ArticlePDF Available

Validity of Self-Evaluation of Ability: A Review and Meta-Analysis

Authors:

Abstract

Reviews 55 studies in which self-evaluations of ability were compared with measures of performance to show a low mean validity coefficient (mean r = .29) with high variability ( SD = .25). A meta-analysis by the procedures of J. E. Hunter et al (1982) calculated sample-size weighted estimates of –- r and SDr and estimated the appropriate adjustments of these values for sampling error and unreliability. Among person variables, high intelligence, high achievement status, and internal locus of control were associated with more accurate evaluations. Much of the variability in the validity coefficients ( R = .64) could be accounted for by 9 specific conditions of measurement, notably (a) the rater's expectation that the self-evaluation would be compared with criterion measures, (b) the rater's previous experience with self-evaluation, (c) instructions guaranteeing anonymity of the self-evaluation, and (d) self-evaluation instructions emphasizing comparison with others. It is hypothesized that conditions increasing self-awareness would increase the validity of self-evaluation. (84 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
... While one could argue that no-one knows their abilities better than the respective people themselves, previous evidence suggests otherwise, with correlations between SAI and psychometric intelligence ranging from r = −0.25 to r = 0.85 (Freund and Kasten 2012). Previous meta-analytic accounts revealed a moderate correlation between both variables (Freund and Kasten 2012;Mabe and West 1982;Zell and Krizan 2014). However, self-assessment accuracy was theorized to be influenced by various aspects related to characteristics of SAI measurement, participants, and test conditions (Freund and Kasten 2012;Zell and Krizan 2014). ...
... While many of these potentially moderating variables have previously been tested (e.g., Freund and Kasten 2012;Mabe and West 1982;Zell and Krizan 2014), their interactions with different analytical approaches in primary or meta-analytical studies have so far not been systematically examined. Study outcomes may vary considerably depending on which variables are analyzed and how these analyses are carried out (Voracek et al. 2019). ...
... To assess the generality of our findings, we used specification curve and combinatorial meta-analyses in our multiverse approach to assess the influence of various analytical choices in terms of which data are analyzed and how this is done. While many potentially moderating variables have previously been tested (e.g., Freund and Kasten 2012;Mabe and West 1982), their interactions with different analytical approaches in primary or meta-analytical studies have so far not been systematically examined. There are many (reasonable) ways in which primary or meta-analytical studies can be designed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Examining the relationship between self-assessed intelligence (SAI) and psychometric intelligence (IQ) is essential for understanding how people's self-evaluations reflect their actual intelligence. Various factors, such as SAI measurement methods, participant characteristics, and testing conditions have been hypothesized to moderate the SAI-IQ link, yet the generality of this association remains unclear. Here, we provide evidence for SAI and IQ associations based on 278 effect sizes from 115 independent samples (N = 36,833) using a multi-level meta-analysis, revealing a moderate positive correlation (r = 0.30; 95% CI [0.27, 0.33]). Multiverse analyses demonstrated remarkable stability of this effect, with most summary effect specifications yielding significant positive correlations (96%), averaging r = 0.32. Notably, ability domain and sample type emerged as significant moderators, with numerical ability showing stronger correlations compared to general cognitive, verbal, and spatial abilities. Importantly, our study found that correlations in student samples were significantly higher than those in general samples. Our findings show a moderate positive association of SAI with IQ, unaffected by participant sex, publication year, administration order, neuroticism, and self-assessment method, yet significantly moderated by ability domain and sample type. Our results illustrate the importance of feedback in educational settings to help students accurately assess their cognitive abilities.
... This decision is motivated by the fact that substantially different correlations with other variables were identified in meta-analyses examining subjective adaptive performance ratings and objective adaptive performance scores (Christian et al., 2017;Stasielowicz, 2019Stasielowicz, , 2020. Furthermore, including objective performance measures addresses the problem of limited validity of self-report measures (Heidemeier & Moser, 2009;Mabe & West, 1982), which studies examining the relationship 7 between focus on opportunities and performance are usually based on. Thus, the present study contributes to solving the problem of the limited utility of findings for organizational and educational processes. ...
... While people's positive perceptions of their occupational future (e.g., opportunities, perceived remaining time) have been linked to better task and contextual performance , the findings are usually based on self-reported performance, which has limited validity (Heidemeier & Moser, 2009;Mabe & West, 1982). Furthermore, research examining the relationship between perceived occupational opportunities and specific performance facets was scarce. ...
Article
Full-text available
People who perceive more opportunities in their occupational future often report better well-being and motivation. A positive correlation with performance has also been reported, but until now, only the relationship with two broad performance dimensions, task and contextual performance, has been examined extensively. Furthermore, performance ratings often rely on self-reports, which can be biased. The present study (N = 258) helps close these research gaps. First, it examines the relationship between focus on opportunities and a specific performance facet (i.e., adaptive performance). Second, performance is assessed both subjectively and objectively. The distinction proved critical. After adjusting for education, focus on opportunities was related to self-reported adaption to change, but there was no substantial evidence that focus on opportunities promotes objective adaptive performance . Thus, it cannot yet be recommended to use focus on opportunities in organizational and educational contexts for selecting and training highly adaptable employees or students.
... Another factor was that educators rated their perceived preknowledge lower than actual pre-knowledge measured (M = 2.4, SD = 0.6 compared to M = 2.8, SD = 1.2). Considering this, and the tendency for people to overestimate their ability, it suggests that it is likely that the pre-knowledge questions used in this study were aimed too low [40,41]. Another factor to consider is the differences in perceived versus actual post-knowledge scores (M = 3.2, SD = 1.4 compared to M = 4, SD = 0.8), suggesting either educators substantially overestimated their knowledge, the questions asked did not capture the breadth of knowledge obtained, or a combination of both. ...
... Another factor to consider is the differences in perceived versus actual post-knowledge scores (M = 3.2, SD = 1.4 compared to M = 4, SD = 0.8), suggesting either educators substantially overestimated their knowledge, the questions asked did not capture the breadth of knowledge obtained, or a combination of both. Given only six pre-knowledge and six post-knowledge questions were asked, and that people tend to overestimate their ability, a combination of both is likely [40,41]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study evaluated the effectiveness of neurodiversity training modules from a mental health literacy program for primary school staff offered over a three-year period (2013–2015), entitled ‘Teaching and Mental Health’. Using archival data, this study aimed to identify how much teachers learned, how confident they were about using this newly acquired knowledge, and how well the training met the teachers’ learning needs. This study also aimed to explore the relationship between knowledge and confidence, and how satisfaction indicators affected this relationship. Key ethical considerations of neurodiversity training and outcome measurement are discussed. A pre-test and post-test survey design was conducted with 99 primary school teachers from the Brisbane region in Australia. Analyses included three paired-samples t-tests, descriptive statistics, a linear regression analysis, and a mediation analysis. Significant increases of knowledge and confidence were found compared to baseline, and mean satisfaction ratings were high. Knowledge was a significant predictor of confidence, and mental health presenter ratings partially mediated this relationship. It is proposed that future delivery of the program should invest in improving presenter effectiveness.
... However, psychological research has shown that personal assessments of one's own level of ability are generally flawed (e.g., Kruger and Dunning 1999), with most studies reporting low correlations between estimated and actual ability (Mabe and West 1982). The problematic nature of self-ratings are recognized in nursing (e.g., Kajander-Unkuri et al. 2015;Liang et al. 2021), and in other healthcare disciplines (e.g., physicians (Davis et al. 2006), psychiatry (Sowden et al. 2017;pharmacy (Gabbard and Romanelli 2021)). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Previous studies have shown that nurses’ spiritual care competence is related to characteristics of personal spirituality, training adequacy, and comfort, confidence, and frequency of provision of spiritual care. However, these studies assumed that all participants understood spiritual care in the same way, and used self-ratings of spiritual care competence, which are problematic. Our previous study found that spiritual care was understood in 4 qualitatively different ways that can be arranged in order of competence. This study aimed to re-examine the relationships between nurse characteristics and spiritual care competence, using spiritual care understanding as a proxy for competence. Methods Data was collected from a convenience sample of nurses who completed an anonymous, online survey. The survey provided qualitative data about what spiritual care means for them. The survey also provided quantitative data regarding nurse characteristics. This study created sub-groups of nurses based on their understanding of spiritual care, and used the quantitative data to construct a profile of nurse characteristics for each sub-group. Kruskal–Wallis statistical tests determined whether nurse characteristics differed across the 4 sub-groups. Results Spiritual care competence was not related to confidence or comfort in providing spiritual care. Relationships with spirituality, training adequacy, and frequency of provision of spiritual care were not linear; i.e., higher competence did not always correspond with higher scores of these characteristics. Significance of results The results raise concerns about the construct validity of using comfort and confidence as estimates of spiritual care competence. That the relationships between competence and spirituality, training adequacy, and frequency of spiritual care provision was not as linear as portrayed in extant literature, suggests that outcomes of training may depend on the type of spiritual care understanding subscribed to by training participants. The findings offer insights about how nurses could achieve high levels of spiritual care performance.
... Second, our data stemmed mainly from participants' self-evaluations. Although self-evaluation is widely used in psychology and management research, self-reports can be influenced by individual subjectivity biases, like social-approval deviation [61]. To enhance the objectivity and accuracy of this research, future researchers might consider co-facilitating other assessments, like peer evaluations. ...
... The relatively recent epidemic of meta-analysis studies (see, for example, Andrews, et a1., 1980;Kazrin, et al , 1979;Kulik, et al , , 1980;Mabe and West, 1982;Resenthal and Rubin, 1978;and Smith and Glass, 1977) is providing the educational researcher with a much-needed quantitative and qualitative synthesis of research findings as well as a multitude of measures of effectiveness of treatments (Glass, 1976;Glass, McGraw and Smith 1981;Hedges, 1982;Hunter, Rosenthal and Rubin 1982; Kraemer and Andrews, 1982;and Schmidt and Jackson, 1982 And only because tradition has such a firm grip on them have they bothered to mention the probability of a Type I Error (a) and then in an implicit form like, "p <.05". ...
Article
A recent meta-analysis on ability grouping by Kulik and Kulik (1982) was used as an example to demonstrate how a researcher might plan for adequate sample sizes and power in future research. Data sufficient to estimate minimum sample sizes and power were gleaned from the meta-analysis study or obtained from its authors. Distributions of harmonic mean sample size and estimated power were displayed and ranges of estimates for each were presented using effect sizes from the meta-analysis with fixed alpha. Although effect sizes were relatively symmetric about .10, power and harmonic mean sample sizes were quite skewed. Recommendations for major professors, re- searchers and journal editors were made to assist in the evaluation and planning of research.
... This finding aligns with previous findings in the fields of psychology (e.g. Dunning, 2011;Mabe and West, 1982) and educational psychology (e.g. Magnus and Peresetsky, 2018;Schl€ osser et al., 2013). ...
Article
Purpose Entrepreneurial self-efficacy (ESE) has a dark side largely ignored in the field of entrepreneurship education. Research in educational psychology indicates that self-efficacy is prone to misjudgment, with novice learners often displaying overconfidence. Furthermore, this misjudgment is gendered; studies suggest that men are more likely to display overconfidence and less likely to correct erroneous self-assessments. However, realistic self-assessments are essential for effective learning strategies, pivotal for performance in the ambiguous entrepreneurial context. Therefore, this study explores whether entrepreneurship education helps mitigate overconfidence, and if this impact varies by gender. Design/methodology/approach Common in educational psychology, but new in the field of entrepreneurship education, a calibration design captures discrepancies between perceived and actual performance. Data from before and after an introductory undergraduate entrepreneurship course ( N = 103) inform descriptive analyses, statistical comparison tests and calibration plots. Findings As expected, nearly all novice students showed significant overconfidence. Curiously, gender difference was only significant at the end of the course, as overconfidence had decreased among female students and increased among male students. Originality/value The paper advocates a more nuanced stance toward ESE, and introduces ESE accuracy as a more fitting measure of entrepreneurial overconfidence. The findings flag the common use of self-perception as a proxy for actual competence, and evoke new research avenues on (gender differences in) learning motivations of aspiring entrepreneurs. Finally, the study shares guidance for entrepreneurship educators on fostering a “healthier” level of self-efficacy for better entrepreneurial learning.
... A meta-analysis by Mabe and West (1982) of 55 studies on the self-evaluation of people's abilities or performance in various fields found that the average correlation between the two was 0.29 (standard deviation, 0.25). When Zell and Krizan (2014) synthesized 22 meta-analyses that measured the correlation between the self-evaluation of ability and actual performance, they found a mean correlation of 0.29 (standard deviation, 0.11). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study proposes the inverse differential information theory, which predicts a positive relationship between misestimation and misplacement, two types of overconfidence. The inverse differential information theory contrasts with the existing theory of differential information, which argues for a negative relationship between these two types of overconfidence. This study shows that these differences arise from opposing perspectives on the accuracy with which individuals assess their own abilities or performance compared to others’. The inverse differential information theory posits that people tend to evaluate others more objectively than they do themselves. A positive relationship between misestimation and misplacement predicts that overestimation and overplacement, as well as underestimation and underplacement, tend to occur together. Analysis using financial literacy data from South Korean adults supports the prediction of the inverse differential information theory. When these two types of overconfidence form a positive relationship, they are expected to have systematically a significant impact on human decision-making and behavior. This study empirically demonstrates that the positive relationship between misestimation and misplacement in financial literacy significantly influences individuals’ financial behavior, specifically in the context of stock market participation experience. The inverse differential information theory requires further empirical validation across various domains, not just in the field of behavioral finance, to establish whether the positive interaction between misestimation and misplacement consistently influences human decision-making and behavior.
... This could condition the ability to generalize the results, and, as mentioned above, it would be appropriate to increase the sample with participants from different contexts. In addition to the sampling limitation, there is the limitation of any survey or self-report research (Karpen, 2018;Mabe & West, 1982), even with validated instruments. ...
Article
Full-text available
The creation of apps with good parameters of applicability and functionality is a complex and highly researched task nowadays, especially if the latest contributions of Neuroeducation and educational technology are taken into account. However, this task will require more attention when apps are created and designed for people with autism, where usability and accessibility are of vital importance. The purpose of this study was to learn the opinion and perception of professionals who work with people with autism on the functionality and applicability of apps specifically for this disorder. Taking a quantitative approach, with a descriptive design, we administered the DPTIC-AUT-Q questionnaire to 159 educators from Granada (Spain) who worked specifically with people with autism. In general, the participants showed a favorable perception of the functionality and applicability of the apps, but highlighted improvements that could be made in their accessibility and how they functioned. We found statistically significant differences according to the age of the participants and the area and educational stage they worked in. This study also underlines the importance of having accessible apps that make it easy for people with autism to use and take advantage of them.
Article
Background Health literacy has evolved from a focus on individual skills to an interactive process influenced by relationships and the health system. Various instruments measure health literacy, developed from different conceptions and often for different measurement purposes. The aim of this study was to compare the properties of four widely used health literacy instruments: Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (TOFHLA), Newest Vital Sign (NVS), European Health Literacy Survey (HLS-EU-Q47), and Health Literacy Questionnaire (HLQ). Methods This was a within-subject study comparing instrument performance. Composite reliability and Cronbach’s alpha was used to measure internal consistency, floor/ceiling effects determined discriminate ability across low-to-high score ranges, and Spearman’s R correlation coefficient was used to assess the relationship between instruments, particularly scales aiming to measure similar constructs. Results Fifty-nine patients consented, with 43 completing all four instruments. Internal consistency was high for all scales (composite reliability range 0.76–0.95). Floor and ceiling effects were observed, with TOFHLA demonstrating the largest ceiling effect (>62) and NVS the only floor effect (18%). Only moderate correlations were found between TOFHLA and NVS (r = 0.60) and between HLS-EU-Q47 and HLQ scales (r ~0.6). Conclusion Our study found low to moderate correlations between the instruments, indicating they measure different constructs of health literacy. Clinicians and researchers should consider the intended measurement purpose and constructs when choosing an instrument. If the purpose of measurement is to understand reading, comprehension, and numeracy skills in individuals and populations, then performance based functional health literacy instruments such as the TOFHLA and NVS will be suitable. However, if the purpose is to generate insights into broader elements of health literacy, including social supports and relationships with health providers, then the HLS-EU and HLQ may be useful. The findings highlight the need for careful instrument selection to ensure meaningful and appropriate data interpretation. As improving population health literacy is a national priority in many countries, it is important that clinicians and researchers understand the measurement differences offered by different instruments to assist them to choose the right instrument for their measurement purpose.
Article
In a study designed to identify the determinants of accurate self-referent behavior, Ss predicted their individual performances on arithmetic and clerical tasks and indicated self-descriptive traits and roles on the Adjective Check List and the Twenty Statements Test. Traits and roles characterizing accurate or inaccurate predictors were then presented to additional Ss before they predicted their scores on the same taks. As hypothesized, the self-ascription of traits and roles associated with baseline accuracy increased predictive accuracy, and inaccurate ascriptions decreased accuracy. The findings are consistent with Mead’s symbolic interactionist approach of self-referent action and suggest that the experimental analysis of behavior may shed further light on the antecedents of accurate self-prediction and self-control.
Article
This study investigated (a) the relationships between college students' semester grade-point averages (SGPA) and their mental ability, reading skills, and three self-predictions of grades, (b) the best predictor of SGPA, (c) the differences between sexes, college status, and mental ability, reading and achievement groups in prediction of grades and changes in predictions, and (d) the relationship of the third prediction with SGPA. For the 151 subjects, most variables studied were significantly related to SGPA. The third prediction was the best predictor of SGPA. Significant differences were found (a) among mental ability, reading and achievement subgroups, (b) over the three predictions and (c) in interaction effects for sexes, college status and achievement subgroups. The third prediction and SGPA were significantly related for all subgroups except for low achievers.
Article
This study is concerned with identifying and measuring the self-made academic predictions of junior college students. Scores on the College Opinion Survey (COS) were related to high school rank (HSR), subsequent first- quarter grades in college (GPA), and scholastic aptitude test scores (MSAT) (ACT). COS scores for junior college students were compared to scores of freshmen in the College of Liberal Arts at the University of Minnesota. Student estimates of relative performance and ability (COSi) were significantly (p≤.01) related to past and future academic performance and aptitude variables. Feelings about the importance of grades (COSj) were not related to scholastic aptitude but were related (p≤.01) to future academic performance. Feelings about the importance of grades (COSa) were related to previous performance only for females.
Article
A preliminary study designed to illustrate the determinants of accurate self-referent behavior was conducted. Ss predicted their individual performances on arithmetic, verbal, and clerical tasks. After the prediction each task was performed for 3 min. Findings were: (1) Ss predicted their verbal task performance with moderate accuracy ( r = .55, p < .01), (2) the arithmetic task was less accurately predicted ( r = .29, p < .05), and (3) the clerical task was not predicted. Ss employed consistent estimates and were consistently accurate across verbal and arithmetic tasks but not over either one of these two tasks and the clerical task. Absolute accuracy of prediction was not correlated across any task pairs. The findings are interpreted as functions of differential access to the symbolic acts required to predict one's own behavior accurately.
Article
Three prediction perspectives are clinical, actuarial, and student. The student self-made prediction would seem to be the most relevant to the development of short term therapeutic programs designed for underachievers. Indeed, the concept of student-made academic predictions can be particularly useful in trying to ascertain those identifiable characteristics that contribute to underachievement. The first objective of this study was to describe a measure of student-made academic predictions. The second objective was to examine the relationships between these predictions and measures of college aptitude and of college freshman grades. Student-made academic predictions were measured using the College Opinion Survey (COS), an adaptation of the Michigan State General Self Concept of Ability to Learn Scale (SCOA). COS is an 8-item inventory including both academic self-acceptance and academic self-evaluation ratings. Individuals are to respond in terms of various academic reference groups. (Information was collected on two samples of college freshman students and on a sample of college sophomore students.) The findings indicate that student-made predictions as measured by the COS are related to college achievement even when differences in college aptitude are controlled.
Article
This paper presents a controlled, and situationally specific, test of a common assertion regarding alienation in modern society. The hypothesis is tested that differences in alienation (i.e., in powerlessness) are associated with differential learning of behavior-relevant information. Comparing patients who differ in their degree of alienation, but who are matched for socio-economic backgrounds and for health and hospital histories, it is shown that the more alienated patients score lower on an objective test of knowledge about tuberculosis. Furthermore, these differences in objective knowledge are reflected in both the staff's description of the patients, and in the patients' attitudes about the information process. In understanding the attitude data, it is shown that the social structure of the ward as well as individual alienation must be taken into account. The relevance of these findings for contemporary viewpoints concerning the significance of alienation is noted.