ArticlePublisher preview available

Elaborative Interrogation Facilitates Adult Learning of Factual Paragraphs

American Psychological Association
Journal of Educational Psychology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The authors evaluated the potency of elaborative interrogation for promoting acquisition of facts in paragraphs. University students studied 6-sentence factual paragraphs about 5 universities (1 fact per sentence). In general, elaborative interrogation facilitated learning better than did self-reference (Experiment 1 only) and reading-control conditions; performances in the elaborative-interrogation conditions equaled performances in the imagery conditions. The elaborative interrogation over control advantage was obtained for both intentional and incidental learning (Experiment 2) and both when subjects processed sentences individually and when they generated answers for each new sentence in a paragraph by considering information presented earlier in the paragraph (Experiment 2). Even when elaborative-interrogation subjects could not recall facts in their entirety, they were more likely than control subjects to have learned the associations between the university and the factual attribute.
Journal
of
Educational Psychology
1990,
Vol. 82, No.
3,
513-524Copyright
1990
by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-0663/90/$00.?5
Elaborative Interrogation Facilitates Adult Learning
of Factual Paragraphs
Vera E. Woloshyn
University
of
Western Ontario
London, Ontario, Canada
Eileen Wood
Wilfred Laurier University
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
Teena Willoughby
University
of
Waterloo
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
Michael Pressley
University
of
Maryland
The authors evaluated the potency of elaborative interrogation
for
promoting acquisition of facts
in paragraphs. University students studied 6-sentence factual paragraphs about
5
universities
(1
fact
per
sentence).
In
general, elaborative interrogation facilitated learning better than
did self-
reference (Experiment
1
only)
and
reading-control conditions; performances
in the
elaborative-
interrogation conditions equaled performances
in the
imagery conditions.
The
elaborative
interrogation over control advantage
was
obtained
for
both intentional
and
incidental learning
(Experiment 2) and both when subjects processed sentences individually and when they generated
answers
for
each
new
sentence
in a
paragraph
by
considering information presented earlier
in
the paragraph (Experiment
2).
Even when elaborative-interrogation subjects could not recall facts
in their entirety, they were more likely than control subjects
to
have learned
the
associations
between
the
university
and the
factual attribute.
Sometimes students lack prior knowledge that might make
obvious the significance
and
meaning of to-be-acquired facts
(e.g., Bransford
et al.,
1982).
If
so, fact learning reduces
to
rote learning
of
arbitrary associations,
a
process known
to be
difficult
(e.g.,
Rohwer,
1973).
Alternatively, even when people
possess relevant prior knowledge that can be used
to
elaborate
novel facts
to
make them more meaningful
and
memorable,
they may fail to activate such knowledge (Pressley, McDaniel,
Turnure, Wood,
&
Ahmad,
1987;
Pressley, Symons,
Mc-
Daniel, Snyder,
&
Turnure,
1988).
One purpose of our studies
was
to
evaluate
one
mechanism that
can
encourage students
to activate relevant prior knowledge
in
response
to
novel facts
and,
in
turn, facilitate acquisition of those facts.
Bransford and his associates (e.g., Stein & Bransford,
1979;
Stein, Littlefield, Bransford,
&
Persampieri, 1984) presented
learners with information about types
of
men: specifically,
statements about actions that were arbitrarily associated with
a number
of man
types (e.g., **The tall
man
bought
the
crackers,"
"The
hungry
man got
into
the
car"). They found
that when adults
and
children were provided elaborations
for
arbitrary facts that made
the
significance
of
the man-action
pairings more obvious
(i.e.,
precise elaborations,
to
use Brans-
ford
et al/s 1982
terminology), arbitrary facts were more
memorable. Thus
"The
tall
man
bought
the
crackers that
were
on the top
shelf
was
more memorable than
"The
tall
man bought the crackers." Bransford
et
al. (1982) argued that
provision of precise elaborations helps to activate the learner's
This research
was
supported
in
part
by an
operating grant
to
Michael
Pressley
from
the
Natural Sciences
and Engineering Research
Council of Canada.
Correspondence concerning this article should
be
addressed
to
Vera
E.
Woloshyn, Department of Psychology, University of Western
Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
N6A
5C2.
prior knowledge
and
promotes integration
of
seemingly arbi-
trary information with previous experience, making the novel
facts more comprehensible
and
memorable. Unfortunately,
memory gains produced
by
providing precise elaborations
to
adults are often small.
More optimistically, Pressley et
al.
(1987) identified another
procedure designed
to
encourage learners
to
make connec-
tions between arbitrary facts
and
their prior knowledge. Pres-
sley
et
al. instructed their subjects
to
generate
an
answer
to a
uwhy" question (e.g., "Why did that particular man do that?")
when presented
man
statements. Pressley
et al.
found that
this approach (elaborative interrogation) produced larger
learning gains that
did
provision of precise elaborations.
One
reason
why the
self-generated elaborations
may be
more
effective than experimenter-provided ones
is
that they
are
more likely
to be
consistent with
the
learner's prior knowl-
edge.
Alternatively, elaborative interrogation
may
involve
more conscious
and
effortful memory processes than does
provision
of
elaborations (Hasher
&
Zacks,
1979,
Jacoby,
1978),
and the
resulting memory trace
may be
more distinc-
tive (e.g., Slamecka
& Graf,
1978).
The
elaborative-interro-
gation effect seems consistent with other demonstrations that
learning
is
better when study includes active generation
of
information (e.g., Jacoby, 1978; Schwartz,
1971;
Slamecka
Sc
Fevrieski,
1983;
Slamecka &
Graf,
1978).
In addition
to
enhancing acquisition
of
essentially random
associations (i.e.,
man
sentences), Pressley
et al.
(1988) also
demonstrated that elaborative interrogation promotes learn-
ing of more educationally relevant content. Canadian univer-
sity students were presented facts about Canadian geography,
history, economics,
and
sociology.
If,
during study, students
answered "why" questions about
the
facts (e.g., "Why would
it make sense that British Columbia
is the
province with
the
highest percentage of its population
in
unions?"), retention
of
513
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Both adult and child populations can be instructed to create mental images to facilitate acquisition of prose materials (Pressley, Symons, McDaniel, Snyder, & Tumure, 1988;Woloshyn, Willoughby, Wood, & Pressley, 1990;Wood, Pressley, & Winne, 1990). Imagery strategies may be particularly advantageous because they allow learners to generate concrete images even from unfamiliar material. ...
... For many studies comparing elaborative interrogation to imagery, there are no significant differences between the two strategies in facilitation of memory performance (Pressley et al., 1988;Woloshyn et al., 1990). However, differences are apparent when object familiarity has been manipulated. ...
Article
Full-text available
The authors examined the role of distinctiveness in the differential memory performance of visual and verbal elaboration strategies. Twenty-eight undergraduates learned information about familiar and unfamiliar animals while using either the visual (imagery) or verbal (answering why questions) elaboration strategies. There was no difference between the 2 strategies in level of organization for the familiar animal material, but imagery students organized unfamiliar animal information into intact sets more than verbal elaboration students. Imagery easily permits the generation of relations and distinctions for unfamiliar material through the creation of unique mental images. In contrast, learners using verbal elaboration may be more likely to access general rather than specific information when answering why questions for unfamiliar information.
... We expected that with familiar domain materials elaborative interrogation would be more beneficial than reading for understanding which, in turn, would be more helpful than no exposure (e.g., Kuhara-Kojima & Hatano, 1991;Woloshyn et al., 1990). We were less certain how large the corresponding effects would be for materials from an unfamiliar domain. ...
... There are several noteworthy features of the data in Table 3. First, for the situation most similar to our previous work (i.e., subjects constructing explanations about facts from a familiar domain), the outcomes in this study are similar to previous outcomes: Subjects were able to generate adequate responses most of the time. In previous work (e.g., Woloshyn et al., 1990), the conditional probabilities of recall differed descriptively such that adequately answered items were remembered better than inadequately answered items, which were remembered better than no-response items. However, these descriptive differences were not very great. ...
Article
Full-text available
Canadian and West German adults were presented facts about Canadian provinces and West German states (at the time of the study, West Germany was a separate nation of what is now the Federal Republic of Germany). Participants in the elaborative-interrogation condition rationalized why each fact was sensible. Reading-to-understand participants read the facts with the goal of comprehending each one. After presentation of all facts, subjects were asked to match provinces and states to facts associated with them. These performances were compared with matching in a no-exposure-control condition. Prior knowledge had a striking effect on learning in both the elaborative-interrogation and reading-to-understand conditions. Elaborative interrogation also promoted learning of all facts for all participants, although when learners lacked prior knowledge, elaborative interrogation did not produce the high levels of performance that were obtained when subjects possessing high prior knowledge simply read the facts. Both strategies and prior knowledge are critical to efficient learning.
... This is only one example of a relationship trigger. These methods are effective in their own right (e.g., Dornisch, Sperling, & Zeruth, 2011;Ozgungor & Guthrie, 2004;Pressley, McDaniel, Turnure, Wood, & Ahmad, 1987;Woloshyn, Willoughby, Wood, & Pressley, 1990). For example, in a study on synthesis writing that De La Paz and Felton carried out over three days, high school students were given the ISSN: 2583-6846 ...
Article
Writing assignments at the collegiate level that emphasize synthesis are quite often assigned. In reading-to-writing tasks, students must choose, organize, and integrate facts from a variety of sources in order to produce an original piece of writing. This might be challenging for certain students. Consequently, it could be challenging for college students to construct a synthesis that has a distinct structure and is tightly connected to their own ideas. This research aimed to get a deeper understanding of the SOAR strategy system, which assists students in selecting, arranging, linking, and controlling various forms of information. This was accomplished in full compliance with the requirements for the synthesis writing process. In the first piece of study, the value of the educational tools that SOAR has made available to its users was investigated. Reading four separate books—four that can be finished with or without the SOAR supplements, depending on the student's preference—helps college students prepare to write synthesis essays. These four readings can be completed with or without the SOAR supplements. Students who did not utilize SOAR as their guide were able to write essays that were less comprehensive (in terms of the selection of relevant materials), less categorically organized (in terms of structuring), and had fewer intertextual relationships than students who did use SOAR as their guide (connecting). The second experiment investigated the degree to which SOA is trainable and how the quality of synthesis writing is affected by student-created SOAR tools. Students in higher education were tasked with completing their first assignment, an essay in the form of basic synthesis. After that, individuals either elected to participate in the SOAR program or decided against it and relied on their methods instead. They finished yet another writing synthesis project as their very final assignment for the day. Students who had participated in SOAR instruction could produce better-structured essays and covered more ground than those who had not participated in the program. During the dialogues, a broad variety of topics were discussed. Some of these topics included the underlying cognitive mechanisms, the limits of the study, prospective directions that future research would pursue, and the practical ramifications.
... Los resultados indicaron que los estudiantes que habían participado de la condición de preguntas de elaboración tuvieron un mejor desempeño en una prueba de verdadero/falso en comparación con aquellos que solo habían leído el material, incluso en relación con información del texto que no había sido cubierta por las preguntas. Por su parte, Woloshyn, Willoughby, Wood y Pressley (1990) encontraron que un grupo universitarios canadienses a los que se había presentado datos sobre cinco universidades y habían recibido preguntas de elaboración tuvieron un mejor desempeño en tareas de recuerdo que aquellos que no habían recibido estas preguntas. Esta facilitación se observó tanto cuando los participantes habían sido informados acerca de las tareas de recuerdo que tendrían que realizar (condición de aprendizaje intencional), como cuando no lo habían sido (condición de aprendizaje incidental). ...
Article
Full-text available
La comprensión del discurso es central para la participación social y el desempeño en el ámbito educativo. Estudios previos sugieren que la realización de tareas de elaboración la facilitan, dado que promueven que los lectores se involucren con el texto, y lo procesen más profundamente. Entre ellas, las preguntas de elaboración (tales como: ‘Por qué la oración que leíste es cierta?’) tienen un rol destacado. Considerando la importancia de facilitar la comprensión de textos para el ámbito educativo, el objetivo de esta revisión narrativa es presentar estudios que han examinado el rol de estas preguntas en la comprensión del discurso narrativo y expositivo por parte de estudiantes de distintos niveles educativos y con distintas lenguas, con el fin de destacar contribuciones y brechas en la investigación actual, y señalar preguntas pendientes para la realización de futuros estudios. Entre ellas, se destacará que estudios previos no han tendido a examinar el interjuego entre la presentación de preguntas y la modalidad de presentación del material, ni a presentar materiales extensos que involucren discurso espontáneo, o que traten acerca de problemáticas sociales actuales. A su vez, se propondrán aplicaciones al ámbito educativo, las cuales pueden brindar herramientas al docente. Se espera que este trabajo contribuya a destacar la necesidad de plantearse nuevas preguntas de investigación que contribuyan a promover la comprensión del discurso que los estudiantes procesan durante el dictado de clases, durante su participación social en el ámbito cotidiano, y que traten acerca de temas de relevancia social actual.
... SOAR is grounded in infor mation-processing theory. rather than their integration (e.g., Carney & Levin, 2014;Chang, Sung, & Chen, 2002;Dye, 2000;Thiede & Anderson, 2003;Woloshyn, Willoughby, Wood, & Pressley, 1990) or has advanced strategy models too complicated to apply (e.g., Hattie & Donoghue, 2016;Thomas & Rohwer, 1986). In either case, students are without clear direction for how to learn and study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Study techniques most commonly used by students are usually the least effective for learning. Therefore, a sample of undergraduate students was trained in the use of Elaborative Interrogation (EI), a more powerful strategy. However, there are no clear guidelines on how to implement this technique, and there are contrasting results on its effectiveness. The present study aimed at analysing the effectiveness of EI in undergraduate students' performance. A pre-test post-test design was used with 3 within-participants conditions: control, self-generated EI and externally-generated EI. The effects that training and type and quality of EI implementation had on students' comprehension (at textbase and situation model levels) were investigated. Results suggest that the EI technique may be useful to improve undergraduates’ learning, but only in very specific situations. Otherwise, it may even hinder performance compared to traditional study techniques. Some insights are offered to better understand how the EI strategy works.
Article
Imagination-to-learn is a specific learning strategy that has been studied in many academic fields. The present study investigated whether imagination is beneficial overall for learning compared to conventional study strategies, while also identifying moderator factors affecting the global effect. A meta-analysis was conducted by scientifically rigorous experiments comparing the learning outcomes of students who were asked to form a mental image of the events described in learning material while reading (imagination condition) or were given no imagination instructions (control condition). A total of 21 experimental studies published on the PsycINFO, Web of Science, ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, ERIC and Google Scholar databases were included, yielding 70 pair-wise comparisons with N = 2625 participants. An overall positive effect of imagination-to-learn was found for both retention (g+ = 0.40, 95% CI [0.23, 0.58], z = 4.63, p < .001) and transfer (g+ = 0.51, 95% CI [0.22, 0.43, z = 3.43, p < .001]) performance. However, analysis of the funnel plots showed that publication bias was present in the reporting of learning outcomes. Analysis of the moderators indicated that the effect sizes differed significantly only with respect to learners’ prior knowledge for transfer performance and their educational level for retention scores. Based on these findings, the present study provides important directions for future research and practices.
Article
Full-text available
La presente investigación tiene como objetivo determinar la dimensión del proceso cognitivo que promueven las preguntas contenidas en los textos escolares de Historia, Geografía y Ciencias Sociales de la educación básica en Chile. Se trató de un estudio cuantitativo en el cual se analizaron 1775 preguntas a la luz de la taxonomía de Anderson y Krathwohl (2001). Se aplicaron pruebas estadísticas no paramétricas a los datos obtenidos. Entre los resultados se evidencia que los textos escolares presentan preguntas de nivel memorístico y reproductivo, lo cual incide en un bajo desarrollo cognitivo de los estudiantes. Las preguntas que demandan niveles cognitivos superiores representan un mínimo porcentaje, a pesar de la importancia que revisten para el desarrollo de competencias intelectuales, específicamente, de pensamiento histórico.
Article
This article extends previous discussions of the potential impact of cognitive psychology on school psychology. Specifically, it reviews themes in cognitive psychology on the relationship of children's knowledge and strategies to learning, memory, and the performance of academic tasks. A review of the literature indicates, not surprisingly, (a) the content-related knowledge of children is often related positively to academic performance; (b) situations exist, however, in which children's knowledge can affect performance on some types of tasks more than others, or in which knowledge can have a deleterious effect on performance; (c) the ability to access knowledge can be a problem, especially in certain content areas and for certain types of children; (d) types of knowledge other than content knowledge, such as metacognitive, can have a substantial impact on academic performance. The literature on strategies indicates that strategic competency is related to changes in the structure and efficiency of strategies with development, the context in which the strategies are used, and the ability to apply and regulate the use of strategies. The implications of some of these data for assessment and consultation with teachers about instruction are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Assigned 216 undergraduates 1 study and 1 recall trial on a list of 10 pairs. In 1 group, Ss were instructed to generate mediators to help them associate the stimulus and response terms; in another, Ss were supplied with mediators which, on a normative basis, had a high probability of being generated. A 3rd group served as control. Orthogonal variables were study-trial presentation rate (2 sec. vs. 6 sec.) and delay of recall (0 delay vs. 15-min delay). In response recall, the generated condition surpassed the supplied, which surpassed the control. Results of a postrecall mediation questionnaire suggest that the stimulus was a more effective retrieval cue for generated than for supplied mediators, although both types of mediators were equally effective as retrieval cues for the response. In all conditions, a strong relationship was found between reported use of a mediator on the recall trial and correct response recall. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Reviews studies that have shown that memory for an item is better when that item is encoded with reference to the self than when it is encoded with reference to some other person and examines the work of C. G. Lord (see record 1981-07095-001), who observed a reversal of the self-reference effect and attributed this reversal to use of imagery. The present series of 6 experiments conducted with 261 undergraduates contradicted Lord's hypothesis that the self-reference effect is reversed when Ss use imagery to encode the material. In these experiments, self-reference effects were consistently observed with imagery instructions. It is suggested that the benefit of self-reference results from the use of episodic memory when forming images of the self. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Two studies with 48 undergraduates examined the contention that the self-reference effect occurs because the self provides a set of organized internal cues in the form of personal experiences that can mediate recall. The 2 properties—constructibility and associability of internal cues—are also important to the self-reference effect. S-generated cues composed of personal experiences representing the internal self were compared with cues composed of names of body parts representing the external self (Exp I). The body-part cues were more easily reconstructed by Ss at recall than were personal-experience cues. Nevertheless, trait words were better recalled after being related to personal experiences, because trait words and personal experiences were easily associated. In Exp II, concrete nouns were presented rather than trait words, and no difference in recall using the 2 types of cues was found. This occurred because concrete nouns can be easily associated with either personal experiences or body parts. (30 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In a study with 62 high school seniors, Ss instructed to form mental images while reading a 2,000-word textbook-like passage learned no more than those merely asked to read carefully. On a postexperiment questionnaire, more than 1/2 of the control group reported using imagery, while about 1/3 of the experimental group did not use imagery. Amount of learning was directly related to the amount of time during which imagery was reportedly used. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Across three experiments adults were presented either base sentences containing arbitrary relations (e.g., The fat man read the sign) or base sentences accompanied by a precise elaboration that made clear the significance of the particular type of man doing the action (e.g., The fat man read the sign warning about thin ice). Subjects either read the sentence alone or answered a question accompanying the sentence. Why did that particular man do that? accompanied base sentences; How does the last part of the sentence make clear why that particular man did that? accompanied precisely elaborated sentences. Both intentional and incidental learning were studied. The positive effects of providing precise elaborations were moderate in size and confined to incidental learning. Much larger incidental and intentional learning gains followed generation of elaborations in response to questions, with generated precise elaborations facilitating acquisition slightly better than generated imprecise elaborations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In two studies we investigated the role of self-reference as a mnemonic for prose material. Prior to reading descriptive passages, college students received self-reference, other-reference, linguistic, or control processing instructions. Overall, the self-reference instructions resulted in the greatest amount of recall. This effect generalized across both within- and between-subjects experimental designs. Further, minor changes in the wording of the processing instructions (question vs. perspective format) did not alter the findings. In addition, subjects' responses (yes or no) to the self-reference question were not related to the level of recall. The discussion focuses on theoretical alternatives and outlines directions for future research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Presents an overview for 3 sets of experiments that examine differences in the way that academically successful and less successful 5th graders approach the problem of learning new information. The overview includes discussions of (a) theoretical approaches to the problem of learning new information, (b) pilot studies that suggest differences in successful and less successful students' approaches to learning, and (c) specific hypotheses about reasons for the differences in learning suggested by the pilot studies. Also described are how these hypotheses will be tested in 3 sets of experiments (B. S. Stein et al, see PA, Vol 70:851 and 850; J. J. Franks et al, see PA, Vol 70:815). (49 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)