Content uploaded by Martha C Shaw
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Martha C Shaw on Oct 20, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
Neuropsychological performance, impulsivity, ADHD symptoms,
and novelty seeking in compulsive buying disorder
Donald Wayne Blacka,*, Martha Shawa, Brett McCormicka, John David Baylessb, and Jeff
Allena
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Iowa Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver College of Medicine,
Iowa City, IA 52242, USA
bDivision of Neuropsychology, Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa Roy J. and Lucille A.
Carver College of Medicine, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA
Abstract
We examined the neuropsychological performance of people with compulsive buying disorder
(CBD) and control subjects, along with trait impulsivity, symptoms of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and selected personality characteristics. Subjects received a
comprehensive neuropsychological test battery, depression and ADHD symptom assessment, the
Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, and a version of the Temperament and Character Inventory. Persons
with CBD (
n
=26) and controls (
n
=32) were comparable in terms of age, sex, and years of
education. Subjects with CBD had a mean age of 36.3 years (S.D.=15.7) and an age at onset of
19.7 years (S.D.=7.0). Compulsive buyers had more lifetime mood, anxiety, and impulse control
disorders. People with Compulsive buying performed significantly better on the Wechsler
Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Picture Completion task, a test of visual perception; otherwise,
there were no consistent differences in neuropsychological measures. They also had elevated
levels of self-reported depression, ADHD symptoms, trait impulsivity, and novelty seeking. In
conclusion, compulsive buyers have greater lifetime psychiatric comorbidity than controls, and
higher levels of self-rated depression, ADHD symptoms, trait impulsivity, and novelty seeking.
The present study does not support the notion that there is a pattern of neuropsychological deficits
associated with CBD.
Keywords
Compulsive buying disorder; Impulsivity; ADHD symptoms; Novelty seeking; Neuropsychology;
Decision-making; Executive function
1. Introduction
Compulsive buying disorder (CBD) is characterized by excessive or poorly controlled
preoccupations, urges, or behaviors regarding shopping and spending that lead to subjective
distress or impaired quality of life (Black, 2007, 2010). The disorder has an estimated rate of
nearly 6% in the adult United States population (Koran et al., 2006). CBD is associated with
co-occurring mood, anxiety, substance use, and other impulse control disorders (Black et al.,
1998; Mller et al., 2009, 2010a). While most epidemiological and clinical research suggests
© 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
*Correspondence address: 2-126b MEB/Psychiatry Research, University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine, Iowa City, IA 52245,
USA. Tel.: +1 319 353 4431. donald-black@uiowa.edu (D.W. Black).
Drs. Allen and Bayless, Mr. McCormick, and Ms. Shaw report no conflicts.
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
Published in final edited form as:
Psychiatry Res
. 2012 December 30; 200(0): 581–587. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.003.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
the disorder has a female preponderance (Black, 2010), the survey reported by Koran et al.
(2006) found nearly equal rates in men and women. The disorder is not included in the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition
(DSM-IV; American
Psychiatric Association, 2000), and there are presently no plans to include it in DSM-5
(www.dsm5.org).
The appropriate classification of CBD has been debated, but some consider it a behavioral
addiction similar in many respects to classic alcohol and drug dependencies (Hollander and
Allen, 2006). The concept of behavioral addiction includes disorders that the National
Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) considers relatively pure models of addiction because the
presence of an exogenous substance does not contaminate their processes (Holden, 2001). In
addition to CBD, potential behavioral addictions include pathological gambling (PG),
kleptomania, compulsive sexual behavior, and Internet addiction (Black et al., 2012). These
disorders share common core clinical features such as the performance of repetitive
behaviors despite negative consequences, diminished control over their urges, craving prior
to engaging in the behavior, and experiencing a pleasurable response while engaged in the
behavior (Grant et al., 2006). Other potential categorizations have been proposed for these
disorders. Hollander (1993a, 1993b) and others (Koran, 1999) have long promoted the
concept of an obsessive–compulsive spectrum, but evidence is limited (Dell’Osso et al.,
2006; Tavares and Gentil, 2007). Others have suggested that behavioral addictions are
related to bipolar disorder (Di Nicola et al., 2010a, 2010b).
Converging evidence from the fields of genetics, neuropharmacology, and brain imaging
suggests that CBD is a neuropsychiatric syndrome (Black, 2007, 2010; Raab et al., 2010).
The disorder appears familial and has a genetic relationship with mood and substance use
disorders (McElroy et al., 1994; Black et al., 1998). Some investigators believe that
disturbed neuro-transmission may underlie CBD. This belief has prompted the use of
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors to treat the disorder (Black et al., 2000; Ninan et al.,
2000; Koran et al., 2003, 2007), though results have been mixed. Dopamine has been
conjectured to play a role in CBD because it is widely believed to mediate reward dependent
behaviors (Holden, 2001). The role of dopamine is further suggested by reports that anti-
parkinsonian medications that modulate dopamine neurotransmission induce compulsive
behaviors including uncontrolled shopping (Lader, 2008). Case reports suggest that
naltrexone may help in treating CBD, leading to speculation about the role of opiate
receptors in CBD, but naltrexone also affects dopamine neurotransmission (Kim, 1998;
Grant, 2003). Finally, a recent functional magnetic resonance imaging study of 23 women
with CBD found increased activation of the nucleus accumbens – the brain’s putative
pleasure center – compared to normal shoppers when subjects were shown products they
could buy (Raab et al., 2010). These findings are in agreement with the work of Knutson et
al. (2007) who studied purchasing decisions (though not in people with CBD), and of studies
of persons with PG or drug addiction whereby images of appropriate stimuli activate the
nucleus accumbens (Berridge, 2003; Reuter et al., 2005).
Neuropsychological studies could contribute to a better understanding of the neurobiology
of CBD. Research investigations with pathological gamblers have suggested the executive
function deficits may be associated with disturbances in fronto-temporal circuitry thereby
contributing to impaired decision-making (Goudriaan et al., 2004; Forbush et al., 2008;
Marraziti et al., 2008). Because both disorders are considered behavioral addictions, it is not
unreasonable to propose that people with CBD might have similar neurop-sychological
profiles to those with PG. Bechara (2003) has described patients with executive function
deficits as having a “myopia of the future” because of their failure to consider future
consequences. This is an apt description of many people with CBD.
Black et al. Page 2
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Knowledge of selected personality traits may also help clinicians better understand and
manage CBD. There is a growing appreciation that, like PG, CBD is associated with trait
impulsivity, despite the fact there have been few formal investigations. Lejoyeux et al.
(1997) reported that a group of 38 depressed inpatients with CBD had elevated trait
impulsivity on all three subscales of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Barratt, 1959).
DeSarbo and Edwards (1996) used the NEO Personality Inventory (NEOPI; Costa and
McCrae, 1985) in a group of 104 self-identified persons with CBD and found high levels of
impulsiveness. Mller et al. (2010b) recently used the NEOPI to assess personality in a group
of 68 compulsive buyers. Cluster analysis yielded two clusters, one of which was associated
with greater CBD severity, higher trait impulsivity, more comorbid psychiatric disorders,
and lower rates of remission.
We have become increasingly interested in assessing childhood and adult symptoms of
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in pathological gamblers, symptoms that
research shows are relatively common in these individuals (Black et al., in press).
Interestingly, we have found not only high levels of ADHD symptoms in people with PG,
but that these levels fall with treatment of the disordered gambling (Black et al., 2007a,
2007b, 2008). This could suggest that ADHD symptoms help mediate disordered gambling
or, possibly, they are behavioral markers of the disorder. The same could be true for CBD.
None of the published CBD comorbidity studies (Christenson et al., 1994; Schlosser et al.,
2004; Black et al., 1998; Mller et al., 2010a) have reported on the prevalence of ADHD
symptoms. This may have more to do with the lack of assessment rather than the lack of an
association, because the instruments used in these studies have not assessed ADHD.
The purpose of this pilot study was to gain a better understanding of the neuropsychological
performance of persons with CBD. Because CBD and PG appear to intertwine (de Zwaan,
2010), we thought it worthwhile to also assess trait impulsivity, ADHD symptoms, and
selected personality characteristics. Based on our experience, and drawing from the
literature on PG, we expected people with CBD to perform more poorly on
neuropsychological tests, including indices of executive function (e.g., cognitive flexibility,
decision-making) than controls, but that general cognition and memory would not differ
between the groups. We further hypothesized that persons with CBD would have higher
levels of trait impulsivity and ADHD symptoms, and that those who were highly impulsive
would perform more poorly on tests of executive function and comprise a subset.
Developing a better understanding of the interrelation of neuropsychological performance,
trait impulsivity, and ADHD symptoms in persons with CBD has the potential to foster new
treatment approaches and preventive strategies.
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects and study design
Men and women ≥18 years who met the criteria of McElroy et al. (1994) for CBD were
recruited through newspaper advertisements and word-of-mouth. Subjects had to score ≥2
standard deviations below the mean on the Compulsive Buying Scale (CBS), shown to
differentiate compulsive from non-compulsive buyers (Faber and O’Guinn, 1992). They also
had to have CBD for ≥1 year. Control subjects were recruited in the course of another study
through advertisements (Black et al., in press). Controls could not have PG or CBD; the
presence of PG and CBD was assessed using the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview
(MIDI) (Christenson et al., 1994; Grant et al., 2005). Exclusions for people with CBD or
controls included having: a current or past diagnosis of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder,
schizoaffective disorder, or a primary neurological disorder (e.g., Parkinson’s disease);
major depression within the last 3 months; a substance use disorder in the past 3 months
(except tobacco dependence); evidence of cognitive impairment (i.e., had a Mini Mental
Black et al. Page 3
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
State Score <23; Folstein et al., 1975); or a history of head injury with loss of consciousness
lasting >10 min. Subjects gave written, informed consent according to procedures approved
by the University of Iowa Institutional Review Board. All received compensation ($25).
2.2. Assessments
The neuropsychological test battery consisted of the following: the Wechsler Abbreviated
Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999) to assess intellectual functioning; the Wide
Range Achievement Test-3 Reading Scale to assess reading skills (WRAT; Wilkinson,
1993); the Stroop Color Word Test (Stroop, 1992) and Letter–Number Sequencing subtest
from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997) to assess sustained
and selective attention, cognitive inhibition, and working memory; Trails A and B (Reitan,
1992) to assess motor planning and cognitive shifting; the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-64
(WCST-64; Berg, 1948) to assess executive function, including the ability to form, test, and
alter problem solving strategies in response to external feedback; the Iowa Gambling Task
(IGT; Bechara et al., 1997) to measure decision-making capacity under differing risk
conditions; the WAIS-III Picture Completion subtest to measure ability to perceive visual
details quickly; the Controlled Word Association Test (COWAT; Benton, 1969) and the
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) Animal Naming Test (Goodglass and
Kaplan, 1983) to assess verbal fluency; the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-
R; Brandt and Benedict, 2004) to assess verbal learning and memory; and the Brief
Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997) to assess visual learning and
memory.
Rater-administered psychiatric instruments included: the Mini International
Neuropsychiatric Interview-Plus (MINI; Sheehan et al., 1998) to assess DSM-IV disorders;
the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview (MIDI; Christenson et al., 1994; Grant et al.,
2005) to assess the presence of impulse control disorders; and the Yale-Brown Obsessive–
Compulsive Scale-Shopping Version (YBOCS-SV; Monahan et al., 1996) to assess CBD
severity. Self-report instruments were administered including the Beck Depression
Inventory (BDI; Beck, 1978) to assess symptoms of depression; the Barratt Impulsiveness
Scale (BIS; Barratt, 1959, 1983) to assess severity of motor, cognitive, and non-planning
impulsiveness; the ADHD Rating Scale (DuPaul, 1991) to evaluate the presence and
severity of symptoms of ADHD; and a version of the Temperament and Character Inventory
(TCI; Cloninger et al., 1993, 1994) to assess novelty seeking, harm avoidance, and reward
dependence. In addition, we collected relevant social and demographic data.
3. Data analysis
χ2 tests (or Fisher’s exact tests) and t-tests were used to compare demographic and clinical
characteristics of the groups. ANOVA was used to compare neuropsychological
characteristics of the two groups (SAS Institute Inc., 2004). The neuropsychological
variables compared included measures of memory (HVLT total recall and delayed recall,
BVMT total recall and delayed recall), executive functioning (WCST total errors,
perseverative responses, non-perseverative errors, perseverative errors, learning to learn, and
categories completed; Trails B, COWAT, Stroop interference, and IGT net total), and
attention (Stroop color naming, word reading, and Trails A). Other measures included the
BDAE Animal Naming Total, WAIS Letter Number Sequencing and Picture Completion,
and the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT).
ANOVA was also used to test for group differences in personality characteristics measured
by the BIS total score, three BIS subscales (Attentional, Motor, and Non-planning), and the
three scales of the TCI (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, and reward dependence). CBD
Black et al. Page 4
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
subjects and controls were also compared on ADHD symptom clusters (Hyperactivity,
Impulsivity, and Inattentiveness), as well as individual items of the ADHD Rating Scale.
For the BIS, the threshold of ≥72 has been suggested to define “high impulsivity” (Stanford
et al., 2009), whereas a score of >8 has been suggested to define “high ADHD symptoms”
with the ADHD Rating Scale (Braaten and Rosen, 1997). For people with CBD, the
neuropsychological variables were compared for those with high levels of impulsivity and
those without. Similarly, the neuropsychological variables were compared for those with
high levels of ADHD symptoms and those without.
4. Results
4.1. Baseline comparison
Twenty-eight persons with CBD were recruited and screened. Two were later dropped from
the analysis for not meeting study criteria (one due to a bipolar disorder, the other due to a
history of head injury and loss of consciousness). Controls were collected in the course of
another study. From a pool of 65 subjects, 32 were selected to approximately match the age,
sex, and educational profile of the group with CBD. The final sample consisted of 26
subjects with CBD and 32 controls.
The groups were comparable for age, gender, race, and years of education (Table 1). Most of
the individuals with CBD were female (88%) and Caucasian (85%); they had a mean age of
36.3 years (S.D.=15.7). Mean age at onset of CBD was 19.7 years (S.D.=7.0). The mean
YBOCS-SV score of 20.2 (S.D.=5.2) suggested moderate severity. Controls had slightly
higher full-scale IQs (
M
=112.0, S.D.=12.0) than subjects with CBD (
M
=107.7, S.D.=12.1),
although the difference was not significant (
p
=0.182). Individuals with CBD were more
likely to have high levels of trait impulsivity (46% vs. 16%; χ2=6.1,
p
=0.01); they also had
significantly higher dimensional levels of depression and ADHD symptoms than controls.
They were significantly more likely than controls to have co-occurring lifetime psychiatric
disorders, including mood, anxiety, and impulse control disorders, but not substance use
disorders, somatoform disorders, antisocial personality disorder, or ADHD.
4.2. Comparison of groups on neuropsychological measures, the BIS, and Cloninger’s
traits
People with CBD performed similarly to controls on neuropsychological measures (Table
2). CBD subjects scored significantly higher on the Picture Completion task of the WASI
(
d
=0.68,
p
=0.009), but no other significant differences were found. Group differences of
0.30 standard deviations and larger, while not statistically significant at
p
=0.05, favored the
Control group on Trails B, COWAT, Stroop Color Naming, and IGT scores. Non-significant
group differences that favored the CBD group with d>0.30 included HVLT total recall,
BVMT total recall, and BVMT delayed recall scores.
People with CBD exhibited greater novelty seeking (
d
=0.75,
p
=0.004), but differences in
harm avoidance and reward dependence were not significant (Table 3). They also exhibited
greater trait impulsivity (
d
=0.85,
p
=0.001). Interestingly, the largest difference existed for
the BIS Motor subscale (
d
=1.14). Moderate differences were observed for the BIS
Attentional (
d
=0.53,
p
=0.044) and Non-planning (
d
=0.52,
p
=0.049) subscales.
4.3. Correlational analyses
BIS total score and ADHD Rating Scales scores were highly correlated (
r
=0.64,
p<
0.001
overall;
r
=0.53,
p
=0.006 among subjects with CBD). Combining data from both groups,
each BIS subscale was correlated with ADHD rating scales (
r
=0.46, 0.54, and 0.46 for BIS
Black et al. Page 5
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Motor, Non-planning, and Attentional, respectively, each
p<
0.001). Among subjects with
CBD, BIS Motor (
r
=0.38,
p
=0.053), Non-planning (
r
=0.40,
p
=0.045) and Attentional
(
r
=0.61,
p<
0.001) were all correlated with ADHD symptoms. Among CBD subjects, CBS
scores were significantly correlated with BIS Motor (
r
=−0.57,
p
=0.002); correlations with
Non-planning (
r
= −0.28) and Attentional (
r
= −0.31) were in the expected direction, but not
significant. CBS scores were significantly correlated with BIS total score (
r
= −0.44,
p
=0.024) and ADHD symptoms (
r
=−0.40,
p
=0.045). The correlation with level of
depression (BDI) was not significant (
r
=−0.23,
p
=0.267).
4.4. ADHD rating scale analyses
We explored the three symptom clusters and items for the ADHD Rating Scale (Table 4).
This comparison revealed that CBD subjects had more ADHD symptoms, but that the group
differences varied across symptom clusters and items. The most pronounced difference was
for the Inattentiveness symptom cluster (
d
=0.52,
p
=0.047), particularly the individual items:
“Fidgety” (
d
=0.61), “Difficulty waiting turn” (
d
=0.71), and “Loses things” (
d
=0.53).
Among CBD subjects, novelty seeking was correlated with BIS impulsivity (
r
=0.45,
p
=0.021) and the ADHD Rating Scale score (
r
=0.44,
p
=0.025 for ADHD total score;
r
=0.40,
p
=0.044 for ADHD hyperactivity subscale;
r
=0.44,
p
=0.025 for ADHD impulsivity
subscale; and
r
=0.37,
p
=0.065 for ADHD inattentive subscale).
4.5. Subjects with CBD with high impulsivity or ADHD symptoms compared to others
Comparisons of neuropsychological variables between CBD subjects with high impulsivity
and those without revealed few significant differences (data not shown). Of the 21
neuropsycho-logical variables tested, only one (Trails B) showed a significant difference for
highly impulsive people with CBD, with the highly impulsive group performing worse.
Comparisons between those with CBD with high ADHD and those without revealed no
significant differences (data not shown).
5. Discussion
People with CBD in this pilot study were similar demographically and clinically to those
described by other investigators, and their disorder was of moderate severity (Christenson et
al., 1994; McElroy et al., 1994; Ninan et al., 2000; Miltenberger et al., 2003; Koran et al.,
2003, 2007). In short, CBD was primarily a disorder of middle-aged women who had
struggled with the condition for nearly 17 years. We were able to confirm some, but not all
of our hypotheses. First, we found no consistent differences in neurop-sychological test
performance between those with CBD and controls, including executive function.
Interestingly, compulsive buyers performed significantly better on the WASI Picture
Completion task, which is a test of visual perception, a finding that may reflect their intense
interest in consumer goods. While controls had higher full scale IQs, this difference was not
statistically significant. Second, we found that those with CBD had significantly higher
levels of trait impulsivity and ADHD symptoms than controls, findings that were not
surprising. In terms of selected personality characteristics described by Cloninger et al.
(1993, 1994), we found high levels of novelty seeking, but not risk aversion or reward
dependence. Lastly, we failed to confirm our hypothesis that high levels of trait impulsivity
or ADHD symptoms would identify a subset of persons with CBD based on
neuropsychological performance.
Our work is consistent with the work of Lejoyeux et al. (1997) and DeSarbo and Edwards
(1996) in confirming that people with CBD are impulsive. Like Lejoyeux et al., we found
elevated scores on all BIS subscales. The BIS is a standard tool for measuring impulsivity,
and its three scales reflect major components of impulsivity identified through factor
Black et al. Page 6
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
analysis. Barratt (1983) conceptualized
cognitive impulsiveness
as making quick decisions;
motor impulsiveness
as acting without thinking, and
non-planning impulsiveness
as lacking
forethought. This finding comes as no surprise considering the behavior of those with CBD
who are often described as spending without adequate reflection, having difficulty delaying
their buying urges, and seeking immediate gratification though their spending (Lejoyeux et
al., 1997; Miltenberger et al., 2003; Black, 2007).
Trait impulsivity is a leading contributor to individual’s loss of control over his or her
shopping and spending behavior. We believe this contributes to a phenomenon analogous to
that seen in pathological gamblers whereby impulsive gamblers seek out games with rapid,
intermittent payout (e.g., slots). With CBD, the person engages in frequent buying of low
cost items, rather than less frequent shopping for higher cost items (Christenson et al., 1994;
Schlosser et al., 2004). Impulsivity may also contribute to the compulsive buyer’s inability
to divide his attention among competing stimuli, and causing him to ignore internal
cognitions focusing on restraint. This is a pattern described in pathological gamblers
(McCown and Chamberlain, 2000). While high scores do not appear to identify a subset
with impaired neuropsychological performance, one use of the BIS may be to help identify
people with CBD who are more treatment-resistant or are more likely to drop out of
treatment (Moeller et al., 2001; Patkar et al., 2004; Black et al., 2009). This information
could have implications for treatment programs.
An important finding was that ADHD symptoms are common in those with CBD, even
though a lifetime diagnosis of ADHD is not. While their ADHD Rating Scale scores are
lower than those reported for people with ADHD (Kuperman et al., 2001; Spencer et al.,
1995), they are much higher than scores of controls, and are similar to scores in persons with
PG (Black et al., 2007a, 2007b). Further, ADHD symptoms were highly correlated with trait
impulsivity. This is not surprising considering that impulsivity is a defining feature of
ADHD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The inattentiveness cluster was
significantly different from controls, specifically the items “fidgety,” “difficulty waiting
turn,” and “loses things.” One can imagine the person with CBD who is anxious and fidgety
as she waits in line to make a purchase, or cannot find an item of clothing she has misplaced
in her crowded closet.
We have also linked CBD to novelty seeking, one of several personality dimensions
described by Cloninger et al. (1993, 1994). Their unified biosocial model of personality
identifies three heritable personality dimensions each thought to represent an independent
behavioral response disposition. Each dimension is hypothesized to be linked with a
different neurotransmitter system. In this case, novelty seeking has been linked to low basal
dopamine activity, which means that a person would need excitement to raise their low level
of arousal, and perhaps for these persons shopping and spending serves that need. To place
these findings in perspective, people who score high on
novelty seeking
are described as
impulsive, extravagant, and disorderly. This description is consistent with the finding of
high levels of both impulsivity and ADHD symptoms. There was no concomitant increase in
harm avoidance or reward dependence.
These findings are preliminary and should be interpreted with caution. There are several
methodological limitations to acknowledge. First, subjects with CBD and controls were
recruited through advertising and word-of-mouth and may not be as representative as if they
had been recruited through a random community survey. Additionally, the controls were not
specifi-cally matched to the individuals with CBD because they were selected in the course
of another study (Black et al., in press). Second, the number of subjects was relatively small
which increases the potential for a Type II error (i.e., finding no difference when one is
present). For example, comparisons of several tests of executive function favored the
Black et al. Page 7
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
controls (Stroop Color Naming, Trails B, IGT), suggesting that with larger samples we may
have found that those with CBD performed significantly worse than controls. Third, testing
was not conducted blind to CBD status, and this may have inadvertently affected the results.
Fourth, while our sample is likely more representative of the general population of
individuals with CBD than treatment-seeking samples, it is possible that a subjects’ reason
for volunteering for the study may have affected our results. Fifth, CBD subjects had a
higher rate of comorbid disorders, and this could have played a role in the study results.
Sixth, the version of the ADHD Rating scale used in this study was originally developed for
use in children and is based on DSM-III-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association,
1987). While valid in adult populations (Spencer et al., 1995; Kuperman et al., 2001), it has
been superseded by a version based on DSM-IV (Murphy and Adler, 2004), the use of
which presumably could have led to different results. Finally, we examined three of
Cloninger’s traits and did not administer the full TCI, so we are unable to comment on the
other personality characteristics assessed with this instrument such as self-directedness or
cooperativeness.
Acknowledgments
The study was funded in part through grants from the National Center for Responsible Gaming and the National
Institute on Drug Abuse (RO1DA021361 to Dr. Black). Dr. Black has received research support from Psyadon and
AstraZeneca.
References
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3.
American Psychiatric Association; Washington DC: 1987. revised
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4.
American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; Washington, DC: 2000. Text Revision
Barratt ES. Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Perceptual and Motor Skills.
1959; 9:191–198.
Barratt, ES. Impulsiveness substrates in Motivation, Emotion, Personality. Spence, JT.; Izard, CE.,
editors. Elsevier; North Holland: 1983. p. 137-146.
Bechara A. Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. Journal of Gambling Studies.
2003; 19:23–51. [PubMed: 12635539]
Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the
advantageous strategy. Science. 1997; 275:1293–1294. [PubMed: 9036851]
Beck, AT. Depression Inventory. Philadelphia Center for Cognitive Therapy; Philadelphia, PA: 1978.
Benedict, RHB. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc;
Lutz, FL: 1997.
Benton AL. Development of a multilingual aphasia battery: progress and problems. Journal of the
Neurological Sciences. 1969; 9:39–48. [PubMed: 5820858]
Berg EA. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General
Psychology. 1948; 39:15–22. [PubMed: 18889466]
Berridge KC. Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition. 2003; 52:106–128. [PubMed: 12812810]
Black DW. Compulsive buying disorder: a review of the evidence. CNS Spectrums. 2007; 12:124–
132. [PubMed: 17277712]
Black, DW. Compulsive Buying Disorder. In: Aboujaoude, E.; Koran, L., editors. Textbook of
Impulse Control Disorders. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2010. p. 5-22.
Black, DW.; Kuzma, J.; Shaw, M. Unique Consequences of Behavioral Expressions of Addiction. In:
Shaffer, H., editor. The American Psychological Association Addiction Syndrome Handbook.
American Psychological Association Press; Washington DC: 2012. p. 329-352.
Black et al. Page 8
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black DW, Gabel J, Hansen J, Schlosser S. A double-blind comparison of fluvoxamine versus placebo
in the treatment of compulsive buying disorder. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 2000; 12:205–211.
[PubMed: 11140921]
Black DW, Repertinger S, Gaffney GR, Gabel J. Family history and psychiatric comorbidity in
persons with compulsive buying: preliminary findings. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;
155:960–963. [PubMed: 9659864]
Black DW, Shaw M, Forbush KT, Allen J. An open-label study of escitalopram in the treatment of
pathological gambling. Clinical Neurophar-macology. 2007a; 30:206–212.
Black DW, Shaw M, Allen J. Extended-release carbamazepine in the treatment of pathological
gambling: an open-label study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
2008; 32:1191–1194.
Black DW, Blum N, St Pfohl B, John D, McCormick B, Allen J. Predictors of response to systems
training to emotional predictability and problem solving (STEPPS) for borderline personality
disorder: an exploratory study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2009; 120:53–61. [PubMed:
19183126]
Black DW, Arndt S, Coryell WH, Argo T, Forbush KT, Shaw M, Perry P, Allen J. Bupropion in the
treatment of pathological gambling: a randomized, placebo-controlled, flexible-dose study. Journal
of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2007b; 27:143–150. [PubMed: 17414236]
Black DW, Smith M, Shaw M, McCormick B, Moser D, Forbush KT, Allen J. Symptoms of attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder, self-rated impulsivity, Cloninger’s personality traits, and
neuropsychological performance in pathological gambling. Addiction Research and Therapy. in
press.
Brandt, J.; Benedict, RHB. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised. Psychological Assessment
Resources, Inc; Lutz, FL: 2004.
Braaten EB, Rosen LA. Emotional reactions in adults with symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Personality and Individual Differences. 1997; 22:355–361.
Christenson GA, Faber RJ, de Zwaan M, Raymond NC, Specker SM, Ekern MD, Mackenzie TB,
Crosby RD, Crow SJ, Eckert ED. Compulsive buying: descriptive characteristics and psychiatric
comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1994; 55:5–11. [PubMed: 8294395]
Cloninger C, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character.
Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993; 50:975–990. [PubMed: 8250684]
Cloninger, CR.; Pryzbeck, TR.; Svakic, DM.; Wetzel, RD. The Temperament and Character Inventory
(TCI): a guide to its development and use. Center of Psychobiology of Personality; St. Louis, MO:
1994.
Costa, PT.; McCrae, RR. NEOPI/FFI Manual Supplement. Psychological Assessment Resources;
Odessa, FL: 1985.
DeSarbo WS, Edwards EA. Typologies of compulsive buying behavior: a constrained clusterwise
regression approach. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 1996; 5:231–262.
Dell’Osso B, Altamura AC, Allen A, Marazitti D, Hollander E. Epidemio-logic and clinical update on
impulse control disorders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;
256:464–475. [PubMed: 16960655]
de Zwaan, M. Psychiatric comorbidity and compulsive buying. In: Mueller, A.; Mitchell, J., editors. In
Compulsive Buying: Clinical Foundations and Treatment. Routledge; New York: 2010. p. 87-104.
Di Nicola M, Martinotti G, Mazza M, Tedeschi D, Pozzi G, Janiri L. Quetiapine as add-on treatment
for bipolar I disorder with comorbid compulsive buying and physical exercise addiction (letter).
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2010a; 34:713–714. [PubMed:
20298734]
Di Nicola M, Tedeschi D, Mazza M, Martinotti G, Harnic D, Catalano V, Bruschi A, Pozzi G, Bria P,
Janiri L. Behavioural addictions in bipolar disorder patients: role of impulsivity and personality
dimensions. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2010b; 125:82–88. [PubMed: 20083309]
DuPaul GJ. Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: psychomentric properties in a community-
based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1991; 20:245–253.
Faber RJ, O’Guinn TC. A clinical screener for compulsive buying. Journal of Consumer Research.
1992; 19:459–469.
Black et al. Page 9
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive
state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975; 12:189–198. [PubMed:
1202204]
Forbush KT, Shaw M, Graeber MA, Hovick L, Meyer VJ, Moser D, Bayless J, Watson D, Black DW.
Neuropsychological characteristics and personality traits in pathological gambling. CNS
Spectrums. 2008; 13:306–315. [PubMed: 18408650]
Goodglass, H.; Kaplan, E. The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 2. Lea and Febigar;
Philadelphia, PA: 1983.
Goudriaan AE, Ossterlaan J, deBeurs E, van den Brink W. Pathological gambling: a comprehensive
review of biobehavioral findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004; 28:123–141.
[PubMed: 15172761]
Grant JE. Three cases of compulsive buying treated with naltrexone. International Journal of
Psychiatry and Clinical Practice. 2003; 7:223–225.
Grant JE, Levine L, Kim D, Potenza MN. Impulse control disorders in adult psychiatric inpatients.
American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005; 162:2184–2188. [PubMed: 16263865]
Grant JE, Brewer JA, Potenza MN. The neurobiology of substance and behavioral addictions. CNS
Spectrums. 2006; 11:924–930. [PubMed: 17146406]
Holden C. Behavioral addictions: do they exist? Science. 2001; 294:980–982. [PubMed: 11691967]
Hollander, E. Obsessive Compulsive Related Disorders. American Psychiatric Press; Washington, DC:
1993a.
Hollander E. Obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders: an overview. Psychiatric Annals. 1993b;
23:355–358.
Hollander E, Allen A. Is compulsive buying a real disorder, and is it really compulsive? American
Journal of Psychiatry. 2006; 163:1670–1672. [PubMed: 17012670]
Kim SW. Opioid antagonists in the treatment of impulse-control disorders. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry. 1998; 59:159–164. [PubMed: 9590665]
Koran, LM. Obsessive–compulsive and Related Disorders in Adults—a Comprehensive Clinical
Guide. Cambridge; New York: 1999.
Koran LM, Faber RJ, Aboujaoude E, Large MD, Serpe RT. Estimated prevalence of compulsive
buying in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006; 163:1806–1812. [PubMed:
17012693]
Koran LM, Chuang HW, Bullock KD, Smith SC. Citalopram for compulsive shopping disorder: an
open-label study followed by a double-blind discontinuation. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2003;
64:793–798. [PubMed: 12934980]
Koran LM, Aboujaoude EN, Solvason B, Gamel N, Smith EH. Escitalo-pram for compulsive buying
disorder: a double-blind discontinuation study (letter). Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology.
2007; 27:225–227. [PubMed: 17414257]
Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron.
2007; 53:147–153. [PubMed: 17196537]
Kuperman S, Perry PJ, Gaffney GR, Lund BC, Bever-Stille KA, Arndt S, Holman TL, Moser DJ,
Paulsen JS. Bupropion SR vs. methylphenidate vs. placebo for attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder in adults. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 2001; 13:129–134. [PubMed: 11791949]
Lader M. Antiparkinsonian medication and pathological gambling. CNS Drugs. 2008; 22:407–416.
[PubMed: 18399709]
Lejoyeux M, Tassian V, Solomon J, Ades J. Study of compulsive buying in depressed persons. Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry. 1997; 58:169–173. [PubMed: 9164428]
Marraziti D, Dell’Osso MC, Conversano C, Consoli G, Vivarelli L, Mungai F, Di Nasso E, Golia F.
Executive function abnormalities in pathological gamblers. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology.
2008; 4:7.
McCown, WG.; Chamberlain, LL. Best Possible Odds: Contemporary Treatment Strategies for
Gambling Disorders. Wiley; New York: 2000.
McElroy S, Keck PE, Pope HG Jr, Smith JMR, Strakowski SM. Compulsive buying: a report of 20
cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1994; 55:242–248. [PubMed: 8071278]
Black et al. Page 10
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Miltenberger RG, Redlin J, Crosby R, Stickney M, Mitchell J, Wonderlich S, Faber R, Smyth J. Direct
and retrospective assessment of factors contributing to compulsive buying. Journal of Behavioral
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2003; 34:1–9.
Monahan P, Black DW, Gabel J. Reliability and validity of a scale to measure change in persons with
compulsive buying. Psychiatry Research. 1996; 64:59–67. [PubMed: 8888365]
Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Schmitz JM, Swann AC, Grabowski J. The impact of
impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment.
2001; 21:193–198. [PubMed: 11777668]
Mller A, Mühlhans B, Müller U, Mertens C, Horbach T, Michell JE, de Zwaan M. Compulsive buying
and psychiatric comorbidity. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2009;
59:291–299.
Müller A, Mitchell J, Black DW, Crosby RD, Berg K, De Zwaan M. Latent profile analysis and
comorbidity in a sample of individuals with compulsive buying disorder. Psychiatry Research.
2010a; 178:348–353. [PubMed: 20471099]
Mller A, Claes L, Mitchell JE, Wonderlich SA, Crosby RD, DeZwaan M. Personality prototypes in
individuals with compulsive buying based on the Big Five Model. Behavior Research and
Therapy. 2010b; 48:930–935.
Murphy KR, Adler LA. Assessing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: focus on rating
scales. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004; 65 (Suppl 3):12–17. [PubMed: 15046530]
Ninan PT, McElroy SL, Kane CP, Knight BT, Casuto LS, Rose SE, Marsteller FA, Nemeroff CB.
Placebo-controlled study of fluvoxamine in the treatment of patients with compulsive buying.
Journal of Clinical Psychophar-macology. 2000; 20:362–366.
Patkar AA, Murray HW, Manelli P, Gottheil E, Weinstein SP, Vergare MJ. Pre-treatment measures of
impulsivity, aggression and sensation seeking are associated with treatment outcome for African–
American cocaine-dependent patients. Journal of Addictive Disease. 2004; 23:109–122.
Raab, G.; Elger, CE.; Neuner, M.; Weber, B. The neural Basis of Compulsive Buying. In: Mueller, A.;
Mitchell, J., editors. Compulsive Buying: Clinical Foundations and Treatment. Routledge; New
York: 2010. p. 63-86.
Reitan, RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology
Laboratory; South Tucson, AZ: 1992.
Reuter J, Raedler T, Rose M, Hand I, Glascher J, Buchel C. Pathological gambling is linked to reduced
activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience. 2005; 8:147–148.
SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT: 9.1 Users Guide. SAS Institute, Inc; Cary, NC: 2004.
Schlosser S, Black DW, Repertinger S, Freet D. Compulsive buying —demography, phenomenology,
and comorbidity in 46 subjects. General Hospital Psychiatry. 2004; 16:205–212. [PubMed:
8063088]
Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim O, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar
GC. The mini international neuropsy-chiatric interview (MINI). Journal of Clinical Psychiatry.
1998; 59 (suppl 20):22–33. [PubMed: 9881538]
Spencer T, Wilens T, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Ablon S, Lapey K. A double-blind, crossover
comparison of methylphenidate and placebo in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995; 52:434–443. [PubMed: 7771913]
Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale:
an update and review. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009; 47:385–395.
Stroop J. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1992;
121:15–23.
Tavares H, Gentil V. Pathological gambling and obsessive compulsive disorder: towards a spectrum of
disorders of volition. Revista Brasileira Psiquiatria. 2007; 29:107–117.
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale®, Third Edition (WAIS®–III). The Psychological
Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997.
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence™ (WASI™). The Psychological
Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1999.
Black et al. Page 11
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Wilkinson, G. The Wide Range Achievement Test 3 (WRAT3). Psychological Assessment Resources,
Inc; Lutz, FL: 1993.
Black et al. Page 12
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 13
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics in persons with CBD and controls.
Variable CBD (n =26) Control (n=32) t p
Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
Age (years) 36.3 15.7 39.4 14.8 −0.77 0.445
Age of CBD onset (years) 19.7 7.0
Education (years) 15.2 2.3 14.8 2.1 0.70 0.485
BDI score 10.8 7.7 6.2 6.8 2.45 0.018
ADHD Rating Scale score 12.7 8.2 8.4 7.1 2.16 0.035
WASI 4 Full IQ 107.7 12.1 112.0 12.0 31.35 0.182
YBOCS-SV score 20.2 5.2
CBS score −3.9 1.5
n % n % χ2p
Gender
Female 23 88 27 84 0.720
a
Male 3 12 5 16 0.113
a
Race/ethnicity
Caucasian 22 85 24 75
African–American 0 0 5 16
Hispanic 3 12 2 8
Asian 1 4 0 0
American Indian 0 0 1 3
Highly impulsive
b
12 46 5 16 6.09 0.014
High ADHD symptoms
c
17 65 11 34 5.52 0.019
Diagnosis (lifetime)
Mood disorder 16 62 2 6 20.49 <0.001
Anxiety disorder 13 50 3 9 11.85 <0.001
Substance use disorder 6 23 4 13 0.319
a
Eating disorder 0 0 1 3 1.000
a
ADHD 1 4 2 6 1.000
a
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 14
n % n % χ2p
ASPD 0 0 0 0 NA
ICD 18 69 2 6 25.19 <0.001
Somatoform disorder 3 12 1 3 0.316
a
Any disorder 21 81 17 53 4.85 0.028
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASPD=antisocial personality disorder; BIS=Barrett Impulsivity Scale; BDI=Beck Depression Inventory; CBS=Compulsive Buying Scale; WASI=Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale; YBOCS-SV=Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; ICD=Impulse Control Disorder.
a
Fisher’s Exact Test.
b
BIS total score ≥72.
c
ADHD Checklist score >8.
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 15
Table 2
Neuropsychological performance in persons with CBD and controls.
Measure CBD (n=26) Control (n=32) d F d.f. p
Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
HVLT-R
Total recall (Raw) 27.7 3.6 26.6 3.7 0.31 1.3 55 0.254
Delayed recall (Raw) 9.8 2.0 9.6 2.1 0.10 0.1 55 0.710
BVMT-R
Total recall (Raw) 26.5 6.2 23.9 6.7 0.39 2.2 55 0.147
Delayed recall (Raw) 10.3 1.8 9.7 2.1 0.31 1.3 54 0.251
WRAT (t-score) 53.2 5.6 52.4 4.9 0.16 0.3 54 0.561
WCST
Total errors (Raw) 12.3 4.5 11.6 4.7 0.15 0.3 53 0.583
Perseverative responses (Raw) 7.3 4.0 6.4 2.7 0.26
a
0.9 54 0.341
Non-perseverative errors (Raw) 5.6 2.4 5.5 3.4 0.03
a
0.0 53 0.906
Perseverative errors (Raw) 6.7 3.0 6.3 2.6 0.14
a
0.3 54 0.601
Categories completed (Raw) 4.1 0.9 4.0 1.1 0.08 0.1 53 0.760
Learning to learn −1.9 6.0 −1.5 5.2 −0.06 0.0 50 0.826
Trails A (s) 22.5 7.1 20.8 5.5 0.28
a
1.1 55 0.290
Trails B (s) 71.4 46.3 55.6 27.0 0.42
a
2.5 54 0.116
BDAE-R animal naming test 21.7 5.2 22.7 5.4 −0.19 0.5 56 0.478
COWAT 40.8 14.0 46.5 10.4 −0.46 3.1 56 0.083
WAIS
Letter number sequencing 11.5 2.7 11.3 2.6 0.08 0.1 56 0.755
Picture completion 22.4 1.9 20.9 2.2 0.68 7.3 55 0.009
Stroop
Color Naming 79.2 12.8 84.1 13.1 −0.38 2.1 56 0.152
Word reading 101.7 18.5 106.4 15.9 −0.27 1.1 56 0.304
Interference 47.0 8.8 47.0 7.2 0.00 0.0 55 0.988
IGT net total (Raw) 14.0 24.9 24.0 27.6 −0.38 1.9 50 0.176
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 16
COWAT=Controlled Oral Word Association Test; IGT=Iowa Gambling Task; BDAE-R=Boston Diagnositic Aphasia Exam; HVLT-R=Hopkins Verbal Learning Task-Revised; BVMT-R=Brief
Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised; WCST=Wisconsin Card Sort Test; WRAT=Wide Range Achievement Test.
a
Positive difference (
d
) suggests Control group performed better.
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 17
Table 3
Selected personality measures in persons with CBD and controls.
Measure CBD {n=26) Control (n=32) d F d.f. p
Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
BIS Total Score 71.5 13.0 61.3 8.9 0.85 12.4 55 0.001
Attentional 18.1 4.2 16.0 3.6 0.53 4.3 56 0.044
Motor 26.9 4.8 21.1 3.6 1.14 27.4 56 <0.001
Non-planning 26.5 6.0 23.7 4.6 0.52 4.1 56 0.049
TCI
Novelty seeking 58.8 10.3 49.9 11.8 0.75 9.2 56 0.004
Harm avoidance 54.2 12.0 55.1 9.9 −0.09 0.1 56 0.733
Reward dependence 50.9 9.4 51.8 8.9 −0.10 0.1 56 0.713
BIS=Barrett Impulsivness Scale; TCI=Temperament and Character Inventory.
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Black et al. Page 18
Table 4
Symptom scores on the ADHD Rating Scale
a
in persons with CBD and controls.
Symptom cluster CBD (n=26) Control (n= 32) d F d.f. p
Mean S.D. Mean S.D.
Hyperactivity (overall) 3.1 2.2 2.2 2.3 0.39 2.2 55 0.146
Difficulty remaining seated 0.6 0. 7 0.4 0.6 0.39 2.1 54 0.150
Fidgety 1.3 1.0 0.7 0.8 0.61 5.8 56 0.019
Difficulty working quietly 0.4 0.7 0.3 0.6 0.14 0.3 55 0.613
Talks excessively 0.9 1.1 0.7 −0.8 0.20 0.6 55 0.458
Impulsivity (overall) 2.6 2.0 1.6 1.9 0.49 3.5 56 0.066
Interrupts or intrudes 0.7 0.8 0.6 0.8 0.21 0.7 56 0.424
Blurts out answers 0.5 0.6 0.5 0.7 30.08 0.1 55 0.764
Difficulty waiting turn 0.8 0.7 0.3 0.5 0.71 7.7 54 0.007
Act before thinking 0.2 0.4 0.1 0.3 0.21 0.6 53 0.450
Inattentiveness (overall) 6.7 4.4 4.6 3.8 0.52 4.1 56 0.047
Difficulty sustaining attention 1.0 1.0 0.6 0.8 0.46 3.2 56 0.080
Shifts activities 1.2 1.1 1.1 0.9 0.11 0.2 56 0.691
Difficulty following instructions 0.4 0.6 0.3 0.6 0.09 0.1 52 0.755
Easily distracted 1.5 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.45 3.0 56 0.088
Loses things 1.0 1.0 0.5 0.7 0.53 4.2 56 0.045
Does not listen 1.3 0.8 1.0 0.8 0.45 2.9 56 0.092
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
a
ADHD Rating Scale symptom scores range from 0 to 3 (0—not at all, 3—very much).
Psychiatry Res
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 28.