Content uploaded by Stefan Stieger
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Stefan Stieger
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Reports: Mental & Physical Health
2012, 111, 1, 97-106. © Psychological Reports 2012
DOI 10.2466/09.07.21.PR0.111.4.97-106 ISSN 0033-2941
PERSONALITY DIFFERENCES BETWEEN TATTOOED
AND NON-TATTOOED INDIVIDUALS1
VIREN SWAMI
Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK
Department of Psychology, HELP University College
Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
JAKOB PIETSCHNIG, BIANCA BERTL, INGO W. NADER,
STEFAN STIEGER, AND MARTIN VORACEK
Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods
School of Psychology, University of Vienna
Summary.—This study examined dierences between tattooed and non-tat-
tooed individuals on a range of personality and individual dierence measures. A
community sample of 540 individuals from the southern German-speaking area of
central Europe completed a survey consisting of measures of the Big Five person-
ality factors, Need for Uniqueness, Self-esteem, sensation seeking, Religious and
Spiritual Beliefs, Attitudes Toward Tattoos, tattoo possession, and demographics.
Preliminary analyses showed that 22% of the total sample possessed at least one
tattoo. Further analyses showed that, compared with non-tattooed (n = 420) indi-
viduals, tattooed participants (n = 120) had signicantly higher scores on Extraver-
sion, Experience Seeking, Need for Uniqueness, and held more positive Attitudes
Toward Tattoos, although eect sizes of these group dierences were generally
small- to medium-sized. These results are considered in relation to the contempo-
rary prevalence of tattoos in socioeconomically developed societies.
In many Western, industrialized nations, the art of tattooing has rap-
idly achieved an unprecedented mainstreaming since the early 1990s
(for a review, see Swami & Harris, 2012). For example, several research-
ers have reported that the incidence of tattooing among respondents in
North America and Europe is about 25% (Greif, Hewitt, & Armstrong,
1999; Armstrong, Roberts, Owen, & Koch, 2004; Laumann & Derick, 2006;
Stieger, Pietschnig, Kastner, Voracek, & Swami, 2010) and expected to rise
in the next decade (Anderson, 2006). Scholarly interest in the psycholo-
gy of tattooing is likewise beginning to develop, with growing interest in
motivations for obtaining a tattoo (e.g., Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011), in-
terpersonal perceptions of tattooed individuals (e.g., Swami & Furnham,
2007), and psychological outcomes of obtaining a tattoo (Seiter & Hatch,
2005; Swami, 2011).
By far the largest body of research has focused on behavioral and in-
1Address correspondence to Jakob Pietschnig, Department of Basic Psychological Research
and Research Methods, School of Psychology, University of Vienna, Liebiggasse 5, 1010 Vi-
enna, Austria or e-mail (jakob.pietschnig@univie.ac.at).
V. Swami, et al.
98
dividual dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed respondents.
Broadly speaking, it is argued that the planning, permanence, and pain
involved in tattooing may reect real-world dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals (Tate & Shelton, 2008). For example, some
researchers have reported that tattoo possession is signicantly associated
with being sexually active (e.g., Carroll, Rienburgh, Roberts, & Myhre,
2002; Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2010), becoming sexually ac-
tive at a younger age (Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2005), and
greater willingness to engage in sexual relations in the absence of commit-
ment (Swami, 2012). Other work has shown signicant associations be-
tween tattoo possession and a range of risk-taking behaviors, such as use
of illegal substances (Carroll, et al., 2002; Brooks, Woods, Knight, & Shrier,
2003; Deschesnes, Fines, & Demers, 2006) and gang aliation among ad-
olescents (Roberts & Ryan, 2002; Deschesnes, et al., 2006). Also, a smaller
body of work has examined personality dierences between tattooed and
non-tattooed individuals, although the results of this work have tended
to be equivocal. For example, some studies have reported that tattooed
individuals have higher scores on measures of extraversion and related
traits, such as sensation seeking (Roberti, Storch, & Bravata, 2004; Stirn,
Hinz, & Brahler, 2006; Wohlrab, Stahl, Rammseyer, & Kappeler, 2007; Swa-
mi, 2012), but other studies yielded statistically non-signicant ndings
(Forbes, 2001; Tate & Shelton, 2008). In a similar vein, some researchers
have reported that tattooed individuals have lower scores on measures on
conscientiousness and agreeableness (Tate & Shelton, 2008), but the latter
association has not been found in at least one further study (Swami, 2012).
Related research has suggested that tattooed individuals have signi-
cantly higher Need for Uniqueness and distinctive appearance investment
compared with non-tattooed individuals (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006; Tate
& Shelton, 2008; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011; Swami, 2012). Nevertheless,
it should be noted that where signicant dierences have been reported,
these have tended to be small and the extent to which such dierences
have real-world implications has been questioned (Tate & Shelton, 2008).
One reason for the equivocal nature of these ndings may be related to
problems with sampling: with few exceptions, in most studies in which
dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals have been ex-
amined have relied on college samples, which have low generalizability.
The present study sought to examine whether there are dierences
between non-tattooed and tattooed individuals recruited from the com-
munity in the southern German-speaking region of central Europe. Pre-
vious work has suggested that the prevalence of tattoos in a sample from
this region is relatively high (about 15%; Stieger, et al., 2010), which makes
this a useful sample to investigate. Specically, in the present study, tat-
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 99
tooed and non-tattooed individuals were compared on their Big Five per-
sonality traits, Need for Uniqueness, sensation seeking, Self-esteem, Spiri-
tual Beliefs, and Attitudes Toward Tattoos.
Method
Participants
In all, 540 individuals (54.4% women; age M = 31.4 yr., SD = 13.7) from
the community participated in the present study. Participants were, for
the most part, from Austria (87.4%), whereas 9.1% reported being German
or of another nationality (3.5%). In terms of marital status, 37.2% were
single, more than half of the participants were in a relationship (38.1%) or
married (21.1%), 3.0% were divorced, and 0.6% widowed. As to highest
educational qualication, 5.6% had completed primary education, 21.9%
had nished their apprenticeship or vocational schooling, 51.0% reported
having completed secondary education, 18.6% had a university degree,
and 3.0% reported some other kind of highest educational qualication.
Materials
Big Five Factors of personality.—The Mini–IPIP (Goldberg, 1999) was
used in order to assess the Big Five Factors of personality. This scale com-
prises ve subscales (Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience,
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) and requires participants to rate
the extent of their agreement with each of 20 statements using a rating
scale with anchors of 1: Strongly disagree and 5: Strongly agree.
Need for uniqueness.—Participants’ need to stand out from others was
measured using the Need for Uniqueness Scale (Snyder & Fromkin, 1977).
On this scale, participants were asked to indicate their agreement on each
of 32 items on a 5-point scale with anchors 1: Strongly disagree and 5:
Strongly agree.
Sensation seeking.—Version V of the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS–V;
Zuckerman, Eysenck, & Eysenck, 1978; German form: Beauducel, Strobel,
& Brocke, 2003) was used to assess participants on the ve domains com-
prising this construct (Boredom Susceptibility, Disinhibition, Experience
Seeking, Thrill and Adventure Seeking). Participants rated a total of 40
items for agreement on a 6-point scale with anchors of 0 and 5: Totally
agree, 1 and 4: Fairly agree, 2 and 3: Somewhat agree (see Voracek, Tran,
& Dressler, 2010, for utilization of this reliability-increasing response for-
mat).
Self-esteem.—The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg,
1965; German form: von Collani & Herzberg, 2003) is the most widely
used measure of Self-esteem. Participants rated its total of 10 items for
agreement on a 4-point Likert-type scale with anchors of 1: Strongly dis-
agree and 4: Strongly agree.
V. Swami, et al.
100
Religious and spiritual beliefs.—The Systems of Beliefs Scale (SBI–15R;
Holland, Kash, Passik, Gronert, Sison, Lederberg, et al., 1998; German
form: Albani, Bailer, Blaser, Geyer, Brähler, & Grulke, 2002) was given to
assess participants’ attitudes towards their Religious and Spiritual Beliefs.
On this scale, 15 items were rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale with an-
chors of 1: Strongly disagree and 4: Strongly agree, yielding two subscales,
namely Attitudes Towards Beliefs and Practices and Attitudes Towards
Social Support.
General attitudes toward tattoos.—Participants’ were asked to rate their
general Attitudes Toward Tattoos on a semantic dierential comprising
eight pairs of characteristics on a 7-point scale with the rst word of each
item reecting a more positive attitude towards tattoos (beautiful vs ugly,
unique vs common, conformant vs rebellious, status symbol vs stigmati-
zation, socially acceptable vs socially unacceptable, makes sense vs does
not make sense, good vs bad, aggressive vs not aggressive). Then, an over-
all score was computed by taking the mean of all eight items, with higher
scores reecting more negative Attitudes Toward Tattoos. Reliabilities for
the used scales in the present sample are provided in the last column of
Table 1.
Demographics.—Participants were asked to provide basic demograph-
ic information, namely sex, age, nationality, marital status, highest edu-
cational qualication, and religious aliation. Additionally, participants
self-reported whether they were tattooed or not (see Stieger, et al., 2010).
When they reported having at least one tattoo, they were asked to state
how many tattoos in total they had across several body locations. For the
present purposes, we computed the sum of tattoos for each participant.
Procedure
For two scales (MINI–IPIP, Need for Uniqueness) German forms were
not available, so the scales were translated into German by the second
to last authors using the parallel-blind technique (Behling & Law, 2000).
Using this approach, the authors translated the scales independently of
each other, subsequently comparing their respective translations. Disput-
ed translations were resolved through discussion. Participants were re-
cruited opportunistically by research assistants through their personal
contacts. Participation was anonymous, voluntary, and without remuner-
ation. After completion of the questionnaire, participants were verbally
debriefed.
Results
Prevalence of Tattoos
Overall, 140 of the total sample of 540 (i.e., 22.2%) participants report-
ed having at least one tattoo. Among tattooed participants, the number of
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 101
TABLE 1
descRiptive statistics and Results of Between-gRoups coMpaRisons foR all study vaRiaBles
Variable Nontattooed Tattooed Women Men Omnibus
Test
Main Eect of
Tattoo
Main Eect
of Sex
Interaction Internal
Consis-
tency,
Cronbach
α
MSD MSD MSD MSD F R²Fηp
2Fηp
2Fηp
2
Extraversion 3.38 0.80 3.62 0.70 3.48 0.77 3.38 0.80 4.38†.02 10.15†.02 0.13 < .01 1.81 < .01 .75
Agreeableness 4.02 0.70 4.03 0.68 4.22 0.61 3.79 0.71 18.81‡.09 0.04 < .01 36.19‡.06 0.12 < .01 .76
Conscientiousness 3.57 0.74 3.51 0.81 3.59 0.74 3.51 0.78 0.81 < .01 0.62 < .01 1.66 < .01 0.08 < .01 .60
Neuroticism 2.76 0.80 2.66 0.76 2.88 0.76 2.56 0.80 9.60‡.05 1.13 < .01 8.19†.02 3.98 < .01 .67
Openness 3.75 0.76 3.78 0.69 3.68 0.76 3.84 0.73 2.86* .01 0.09 < .01 1.97 < .01 1.74 < .01 .69
Self-esteem 3.34 0.53 3.46 0.45 3.32 0.55 3.42 0.47 3.70* .02 5.58* .01 2.95 < .01 0.13 < .01 .87
Need for Uniqueness 3.18 0.41 3.44 0.37 3.19 0.43 3.30 0.39 17.83‡.09 37.29‡.07 5.48* .01 1.50 < .01 .79
Thrill and Adventure
Seeking
3.85 1.20 4.06 1.13 3.64 1.11 4.20 1.21 11.75‡.06 4.40* < .01 27.23‡.05 1.22 < .01 .86
Experience Seeking 3.84 0.82 4.17 0.77 3.92 0.80 3.92 0.85 5.00†.02 14.37‡.03 0.01 < .01 0.03 < .01 .68
Disinhibition 3.48 0.89 3.69 0.84 3.37 0.85 3.71 0.88 10.10‡.05 7.79†.02 23.55‡.04 3.25 < .01 .77
Boredom Susceptibility 3.09 0.69 3.23 0.71 2.98 0.68 3.30 0.68 11.11‡.05 4.63* .01 20.88‡.04 0.01 < .01 .57
Religious Beliefs and
Practices
1.92 0.84 1.70 0.75 1.95 0.83 1.78 0.80 4.74†.02 5.82* .01 1.85 < .01 1.56 < .01 .94
Social Support 1.71 0.72 1.52 0.62 1.62 0.67 1.72 0.75 5.33†.02 4.67* .01 7.54†.01 6.59* .01 .82
General Attitudes
Toward Tattoos
4.30 0.91 3.25 0.69 4.09 0.94 4.05 1.00 101.12‡.20 139.12‡.21 1.66 < .01 0.56 < .01 .73
Note.—R² = corrected explained variance. *p < .05. †p < .01. ‡p < .001.
V. Swami, et al.
102
tattoos per individual ranged from one to 15, with a mean of 2.7 (SD = 3,
Mdn = 1.5, Mode = 1). There was no statistically signicant dierence be-
tween tattooed women (n = 70) and men (n = 50; 23.8 vs 20.3% respective-
ly reported having at least one tattoo) in the number of tattoos they pos-
sessed (Mann-Whitney U11 0 = 1,390.5, Z = −0.53, p = .60; 209 vs 262 tattoos,
respectively).
Preliminary Between-groups Comparisons
Preliminary analyses showed that there were no statistically signi-
cant dierences in mean age between tattooed and non-tattooed partici-
pants (t538 = 0.59, p = .55, d = 0.05). In addition, there were no signicant dif-
ferences between tattooed (n = 120) and non-tattooed groups (n = 420) in
the distribution of sex, nationality, marital status, and highest educational
qualication (Table 2).
Between-groups Comparisons
Next, it was examined whether there were dierences on key vari-
ables between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals. To do so, 2 (tattoo
status: tattooed versus non-tattooed) × 2 (sex: women versus men) analy-
ses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted with the Big Five personal-
ity factors, Self-esteem, Need for Uniqueness, the sensation seeking sub-
scales, the systems of beliefs subscales, and the general attitude towards
tattoo score, respectively, as dependent variables. To control for Type
I error, a Bonferroni correction was applied, such that alpha was set at
.05/14 = .004.
Means and standard deviations and the results of all ANOVAs are
reported in Table 1. As can be seen, none of the tattoo status by sex in-
teractions reached statistical signicance following Bonferroni correction,
which led us to examine the main eects. Following Bonferroni correc-
tions, there were signicant main eects of tattoo status on Extraversion,
Need for Uniqueness, the Experience Seeking subscale of the SSS–V, and
the general attitude toward tattoos. In the rst three cases, tattooed indi-
viduals had higher scores than non-tattooed individuals, whereas in the
latter case, tattooed individuals had more positive Attitudes Toward Tat-
toos than non-tattooed individuals. There were also statistically signi-
TABLE 2
coMpaRisons Between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals
χ2df p φ
Sex 0.94 1.33 < .001
Nationality 1.27 2 .53 < .001
Marital status 2.17 4 .70 .009
Highest educational qualication 8.79 5 .12 .016
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 103
cant main eects of sex on Agreeableness and three of the four subscales
of the SSS–V.
discussion
The present results showed that there were a number of statistical-
ly signicant dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals
in terms of their personality and individual-dierence traits. Specically,
those tattooed had signicantly higher scores than non-tattooed individ-
uals on Extraversion, Need for Uniqueness, and the Experience Seeking
subscale of the SSS–V. In addition, compared with non-tattooed individ-
uals, tattooed participants also had signicantly more positive Attitudes
Toward Tattoos, which is unsurprising. These results are discussed in
greater detail below.
In terms of the Big Five, present results corroborate previous work
showing that tattooed individuals scored more extraverted than non-
tattooed individuals (e.g., Stirn, et al., 2006; Wohlrab, et al., 2007; Swami,
2012). Moreover, tattooed participants had higher scores on a subscale of
sensation seeking, which was related to the trait of Extraversion (see Ey-
senck, 1990). Importantly, however, there were no statistically signicant
dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals on the remain-
ing Big Five factors, which stands in contrast to some previous work (e.g.,
Tate & Shelton, 2008). Overall, then, converging evidence appears to sug-
gest that tattooed individuals score more extraverted than non-tattooed
individuals, with this contrast being driven by their sensation seeking
scores.
The present results also corroborate previous work showing that tat-
tooed individuals have higher Need for Uniqueness than non-tattooed in-
dividuals (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011; Swa-
mi, 2012). In general, these results support the idea that tattoos are now
used as a means of self-expression or construction of identity. Specically,
in societies in which the body is increasingly commodied, tattoos may
aord some individuals an opportunity to mark the self as being dier-
ent and thereby attain improved self-perceptions of uniqueness (Swami,
2011). Put dierently, tattoos may now be an important means through
which individuals can develop unique identities, particularly in the ap-
pearance domain (Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011).
Although the results suggest that there are statistically signicant dif-
ferences between tattooed and non-tattooed participants in a number of
examined variables, it should be noted that the eect sizes of observed dif-
ferences were generally small. Specically, eect sizes of dierences were
smallest for Extraversion and Experience seeking (ηp
2 = .02–.03), slightly
larger for Need for Uniqueness (ηp
2 = .07), and largest for Attitudes To-
ward Tattoos (ηp
2 = .21). As discussed by Tate and Shelton (2008), real-
V. Swami, et al.
104
world implications of such and mostly small dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals may well be negligible. Moreover, present
results suggest no statistically signicant dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals on a number of other variables, including
Self-esteem and Religious and Spiritual Beliefs. However, small eect siz-
es of dierences may well be due to secular trends in types of body orna-
mentation and rising prevalence rates of tattoos (e.g., Armstrong & Kelly,
2001; Stieger, et al., 2010) thus reecting increased acceptance of tattoos in
the general population and, in consequence, levelling o of dierences.
The main limitation of this study is that, although we were able to
avoid relying on college samples, it is possible the recruitment technique
introduced sampling biases. In any case, the sample should not be consid-
ered representative of the wider population and should not be extrapo-
lated to other German-speaking regions of Europe. In addition, a limited
number of variables were included in this study. So it is possible that this
limited range masks other, more important dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals. Given the cross-sectional nature of the de-
sign, inferring causation in the present instance (e.g., to say that extrav-
ertive scorers will be more likely to obtain a tattoo) is not possible.
These limitations notwithstanding, the present results suggest there
are statistically signicant, albeit for the most part small, dierences be-
tween tattooed and non-tattooed individuals on a small number of per-
sonality and individual dierence traits. Given the rapid mainstreaming
of tattoos in socioeconomically developed societies (including almost a
quarter of participants in the present study), ndings such as those report-
ed here may prove useful for scholars seeking to understand motivations
for obtaining a tattoo. That is, an individual dierences approach may
help scholars identify certain proles of individuals who are more likely
to obtain a tattoo, which can only add to research in which motivational
aspects of tattooing have been approached from a sociological perspective
(e.g., Kosut, 2006).
REFERENCES
alBani, c., BaileR, h., BlaseR, g., geyeR, M., BRähleR, e., & gRulke, n. (2002) [Reli-
gious and spiritual beliefs: validation of the German version of the “Systems of
Beliefs Inventory” (SBI-15R-D) by Holland, et al. in a population-based sample].
Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie, 52, 306-313. [in German]
andeRson, R. R. (2006) Commentary: tattoos and body piercing. Journal of the American
Academy of Dermatology, 55, 422.
aRMstRong, M. l., & kelly, l. (2001) Tattooing, body piercing, and branding are on
the rise: perspectives for school nurses. The Journal of School Nursing, 17, 12-23.
aRMstRong, M. l., RoBeRts, a. e., owen, d. c., & koch, J. R. (2004) Toward building
a composite of college student inuences with body art. Issues in Comprehensive
Pediatric Nursing, 27, 277-295.
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 105
Beauducel, a., stRoBel, a., & BRocke, B. (2003) [Psychometric properties and norms of
a German version of the sensation seeking scales, form V]. Diagnostica, 49, 61-72.
[in German]
Behling, o., & law, k. s. (2000) Translating questionnaires and other research instruments:
problems and solutions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
BRooks, t. l., woods, e. R., knight, J. R., & shRieR, l. a. (2003) Body modication and
substance use in adolescents: is there a link? Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 44-49.
caRRoll, s. t., RiffenBuRgh, R. h., RoBeRts, t. a., & MyhRe, e. B. (2002) Tattoos and
body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk-taking behaviors. Pediatrics, 109,
1021-1027.
deschesnes, M., fines, p., & deMeRs, s. (2006) Are tattooing and body piercing indica-
tors of risk-taking behaviours among high school students? Journal of Adolescence,
29, 379-393.
eysenck, h. J. (1990) Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Hand-
book of personality: theory and research. New York: Guilford. Pp. 244-276.
foRBes, g. B. (2001) College students with tattoos and piercings: motives, family ex-
periences, personality factors, and perception by others. Psychological Reports, 89,
774-796.
goldBeRg, l. R. (1999) A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality inventory
measuring the lower-level facets of several ve-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I.
J. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality Psychology in Europe. Vol. 7.
Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg Univer. Press. Pp. 7-28.
gReif, J., hewitt, w., & aRMstRong, M. l. (1999) Tattooing and body piercing: body art
practices among college students. Clinical Nursing Research, 8, 368-385.
holland, J. c., kash, k. M., passik, s., gRoneRt, M. k., sison, a., ledeRBeRg, M., Russak, s.
M., BaideR, l., & fox, B. (1998) A brief Spiritual Beliefs Inventory for use in qual-
ity of life research in life-threatening illness. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 460-469.
koch, J. R., RoBeRts, a. e., aRMstRong, M. l., & owen, d. c. (2005) College students,
tattoos, and sexual activity. Psychological Reports, 97, 887-890.
koch, J. R., RoBeRts, a. e., aRMstRong, M. l., & owen, d. c. (2010) Body art, deviance,
and American college students. Social Science Journal, 47, 151-161.
kosut, M. (2006) An ironic fad: the commodication and consumption of tattoos. The
Journal of Popular Culture, 39, 1035-1048.
lauMann, a. e., & deRick, a. J. (2006) Tattoos and body piercings in the United States:
a national data set. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 55, 413-421.
RoBeRti, J. w., stoRch, e. a., & BRavata, e. a. (2004) Sensation seeking, exposure to
psychological stressors, and body modications in a college population. Personal-
ity and Individual Dierences, 37, 1167-1177.
RoBeRts, t. a., & Ryan, s. a. (2002) Tattooing and high-risk behavior in adolescents.
Pediatrics, 110, 1058-1063.
RosenBeRg, M. (1965) Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Univer. Press.
seiteR, J. s., & hatch, s. (2005) Eect of tattoos on perceptions of credibility and at-
tractiveness. Psychological Reports, 96, 1113-1120.
snydeR, c. R., & fRoMkin, h. l. (1977) Abnormality as a positive characteristic: the
development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 86, 518-527.
V. Swami, et al.
106
stiegeR, s., pietschnig, J., kastneR, c. k., voRacek, M., & swaMi, v. (2010) Prevalence
and acceptance of tattoos and piercings: a survey of young adults from the south-
ern German-speaking area of southern Europe. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110,
1065-1074.
stiRn, a., hinz, a., & BRahleR, e. (2006) Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing
in Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking
among tattooed and body-pierced individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
60, 531-534.
swaMi, v. (2011) Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance anxi-
ety and dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Body Image, 8,
237-244.
swaMi, v. (2012) Written on the body? An examination of the personalities and indi-
vidual dierences of British adults who do and do not obtain a rst tattoo. Un-
published manuscript.
swaMi, v., & fuRnhaM, a. (2007) Unattractive, promiscuous, and heavy drinkers: per-
ceptions of women with tattoos. Body Image, 4, 343-352.
swaMi, v., & haRRis, a. s. (2012) Body art (tattoos and piercings). In T. Cash (Ed.), En-
cylopedia of human appearance and body image. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
tate, J. c., & shelton, B. l. (2008) Personality correlates of tattooing and body pierc-
ing in a college sample: the kids are alright. Personality and Individual Dierences,
45, 281-285.
tiggeMann, M., & goldeR, f. (2006) Tattooing: an expression of uniqueness in the ap-
pearance domain. Body Image, 3, 309-315.
tiggeMann, M., & hopkins, s. (2011) Tattoos and piercings: bodily expressions of
uniqueness? Body Image, 8, 245-250.
von collani, g., & heRzBeRg, p. y. (2003) [A revised version of the German adapta-
tion of Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale]. Zeitschrift für Dierentielle und Diagnostische
Psychologie, 24, 3-7. [in German]
voRacek, M., tRan, u. s., & dRessleR, s. g. (2010) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sensation
seeking: new data and meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Dierences, 48, 72-
77.
wohlRaB, s., stahl, J., RaMMsayeR, t., & kappeleR, p. M. (2007) Dierences in personal-
ity characteristics between body modied and nonmodied individuals and pos-
sible evolutionary implications. European Journal of Personality, 21, 931-951.
zuckeRMan, M., eysenck, s. B. g., & eysenck, h. J. (1978) Sensation seeking in England
and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 46, 139-149.
Accepted July 18, 2012.