ChapterPDF Available

Noun Bias in Chinese Children: Novel Noun and Verb Learning in Chinese, Japanese, and English Preschoolers

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Noun Bias in Chinese Children: Novel Noun and Verb
Learning in Chinese, Japanese, and English Preschoolers
Etsuko Haryu1, Mutsumi Imai2, Hiroyuki Okada3, Lianjing Li4, Meredith A.
Meyer5, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek6 and Roberta Michnick Golinkoff7
1University of Tokyo, 2Keio University, 3Tokai University, 4Peking University,
5University of Oregon, 6Temple University and 7University of Delaware
1. Introduction
Young children acquire words with amazing rapidity, and this rapid growth
is mostly due to the increase of the noun vocabulary (e.g., Bates et al., 1994).
To explain this, Gentner (1982) proposed that children acquire nouns more
easily and earlier than verbs because the concepts denoted by nouns are easier to
access than those denoted by verbs (the universal noun advantage view). Some
researchers, however, challenged this view, arguing that verbs can be learned
more easily and earlier than nouns if the input language has properties to foster
verb learning (the input-dependent view). For example, in argument-dropping
languages such as Korean, Japanese and Chinese, verbs may be heard more
frequently than nouns in the input, and tend to appear in the salient position in
the sentence (Choi & Gopnik,1995; Gentner, 1982). Another factor that has
been noted to foster verb learning is morphological simplicity of verbs (Gentner,
1982; Tardif,1996). In fact, researchers advancing the input-dependent view
have presented data showing that Korean- and Chinese-speaking children have
more verbs than nouns in their early vocabularies (Choi & Gopnik,1995,
Tardif,1996). However, data contradicting this view also exists in the literature.
In some studies, children who are learning these ‘verb-friendly’ languages know
more nouns than verbs (Au et al.,1994 and Kim et al.,2000 for Korean children;
Ogura,2001 for Japanese children).
The discrepancies seem to have arisen in large part from the fact that these
studies relied on either maternal reports or production data. In these methods, a
produced word must be classified into an appropriate form class by a caregiver
or a researcher. However, it is difficult to set up clear criteria for determining
whether the produced word is a noun or a verb, for multiple reasons: (1) when
children are at a one- or two-word stage, it is difficult to determine whether a
given word is in fact produced as a noun or a verb; (2) the fact that children
“say” a given word does not guarantee that they have acquired the full meaning
of the word (see Imai et al, in press b, for further discussion). In addition,
relative use of nouns and verbs may vary across different contexts.
Given these problems inherent in using the relative proportion of nouns and
verbs in the vocabulary inventory or corpus data as a measure of the difficulty of
learning words, Imai et al. (in press a) directly compared the ease of
fast-mapping novel nouns and verbs, studying Japanese children. Japanese is
considered to be one of ‘verb-friendly’ languages, as it is an argument-dropping
as well as an SOV language. They showed Japanese 3- and 5-year-olds a
dynamic video scene in which a woman was performing a novel action with a
novel object, and introduced either a novel noun or verb. The children were
then presented with two test scenes. One of the test scenes was the
Action-Same-Object-Different (AS) scene in which the same woman was doing
the same action but with a different object from the original scene. The other
was the Object-Same-Action-Different (OS) scene in which the same woman
was performing a different action with the same object. The children were
asked to which of the two test scenes the newly introduced word should be
extended. Imai et al. (in press a) found that both 3- and 5-year-olds could map
a novel noun onto a novel object, selecting OS events significantly above chance.
However, the 3-year-olds failed to extend a novel verb to the same action
performed with a different object; only 5-year-olds successfully generalized
novel verbs.
This research extended Imai et al.’s (in press a) research crosslinguistically,
comparing Japanese-, English-, and Mandarin-Chinese-speaking children.
Comparing children learning these three languages is extremely interesting
because the three languages are different from one another along two dimensions
that have been assumed to affect the difficulty of verb learning. On the first
dimension, argument-dropping is allowed in Japanese and Chinese, but not in
English. As a consequence, children learning Japanese or Chinese tend to hear
verbs more frequently than children learning English do. Because of this
distributional property, some researchers predict that children learning Japanese
or Chinese will learn verbs more easily (and hence earlier) than nouns (Choi &
Gopnik, 1995; Tardif, 1996). However, at the same time, this property may
lead to the opposite prediction. It has been proposed that inferring the meaning
of a verb is very difficult even for adults without cues from the argument
structure (Gillette et al., 1999), and that children do utilize the structural cues in
inferring verb meanings (e.g., Fisher, 1996; Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996).
Thus, one could make the prediction that verb learning should be more difficult
for children learning a language that occasionally allows argument-dropping. (In
fact, in Japanese, arguments-dropping occurs more than occasionally—it is
usually dropped when the arguments can be inferred from the context.)
The second dimension is the presence of morphological inflection in verbs.
On this dimension, Chinese contrasts not only to English but also to Japanese.
While verbs are inflected in both English and Japanese, they are not in Chinese.
In other words, verbs cannot be morphologically distinguished from nouns in
Chinese. Remember that in Chinese and Japanese, verb arguments are often
dropped, and a verb alone can constitute a sentence. Even when this occurs,
verbs can be identified by inflectional morphology in Japanese. That is, when a
verb is produced without any arguments, as in “Mite (Look), X-teiru (X-ing),”
one can tell that the word X is a verb attending to the morphological affix
“-teiru.” However, in Chinese, when a word is produced by itself, it is difficult
to tell whether it is a noun or a verb. In other words, one can identify a novel
word as a verb only when it is embedded in an argument structure (see Li, Bates
& MacWhinney, 1993). It is of great theoretical interest to see whether the
morphological simplicity makes verb learning even easier, as argued by some
researchers (Tardif,1996), by comparing Chinese- and Japanese-speaking
children.
If children in all the three languages perform better in fast-mapping novel
nouns than in fast-mapping novel verbs, it will be the strongest evidence for the
universal noun advantage view. If the difficulty of noun and verb learning
varies across the three languages, we can proceed to identify what properties of
languages affect the ease of children’s word learning.
2. Main Crosslinguistic Study
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants. Thirty six 3-year-old (mean=3;5) and 38 5-year-old
(mean=5;6) monolingual Mandarin-Chinese-speaking children and 49 3-year-old
(mean=3;5) and 52 5-year-old (mean=5;2) monolingual English-speaking
American children participated in this study. The children were randomly
assigned to one of the three conditions, the noun condition, the verb with
arguments condition, and the bare verb (word) condition (see Section 2.1.3.).
Thirteen 3-year-old and 14 5-year-old monolingual Japanese children were
tested on the verb with arguments condition. (The data of the noun and the bare
verb conditions for the Japanese group was borrowed from Imai et al. (in press
a).
2.1.2. Stimuli. The stimuli used in this research were the same as those used in
Imai et al. (in press a). Six sets of video action events were used as stimulus
materials. Each set consisted of a standard event and two test events. In each
standard event, a young woman did a novel repetitive action with a novel object.
The two test events were variants of the standard event. In one test event, the
same woman did the same repetitive action but with a different object
(Action-Same-Object-Different scene; AS). In the other test event, a different
repetitive action was performed by the same woman with the same object
(Object-Same-Action-Different scene; OS). (For the full description of the
objects and the actions, see Imai et al., in press a.)
2.1.3. Conditions and Instructions. The children were asked to fast-map six
novel nouns or six novel verbs depending on the condition, since the major goal
was to examine whether English-, Japanese- and Mandarin-Chinese-speaking
children understood the basic principles governing noun extension and verb
extension. In the noun condition, a novel noun was introduced to
English-speaking children by saying, “Look, there is an X.” The equivalents of
this expression were used for Japanese and Mandarin Chinese children.
The verbs were presented in two different forms, one with full arguments
(verb with arguments condition) and the other with no arguments (bare verb
condition), in order to see whether dropping of the verb arguments affects
children’s performance in learning novel verbs. In providing the arguments, in
English, the pronoun “she” served as the subject, and “it” as the object of the
sentence (e.g., “Look, she is X-ing it”). In Japanese, the word “oneesan
(‘young woman’) was used for the subject, and “nanika (‘something’) was used
to refer to the object. For Chinese children, the word “a(1) yi(2) (‘young
woman’) served as the subject and “yi(2) ge dong(1) xi ” (‘one thing’) was used
for the object. In addition, an aspect marker “zai(4),” which marks
imperfective aspect and is usually used to express an ongoing action, was put
just before a novel verb, to make it clear that the target word is a verb.
In the bare verb condition, the subject and the object were not specified in
the sentence, but a novel word could be identified as a verb by the
morphological affix “-ing” and “-teiru,” in English and Japanese, respectively.
However, since verbs are not morphologically distinguished from nouns in
Chinese, one cannot tell whether the novel word is a verb or a noun when both
arguments are dropped in Chinese. Therefore, the bare verb condition in which
a novel verb is presented without any arguments is in actuality a bare word
condition for Chinese children. We include this condition in order to examine
onto which elements of the event Chinese children are likely to map a novel
word whose grammatical class is unknown.
2.1.4. Procedure. Participants were tested individually either at a university
laboratory or in a quiet room at the preschool they attended. The experiment
was conducted by a native speaker of the children’s language. While the
standard event was presented for about 30 seconds, participants heard a novel
verb or a novel noun depending on the condition. Then the two test events
were presented simultaneously and side by side on the screen and the
participants were asked to which scene of the two test scenes the novel word
should be extended.
2.2. Results and Discussion
The number of AS (Action-Same) responses in the six test trials was
counted and the proportion of the AS responses calculated. Since it was a
forced choice task, the proportion of the OS (Object-Same) responses was 100
minus the proportion of the AS responses. We report the results from the three
language groups separately, in the order of English, Japanese, and Chinese.
2.2.1. Results from English-speaking preschoolers. Figure 1 shows the mean
proportions of AS responses by 3- and 5-year-old English-speaking children in
the noun condition, in the verb with arguments condition and in the bare verb
condition. Both the 3- and the 5-year-olds in the noun condition successfully
Figure 1. Mean proportions of AS responses in English-speaking children
mapped novel nouns to novel objects, selecting the OS event significantly above
chance (82.4%, t(17)=6.12, p<.001 for the 3-year-olds and 89.5%, t(18)=13.6,
p<.001 for the 5-year-olds). In extending novel verbs, the 5-year-olds in the
verb with arguments condition successfully selected AS events 72.6% of the
time, t(16)=2.8, p<.05, although the children from the same age group performed
at chance in the bare verb condition (56.2%, t(15)=.61, p>.1). The
performance of the 3-year-olds did not differ from chance, whether they were in
the verb with arguments condition or in the bare verb condition, 39.6%,
t(15)=1.18, p>.1 and 53.3%, t(15)=.35, p>.1, respectively.
To see if there was any difference across ages and conditions, an age (2) x
condition (3) ANOVA was conducted on the proportions of AS responses. It
revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(2,95)=20.7, p<.001, indicating
that the children in the bare verb condition and the verb with arguments
condition selected AS events more often than the children in the noun condition.
Since an interaction between age and condition was also found to be significant,
F(2,95)=3.83, p<.05, the proportions of AS responses were compared across two
age groups for each condition. The age difference was significant only in the
verb with arguments condition, F(1,95)=9.25, p<.01.
The results showed that English-speaking 3-year-olds could fast-map a
novel noun to the object. But at the same time, children of the same age were
not willing to extend a novel verb to the event in which the same action was
performed with a different object. This suggests that noun learning is easier
than verb learning for English-speaking children --as expected.
Three-year-olds do not yet understand one of the most basic principles of verb
extension: verbs should be extended on the basis of the action independent of the
object. By contrast, the 5-year-olds could extend a novel verb properly when it
was embedded in a sentence frame with the subject and the object.
Performance declined to chance-level, however, when the verb was presented
without the explicit mention of the arguments (just saying “X-ing”). As
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Noun Verb
Arguments Bare Verb
% AS responses
3-year-olds
5-year-olds
*
*
*
previous research has shown, argument structure is one of the important
resources children can rely on to infer the meaning of a novel verb (Fisher, 1996;
Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff,1996; Naigles,1990). In addition, argument-dropping
is not allowed in English and verbs always appear with arguments. As a result,
English-speaking children might come to heavily rely on the information about
argument structure for learning a novel verb.
2.2.2. Results from Japanese-speaking preschoolers. The mean proportions
of AS responses by Japanese children in each condition are shown in Figure 2.
In the noun condition, both the 3- and the 5-year-olds selected OS events
significantly above chance (73.4%, t(14)=2.88, p<.05 for the 3-year-olds and
92%, t(13)=5.83, p<.001 for the 5-year-olds), indicating that they could fast-map
a novel noun to a novel object. By contrast, in extending novel verbs, only the
5-year-olds in the bare verb condition showed successful performance, selecting
AS events 83.3% of the time, t(12)=4.56, p<.001. The 5-year-olds in the verb
with arguments condition and the 3-year-olds both in the bare verb condition and
in the verb with arguments condition performed at chance, selecting AS events
69.0% (t(13)=1.56, p>.1), 64.2% (t(12)=1.53, p>.1) and 41.0% (t(12)=.7, p>.1)
of the time, respectively.
In order to compare the mean proportions of AS responses across ages and
conditions, an age (2) x condition (3) ANOVA was conducted. It revealed a
significant main effect of condition, F(2,76)=17.79, p<.001, suggesting that the
children in the two verb conditions selected AS events more often than those in
the noun condition. The interaction between age and condition was also found
to be significant, F(2,76)=3.34, p<.05. The proportions of AS responses were
compared across two age groups for each condition and it was found that the
5-year-olds performed significantly better than the 3-year-olds in the verb with
arguments condition, F(1,76)=4.14, p<.05. No age difference was found in the
noun condition, F(1,76)=1.90, p<.1, and in the bare verb condition,
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Noun Verb
Arguments Bare Verb
% AS responses
3-year-olds
5-year-olds
*
*
*
Figure 2. Mean proportions of AS responses in Japanese-speaking children
F(1,76)=1.88, p>.1.
It was found again that verb learning was more difficult than noun learning
even for children learning Japanese, a language thought to have linguistic
properties that foster verb learning. Even 3-year-olds could extend nouns
properly, while the children of the same age performed only at chance in
extending novel verbs. Five-year-olds successfully mapped a novel verb to the
action above chance when the verb was presented by itself without any
arguments, although their performance was slightly lowered if the verb was
introduced embedded in the sentence frame with the subject and the object.
Thus, Japanese children most successfully extended a novel verb when the verb
was presented by itself without arguments. This contrasts to English
5-year-olds who need information about argument structure in extending novel
verbs properly. Certainly the information about argument structure is useful
when determining what element of the scene should be incorporated into the
meaning of a novel verb. However, this information is not always available to
Japanese children, as arguments are very usually dropped in Japanese when the
arguments can be inferred from the context. As a consequence, Japanese
children might develop another strategy to be more attentive to the morpheme
‘-teiru (-ing)’ which suggests that the word is a verb, instead of exclusively
relying on the argument structure.
2.2.3. Results from Mandarin Chinese preschoolers. As shown in Figure 3,
Chinese children, both the 3- and the 5-year-olds, selected AS events
significantly below chance in all the three conditions. An age (2) x condition
(3) ANOVA conducted on the proportions of AS responses revealed that neither
the main effects nor the interaction was significant. The results indicate that
Chinese 3-year-olds, like Japanese- and English-speaking counterparts, were
able to fast-map a novel noun. However, in the two verb conditions, both 3-
Figure 3. Mean proportions of AS responses by Mandarin-Chinese-speaking
children in the main study
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Noun Verb
Arguments Bare Word
% AS responses
3-year-olds
5-year-olds
******
and 5-year-olds failed to fast-map a novel verb to the action. Rather, they
selected OS events significantly above chance.
The results suggest that Chinese preschoolers, both 3- and 5-year-olds, have
a strong tendency to fast-map a novel word to a novel object, whether the word
was presented as a noun, a verb, or a bare word whose grammatical form class
was not clear. Since these results from Chinese preschoolers were totally
surprising, we conducted a series of follow-up studies with Mandarin Chinese
speakers. First, we replicated the verb with arguments condition with older
children and adults to see whether they could extend novel verbs to the same
action performed with a different object. In Follow-up Study 2, two additional
versions of the verb with arguments condition were conducted with Chinese
preschoolers in order to see whether their performance in generalizing novel
verbs would be improved when provided with additional linguistic cues.
3. Follow-up Study 1: Replication of the Verb with Arguments condition with
Chinese 6- and 8-year-olds and adults
3.1. Method.
3.1.1. Participants. Twenty adults (mean =23), 15 6-year-olds (mean=6;9) and
15 8-year-olds (mean age=8;7) participated in this study. All the participants
were monolingual Mandarin Chinese speakers and lived in Beijing.
3.1.2. Stimuli and Procedure. The stimuli and the procedure were exactly the
same as those for the verb with arguments condition in the main study.
3.2. Results
The adults extended novel verbs to AS events 100% of the time, indicating
that a novel word introduced in the verb with arguments condition was clearly a
verb from the adults’ point of view. The 8-year-olds could also map a novel
verb to the action, selecting AS events 72.2% of the time, t(14)= 2.17, p<.05.
However, the performance of the 6-year-olds did not differ from chance, 52.2%,
t(14)=.19, p>.1.
4. Follow-up Study 2: Providing additional linguistic cues to Chinese
preschoolers
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants. Twenty seven 3-year-old and 27 5-year-old monolingual
Mandarin-Chinese-speaking children who had not been tested in the main study
participated. They were randomly assigned either to the monosyllabic word
condition or to the monosyllabic word + 3 sentence frames condition (see
below).
4.1.2. Linguistic cues. Additional linguistic cues were provided in two stages.
First, the number of syllables in the word was changed. In the original study
for Chinese children, we prepared novel words with two syllables both for novel
nouns and for novel verbs. This was because two syllable words were most
common for both nouns and verbs. However, verbs referring to simple actions
such as “jump” and “kick” tend to be monosyllabic words. Thus, we
constructed monosyllabic nonsense words and replicated the verb with
arguments condition with 3- and 5-year-olds (the monosyllabic word condition).
In addition to this manipulation, in the monosyllabic word + 3 sentence frames
condition, we provided additional linguistic cue to further stress that the novel
word was not a noun but a verb. In the instruction for the verb with arguments
condition in the main study, the novel word X could be unambiguously
identified as a verb by the structure of the sentence, in particular, by the word
order and the presence of the aspect marker “zai(4)”. However, zai(4) is also
used as a verb, meaning roughly “exist” or “be present at a place.” In this case,
the word that comes after “zai(4)” is usually a noun. Young children might
have been confused because of this homonymous use ofzai(4)” and mistakenly
assumed that the given word was a noun. In order to show even more clearly
that the newly introduced word was a verb, we presented novel verbs in three
sentence frames, using three different auxiliaries, namely, “zai(4),” “zheng(4)
zai(4),” and “yi(4) zhi(2) zai(4),” all of which marked the progressive aspect.
4.1.3. Stimuli and Procedure. The stimuli and the procedure were exactly the
same as those in the main study, except that additional linguistic cues were
provided to emphasize that the novel word was a verb, as described above.
4.2. Results and Discussion.
The mean proportions of AS responses are shown in Figure 4. Both the 3-
and the 5-year-olds in both the monosyllabic word and the monosyllabic word +
3 sentence frames conditions selected AS events at chance. Although the
children in both the monosyllabic word and the monosyllabic word + 3 sentence
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
monosyllabic word monosyllabic word +3
sentences
%AS response
3-year-olds
5-year-olds
Figure 4. Mean proportions of AS responses in Follow-up Study 2.
frames conditions performed slightly better than their counterparts in the verb
with arguments condition in the main study, the difference was not statistically
reliable. In sum, Chinese preschoolers, not only 3-year-olds but also
5-year-olds, failed to fast-map a novel verb to the action even when linguistic
cues were provided to the maximum. These results are striking given that
Chinese has been considered as one of the most ‘verb-friendly’ languages. We
will discuss this in the general discussion.
5. General discussion
Both the 3- and the 5-year-olds in all the three languages could fast-map a
novel noun to a novel object. By contrast, children had much difficulty in
generalizing novel verbs. All the 3-year-olds from the three language groups
were reluctant to extend the novel verb to the scene in which the same action as
the standard event was performed but with a different object. Japanese- and
English-speaking 5-year-olds could generalize a novel verb to the Action-Same
event, although Chinese 5-year-olds selected the Object-Same event in the verb
extension task. Thus, the results from this research strongly supported the
universal noun advantage view.
At the same time, we also found that linguistic properties did influence how
children learn verbs, although not in the expected way. Children learning
Chinese, which had been thought to be the most ‘verb-friendly,’ had the most
difficulty in fast-mapping novel verbs. In addition, while both Japanese and
English 5-year-olds successfully fast-mapped novel verbs, the condition under
which English-speaking children showed the best performance was different
from that for Japanese-speaking children. English-speaking children attended
to the cues from the argument structure, while Japanese-speaking children
utilized the verb morphology. In other words, both Japanese- and
English-speaking children rely on some linguistic cues that are useful and
available in their own language, in order to identify a given novel word as a verb
and to determine what element of the scene should be incorporated into the
meaning. From this point of view, Chinese lacks important linguistic cues
children can utilize in learning verbs.
The present research has shown that children cannot fast-map a novel verb
to its meaning properly until five years of age. These findings seem to
contradict the fact that children younger than five years of age know and use
many verbs. However, producing a word in an appropriate situation does not
guarantee that the child knows the full, adult-like meaning of the word, as we
discussed earlier. If the child can associate a novel verb with the situation in
which the verb is introduced, and if she limits the use of the word only to the
situations that are very similar to the original situation, she can use the word
correctly. In fact, while Casasola and Cohen (2000) found that 18-month-olds
could form the linkage between a word and an action on the one hand, other
studies, on the other hand, have shown that it is difficult not only for
18-month-olds but also for 3-year-olds to generalize a newly learned verb on the
basis of the same action, ignoring the sameness of the actor or the object (Imai et
al., in press a; Kersten & Smith,2002; Maguire et al.,2002). Children seem to
start out with a very conservative verb meaning representation in which the
“core” meaning is not yet separated from other non-core elements (such as the
object involved) of the event. The process of extracting the adult-like meaning
seems to take place only gradually and progressively, requiring many instances
of the verb with different objects in different contexts.
5.1. Reconsidering factors affecting the ease of verb learning
Our results from Chinese children are really surprising as Chinese has been
considered to be one of the most ‘verb-friendly’ languages. These results led us
to reconsider what properties of languages really help children learn verbs. In
order for children to fast-map a novel word to its meaning, they need to identify
the form class the word belongs to and find the appropriate referent based on
their knowledge about the correspondence between each form class and the kind
of concepts it encodes. If the novel word is a noun, it should be associated with
an object and be extended to the same kind of objects. When the novel word is
a verb, an action or a relation between objects should be attended to as a
meaning of the word. Now that the results from the bare word condition
suggest that children have a default bias to map a novel word to a novel object,
what is important in verb learning is that this noun bias should be suspended in
face of a novel verb. For this decision, children need some cues that tell them
that the novel word is a verb. For such cues, English-speaking children utilize
the argument structure of the sentence and Japanese children attend to the
morphological inflection of the novel word. Since Chinese is an
argument-dropping language, however, the arguments are often omitted from the
sentence. In addition, verbs are not inflected, and therefore cannot be
morphologically distinguished from nouns. The combination of these
properties means that the linguistic cues children should utilize in determining
whether they should map the novel word to the action or to the object are not
always available. As a result, Chinese children might take more time to extract
the linguistic cues they should rely on in learning verbs. Thus, the lack of verb
morphology, especially when it is combined with the argument-dropping
property of the language, seems to make verb learning even more difficult.
However, at the same time, we believe that there must be conditions under
which Chinese preschoolers, especially 5-year-olds, can extend novel verbs to
the action performed with a different object. We suspected that the difficulty in
identifying a word’s grammatical form class solely from structural cues such as
morphological marking or word order leads Chinese children to rely heavily on
extra-linguistic cues. We are currently investigating this possibility (see Imai et
al., in press b, for some preliminary report on this).
References
Au, Terry K., Dapretto, Mirella & Song, You-Kyung (1994). Input vs. constraints:
Early word acquisition in Korean and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 33,
567-582.
Bates, Elizabeth., Marchman, Virginia, Thal, Donna, Fenson, Larry, Dale, Philip,
Reznick, J. Steven, Reilly, Jeff & Hartung, Judy (1994). Developmental and
stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child
Language, 21, 85-123.
Casasola, Marianella & Cohen, Leslie B. (2000). Infants’ association of linguistic
labels with causal actions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 155-168.
Choi, Soonja & Gopnik, Alison (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A
cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 22, 497-529.
Fisher, Cynthia (1996). Structural limits on verb meaning: The role of analogy in
children’s understanding of familiar verb meaning. Journal of Child Language, 24,
389-406.
Gentner, Dedre (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity and
natural partitioning. In S.A.Kuczaj(Ed.), Language development, vol.2: Langauge,
thought and culture (pp301-334). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.
Gillette, Jane, Gleitman, Henry, Gleitman, Lila & Lederer, Anne (1999). Human
simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73, 135-176.
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy. & Golinkoff, Roberta M. (Eds.) (1996). The origins of grammar:
Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press.
Imai, Mutsumi, Haryu, Etsuko & Okada, Hiroyuki (in press a). Mapping novel
nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: Are verb meanings easier to learn than
noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development.
Imai, Mutsumi, Haryu, Etsuko, Okada, Hiroyuki, Li, Lianjing & Shigematsu,
Jun (in press b). Revisiting the noun-verb debate: A crosslinguistic comparison of
novel noun and verb learning in English-, Japanese- and Chinese-speaking children.
In K.Hirsh-Pasek & R.Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn
verbs. Oxford University Press.
Kersten, Alan W. & Smith, Linda B. (2002). Attention to novel objects during verb
learning. Child Development, 73, 93-109.
Kim, Mikyong, McGregor, Karla K. & Thompson, Cynthia K. (2000). Early lexical
development in English- and Korean speaking children: Language-general and
language-specific patterns. Journal of Child Language, 27, 225-254.
Li, Ping, Bates, Elizabeth & MacWhinney, Brian (1993). Processing a language
without inflections: A reaction time study of sentence interpretation in Chinese.
Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 169-192.
Maguire, Mandy J., Hennon, Elizabeth A., Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy, Golinkoff,
Roberta .M, Slutzky, Carly B. & Sootsman, Jennifer (2002). Mapping words to
actions and events: How 18-month-olds learn a verb? The Proceedings of the 26th
Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 371-392.
Naigles, Letitia. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child
Language, 17, 357-374.
Ogura, Tamiko (2001). Meishi yuui, doushi yuui ni oyobosu hahaoya no gengo nyuuryoku
no kentou (Influence of maternal input in the distribution of nouns and verbs in early
vocabulary). Ministry of Education, Science and Sport grant report. Kobe
University.
Tardif, Twila. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from
Mandarin speaker’s early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology, 32, 492-504.
... In this study, 3-and 5-year-old children from three language groups—Japanese, Mandarin Chinese, and English—were tested (Imai, Haryu, & Okada, in press; Mayer et al., 2003; Haryu et al., 2004). The children were all from monolingual families living in Japan (a suburban Tokyo metropolitan area), China (Beijing), and the United States (Philadelphia), respectively. ...
... While the 5-year-olds extended a novel verb to the action-sameobject-change test at reliably above chance level, the 3-year-olds showed only chance-level performance (Imai et al., in press). To our surprise, Japanese children performed better when the verb was presented without the arguments than when it was presented with an explicit mention of the arguments (Haryu et al., 2004). A series of follow-up studies were conducted to specify the nature of the Japanese 3-year-olds' chance level performance in novel verb generalization (Imai et al., in press, Study 2). ...
... Golinkoff et al. (1996) demonstrated that 3-year-old English-speaking children could extend a novel verb to the same intransitive action by a different actor, but the 3-year-olds in Kersten and Smith's (2002) study failed to do so. 4 In our studies (Haryu et al., 2004; Imai et al., in press; Mayer et al., 2003), in none of the three languages were 3-year-olds willing to extend a verb to the same action when an object (the agent in their case and the theme object in our case) was changed. In summary, previous results from novel verb learning studies suggest that children do learn verbs as early as 18 months, but their representation of verb meanings at initial stages is incomplete and fragile. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Words are the building blocks of language. An understanding of how words are learned is thus central to any theory of language acquisition. Although there has been a surge in our understanding of children's vocabulary growth, theories of word learning focus primarily on object nouns. Word learning theories must explain not only the learning of object nouns, but also the learning of other, major classes of words — verbs and adjectives. Verbs form the hub of the sentence because they determine the sentence's argument structure. Researchers throughout the world recognize how our understanding of language acquisition can be at best partial if we cannot comprehend how verbs are learned. This book enters the relatively uncharted waters of early verb learning, focusing on the universal, conceptual foundations for verb learning, and how these foundations intersect with the burgeoning language system.
... Three-and 5-year-old children from three language groups–Japanese, Mandarin Chinese, and English–were tested (Imai et al., in press; Mayer et al., 2003; Haryu et al., 2005). The children were all from monolingual families, ...
... Clearly, the lexical statistics hypothesis implicates learning. Recent literature documents the slow emergence of other statistically based language-specific biases (e.g., Haryu et al., 2005; Hohenstein, 2005 ). Similarly, Samuelson and Smith (1999) found that a shape-match bias in generalizing the referents of newly learned nouns is only be detected in children who have acquired a substantial number of nouns in their vocabularies. ...
Article
Shown an entity (e.g., a plastic whisk) labeled by a novel noun in neutral syntax, speakers of Japanese, a classifier language, are more likely to assume the noun refers to the substance (plastic) than are speakers of English, a count/mass language, who are instead more likely to assume it refers to the object kind [whisk; Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169-200]. Five experiments replicated this language type effect on entity construal, extended it to quite different stimuli from those studied before, and extended it to a comparison between Mandarin speakers and English speakers. A sixth experiment, which did not involve interpreting the meaning of a noun or a pronoun that stands for a noun, failed to find any effect of language type on entity construal. Thus, the overall pattern of findings supports a non-Whorfian, language on language account, according to which sensitivity to lexical statistics in a count/mass language leads adults to assign a novel noun in neutral syntax the status of a count noun, influencing construal of ambiguous entities. The experiments also document and explore cross-linguistically universal factors that influence entity construal, and favor Prasada's [Prasada, S. (1999). Names for things and stuff: An Aristotelian perspective. In R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, & K. Wynn (Eds.), Language, logic, and concepts (pp. 119-146). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press] hypothesis that features indicating non-accidentalness of an entity's form lead participants to a construal of object kind rather than substance kind. Finally, the experiments document the age at which the language type effect emerges in lexical projection. The details of the developmental pattern are consistent with the lexical statistics hypothesis, along with a universal increase in sensitivity to material kind.
Article
Full-text available
This article examines D. Gentner's (1982) claim that nouns are universally predominant in children's early vocabularies. When a conservative method of counting nouns was used, 9 out of 10 22-month-old monolingual Mandarin-speaking children produced more verbs or action words than nouns or object labels in their naturalistic speech. When a more liberal definition of nouns was used, neither a noun nor a verb bias was found. Importantly, there was no difference in the type-token ratios of the children's use of nouns and verbs. Thus, a sampling bias type of explanation cannot explain the prevalence of verbs in these data. Instead, these data suggest the importance of a variety of linguistic and sociocultural input factors in early word learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Verb learning is clearly a function of observation of real-world contingencies; however, it is argued that such observational information is insufficient to account fully for vocabulary acquisition. This paper provides an experimental validation of Landau & Gleitman's (1985) syntactic bootstrapping procedure; namely, that children may use syntactic information to learn new verbs. Pairs of actions were presented simultaneously with a nonsense verb in one of two syntactic structures. The actions were subsequently separated, and the children (MA = 2;1) were asked to select which action was the referent for the verb. The children's choice of referent was found to be a function of the syntactic structure in which the verb had appeared.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the composition of the early productive vocabulary of eight Korean- and eight English-learning children and the morpho-syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic characteristics of their caregivers' input in order to determine parallels between caregiver input and early lexical development. Vocabulary acquisition was followed using maternal diary and checklists for the Korean-learning children (from a mean age of 1;6 to 1;9) and for the English-learning children (from a mean age of 1;4 to 1;8). Results showed that both Korean-learning and English-learning children acquired significantly more nouns than verbs at the 50-word mark. However, Korean children learned significantly more verbs than did English-learning children. The relative ease with which Korean learners, as compared to English learners, acquired verbs parallels several differences in the linguistic and socio-pragmatic characteristics of the input addressed to them. Korean-speaking caregivers presented more activity-oriented utterances, more verbs, and more salient cues to verbs than did English-speaking caregivers. These data suggest that both general and language-specific factors shape the early lexicon.
Article
At head of title: Center for the Study of Reading Bibliography: p. 62-73 Research was conducted under a contract from the National Institute of Education under contract no. HEW-NIE-C-400-76-0116
Article
Three experiments provided evidence that 3.5- to 4-year-old English-speaking children (N = 72) attend to the appearances of novel objects, not only when they hear a novel noun, but also when they hear a novel verb. Children learning nouns in the context of novel, moving objects attended exclusively to the appearances of objects, even though nouns were also related to the motions of those objects. Children learning verbs attended equally to the appearances of objects and their motions. The latter result contrasted with the results from adults (N = 20), who focused more strongly on motions than on the appearances of objects when learning verbs. When familiar objects were instead employed, child verb learners attended more to motions than to the appearances of objects. Children may attend to novel objects during verb learning because knowledge of an object may be prerequisite to understanding what a verb means in the context of that object.
Structural limits on verb meaning: The role of analogy in children's understanding of familiar verb meaning
  • Cynthia Fisher
Fisher, Cynthia (1996). Structural limits on verb meaning: The role of analogy in children's understanding of familiar verb meaning. Journal of Child Language, 24, 389-406.
Meishi yuui, doushi yuui ni oyobosu hahaoya no gengo nyuuryoku no kentou (Influence of maternal input in the distribution of nouns and verbs in early vocabulary) Ministry of Education, Science and Sport grant report
  • Tamiko Ogura
Ogura, Tamiko (2001). Meishi yuui, doushi yuui ni oyobosu hahaoya no gengo nyuuryoku no kentou (Influence of maternal input in the distribution of nouns and verbs in early vocabulary). Ministry of Education, Science and Sport grant report. Kobe University.