Content uploaded by Patricia Benner
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Patricia Benner on Jun 24, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Benner Page
III. Using- the Dreyfus Model of Skill Acquisition to Describe and Interpret Skill
Acquisition and Clinical Judgment in Nursing Practice and Education
2004 Benner, P. Using the Dreyfus Model of Skill Acquisition to describe and
interpret skill acquisition and clinical judgment in nursing practice and
education. The Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society Special Issue:
Human Expertise in the Age of the Computer, Vol. 24 (3) 188-199
Patricia Benner
University of California, San Francisco Three studies using the Dreyfus model of skill
acquisition were conducted over a period of 21 years. Nurses with a range of experience and
reported skillfulness were interviewed. Each study used nurses' narrative accounts of actual
clinical situations. A subsample of participants were observed and inter-viewed at work. These
studies extend the understanding of the Dreyfus model to complex, under-determined, and fast
paced practices. The skill of involvement and the development of moral agency are linked with
the development of expertise, and change as the practitioner becomes more skillful. Nurses who
had some diiculty with understanding the ends of practice and difficulty with their skills of
interpersonal and problem engagement did not progress to the level of expertise. Taken together;
these studies demonstrate the usefulness of the Dreyfus model for understanding the learning
needs and styles of learning at different levels of skill acquisition.
Keywords: nursing practice; phronesis
Stuart E. Dreyfus, an applied mathematician, and Hubert L. Dreyfus, a philosopher,
developed a model of skill acquisition based on the study of chess players, air force pilots,
and army tank drivers and commanders (S. E. Dreyfus, 1982; H. L. Dreyfus & Dreyfus,
1977, 1986; S. E. Dreyfus & Dreyfus, 1979, 1980). The Dreyfus model of skill acquisition
has illuminated ongoing research on skill acquisition and articulation of knowledge
embedded in expert practice in nursing. The Dreyfus model is developmental, based on situ-
ated performance and experiential learning. Three studies of skill acquisition in nursing have
been guided by the Dreyfus model of skill acquisition (Benner, 1982, 1984; Benner, Hooper-
Kyriakidis, Stannard, 1999; Benner, Tanner, & Chesla et al. 1992, 1996). Hubert and Stuart
Dreyfus served as consultants in each of these three studies.
The first study, conducted between 1978 and 1981 (Benner, 1982, 1984), was based on
21 paired inter-views with newly graduated nurses and their preceptors, and interviews
and/or participant observations were conducted with 51 additional experienced nurse
clinicians, 11 newly graduated nurses, and 5 senior nursing students to further delineate
and describe characteristics of nurse performance at different levels of education and
experience. The interviews (small group and individual) were conducted in six hospitals:
two private community hospitals, two community teaching hospitals, one university
medical center, and one inner-city general hospital. A second study of skill acquisition and
clinical knowledge of critical-care nurses was conducted between 1988 and 1994 (Benner
et al., 1992; Benner et al., 1996). One-hundred and thirty nurses practicing in intensive
188
Benner Page
care units and general floor units from eight hospitals, seven of which are located in the
far western and one in the eastern region of the country, comprised the study population.
Small group narrative interviews, individual interviews, and participant observation were
used as data collection strategies. The two aims of the study, relevant to this article, were
(a) to describe the nature of skill acquisition in critical-care nursing practice and (b) to
delineate the practical knowledge embedded in expert practice. The third study was an
extension of this study, conducted between 1996 and 1997 to include other critical.-care
areas (including emergency departments, flight nursing, home health, the operating room,
and postanesthesia care units [N = 75 nurses]) and to enlarge our sample of advanced
practice nurses.' This article presents key findings of these three studies using data from
each of the studies.
Nursing, like other practice disciplines, is not merely an applied field in the sense that the
practice is complex, varied, and underdetermined. Good practice requires that the nurse develop
skillful ethical comportment as a practitioner and that the nurse use good clinical judgment
informed by scientific evidence and technological development. The sciences of medicine and
nursing are broad and multidisciplinary and require translation into the particular practice situa-
tion. Basic sciences of biochemical, physical, and biological processes; physiological processes;
research and development of specific therapies and technologies; and finally clinical trials and
more make up a broad range of relevant science used in the practice of medicine and nursing.
A recent development in nursing and medical practice has been to aggregate clinical trial
research out-comes to summarize and recommend the best evidence for treatment of specific
clinical conditions. However, the logic of scientific decision making and the logic of the
practitioner working with single cases or unique populations are necessarily different. The
practitioner must reason across time about the particular through changes in the patient's
condition and changes in the clinician's understanding of the patient's condition. Because
practice in the individual case is underdetermined (i.e., open to variations not accounted for by
science), the practitioner must use good clinical reasoning to intelligently select and use the
relevant science. Perceptual acuity in recognizing salient signs, symptoms, and responses to
therapies are required for the clinician to use good clinical judgments in particular clinical cases.
Recognizing and keeping track of clinical changes in the patient over time requires the
logic of reasoning in transition (Benner, 1994; Taylor, 1993). This is a form of argument
about the outcomes of successive changes. Patient changes must be evaluated as
improved, stable, or deteriorating over time. Clinicians call this recognizing trends in the
patient. Some aspects of practice can be subjected to more standardization and to what
Aristotle described as techne. Standard measurements of vital signs and laboratory metrics
are examples of clinical assessments that can be reduced to techne. But note that
skillfulness and craft based on experience may still be essential to successful
performance of techne. In situations where the patient's particular response must be
considered and perceptual acuity is required to recognize salient changes in the patient,
as well as situations where attuned relationships and judgment require skillful
comportment, both techne and phronesis (situated actions based on skill, judgment,
character, and wisdom) are essential.
At the heart of good clinical judgment and clinical wisdom lies experiential learning from
particular cases. Bad judgments roust be refined and corrected in particular cases; anomalies and
distinctions must be noticed. The Dreyfus model of skill acquisition addresses this kind of
experiential learning in a complex, underdetermined field over time. The model is situational
rather than being a trait or talent model because the focus is on actual performance and out-
comes in particular situations. The model is develop-mental in that changes in the performance in
particular situations can be compared across time. However, the model does not focus or identify
189
Benner Page
particular traits or talents of the person that generates the skillful performance.
Nursing, as a practice, requires both techne and phronesis as described by Aristotle. Techne
can be captured by procedural and scientific .knowledge, knowledge that can be made formal,
explicit, and certain, except for the necessary timing and adjustments made for particular
patients. Phronesis, in contrast to techne, is the kind of practical reasoning engaged in by an
excellent practitioner lodged in a community of practitioners, a practitioner who, through
experiential learning and for the sake of good practice, continually lives out and improves practice
(Benner et al., 1999; Dunne, 1997.; Gadamer,1960/1975; MacIntyre, 198 1; Shulman, 1993).
Techne, or the activity of producing outcomes, is governed by a means-ends rationality where
the maker or producer governs the thing produced or made by gaining mastery over the means of
producing the outcomes. By contrast, phronesis is lodged in a practice and so cannot rely solely
on a means-ends rationality because one's acts are governed by concern for doing good in
particular circumstances, where being in relationship and discerning particular human concerns
are at stake must guide action. For example, nurses describe the excessive use of power over a
patient that subjects said patient to unwanted, futile therapies to prolong his or her life at any cost
as flogging the patient, violating both good nursing and good doctoring (Benner et al., 1999, pp.
363-403). In such bad practice, means and ends are violently separated so that both are distorted.
Doctoring and nursing particular patients requires relational and communication skills and art.
The relationship between the patient and nurse, for example, determines what will be disclosed,
what can be thought about and talked about together, and what level of acceptance and
endorsement of the therapies will be acceptable to patients and clinicians alike. Patients often
rehearse their most fearful concerns with nurses who they expect to be more approachable and
effective in helping them communicate with physicians. Technique alone cannot address
interpersonal and relational responsibilities, discernment, and situated possibilities required by
caring for persons made vulnerable by illness and injury. Phronesis is required. Means and ends
are inextricably related in caring for the ill. Clinician and patient bend and respond to the other so
that horizons and world are opened and reconstituted so that new possibilities can emerge.
As the Dreyfus model suggests, experiential learning requires the stance of an engaged learner
rather than a stance of one expert in techne who skillfully applies well-established knowledge in
prespecified, clear circumstances. Experiential learning requires openness and responsiveness by
the learner to improve practice over time. The learner who develops an attuned, response-based
practice learns to recognize whole situations in terms of past concrete experiences, as pointed out
by the Dreyfus model,
We found that responding to the situation as an instance of particular concern is central
to the logic of excellent practice. For example, this is a situation of heart-pump failure or
fluid depletion. Interventions depend on clarifying and confirming the nature of the
clinical situation at hand. The skillful practitioner learns to hold his or her background
understandings in a fluid or semipermeable way so that he or she can recognize when
these tacit expectations are not met. For example, a nurse with expertise in detecting heart
arrhythmias on a unit where all patients' cardiac functioning is monitored will only notice
aberrations in sound patterns rather than attending to the familiar sounds in the
foreground of attention. Whereas in some skill situations, such as playing chess or driving
a car, experts would not need to articulate their perspectives before taking action, must
make a case, that includes articulating their perspective and evidence to get the
appropriate physician intervention. In emergencies, when there is no physician available,
the nurse must be able to articulate clearly the reason for using a standing order or
protocol or going beyond the usual boundaries of usual nursing practice. This is expected
and defensible when it is critical for the patient's survival. Recognizing the unexpected
190
Benner Page
(i.e., when tacit global expectations of patients' recovery are not met) is also a hallmark of
expert practice.
Major Shifts in the Style of Practice With the Development of Expertise
As noted above, the nurse's capacity for effective moral agency changes with developing
practice skills and insights from experience. Also, the skills of problem and person engagement
grow more attuned. The development of agency and skills of involvement can be seen at each
stage of skill acquisition. The nurse increasingly is able to recognize when he or she does not
have a good grasp of the clinical situation, and this lack of sense of understanding guides the
nurse's questioning and problem solving.
As the skill model predicts, with more experience comes increased grasp of the nature of
particular clinical situations, including opportunities and constraints, which then guides the nurse's
actions and interactions. Consequently, responses to patients become more contextualized and
attuned. Recognition of clinical situations moves from abstract textbook accounts of general
features to an experience-based response to the situation. Grasp of the situation, with its
possibilities and constraints, enables the competent nurse to move from rule-governed thinking
to an intuitive grasp of the situation (Dreyfus & Dreyfus, 1986),
This intuitive grasp is based on experience and not based on extrasensory powers or wild
hunches. It is situated in the clinician's grasp of the situation. Improved skills of involvement
create disclosive spaces in which pressing concerns or the most plausible actions can be
discovered. Relational skills are schooled by learning to be at home in a highly differentiated
clinical world where some actions are plausible and effective and others are experienced as ill
timed or implausible, A sense of salience develops over time so that some things stand out as
more plausible and appropriate than others. The proficient practitioner develops a richer sense of
the ends and possibilities of practice based on shared notions of good practice within the
profession (Rubin, 1996).
Because the Dreyfus model of skill acquisition is a situated and descriptive phenomenological
account of the development of skill over time, it does not point to isolated competencies nor
enabling traits or talent. Consequently, it allows that a practitioner may be at different levels of
skill in different areas of practice based on the particular practitioner's background experience
and knowledge. For example, a practitioner skilled in caring for adults at an expert level will not
be at that level of skill when caning for young children or premature infants. The continuities in
patient populations one cares for determine the opportunities for experiential learning.
Novice: First Year of Education
The novice stage of skill acquisition occurs in areas on which the student has no
experiential background to base approach or understanding of the clinical situation. For
example, the art and skill of a range of medical and nursing interventions on particular
patients will be new. The educator must offer good descriptions of features and attributes
of the situation that the novice can recognize. For example, to determine fluid balance,
students are given clear parameters and guidelines:
191
Benner Page
To determine fluid balance, check the patient's morning weights and daily intake and output
for the past three days. Weight gain and an intake that is consistently greater than 500 cc.
could indicate water retention, in which case fluid restriction should be started until the
cause of the imbalance can be found. (Benner, 1954, p. 21)
An experienced clinician will immediately think of all the situations where this evaluation
would be inappropriate or too stringent. But the novice is given clear directions of safe ways to
proceed until the significance of fluid balance for different clinical conditions can be Iearned. The
rules and guidelines must not re-quire prior experience for their recognition. They must provide a
safe beginning point for specific, situated learning in the clinical situation. Fluid balance is sa -
lient, but what the novice must learn is the particular salience of fluid balance for particular
patients.
The rule-governed behavior of the novice is extremely limited and inflexible. The
student is coached in comparing and matching textbook examples with actual clinical
cases. Skills that are per-formed easily on a mannequin in a skills lab require adaptation
and communication and reassurance skills when performed on a range of patients who
may be calm or highly anxious. The nursing instructor must carefully select patient care
situations that are relatively stable and that provide coaching about possible changes in
the patient's condition. The instructor fore-casts for the student what he or she should
expect, and students typically rely on standard nursing care plans to guide their planned
care activities. Exceptions and contraindications must be identified for the student by the
nursing instructor or staff nurse caring for the patient. The meanings of vital signs in the
particular situation must be reviewed with the instructor or practicing nurse and the
range of relevant signs and symptoms are reviewed in terms of relevance and are
assessed in the particular patient. A large number of signs and symptoms (e.g., Iethargy,
skin turgor, mental status, etc.) can only be recognized and assessed after they have been
seen in a range of patients.
The best clinical educators are good ethnographers who can give the students access to the
culture and expectations of the clinical units where they are gaining clinical experience. The
clinical educator offers broad guidelines and timelines to guide the student's understanding of the
task world and of the subculture of expectations of a particular unit. Good informants are
identified as resources for the new students in the unit, and the clinical supervision is on call to
deal with questions or emergencies encountered by the student. Novices have only a very limited
ability to forecast futures because of the student's lack of experience with other patients. Usually,
the student must rely on textbook forecasts.
Advanced Beginner, New Graduate
The newly graduated nurse has usually functioned very close to the level of a beginning staff
nurse in his or her final year of nursing education. Typically, newly graduated nurses will not have
functioned in any administrative or managerial functions, though they will have studied principles
and practices related to these roles. The striking change for the newly graduated nurse is that he
or she now has full legal and professional responsibility for patients. This new level of re-
sponsibility and entitlement brings with it changes in the way nurses experience themselves and
the practice environment. They no longer feel that they can always look to other nurses to tell
them what to do or to bear their responsibility. This level of individual and team professional
responsibility heightens the new nurse's sense of engagement with the patient and with clinical
192
Benner Page
problems. This new level of felt responsibility in-creases the beginning nurse's attentiveness to
his or her recognition of features and relevant aspects of the situation; however, the style of
evaluation remains detached and typically lacks integration with other objectively evaluated
signs and symptoms. Beginning nurses look to patients and family members to fill in
expectations of them in their newly forming role. This heightened and qualitatively different kind
of engagement heightens experiential learning and spurs the de velopment of a sense of moral
agency in the professional role (Benner et al., p. 93):
The quality of learning is quite different for new as opposed to more experienced nurses.
Beginners have a level of trust in the environment and in the legitimacy of co-workers'
knowledge, which allows them to absorb information as fact. This trust sets up qualities of
freedom and exhilaration in learning that are probably only avail-able to those who do not
yet comprehend the contingent nature of both the situation and what is known about it.
This freedom in learning is furthered because advanced beginners do not yet feel responsible
for managing clinical situations with which they are unfamiliar.
In what follows, an advanced beginner evidences this "lightness of being" about learning
as he describes a post-operative patient who had undergone complex GI surgery. His entire
statement was delivered in an excited, enthusiastic tone.
1 had learned so much. There are two clinical nurse specialists involved right now. There
are people on the unit who are CNII's and CNIII's who are just really knowledgeable on
major GI surgery on infants. I talked to all these people and pediatric surgery were really
helpful, and our Attendings and fellows were ... I mean, I just learned so much in the last
three days, I couldn't even tell you. (Benner et al., 1996, p. 52)
The advanced beginner has a heightened awareness of any feedback on performance
and pays close attention to the practice of colleagues. He or she actively searches for
credible sources of good and useful information. The nurses now attend their ability to
recognize these aspects of the situation as they are pointed out by colleagues and as they
come to notice them on their own. In the situation above, clinical nurse specialists
assisted with the care of the infant but also engaged in intensive teaching of the new
nurse. The advanced beginner can experience each situation as a myriad of competing
tasks, all of which may feel of equal priority to the new nurse. Anxiety and excessive
fatigue are frequent experiences for new nurses. Worry and anxiety tend to be more global
because the advanced beginner does not yet have a sense of salience with a range of
situations, and the anxiety of learning to perform new tasks is ever present:
And I just talked to myself and I had a great night because this was the first time I did it.... I
was (saying to myself) "Okay. Just take it one step at a time. You're only human, do one
thing then go onto the next thing. It will all get done, it will get done easier if you're calm
and because you think better that way".... And the shift went great. (Benner et al., p. 50)
In coaching an advanced beginner, strategies for keeping anxiety at bay and staying calm
enhance performance capacity because the anxiety is so general. The sense of foreboding and
anxiousness over particular clinical situations is not yet very attuned to the demands, possibilities,
and constraints of the situation simply because of a lack of experienced past similar situations.
Anxiety is ameliorated by this very lack of attunement and sense of salience. Therefore, much
of the experiential learning required of an advanced be-ginner has to do with recognizing more
subtle aspects of the situation. Advanced beginners rely on textbook accounts of patient signs
and symptoms related to diseases, injuries, and therapies, but they may have difficulty
193
Benner Page
recognizing subtle variations and cannot gauge the level of severity in comparison with other
cases simply because of their lack of experienced past and future trajectories with similar
patients. For example, advanced beginners collect their assessment data care-fully and then
consult about the meanings of the numbers and signs and symptoms in a particular case. They will
need to ask questions such as the following: Is this the usual amount of bleeding? Is this a frank
hemorrhage in a postoperative or obstetrical patient? Drain-age from wounds and tubes must be
evaluated in relation to usual quantities and qualities. But these quantities may vary with specific
procedures, patient conditions, and characteristics. The range and variegations cannot be
captured fully in textbook, a problem known in philosophy as the limits of formalism. Also, the
perceptual skills associated with recognizing fuzzy or family resemblances, qualitative
distinctions, and real-life presentations complete with their range of manifestations cannot be
captured in two-dimensional textbooks or single case presentations. This relationship to clinical
mentors is vividly illustrated in the following nursing student's account of a clinical emergency:
This man is a very pleasant fellow, very bright, very alert and awake, and was
unfortunately requiring tracheal suctioning approximately every hour to two hours for
moderate amounts of tracheal secretions which were relatively tena cious in character,
relatively white tannish in color. He unfortunately did not tolerate the suctioning extremely
well. It was relatively uncomfortable for him, caused a moderate amount of cough and gag
reflex, which in turn caused a transient increase in blood pressure. Following suctioning on
one occasion, as I was replacing his tracheal mist mask, he began coughing up very copious
amounts of bright red blood per mouth. I mildly panicked, called for help from the nurse
next door, placed him a moderate Trendelenberg position, opened his I.V. to a rapid rate, and
continued to experience mild panic. Perhaps more like moderate panic. (Benner, 1984, p.
19)
This advanced beginner nurse performed well considering the enormity of the situation. The
student wonders tacitly whether his suctioning technique was too traumatic and therefore
whether it caused the bleed. But notice that the advanced graduate cannot know this because he
has had little experience with patients with similar compromised situations and with the skill of
suctioning itself. There are extraneous details of the story, and the language is couched in
textbook terms. His account responds to the immediate situation, with little or no forecasting of the
future. He gives a full ac-count of his own anxiety in the situation that results, in part, from
hislack of experiential knowledge about what can be done in the situation. Like the novice, the
advanced beginner is dependent on others for filling in his or her experience-based comparisons,
interpretations, and qualitative distinctions. Later, we will contrast this episode with the account
from the expert's perspective.
Competent Stage: 1 to 2 Years in Practice
Developing skill and clinical grasp in particular cases is dependent on experiential learning.
Consequently, how fast someone can gain competence depends on how varied and complex his
or her patient population is. Obviously, nurses working in a high-volume heart surgery center
will gain more experience sooner. But even in the high-volume center, it is usu ally a while
before the newer nurse is assigned complete responsibility for complicated postoperative
patients. Competence with particular patient populations will develop unevenly depending on
experience with that population and with the quality of clinical teaching available in the
institution.
194
Benner Page
The competent stage of skill acquisition is typically a time of heightened planning for what are
now more predictable immediate futures. The competent nurse now decides what is more or less
important based on informal yardsticks learned from past experiences with other patients. The
competent nurse tries to limit the unexpected through planning and analysis and by forecasting
the needs and contingencies of the immediate future, but he or she realizes that there are no rules
to help him or her do it. Anxiety is now more at-tuned to the situation, as illustrated in the
following example where the nurse describes her discovery (experiential learning) that a
postheart transplant surgery patient could not maintain good oxygenation when placed on the
nonoperative side, a physiological principle that the new graduate would probably recognize in a
formal, written test but that is more ambiguous to her in the actual situation:
Nurse: It kind of humbles you. [She realizes that the physiological explanation for this
occurrence was straight forward, but that she had not been able to recognize the problem as
manifested in the particular patient.] At one point, I'm feeling like I have things straight
now, and I can handle the situations, and when something like this hap-pens, I think, well, I
still have a lot of learning to do. I can handle the situations that are status quo; it's the
unexpected that I have to learn to deal with now. But then I think back to situations when I
was brand new. Things that are status quo now weren't back then. Things I can trouble-shoot
and solve now were much different back then. I usually needed help. (Benner et al., 1996, p.
95)
Anxiety is now more tailored to the situation than it was at the novice or advanced beginner
stage when a general anxiety exists over learning and performing well without making mistakes.
Coaching at this point should encourage competent-level nurses to follow through on a sense that
things are not as usual, or even on vague feelings of foreboding or anxiety, because they have to
learn to decide what is relevant with no rules to guide them. There is now enough of an experi-
ential base to have these emotional responses to act like fuzzy recognition of similar and/or past
clinical dissimilar situations. Nurses at this stage feel exhilarated when they perform well and
feel remorse when they recognize that their performance could have been more effective or more
prescient because they had paid attention to the wrong things or had missed relevant subtle signs
and symptoms. These emotional responses are the formative stages of aesthetic appreciation of
good practice. These feelings of satisfaction and uneasiness with performance act as a moral
compass that guides experiential ethical and clinical learning. There is a built-in tension between
the deliberate rule- and maxim-based strategies of organizing, planning, and prediction and
developing a more response-based practice, as pointed out in our study of critical-care nurses:
Not needing help, ordering the task world, and planning based on goals and predictions
structure what the nurse notices, and what are considered issues. It is not accidental that this
vision of performance and agency is institutionally rewarded and encouraged as "standard."
Structuring the day by goals and plans, how-ever, interferes with perceiving the demands
of the situation and with timing interventions in response to the patient's responses and
readiness. The competent nurse seldom sees that signs and symptoms have taken on a new
relevance in a clinical situation due to changes in the patient's condition. Their skill of
seeing is hampered by the need to organize data collection and to achieve goals. Inevitably
the clinical situation intrudes by not matching the goals and plans and the nurse must
adapt ... conceptual descriptions do not automatically lead to recognition of actual signs, and
varied responses require time to assimilate and interpret. Slavishly following one's plans
and holding on to preset expectations can limit perceptual grasp.... Holding on to this de-
liberative form of agency (sense of personal influence in the situation) prevents the nurse
195
Benner Page
from having expert clinical and ethical comportment because response-based organization is
not yet achieved. (Benner et al., pp. 95-96)
Experiential learning with past patient care enables the nurse to develop a greater sense of
salience. As illustrated in the nurse's statement above, there is an in-creasing sense of when the
nurse has or does not have a good clinical grasp of the situation. He or she can use his or her
sense of confusion or questioning to propel his or her understanding of the clinical situation. Be-
cause nurses have now lived through more clinical futures, they can now better predict
immediate likely events and needs of patients and can plant for them.
Toward the upper limits of competent performance, the nurse may begin to apprehend the limits
of formal and practical knowledge. Nurses can now recognize that not everyone is a proficient or
expert clinician, just at the point that they realize that they must develop a perspective on the
situation to perform well in the situation. They typically buy more comprehensive reference
works and medical and nursing textbooks at this point because loss in confidence in the advice of
specific others may be overgeneralized, and consequently, the nurse may feel hyper-responsible.
This inability to trust colleagues can be aggravated by encountering incompetence and a lack of
social integration and informal coaching in the particular clinical unit.
Proficiency: A Transitional Stage on the way to Expertise
It is the felt crisis in the limits of formalism and the limits of planning and prediction along
with an enhanced ability to read the situation that may propel the nurse into the proficient stage of
performance. Whereas skill development up until this point has been incremental, now, to
progress, the learner must make a qualitative leap in the way he or she engages and per-forms in
the situation. The nurse must literally learn to situate himself or herself differently in relation to
his or her work. At this stage, first-person, experience-near narratives (Geertz, 1987) often take
the form of describing changes in the perspective of a situation. The narrative structure is often as
follows: "I went into the situation thinking that I knew what was going on, or that this particular
thing was going on, only to have it disconfirmed by the patient's responses to my assessment."
This is evidence for developing the ability to let the situation guide nurses' responses:
The nurse is now synthesizing the meaning of patient's responses through time. She imagines
that a computer could capture all her readings, but she fails to recognize that her understanding of
the patient is now situated, and based upon a practical understand ing of the patient's responses
[and qualitative changes] made over time rather than a collection of data points. The clinician
struggles with articulating this practical grasp:
Nurse: I had drawn a [blood] gas on a person and the gas was pretty poor and I took another gas
to the house officer and he looked at it and said: "I don't believe this gas, the patient hasn't
changed. And at that point i t takes a while to get to this point, but I felt comfortable in saying
to him: "What do you mean, this patient hasn't changed?" This patient's blood pressure has gone
up to 200, and I presented hire with a picture of this patient that he had obviously over-looked. It
takes a while to get to the point where you can feel comfortable saying this to the doctor and
feeling comfortable, feeling that you can go with your instincts.
Int: What happened in that situation?
Nurse: I was right .. .
Int: How did you learn that the objective signs that you were seeing were correlated with blood
gas?
Nurse: Just experience and seeing different patients and different breathing patterns and
knowing by looking at the patient that this breathing pattern is effective and this one isn't,
knowing whether there is air exchange there or not. These breaths aren't effective and he's wear -
ing himself out and that could be the cause of his deterioration in his gas, and just experience and
seeing different cases and how people adjust to physical things that are going on.
196
Benner Page
Though difficult to articulate, this practical grasp is not mystical. It reflects the skill of seeing
practical manifestations of changed physiological states, patient responses and noticing these
transitions. The nurse actively interprets the direction of the change and keeps track of what can
be ruled in and ruled out. Practical grasp is perceptually grounded and response based and
requires being open to correction and disconfirn,iation as the situation unfolds. The clinician is
always in the situation with some practical under-standing, and it is that practical understanding
that is revised or confirmed. When the practitioner's grasp of the patient's clinical situation is
jarred by changes or unexpected patient responses, the practitioner searches for a new grasp and
deliberation becomes
necessary and if all goes well, experiential clinical learning occurs. They describe the frustrating
situation of "chasing a problem" and never being quite "in synch" with the situation when they do
not have a good perceptual grasp of the situation at hand (Benner et al., 1999, pp. 23-87; Benner et
al., 1996, pp. 146-147). Engaged reasoning through transitions requires being open to correction
and disconfirmation. The ethos of openness, rather than prediction and control, and fidelity to
what one sees and hears, rather than excessive suggestibility and confusion, are embodied and
linked to emotional responses to the situation. Thus, one's skilled emotional responsiveness
guides perceptual acuity and responsiveness to changes in the situations that are similar or
dissimilar to past situations, but when novelty or surprise occurs, the nurse tries to figure out why
and how this situation is different. (Benner et al., 1996, pp. 116-117)
The nurse gains a much more differentiated world of practice at the proficient level. The nurse
feels increasingly at home in the situation and can now recognize when she or he has a good
sense of the situation. In the following excerpt the nurse demonstrates this new comfort level by
describing open-heart surgery patients' trajectories:
Nurse: I feel pretty comfortable, and you learn when they're warming to start giving the
volume and when to stop because now maybe they need a little bit of Levophed to keep their
blood pressure up, when to shut off the Levophed because they're waking up and you know
their catecholamines have kicked in and that kind of thing. It's almost routine, whereas
before it took a lot of trial and asking questions.
This change is based on procedural knowledge and protocols, but the transition being de-
scribed is the flexible recognition of patient changes in particular situations. This recognition
occurs in the context of the predictable changes over time in a recovering heart surgery
patient. These decisions cannot be based on quantitative physiological measures alone, but
must be based on understanding the relationship between the numbers and the way the
patient looks and responds. This form of response-based action is crucial for performing
well in a rapidly changing emergency ... (Benner et al., P.123).
Because the proficient level nurse is learning to adjust his or her responses to the
situation, the skill of both problem and person engagement becomes more differentiated and
attuned. Observing nurses across situations reveals that they vary their relationships with
patients and families based upon their understanding of what the situation requires. Timing
becomes much more re-fined, and recognizing opportunities in the situation for patient
learning or for supporting a patient is now more attuned to the needs and concerns of the
patient.
Nurse: Transplant patients become so de-pendent on you for everything—Can I brush my
teeth now? Do you think I should do this? And you have to really encourage them to take control
back. It's a hard concept for a lot of them because they need to be dependent because it's safe for
them to be dependent.... I've learned how to give them control back slowly and how to en-
courage them to take that control back over their own life. (Benner et al., pp. 125-126)
Once begun, the proficient nurse usually continues to refine his or her reading of particular
197
Benner Page
situations. Re-fining discriminations through deliberate comparisons with past experiences and
other patients improves the nurse's grasp of the situation.
Expertise: Phronesis (Practical Wisdom)
Once a nurse has progressed to proficiency, the style of being a situated, response-based
performer propels experiential learning and the ability to switch from taken-for-granted tacit
expectations to switching to focusing on aspects of the situation that are changing and creating an
altered sense of the situation. The expert nurse is response based in using techne and phronesis.
The expert can now integrate his or her grasp of the situation with his or her responses.
The expert is able to take up theories and ends of practice in multiple ways, often
creating new possibilities in the situation (Taylor, 1991). These situated practical
innovations or sensible variations in practice seem intuitively obvious to the practitioner
and might not be captured easily in a narrative description of the situation. This is why
observation and informal interviewing in actual situations are required to discover and
describe all levels of practice, but particularly proficient and expert levels of practice.
The innovations or sensible variations in practice typically make sense to others as the
most effective response in the situation. They are not breaks with the understanding of
good practice; rather, they extend good practice in challenging underdetermined
situations. As noted in our research, ways of seeing the situation increasingly just call for
appropriate actions.
Intuitive links develop between seeing and responding to the situation. This is revealed by
observing the nurse in the situation and is partially captured in the following account of an
emergency situation where a patient who was hemorrhaging stopped breathing. The links between
the patient's condition and action are sufficiently strong that the nurse attends primarily to actions
rather than assessment of signs and symptoms. This is reasonable because, in extreme
circumstances, the possible responses are fewer, but experience is required to make this shift in
performance:
Nurse: So we didn't even call the code. We just called the doctor stat [emergency] and got
him up there. (They had sufficient people available to resuscitate the patient, so no formal
page for additional help was needed.] I looked at his heart rate and I said: "O.K. he is
bradying down. Someone want to give me some atropine?" I just started calling out the drugs
that I needed to get for this guy, so we started to push these drugs in. In the meantime, I said,
can we have some more blood?" I was just barking out this stuff [the things that were
needed and had to be done]. I can't even tell you the sequence. I was saying, "We need this."
I needed to anticipate what was going to happen and I could do this because I had been
through this a week before with this guy and knew what we had done [and what had
worked].
The recognition and assessment language are minimal, in part, because the number of
actions per problem are limited, but also because recognition and assessment language
become so linked with actions and outcomes that they b-come self-evident or "obvious" for
the expert practitioner. This is the kind of "maximum grasp" of the expert that is not
available to the proficient performer. Immediate futures obvious to the expert order the
situation. In this case the nurse becomes the situational leader because of this maximum
grasp of this particular patient, and the sequence of events. The integrated rapid response is
the hallmark of expertise. (Benner et al., 1996, pp. 142-143)
Based on enriched experiential learning spawned by increasing ability to read the current
situation in terms of their deep familiarity with similar and dissimilar situations, nurses develop a
198
Benner Page
sense of whether they have a good (better or poorer) grasp of the situation. Skilled know-how
now allows for more fluid and rapid performance of procedures. Narratives often focus on new
clinical learning or troubling moral dilemmas or conflicts in the situation. Qualitative
distinctions associated with nuanced responses make the nurse able to know and do more than he
or she can tell or think to de-scribe (Polanyi, 195811962). [Good. This is expertise alright. Notice
there is no talk her of innovation.]
Attunement allows for flexible fusion of thought, feeling, and action. Seeing the unexpected
based on having a rich set of expectations as well as a rich sense of the particular situation
requires engagement with the patient and openness to notice when things do not go as implicitly
expected, and evidence that disconfirm one's assumptions can be encountered (Benner et al.,
1999, pp. 85-86). The relational skills of attunement to the patient's concerns and to the clinical
situation create the possibility for patients and family members to disclose or reveal their
concerns and fears to the nurse and for the nurse to notice changes in the patient or family across
time (transitions). The quality of attentiveness and of the relationship literally creates different
disclosive spaces and moods for the patient and family so that different clinical issues are
noticed based on qualitatively different disclosive possibilities. [This is as innovative as
expertise gets.]
Now it is possible to compare the expert's narrative below of the situation of the patient who
developed a carotid hemorrhage with the advanced beginner's ac-count given earlier:
I had worked late and was just about ready to go home, when a nurse preceptor said to me,
"Jolene, come here." Her voice had urgency in it, but not Code Blue. I walked in and I looked
at the patient and his heart rate was about 120, and he was on the respirator and breathing.
And I asked her: "What's wrong?" There was a new graduate taking care of him. And he just
pointed down to the patient who was lying in a pool of blood. There was a big stream of
blood drooling out of his mouth. This man's diagnosis was mandibular cancer, which had
been resected, and about a week previous to that he had had a carotid bleed from external
carotid, which had been, ligated secondary to radiation erosion. That wound had become
septic and he had developed respiratory failure and he was in ICU for that. So I looked at the
dressing and it was dry, the blood was coming out of his mouth. The man had a tracheostomy
because of the type of surgery that had been done. He also had an N.G. tube in for feedings,
and I got to thinking that it might be the innominate or the carotid artery that had eroded. So
we took him off the ventilator to see if any-thing was going to pump out of his trach. There
was a little blood, but it looked mostly like it had come down from the pharynx into the
lungs. So we began ventilating him, trying to figure what was inside his mouth that was
pumping out his tremendous amount of blood. (Benner, 1984, p. 17)
This nurse went on to describe her quick actions to draw blood for a cross-match and typing
and preparing the man for an immediate transfer to the operating room after marshalling all the
resources for the surgical team. She gives us an immediate, direct grasp of the nature of the
situation. Action, thought, and feeling are fused. She evaluates the resistance in the lungs by hand
ventilating the patient. Fortunately, because of her rapid responses, the patient survived the
hemorrhage.
Expert practice, by its very nature, is of local, specific knowledge; know-how; and technical
and scientific knowledge that is more transferable to other practice contexts. Because practice is a
way of knowing through experiential learning and embodied know-how, it is highly valuable to
study and articulate the knowledge embedded in highly complex practices such as nursing and
medicine. Articulation of the knowledge embedded in proficient and expert practice, plus
articulation the range of practical knowledge learned by beginning practitioners in local settings,
creates the possibility of self-improving practice based on making experiential learning public
199
Benner Page
and therefore open to development so that experiential learning and practical wisdom becomes
cumulative and shared.
Summary
Each of the studies was based on extensive first-person, experience-near narrative
accounts of clinical situations that stood out in the participants' minds. In addition, a
subsample of participants was observed and informally interviewed in its practice. We
deliberately sampled nurses with a range of experience and reported skillfulness and interviewed
nurses with like backgrounds in small group narrative interviews. We created an open dialogue
with the tenets of the Dreyfus model of skill acquisition and the philosophical basis for this
model. We found that the model was predictive and descriptive of distinct stages of skill
acquisition in nursing practice. The most qualitatively distinct difference lies between the
competent and proficient level, where the practitioner begins to read the situation. The proficient
performer begins to increasingly change his or her perception of the nature of the situation and
then deliberates about changing plans or strategies in response to the new understanding of the
situation. The expert develops yet another qualitatively distinct way of being in the situation by
developing the capacity to fluidly respond to the situation, even as the situation changes and the
relevance of the actions taken change. The study of nursing practice, because it is an
underdetermined, complex practice that requires skillful comportment, articulation, and highly
developed relational skills, allowed us to identify qualitatively distinct forms of moral agency
and skills of involvement at different levels of skill acquisition. The development of moral agency
and the influence of emotional engagement with the person and the problem, as well as
emotional climate, on skill acquisition vary distinctly at each stage of skill acquisition. For
example, the advanced beginner focuses on getting everything done adequately. The competent
nurse increases his or her ability to advocate for the patient, getting what the patient needs or
requests. At the expert level, the moral agency and skills of involvement create disclosive spaces
that would not have even been imagined at the earlier stages. New possibilities and notions of
good practice are instantiated in more skillful, ethical comportment and relational. capacities.
Expert nurses are extremely pleased when they are able to comfort or assist patients in coping
with the demands of the illness.
We found that nurses who had some difficulty with understanding the ends of practice
and difficulty with their skills of interpersonal and problem engagement did not go on to
become expert nurses (see Rubin, 1996). They literally thought of rational calculation as
the scientific and objective way of practicing, and thus, they failed to see significant moral
concerns and failed to recognize qualitative distinctions between situations because they
attempted to apply the same metric of rationally calculating odds, prevalence, and
evidence in each situation. This computational and calculative approach to practice,
coupled with a disciplined stance of detachment, blocked experiential learning. The
model was also useful in helping us articulate knowledge and skill embedded in the
practice of nursing. The rational-technical vision of performance is that of a practitioner
or technical expert developing mastery of a body of knowledge and applying that
knowledge in prespecified ways for prespecified outcomes. The rational-technical model
does not account for development of relational, perceptual, or skillful comportment
overtime. It also does not account for the role of experiential learning in learning to
practice in a dynamic, underdetermined, and complex practice such as nursing and
medicine. A strict technical application of knowledge does not take into account the skills
200
Benner Page
required for discerning the nature of the situation and its possibilities and constraints.
Even the expert in the Dreyfus model of skill acquisition must stay attuned to the situation
and must remain open to the unexpected. Practitioners on the account of expert
comportment provided by the Dreyfus model must remain open to experiential learning
and reading changes in transitions in fast-paced, open-ended environments. In the
Dreyfus model, the practitioner is assumed to dwell with increasing skill and finesse in a
meaningful, intelligible, but changing, world.
Note
I. The first study was sponsored by a grant from the Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health
Service, Division of Nursing, Bureau of Health Professions (Grant No. 7 D10 NU 29104-03). The Helene Fuld Health
Trust funded the second and third studies.
References
Benner, P. (1982). From novice to expert. American Journal of Nursing, 82,402-407.
Benner, P. (1984). From novice to expert: Excellence and power in clinical nursing practice. Reading, NIA: Addison-
Wesley-
Benner, P. (1994). The role of articulation in understanding practice and experience as sources of knowledge in clinical
nursing. In J. Tully (Ed.), Philosophy in an age of pluralism: The philosophy of Charles Taylor in question (pp. 136-
155). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Benner, P., Hooper-Kyriakidis, P., & Stannard, D. (1999) Clinical wisdom and interventions in critical care: A thinking-
in-action approach. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Benner, P., Tanner, C. A., & Chesla, C. A. (1992). From beginner to expert: Gaining a differentiated clinical world in
critical care nursing. Advances in Nursing Science, 14(3), 13-28.
Benner, P., Tanner, C. A., & ChesIa, C. A. (1996). Expertise in nursing practice: Caring, clinical judgment, and ethics.
New York: Springer.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S.E. (1977). Uses and abuses of multi-attribute and multi-aspect model of decision
making. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of
the computer. New York: Free Press.
Dreyfus, S. E. (1982) Formal models vs. human situational under-standing: Inherent limitations on the modeling of
business expertise. Office: Technology and People, I, 133-155.
Dreyfus, S. E., & Dreyfus, H. L. (1979). The scope, limits, and training implications of three models of aircraft pilot
emergency response behavior: Unpublished report, Uniyersity of California, Berkeley.
Dreyfus, S. E., & Dreyfus, H. L. (1980). A five-stage model of the mental activities involved in directed skill
acquisition. Unpublished report, University of California, Berkeley.
Dunne, J. (1997). Back to the rough ground, practical judgment and the lure of technique. Notre Dame, IN: University
of Notre Dame Press.
Gadamer, H. (1975). Truth and method (G. Barden & J. Cumming, Trans.). New York: Seabury. (Original work
published 1960)
Geertz, C. (1987). Deep play: Notes on the Balinese cockfight. In P. Rabinow & W. Sullivan (Eds.), Interpretive social
science: A second look. Berkeley: University of California Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1981). After virtue: A study in moral theory. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
Polanyi, M. (195811962). Personal knowledge, Towards a post-critical philosophy. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Rubin, J. (1996) Impediments to the development of clinical knowledge and ethical judgment in critical care nursing. In
P. Benner, C. Tanner, & C. Chesla (Eds.), Expertise in nursing
practice, caring, clinical judgment and ethics (pp. 170-192).
New York: Springer.
Shulman, L. S. (1993). Teaching as community property. Change, 25,6-7.
201
Benner Page
Taylor, C. (1991). Ethics of authenticity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Taylor, C. (1993). Explanation andpractical reason. In M. Nussbaum &
A. Sen (Eds.), The quality of life. Oxford UK: Clarendon.
Patricia Benner is professor and chair of the Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, University of
California, San Francisco, where she teaches in the nursing and sociology doctoral programs. She is the
author or coauthor of three research-based books that draw on the work of Hubert L Dreyfus and Stuart E.
Dreyfus: From Novice to Expert: Excellence and Power in Clinical Nursing Practice (1984); From
Beginner to Expert: Caring, Ethics and Clinical Judgment in Clinical Nursing Practice (1996 ); and Clinical
Wisdom and Interventions in Critical Care: A Thinking-in-Action Approach (1999). All three of these books
have won the American Journal of Nursing Book of the Year Award and have been translated into other
languages.
Citation for this paper:
Benner, P. 2005 Using the Dreyfus Model of Skill Acquisition to describe and interpret
skill acquisition and clinical judgment in nursing practice and education.
The Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society Special Issue: Human
Expertise in the Age of the Computer. Vol. 24 (3) 188-199.
202