ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

This study examined differences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals on a range of personality and individual difference measures. A community sample of 540 individuals from the southern German-speaking area of central Europe completed a survey consisting of measures of the Big Five personality factors, Need for Uniqueness, Self-esteem, sensation seeking, Religious and Spiritual Beliefs, Attitudes Toward Tattoos, tattoo possession, and demographics. Preliminary analyses showed that 22% of the total sample possessed at least one tattoo. Further analyses showed that, compared with non-tattooed (n = 420) individuals, tattooed participants (n = 120) had significantly higher scores on Extraversion, Experience Seeking, Need for Uniqueness, and held more positive Attitudes Toward Tattoos, although effect sizes of these group differences were generally small- to medium-sized. These results are considered in relation to the contemporary prevalence of tattoos in socioeconomically developed societies.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Reports: Mental & Physical Health
2012, 111, 1, 97-106. © Psychological Reports 2012
DOI 10.2466/09.07.21.PR0.111.4.97-106 ISSN 0033-2941
PERSONALITY DIFFERENCES BETWEEN TATTOOED
AND NON-TATTOOED INDIVIDUALS1
VIREN SWAMI
Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, UK
Department of Psychology, HELP University College
Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
JAKOB PIETSCHNIG, BIANCA BERTL, INGO W. NADER,
STEFAN STIEGER, AND MARTIN VORACEK
Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods
School of Psychology, University of Vienna
Summary.—This study examined dierences between tattooed and non-tat-
tooed individuals on a range of personality and individual dierence measures. A
community sample of 540 individuals from the southern German-speaking area of
central Europe completed a survey consisting of measures of the Big Five person-
ality factors, Need for Uniqueness, Self-esteem, sensation seeking, Religious and
Spiritual Beliefs, Attitudes Toward Tattoos, tattoo possession, and demographics.
Preliminary analyses showed that 22% of the total sample possessed at least one
tattoo. Further analyses showed that, compared with non-tattooed (n = 420) indi-
viduals, tattooed participants (n = 120) had signicantly higher scores on Extraver-
sion, Experience Seeking, Need for Uniqueness, and held more positive Attitudes
Toward Tattoos, although eect sizes of these group dierences were generally
small- to medium-sized. These results are considered in relation to the contempo-
rary prevalence of tattoos in socioeconomically developed societies.
In many Western, industrialized nations, the art of tattooing has rap-
idly achieved an unprecedented mainstreaming since the early 1990s
(for a review, see Swami & Harris, 2012). For example, several research-
ers have reported that the incidence of tattooing among respondents in
North America and Europe is about 25% (Greif, Hewitt, & Armstrong,
1999; Armstrong, Roberts, Owen, & Koch, 2004; Laumann & Derick, 2006;
Stieger, Pietschnig, Kastner, Voracek, & Swami, 2010) and expected to rise
in the next decade (Anderson, 2006). Scholarly interest in the psycholo-
gy of tattooing is likewise beginning to develop, with growing interest in
motivations for obtaining a tattoo (e.g., Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011), in-
terpersonal perceptions of tattooed individuals (e.g., Swami & Furnham,
2007), and psychological outcomes of obtaining a tattoo (Seiter & Hatch,
2005; Swami, 2011).
By far the largest body of research has focused on behavioral and in-
1Address correspondence to Jakob Pietschnig, Department of Basic Psychological Research
and Research Methods, School of Psychology, University of Vienna, Liebiggasse 5, 1010 Vi-
enna, Austria or e-mail (jakob.pietschnig@univie.ac.at).
V. Swami, et al.
98
dividual dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed respondents.
Broadly speaking, it is argued that the planning, permanence, and pain
involved in tattooing may reect real-world dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals (Tate & Shelton, 2008). For example, some
researchers have reported that tattoo possession is signicantly associated
with being sexually active (e.g., Carroll, Rienburgh, Roberts, & Myhre,
2002; Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2010), becoming sexually ac-
tive at a younger age (Koch, Roberts, Armstrong, & Owen, 2005), and
greater willingness to engage in sexual relations in the absence of commit-
ment (Swami, 2012). Other work has shown signicant associations be-
tween tattoo possession and a range of risk-taking behaviors, such as use
of illegal substances (Carroll, et al., 2002; Brooks, Woods, Knight, & Shrier,
2003; Deschesnes, Fines, & Demers, 2006) and gang aliation among ad-
olescents (Roberts & Ryan, 2002; Deschesnes, et al., 2006). Also, a smaller
body of work has examined personality dierences between tattooed and
non-tattooed individuals, although the results of this work have tended
to be equivocal. For example, some studies have reported that tattooed
individuals have higher scores on measures of extraversion and related
traits, such as sensation seeking (Roberti, Storch, & Bravata, 2004; Stirn,
Hinz, & Brahler, 2006; Wohlrab, Stahl, Rammseyer, & Kappeler, 2007; Swa-
mi, 2012), but other studies yielded statistically non-signicant ndings
(Forbes, 2001; Tate & Shelton, 2008). In a similar vein, some researchers
have reported that tattooed individuals have lower scores on measures on
conscientiousness and agreeableness (Tate & Shelton, 2008), but the latter
association has not been found in at least one further study (Swami, 2012).
Related research has suggested that tattooed individuals have signi-
cantly higher Need for Uniqueness and distinctive appearance investment
compared with non-tattooed individuals (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006; Tate
& Shelton, 2008; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011; Swami, 2012). Nevertheless,
it should be noted that where signicant dierences have been reported,
these have tended to be small and the extent to which such dierences
have real-world implications has been questioned (Tate & Shelton, 2008).
One reason for the equivocal nature of these ndings may be related to
problems with sampling: with few exceptions, in most studies in which
dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals have been ex-
amined have relied on college samples, which have low generalizability.
The present study sought to examine whether there are dierences
between non-tattooed and tattooed individuals recruited from the com-
munity in the southern German-speaking region of central Europe. Pre-
vious work has suggested that the prevalence of tattoos in a sample from
this region is relatively high (about 15%; Stieger, et al., 2010), which makes
this a useful sample to investigate. Specically, in the present study, tat-
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 99
tooed and non-tattooed individuals were compared on their Big Five per-
sonality traits, Need for Uniqueness, sensation seeking, Self-esteem, Spiri-
tual Beliefs, and Attitudes Toward Tattoos.
Method
Participants
In all, 540 individuals (54.4% women; age M = 31.4 yr., SD = 13.7) from
the community participated in the present study. Participants were, for
the most part, from Austria (87.4%), whereas 9.1% reported being German
or of another nationality (3.5%). In terms of marital status, 37.2% were
single, more than half of the participants were in a relationship (38.1%) or
married (21.1%), 3.0% were divorced, and 0.6% widowed. As to highest
educational qualication, 5.6% had completed primary education, 21.9%
had nished their apprenticeship or vocational schooling, 51.0% reported
having completed secondary education, 18.6% had a university degree,
and 3.0% reported some other kind of highest educational qualication.
Materials
Big Five Factors of personality.—The Mini–IPIP (Goldberg, 1999) was
used in order to assess the Big Five Factors of personality. This scale com-
prises ve subscales (Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience,
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) and requires participants to rate
the extent of their agreement with each of 20 statements using a rating
scale with anchors of 1: Strongly disagree and 5: Strongly agree.
Need for uniqueness.—Participants’ need to stand out from others was
measured using the Need for Uniqueness Scale (Snyder & Fromkin, 1977).
On this scale, participants were asked to indicate their agreement on each
of 32 items on a 5-point scale with anchors 1: Strongly disagree and 5:
Strongly agree.
Sensation seeking.—Version V of the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS–V;
Zuckerman, Eysenck, & Eysenck, 1978; German form: Beauducel, Strobel,
& Brocke, 2003) was used to assess participants on the ve domains com-
prising this construct (Boredom Susceptibility, Disinhibition, Experience
Seeking, Thrill and Adventure Seeking). Participants rated a total of 40
items for agreement on a 6-point scale with anchors of 0 and 5: Totally
agree, 1 and 4: Fairly agree, 2 and 3: Somewhat agree (see Voracek, Tran,
& Dressler, 2010, for utilization of this reliability-increasing response for-
mat).
Self-esteem.—The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg,
1965; German form: von Collani & Herzberg, 2003) is the most widely
used measure of Self-esteem. Participants rated its total of 10 items for
agreement on a 4-point Likert-type scale with anchors of 1: Strongly dis-
agree and 4: Strongly agree.
V. Swami, et al.
100
Religious and spiritual beliefs.—The Systems of Beliefs Scale (SBI–15R;
Holland, Kash, Passik, Gronert, Sison, Lederberg, et al., 1998; German
form: Albani, Bailer, Blaser, Geyer, Brähler, & Grulke, 2002) was given to
assess participants’ attitudes towards their Religious and Spiritual Beliefs.
On this scale, 15 items were rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale with an-
chors of 1: Strongly disagree and 4: Strongly agree, yielding two subscales,
namely Attitudes Towards Beliefs and Practices and Attitudes Towards
Social Support.
General attitudes toward tattoos.—Participants’ were asked to rate their
general Attitudes Toward Tattoos on a semantic dierential comprising
eight pairs of characteristics on a 7-point scale with the rst word of each
item reecting a more positive attitude towards tattoos (beautiful vs ugly,
unique vs common, conformant vs rebellious, status symbol vs stigmati-
zation, socially acceptable vs socially unacceptable, makes sense vs does
not make sense, good vs bad, aggressive vs not aggressive). Then, an over-
all score was computed by taking the mean of all eight items, with higher
scores reecting more negative Attitudes Toward Tattoos. Reliabilities for
the used scales in the present sample are provided in the last column of
Table 1.
Demographics.—Participants were asked to provide basic demograph-
ic information, namely sex, age, nationality, marital status, highest edu-
cational qualication, and religious aliation. Additionally, participants
self-reported whether they were tattooed or not (see Stieger, et al., 2010).
When they reported having at least one tattoo, they were asked to state
how many tattoos in total they had across several body locations. For the
present purposes, we computed the sum of tattoos for each participant.
Procedure
For two scales (MINI–IPIP, Need for Uniqueness) German forms were
not available, so the scales were translated into German by the second
to last authors using the parallel-blind technique (Behling & Law, 2000).
Using this approach, the authors translated the scales independently of
each other, subsequently comparing their respective translations. Disput-
ed translations were resolved through discussion. Participants were re-
cruited opportunistically by research assistants through their personal
contacts. Participation was anonymous, voluntary, and without remuner-
ation. After completion of the questionnaire, participants were verbally
debriefed.
Results
Prevalence of Tattoos
Overall, 140 of the total sample of 540 (i.e., 22.2%) participants report-
ed having at least one tattoo. Among tattooed participants, the number of
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 101
TABLE 1
descRiptive statistics and Results of Between-gRoups coMpaRisons foR all study vaRiaBles
Variable Nontattooed Tattooed Women Men Omnibus
Test
Main Eect of
Tattoo
Main Eect
of Sex
Interaction Internal
Consis-
tency,
Cronbach
α
MSD MSD MSD MSD F R²Fηp
2Fηp
2Fηp
2
Extraversion 3.38 0.80 3.62 0.70 3.48 0.77 3.38 0.80 4.38.02 10.15.02 0.13 < .01 1.81 < .01 .75
Agreeableness 4.02 0.70 4.03 0.68 4.22 0.61 3.79 0.71 18.81.09 0.04 < .01 36.19.06 0.12 < .01 .76
Conscientiousness 3.57 0.74 3.51 0.81 3.59 0.74 3.51 0.78 0.81 < .01 0.62 < .01 1.66 < .01 0.08 < .01 .60
Neuroticism 2.76 0.80 2.66 0.76 2.88 0.76 2.56 0.80 9.60.05 1.13 < .01 8.19.02 3.98 < .01 .67
Openness 3.75 0.76 3.78 0.69 3.68 0.76 3.84 0.73 2.86* .01 0.09 < .01 1.97 < .01 1.74 < .01 .69
Self-esteem 3.34 0.53 3.46 0.45 3.32 0.55 3.42 0.47 3.70* .02 5.58* .01 2.95 < .01 0.13 < .01 .87
Need for Uniqueness 3.18 0.41 3.44 0.37 3.19 0.43 3.30 0.39 17.83.09 37.29.07 5.48* .01 1.50 < .01 .79
Thrill and Adventure
Seeking
3.85 1.20 4.06 1.13 3.64 1.11 4.20 1.21 11.75.06 4.40* < .01 27.23.05 1.22 < .01 .86
Experience Seeking 3.84 0.82 4.17 0.77 3.92 0.80 3.92 0.85 5.00.02 14.37.03 0.01 < .01 0.03 < .01 .68
Disinhibition 3.48 0.89 3.69 0.84 3.37 0.85 3.71 0.88 10.10.05 7.79.02 23.55.04 3.25 < .01 .77
Boredom Susceptibility 3.09 0.69 3.23 0.71 2.98 0.68 3.30 0.68 11.11.05 4.63* .01 20.88.04 0.01 < .01 .57
Religious Beliefs and
Practices
1.92 0.84 1.70 0.75 1.95 0.83 1.78 0.80 4.74.02 5.82* .01 1.85 < .01 1.56 < .01 .94
Social Support 1.71 0.72 1.52 0.62 1.62 0.67 1.72 0.75 5.33.02 4.67* .01 7.54.01 6.59* .01 .82
General Attitudes
Toward Tattoos
4.30 0.91 3.25 0.69 4.09 0.94 4.05 1.00 101.12.20 139.12.21 1.66 < .01 0.56 < .01 .73
Note.—R² = corrected explained variance. *p < .05. p < .01. p < .001.
V. Swami, et al.
102
tattoos per individual ranged from one to 15, with a mean of 2.7 (SD = 3,
Mdn = 1.5, Mode = 1). There was no statistically signicant dierence be-
tween tattooed women (n = 70) and men (n = 50; 23.8 vs 20.3% respective-
ly reported having at least one tattoo) in the number of tattoos they pos-
sessed (Mann-Whitney U11 0 = 1,390.5, Z = −0.53, p = .60; 209 vs 262 tattoos,
respectively).
Preliminary Between-groups Comparisons
Preliminary analyses showed that there were no statistically signi-
cant dierences in mean age between tattooed and non-tattooed partici-
pants (t538 = 0.59, p = .55, d = 0.05). In addition, there were no signicant dif-
ferences between tattooed (n = 120) and non-tattooed groups (n = 420) in
the distribution of sex, nationality, marital status, and highest educational
qualication (Table 2).
Between-groups Comparisons
Next, it was examined whether there were dierences on key vari-
ables between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals. To do so, 2 (tattoo
status: tattooed versus non-tattooed) × 2 (sex: women versus men) analy-
ses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted with the Big Five personal-
ity factors, Self-esteem, Need for Uniqueness, the sensation seeking sub-
scales, the systems of beliefs subscales, and the general attitude towards
tattoo score, respectively, as dependent variables. To control for Type
I error, a Bonferroni correction was applied, such that alpha was set at
.05/14 = .004.
Means and standard deviations and the results of all ANOVAs are
reported in Table 1. As can be seen, none of the tattoo status by sex in-
teractions reached statistical signicance following Bonferroni correction,
which led us to examine the main eects. Following Bonferroni correc-
tions, there were signicant main eects of tattoo status on Extraversion,
Need for Uniqueness, the Experience Seeking subscale of the SSS–V, and
the general attitude toward tattoos. In the rst three cases, tattooed indi-
viduals had higher scores than non-tattooed individuals, whereas in the
latter case, tattooed individuals had more positive Attitudes Toward Tat-
toos than non-tattooed individuals. There were also statistically signi-
TABLE 2
coMpaRisons Between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals
χ2df p φ
Sex 0.94 1.33 < .001
Nationality 1.27 2 .53 < .001
Marital status 2.17 4 .70 .009
Highest educational qualication 8.79 5 .12 .016
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 103
cant main eects of sex on Agreeableness and three of the four subscales
of the SSS–V.
discussion
The present results showed that there were a number of statistical-
ly signicant dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals
in terms of their personality and individual-dierence traits. Specically,
those tattooed had signicantly higher scores than non-tattooed individ-
uals on Extraversion, Need for Uniqueness, and the Experience Seeking
subscale of the SSS–V. In addition, compared with non-tattooed individ-
uals, tattooed participants also had signicantly more positive Attitudes
Toward Tattoos, which is unsurprising. These results are discussed in
greater detail below.
In terms of the Big Five, present results corroborate previous work
showing that tattooed individuals scored more extraverted than non-
tattooed individuals (e.g., Stirn, et al., 2006; Wohlrab, et al., 2007; Swami,
2012). Moreover, tattooed participants had higher scores on a subscale of
sensation seeking, which was related to the trait of Extraversion (see Ey-
senck, 1990). Importantly, however, there were no statistically signicant
dierences between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals on the remain-
ing Big Five factors, which stands in contrast to some previous work (e.g.,
Tate & Shelton, 2008). Overall, then, converging evidence appears to sug-
gest that tattooed individuals score more extraverted than non-tattooed
individuals, with this contrast being driven by their sensation seeking
scores.
The present results also corroborate previous work showing that tat-
tooed individuals have higher Need for Uniqueness than non-tattooed in-
dividuals (Tiggemann & Golder, 2006; Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011; Swa-
mi, 2012). In general, these results support the idea that tattoos are now
used as a means of self-expression or construction of identity. Specically,
in societies in which the body is increasingly commodied, tattoos may
aord some individuals an opportunity to mark the self as being dier-
ent and thereby attain improved self-perceptions of uniqueness (Swami,
2011). Put dierently, tattoos may now be an important means through
which individuals can develop unique identities, particularly in the ap-
pearance domain (Tiggemann & Hopkins, 2011).
Although the results suggest that there are statistically signicant dif-
ferences between tattooed and non-tattooed participants in a number of
examined variables, it should be noted that the eect sizes of observed dif-
ferences were generally small. Specically, eect sizes of dierences were
smallest for Extraversion and Experience seeking (ηp
2 = .02–.03), slightly
larger for Need for Uniqueness (ηp
2 = .07), and largest for Attitudes To-
ward Tattoos (ηp
2 = .21). As discussed by Tate and Shelton (2008), real-
V. Swami, et al.
104
world implications of such and mostly small dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals may well be negligible. Moreover, present
results suggest no statistically signicant dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals on a number of other variables, including
Self-esteem and Religious and Spiritual Beliefs. However, small eect siz-
es of dierences may well be due to secular trends in types of body orna-
mentation and rising prevalence rates of tattoos (e.g., Armstrong & Kelly,
2001; Stieger, et al., 2010) thus reecting increased acceptance of tattoos in
the general population and, in consequence, levelling o of dierences.
The main limitation of this study is that, although we were able to
avoid relying on college samples, it is possible the recruitment technique
introduced sampling biases. In any case, the sample should not be consid-
ered representative of the wider population and should not be extrapo-
lated to other German-speaking regions of Europe. In addition, a limited
number of variables were included in this study. So it is possible that this
limited range masks other, more important dierences between tattooed
and non-tattooed individuals. Given the cross-sectional nature of the de-
sign, inferring causation in the present instance (e.g., to say that extrav-
ertive scorers will be more likely to obtain a tattoo) is not possible.
These limitations notwithstanding, the present results suggest there
are statistically signicant, albeit for the most part small, dierences be-
tween tattooed and non-tattooed individuals on a small number of per-
sonality and individual dierence traits. Given the rapid mainstreaming
of tattoos in socioeconomically developed societies (including almost a
quarter of participants in the present study), ndings such as those report-
ed here may prove useful for scholars seeking to understand motivations
for obtaining a tattoo. That is, an individual dierences approach may
help scholars identify certain proles of individuals who are more likely
to obtain a tattoo, which can only add to research in which motivational
aspects of tattooing have been approached from a sociological perspective
(e.g., Kosut, 2006).
REFERENCES
alBani, c., BaileR, h., BlaseR, g., geyeR, M., BRähleR, e., & gRulke, n. (2002) [Reli-
gious and spiritual beliefs: validation of the German version of the “Systems of
Beliefs Inventory” (SBI-15R-D) by Holland, et al. in a population-based sample].
Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie, 52, 306-313. [in German]
andeRson, R. R. (2006) Commentary: tattoos and body piercing. Journal of the American
Academy of Dermatology, 55, 422.
aRMstRong, M. l., & kelly, l. (2001) Tattooing, body piercing, and branding are on
the rise: perspectives for school nurses. The Journal of School Nursing, 17, 12-23.
aRMstRong, M. l., RoBeRts, a. e., owen, d. c., & koch, J. R. (2004) Toward building
a composite of college student inuences with body art. Issues in Comprehensive
Pediatric Nursing, 27, 277-295.
PERSONALITY AND TATTOOS 105
Beauducel, a., stRoBel, a., & BRocke, B. (2003) [Psychometric properties and norms of
a German version of the sensation seeking scales, form V]. Diagnostica, 49, 61-72.
[in German]
Behling, o., & law, k. s. (2000) Translating questionnaires and other research instruments:
problems and solutions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
BRooks, t. l., woods, e. R., knight, J. R., & shRieR, l. a. (2003) Body modication and
substance use in adolescents: is there a link? Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 44-49.
caRRoll, s. t., RiffenBuRgh, R. h., RoBeRts, t. a., & MyhRe, e. B. (2002) Tattoos and
body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk-taking behaviors. Pediatrics, 109,
1021-1027.
deschesnes, M., fines, p., & deMeRs, s. (2006) Are tattooing and body piercing indica-
tors of risk-taking behaviours among high school students? Journal of Adolescence,
29, 379-393.
eysenck, h. J. (1990) Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Hand-
book of personality: theory and research. New York: Guilford. Pp. 244-276.
foRBes, g. B. (2001) College students with tattoos and piercings: motives, family ex-
periences, personality factors, and perception by others. Psychological Reports, 89,
774-796.
goldBeRg, l. R. (1999) A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality inventory
measuring the lower-level facets of several ve-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I.
J. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality Psychology in Europe. Vol. 7.
Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg Univer. Press. Pp. 7-28.
gReif, J., hewitt, w., & aRMstRong, M. l. (1999) Tattooing and body piercing: body art
practices among college students. Clinical Nursing Research, 8, 368-385.
holland, J. c., kash, k. M., passik, s., gRoneRt, M. k., sison, a., ledeRBeRg, M., Russak, s.
M., BaideR, l., & fox, B. (1998) A brief Spiritual Beliefs Inventory for use in qual-
ity of life research in life-threatening illness. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 460-469.
koch, J. R., RoBeRts, a. e., aRMstRong, M. l., & owen, d. c. (2005) College students,
tattoos, and sexual activity. Psychological Reports, 97, 887-890.
koch, J. R., RoBeRts, a. e., aRMstRong, M. l., & owen, d. c. (2010) Body art, deviance,
and American college students. Social Science Journal, 47, 151-161.
kosut, M. (2006) An ironic fad: the commodication and consumption of tattoos. The
Journal of Popular Culture, 39, 1035-1048.
lauMann, a. e., & deRick, a. J. (2006) Tattoos and body piercings in the United States:
a national data set. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 55, 413-421.
RoBeRti, J. w., stoRch, e. a., & BRavata, e. a. (2004) Sensation seeking, exposure to
psychological stressors, and body modications in a college population. Personal-
ity and Individual Dierences, 37, 1167-1177.
RoBeRts, t. a., & Ryan, s. a. (2002) Tattooing and high-risk behavior in adolescents.
Pediatrics, 110, 1058-1063.
RosenBeRg, M. (1965) Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Univer. Press.
seiteR, J. s., & hatch, s. (2005) Eect of tattoos on perceptions of credibility and at-
tractiveness. Psychological Reports, 96, 1113-1120.
snydeR, c. R., & fRoMkin, h. l. (1977) Abnormality as a positive characteristic: the
development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 86, 518-527.
V. Swami, et al.
106
stiegeR, s., pietschnig, J., kastneR, c. k., voRacek, M., & swaMi, v. (2010) Prevalence
and acceptance of tattoos and piercings: a survey of young adults from the south-
ern German-speaking area of southern Europe. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110,
1065-1074.
stiRn, a., hinz, a., & BRahleR, e. (2006) Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing
in Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking
among tattooed and body-pierced individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
60, 531-534.
swaMi, v. (2011) Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance anxi-
ety and dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Body Image, 8,
237-244.
swaMi, v. (2012) Written on the body? An examination of the personalities and indi-
vidual dierences of British adults who do and do not obtain a rst tattoo. Un-
published manuscript.
swaMi, v., & fuRnhaM, a. (2007) Unattractive, promiscuous, and heavy drinkers: per-
ceptions of women with tattoos. Body Image, 4, 343-352.
swaMi, v., & haRRis, a. s. (2012) Body art (tattoos and piercings). In T. Cash (Ed.), En-
cylopedia of human appearance and body image. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
tate, J. c., & shelton, B. l. (2008) Personality correlates of tattooing and body pierc-
ing in a college sample: the kids are alright. Personality and Individual Dierences,
45, 281-285.
tiggeMann, M., & goldeR, f. (2006) Tattooing: an expression of uniqueness in the ap-
pearance domain. Body Image, 3, 309-315.
tiggeMann, M., & hopkins, s. (2011) Tattoos and piercings: bodily expressions of
uniqueness? Body Image, 8, 245-250.
von collani, g., & heRzBeRg, p. y. (2003) [A revised version of the German adapta-
tion of Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale]. Zeitschrift für Dierentielle und Diagnostische
Psychologie, 24, 3-7. [in German]
voRacek, M., tRan, u. s., & dRessleR, s. g. (2010) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sensation
seeking: new data and meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Dierences, 48, 72-
77.
wohlRaB, s., stahl, J., RaMMsayeR, t., & kappeleR, p. M. (2007) Dierences in personal-
ity characteristics between body modied and nonmodied individuals and pos-
sible evolutionary implications. European Journal of Personality, 21, 931-951.
zuckeRMan, M., eysenck, s. B. g., & eysenck, h. J. (1978) Sensation seeking in England
and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 46, 139-149.
Accepted July 18, 2012.
... Wytatuowani ludzie w porównaniu z osobami bez modyfikacji ciała częściej prezentują wyższe wyniki na skali ekstrawersji, poszukiwania doświadczeń oraz silniej deklarują potrzebę bycia wyjątkowym [15]. Kobiety z tatuażami -w porównaniu z kobietami bez tatuażu -wyrażają większą chęć do angażowania się w niezobowiązujące relacje seksualne, a także większe poparcie dla egalitaryzmu i poszukiwania doznań [16]. ...
... Wytatuowanych dorosłych charakteryzuje buntowniczość, złość i agresja werbalna [19]. Jednakże badacze w swoich analizach coraz częściej ujawniają niewielkie efekty obserwowanych różnic, które niekoniecznie świadczą o dysfunkcjonalnym charakterze owych cech, a tym bardziej o patologii wytatuowanych osób [15]. ...
... Osoby, które posiadały modyfikacje ciała, nie różniły się w sposób znaczący od osób bez takich modyfikacji pod względem samooceny [15,35], co potwierdzono w niniejszym badaniu. Jest wysoce prawdopodobne, że w związku z dużą liczebnością grup oraz wysoką mocą statystyczną zastosowanych testów wykazane różnice mię- dzygrupowe w samoocenie własnego funkcjonowania były przypadkowe [32]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cel pracy Tatuaż i kolczykowanie ciała stają się coraz bardziej popularne. Psychologiczne podejście do takich modyfikacji ciała wciąż pozostaje heterogeniczne. Celem niniejszej replikacji było oszacowanie poziomu satysfakcji z życia i samooceny oraz ujawnienie subiektywnie doświadczanych objawów zaburzeń zdrowia psychicznego u osób, które deklarowały posiadanie tatuażu i/lub piercingu w trakcie epidemii koronawirusa. Metoda Badania zostały przeprowadzone w okresie od kwietnia do czerwca 2020 w formie on-line. Uczestnicy (N = 557) byli w wieku 15-68 lat. Wyniki Nie stwierdzono istotnych różnic w postrzeganej satysfakcji z życia, samoocenie i ocenie zdrowia psychicznego pomiędzy osobami z modyfikacjami ciała i bez nich. Ujawnione różnice w wymiarach samooceny oraz ilości subiektywnie odczuwanych objawów depresji okazały się mieć charakter przypadkowy. Wnioski Wszyscy uczestnicy badania (niezależnie od posiadania modyfikacji ciała) mieli świadomość posiadania i umięjętności wykorzystania zasobów osobistych, by radzić sobie z sytuacją pandemii COVID-19. Modyfikacje ciała nie należy traktować jako czynnik ryzyka. Szczególnie wśród osób wytatuowanych wraz ze wzrostem satysfakcji z życia wzrastała samoocena funkcjonowania psychologicznego.
... Considering tattoos as a form of risk-taking behaviour, the association between self-esteem and tattoos may also be complex. For example, Swami et al. (2012) found that individuals with multiple tattoos reported higher self-esteem and a greater need for uniqueness compared to those with a single tattoo or no tattoos at all. This suggests that the relationship between self-esteem and tattoos may not be linear, and factors such as the need for uniqueness or self-expression might play a role, which can be linked to locus of control, body image and getting tattoos (Ball & Elsner, 2019;Kluger, 2021). ...
... There is some evidence that has suggested that people with tattoos tend to have a higher external locus of control Koch et al, 2015). In their study, Swami et al. (2012) found that individuals with tattoos exhibited a more external locus of control compared to those without tattoos. However, it is important to consider the context in which this study was conducted, as it took place in a different cultural and temporal setting than the present research, Swami et al (2012) conducted their research in-person on 540 adults in southern-Germany and was geographically restricted, while the present study was conducted after a global pandemic, and was conducted online and did not control for geographical boundaries. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This study explores how the covid-19 pandemic affected some psychological factors in people who got tattoos during or after the lockdown, the psychological factors assessed were self-esteem, risk taking behaviour, body image and locus of control. The study used a quantitative, between-subjects design to investigate the experiences of participants who acquired tattoos during the pandemic compared to those who did not. The results showed that participants who got a tattoo during the pandemic reported higher levels of self-esteem and body image satisfaction compared to those who did not get a tattoo. Furthermore, the findings suggest that individuals with tattoos may have a more external locus of control. Participants with tattoos exhibited higher levels of risk-taking behaviour compared to those without tattoos, furthermore sex affected risk-taking behaviour in tattooed individuals, and females with tattoos displayed a higher risk taking tendency than their tattooed male counterparts. Further research needs to be done while acknowledging the limitations of this study. Overall, this study is pivotal in establishing how the pandemic may have affected the aforementioned psychological factors in people with tattoos. It also adds to the growing body of literature in body modification and psychology.
... There is no longer any significant difference between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals other than being more unique and outgoing (Swami et al., 2012). Thus, indicating that all the negative ideas associated with tattooed people are only stereotypical thoughts that have been fostered over the years rather than being scientific or accurate facts. ...
... Their ideas revolved around freedom of expressing personality (stories 2, 4, and 6), finding tattoos appealing, and emotional connections (stories 1,3, and 5). Individuals make their decision to have a tattoo based on a meaningful purpose (Schlösser et al., 2020;Swami et al., 2012). However, as revealed in previous themes, tattooed individuals are discriminated against in different ways leaving them lost between what they want and what others want them to be or look like. ...
Article
Full-text available
Body modifications such as tattoos have been a diabolical topic throughout the world, especially in the business world, and how accepting a workplace culture can be of such appearances. With all the strict values and traditions that a conservative culture imposes on its companies’ inner cultures, the consequences of having a tattoo may surpass individuals’ expectations. Jordan, as a Middle Eastern country, has a conservative culture, and tattoos are not a culturally acceptable trend. This study aims to explore whether tattooed individuals in Jordan experience any discrimination throughout their careers in different sectors in the country due to their different physical appearance and if such body modifications create any risks in being treated fairly in an organization. This research adopts a social constructivist approach that integrates narratives from six Jordanian tattooed individuals who faced discriminatory actions in a workplace due to their appearance. The findings revealed that (i) tattooed individuals have fears of not getting employed or not having job security when employed. (ii) Tattooed individuals are being judged based on how they look rather than their performance. (iii) Tattooed individuals may suffer from psychological impacts. (IV) Tattooed individuals are lost between who they are (what they want) and who they should be (what others want). This research increases awareness of the possible impacts of discriminatory actions on tattooed individuals in the workplace and recommends actions to eliminate this impact.
... njem, antisocijalnim poremećajem ličnosti, te samoozljeđivanjem. Studija Swamija i sur., na uzorku od 450 ispitanika iz središnje Europe, pokazala je da tetovirani pojedinci u odnosu na netetovirane u testovima ličnosti češće pokazuju znakove ekstrovertiranosti, traženja novih iskustava i potrebe za osjećajem posebnosti (15). ...
... Measey je pokazao da vjerojatnost prisutnosti poremećaja ličnosti kod nositelja tetovaža ra-Tattoos are usually associated with certain personality characteristics, risky behaviour, antisocial personality disorder, and self-harming. Swami et al. conducted a study on a sample of 450 respondents from Central Europe and found that in personality tests tattooed individuals were more likely than non-tattooed individuals to show signs of extroversion, seek new experiences, and express a need to feel special (15). Similar personality traits in tattooed individuals were found in a study conducted by Tate and Shelton on the student population. ...
Article
Full-text available
Tetovaže su prisutne u gotovo svakom dijelu svijeta, kao i u gotovo svakom razdoblju povijesti. Njihova se uloga mijenjala tijekom povijesti, od funkcije obilježavanja prijestupnika preko omogućavanja međusobnog prepoznavanja pripadnika određenih zajednica do konačne uloge ukrašavanja tijela i popratne ekspresije emocija i stajališta. Premda su nositelji tetovaža nekoć bili izrazito stigmatizirani, taj se trend znatno promijenio te trenutno svjedočimo naglom porastu popularnosti tetoviranja. Opisani su brojni razlozi za tetoviranje, od potrebe za uljepšavanjem, iskazivanjem individualnosti ili ekspresijom emocija do označavanja pripadnosti različitim društvenim skupinama ili pak izražavanja otpora prema autoritetu. Tetovaže su povezane i s nekim psihijatrijskim poremećajima. Među tetoviranom populacijom veća je učestalost disocijalnog poremećaja ličnosti, zlouporabe droga i alkohola te graničnog i drugih poremećaja ličnosti. Ipak, one dopuštaju pojedincu izražavanje osjećaja, vrijednosti i stavova, kao i održavanje pozitivne slike vlastitog identiteta. U ovom je radu opisan bolesnik s brojnim tetovažama, njihovo značenje u tijeku liječenja pacijenta te važnost koje tetovaže imaju za njega. Posebna je pažnja obraćena tetovaži zgrade Klinike za psihijatriju Vrapče, gdje je pacijent hospitaliziran u više navrata, kojom je nastojao izraziti zahvalnost za pruženo liječenje. / Tattoos are present in almost every part of the world, as well as in almost every period of history. The role of tattoos has changed throughout history, from marking offenders and enabling the members of a particular community to recognize each other to the ultimate role of adorning the body and thus expressing certain emotions or attitudes. Although tattoo wearers were once highly stigmatized, this trend has changed significantly and we are currently witnessing a sharp rise in the popularity of tattooing. Many reasons for tattooing have been described, ranging from the need to beautify, express individuality or emotions to the need to mark one's affiliation to various social groups or to express resistance to authority. Tattoos are also linked to certain psychiatric disorders. In the tattooed population, the incidence of dissocial personality disorder, drug and alcohol abuse, and borderline and other personality disorders is higher. Yet, tattoos allow the individual to express their feelings, values and attitudes, as well as to maintain a positive image of their own identity. This paper describes a patient with numerous tattoos, their meaning during the treatment of the patient as well as the importance that tattoos have for him. Special attention was paid to the tattoo of the building of the University Psychiatric Hospital Vrapče, where the patient was hospitalized on several occasions, since this tattoo was the patient's attempt to express gratitude for the treatment provided.
... Rebelliousness, anger, and verbal aggression have been associated with tattooed individuals (Raspa et al., 1990). Another study showed high scores on extraversion and experience seeking for tattooed individuals (Swami et al., 2012). Similarly, Lamberg (1996) supported the conclusion that tattooed individuals do exhibit aggression, impulsivity, and drug use. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present research work titled, ‘A Comparative Study of Mental Health Issues Among Tattooed and Non-Tattooed Young Adults’, is based on the enquiry, could many tattoos, in different parts of the body done over a period of time, an indication of onset of mental illness? Why people willingly inflict pain on their body? Do the young adults who get multiple tattoos on different parts of their body have mental health issues? The objectives of the study wereto study themental health of young tattooed adults with the Hypotheses: a) There will be an effect of tattoos on the mental health of young adults with tattoos on different parts of the body, and b) There will be a significant difference between the mental health of tattooed and non-tattooed young adults and the Variablesbeingtattoo (IV) and mental health (DV). Tool: Mental Health Inventory by Jagdish and A.K. Srivastava (1983) that has 56 questions covering six dimensions i.e., positive self-evaluation (10 questions), perception of reality (8 questions), integration of personality (12 questions), autonomy (6 questions), group-oriented attitudes (10 questions), and environmental mastery (10 questions). The Sample Populationwas 60 young adults between the age range of 18 and 25 years (30 with tattoos on different parts of the body and 30 without tattoos) using purposive sampling and Exploratory Research Design. The statistical analysis was done using Descriptive Statisticst test and correlation. This study clearly indicates that tattooed young adults scored poorly in mental health status.
... 14 Despite this, individuals with and without tattoos have been assessed to be similar in terms of intelligence, honesty, and other traits by which society determines one's character. 16,17 This further dismantles the ideology that tattooed persons are of low moral character. ...
Article
Tattoos are common practice in underrepresented groups. However, institutional policies often prohibit visible tattoos of healthcare professionals. This affects marginalized groups where tattoos may be the cultural norm. There are conflicting findings on perceptions of tattoos on medical professionals from the perspectives of peers, patients, and learners. Tattoo restriction can be discriminatory against already marginalized persons and send a message of exclusion. Policies surrounding tattoo restriction should be re-evaluated to create an inclusive environment for all.
... 20 In addition differences in personality traits have been demonstrated between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals and this may include increased anger and verbal aggression in the former. 21,22 While it has been suggested that individuals with multiple tattoos may have certain differences such as personality disorders, 8 the current study has demonstrated that the number of tattoos did not have an effect on the likelihood of a specific manner of death in a medicolegal context, despite a trend for this being observed in earlier work. 16 Specifically the percentage of unnatural deaths in the study group of individuals with five or more tattoos of 48% was not statistically different to a control group of non-tattooed individuals from the same medicolegal population of 44% (p = 0.3). ...
Article
A prospective study was undertaken of 150 medicolegal cases where five or more tattoos were identified in anatomically separate areas. All cases were the subject of full police and coronial investigations with examination by forensic pathologists. There were 120 males and 30 females (M:F = 4:1) with an age range of 22–86 years (mean = 48.1 years). 78 cases were found where deaths were due to natural diseases (52%) (age range 27–82 years; mean 55.3 years; M:F = 4.2:1). 72 cases (48%) were found where deaths were classified as unnatural – 23 drug/alcohol related, 37 suicides, 12 accidents and 0 homicides (age range 20–66 years; mean 39.8 years; M:F = 3.8:1). This distribution was not shown to be statistically different to a control group of 100 non-tattooed individuals where there were 56 natural and 44 unnatural deaths (p = 0.3). Thus, although certain types of tattoos may be associated with an increased number of unnatural deaths in a medicolegal environment, the actual number of tattoos appears to have minimal effect.
Article
Full-text available
Autobiographical memories play a vital role in shaping personal identity. Therefore, individuals often use various methods like diaries and photographs to preserve precious memories. Tattoos also serve as a means of remembering, yet their role in autobiographical memory has received limited attention in research. To address this gap, we surveyed 161 adults (68.9 per cent female, M = 26.93, SD = 6.57) to explore the life events that motivated their tattoos and to examine their most significant memories. We then compared these findings with significant memories of 185 individuals without tattoos (80.0 per cent female, M = 31.26, SD = 15.34). The results showed that the majority of tattoos were inspired by unique life events, including specific events about personal growth, relationships, leisure activities, losses, or diseases. Even when not directly tied to specific events in life, tattoos still reflect autobiographical content, such as mottos, beliefs, and values. Furthermore, the most significant memories of younger tattooed individuals (20-24 years) tended to be more normative and less stressful compared to those of their non-tattooed counterparts in the same age group, though the nature of these memories varied. This difference was not found among older participants (30-54 years). Additionally, those without tattoos indicated to use specific objects and methods for preserving important events, suggesting tattoos are only one of several ways to reminisce. However, tattoos uniquely allow for the physical embodiment of autobiographical memories, indicating that engraving significant life events in the skin aids in reflecting on one's life story.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Few population-based studies have examined the perception and prevalence of tattoos and tattoo regret in the general United States (US) adult population. Our objective was to report the perception of people with tattoos and describe the prevalence, socio-demographics, health-related quality of life, and the extent of tattoo regret in US adults. Methods Data were assessed from a cross-sectional study of US adults. Participants were recruited using a random stratified sampling framework similar to the US Census. Data collected for all participants included socio-demographic and clinical characteristics, general health-related quality of life (Veterans RAND 12‐item), depression (Patient Health Questionnaire 9-item), anxiety (Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7-item), and perceptions of those with tattoos. Those with tattoos also answered questions about their tattoo(s), including age when first tattooed, reasons for getting a tattoo, and tattoo regret. Categorical data were described by percentages, and continuous data by mean and standard deviation. Proportions were compared with Chi-squared tests and the means with ANOVA. A logistic regression controlling for confounding variables was carried out to assess factors predictive of tattoo regret. Results Of the 3033 participants, 35.3% (1,072) reported having a tattoo. Those more likely to have a tattoo were female (58% vs. 45%), younger (38 vs. 46 years), smoked cigarettes (38% vs. 19% non-smoker), and/or reported an alcohol or drug problem (10% vs. 5%). Those without tattoos were more likely to perceive those with tattoos as less attractive, intelligent, professional, and more rebellious. More time (in years) with a tattoo, having a tattoo on the face, neck, hands, wrist, or fingers, getting a tattoo because of peer pressure, being impaired when getting a tattoo, and experiencing an adverse event related to a tattoo were predictive of tattoo regret. Older age and remembrance as the reason for a tattoo were predictive of not having tattoo regret. Conclusion More than one-third of the study sample comprised of adults in the US reported having at least one tattoo. While most people, regardless of their tattoo status, perceived tattooed and non-tattooed individuals equally, tattooed individuals were more likely to be perceived negatively than positively by those without tattoos. Whether tattooed or not, being aware of varying perceptions of tattoo status may be helpful in facilitating positive outcomes.
Book
Full-text available
This is the next book in the scientific book series entitled "Advances in biomedical research". This book series is the result of the meetings and work of scientists from various universities and institutes in Poland. In this monograph we present the topics in Biomedical Research from 2021. All presented articles have passed the peer-review process positively. The articles come from various fields of biomedicine from Cell-inCell phenomena throughout cancer research to skin disorders. Wishing you enjoyable and productive reading. Editors, L.Bialy, I Mlynarczuk-Bialy https://bc.wydawnictwo-tygiel.pl/publikacja/4D2E20CF-4ADE-0211-0229-3237E8326A1A
Article
Full-text available
Defines need for uniqueness as a positive striving for abnormality relative to other people. Recent research regarding situational determinants of uniqueness motivation is described, and a dispositional individual-differences measure of need for uniqueness is presented. The development of the Uniqueness Scale aims at insuring construct validity as a guide for the item selection. The internal reliabilities, item-remainder coefficients, test–retest reliabilities, cross-validation information, factor analysis, and discriminant validation data are presented, and all meet the normal psychometric criteria expected of an individual-differences measure. Additionally, 8 separate validational studies, conducted with a total of 1,523 US and Israeli college students, are presented. (28 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
After a long history of negative stigmatisation, the practices of tattooing and body piercing have become fashionable in the last decade. Today, 10% of the population in modern western societies have some form of body modification. The aim of this study was to quantify the demographic and personality traits of tattooed and pierced individuals and to compare them with a control group of individuals without body modifications. These comparisons are based on questionnaires completed by 359 individuals that investigate the details of body modification, and which incorporate five personality scales. We describe several sex differences in ornament style and location. We found no relevant differences between modified and non-modified individuals in relation to demographic variables. This indicates that some of the traditional attitudes towards tattoos and piercings appear to be outdated. However, we found striking differences in personality traits which suggest that body-modified individuals are greater sensation seekers and follow a more unrestricted mating strategy than their non-modified contemporaries. We discuss these differences in light of a potential signalling function of tattoos and piercings in the mating context. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Full-text available
This research examines the relationship between body art (tattoos and piercings) and deviance. With the increasing mainstream presence of visible tattoos and piercings among entertainers, athletes, and even in corporate boardrooms, we wonder the extent to which long-time enthusiasts and collectors regard the phenomenon as encroachment. We use sub-cultural identity theory to propose that individuals with increasing evidence of body art procurement will also report higher levels of deviant behavior in order to maintain and/or increase social distance from the mainstream. We tested this proposition by surveying 1753 American college students, asking them to report their level of body art acquisition and their history of deviance. Results indicate that respondents with four or more tattoos, seven or more body piercings, or piercings located in their nipples or genitals, were substantively and significantly more likely to report regular marijuana use, occasional use of other illegal drugs, and a history of being arrested for a crime. Less pronounced, but still significant in many cases, was an increased propensity for those with higher incidence of body art to cheat on college work, binge drink, and report having had multiple sex partners in the course of their lifetime.
Article
Full-text available
A large (N = 1375; 873 women, 502 men) sample of college students completed measures of demographics, body modification (i.e., tattoos and body piercings), and measures of the Big Five personality traits, social desirability, and need for uniqueness. Tattooed participants, as compared to their non-tattooed counterparts, scored significantly lower on agreeableness and conscientiousness and significantly higher on need for uniqueness. Body pierced participants, as compared to their non-pierced counterparts, scored significantly lower on conscientiousness and significantly higher on openness to experience. Although statistically significant, these relationships explain very small amounts of variance and most likely reflect inconsequential real world differences between those with and without these two forms of body modification. Our findings suggest caution when attributing psychopathology to adults who possess tattoos and/or body piercings.
Article
Zusammenfassung. Die Sensation Seeking-Skalen, Form V (SSS-V) sind eines der am haufigsten eingesetzten Inventare zur Erfassung von Sensation Seeking. Allerdings wurden immer wieder geringe interne Konsistenzen und Probleme bei der Replikation der Faktorenstruktur, insbesondere fur die Subskalen Experience Seeking und Boredom Susceptibility berichtet. Im Vergleich zu Alternativen hat die SSS-V allerdings den Vorteil einer differenzierteren Erfassung des Konstrukts, weshalb die neueren Inventare sich nicht durchsetzen konnten. Vor dem Hintergrund dieser Situation bestanden die Ziele der vorliegenden Arbeit in der Untersuchung der psychometrischen Eigenschaften einer von Marvin Zuckerman autorisierten deutschsprachigen SSS-V, der Gewinnung von Normen sowie optimierter Personenkennwerte anhand von Faktorwerten. Dazu wurde eine Stichprobe von 1526 Probanden (823 weiblich) im Alter von 16 bis 68 Jahren mit der SSS-V untersucht. Die vierfaktorielle Struktur der SSS-V konnte anhand einer exploratorischen Faktore...
Article
A sample of 281 (females=160) college students with body modifications completed a demographics questionnaire, questions about body modification practices, the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS-V; Zuckerman, 1994), the Impulsivity subscale of the Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ; Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, & Kraft, 1993), and a selected portion of the History of Psychosocial Stressors (HPS; Scotti, 1992, 1999). Gender differences were found on the number of body modifications and when they were first obtained. In general, males with tattoos and piercings had higher scores than females on subscales of the SSS-V. Females with piercings and tattoos had higher scores on the Impulsive Sensation Seeking subscale of the ZKPQ. No gender differences were found when participants were asked if he/she would obtain another body modification or if his/her friends had a body modification. Gender and sensation seeking preference were predictive of number of tattoos but not number of piercings. Exposure with psychosocial stressors was predictive of number of piercings but not number of tattoos. These results suggest that sensation seeking preferences and exposure to a psychosocial stressor may be associated with obtaining a body modification in some college students.
Article
Previously reported associations between low (male-typical) digit ratio (2D:4D), a putative pointer to prenatal testosterone exposure, and high (male-typical) sensation seeking have been inconsistent across studies (alternately present for men, women, either sex, or neither). Addressing this question again in three new studies (N = 198, 188, 1118) produced similarly erratic findings. Meta-analysis of the entire literature (13 studies with nearly 3000 individuals, including unpublished accounts) showed that the current cumulative evidence does not support any negative correlations between 2D:4D and sensation seeking traits. The only significant meta-analytical finding was for right-hand 2D:4D and the experience seeking facet of sensation seeking in both sexes, but this effect accounted for merely 0.4% attributable variance, and moreover was directionally opposite to expectation (i.e., a positive correlation). Discussed are inherent limitations of narrow-scoped approaches (such as via 2D:4D) for elucidating the biological bases of individual difference variables with evidentially intricate neurochemical underpinnings (such as sensation seeking).
Book
There are few topics so fascinating both to the research investigator and the research subject as the self-image. It is distinctively characteristic of the human animal that he is able to stand outside himself and to describe, judge, and evaluate the person he is. He is at once the observer and the observed, the judge and the judged, the evaluator and the evaluated. Since the self is probably the most important thing in the world to him, the question of what he is like and how he feels about himself engrosses him deeply. This is especially true during the adolescent stage of development.