ArticlePDF Available

Improving multi-tasking ability through action videogames

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study examined whether action videogames can improve multi-tasking in high workload environments. Two groups with no action videogame experience were pre-tested using the Multi-Attribute Task Battery (MATB). It consists of two primary tasks; tracking and fuel management, and two secondary tasks; systems monitoring and communication. One group served as a control group, while a second played action videogames a minimum of 5 h a week for 10 weeks. Both groups returned for a post-assessment on the MATB. We found the videogame treatment enhanced performance on secondary tasks, without interfering with the primary tasks. Our results demonstrate action videogames can increase people's ability to take on additional tasks by increasing attentional capacity.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Improving multi-tasking ability through action videogames
Dan Chiappe
a
,
*
, Mark Conger
b
, Janet Liao
a
, J. Lynn Caldwell
c
, Kim-Phuong L. Vu
a
a
California State University Long Beach, Department of Psychology, 1250 N Bellower Blvd, Long Beach, CA 90840, USA
b
Air Force Research Lab, 711 Human Performance Wing, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, OH 45433, USA
c
Northrop Grumman Corporation, Advanced Programs & Technology, El Segundo, CA 90245, USA
article info
Article history:
Received 25 November 2011
Accepted 10 August 2012
Keywords:
Multi-tasking
Videogame training
Attention
abstract
The present study examined whether action videogames can improve multi-tasking in high workload
environments. Two groups with no action videogame experience were pre-tested using the
Multi-Attribute Task Battery (MATB). It consists of two primary tasks; tracking and fuel management,
and two secondary tasks; systems monitoring and communication. One group served as a control group,
while a second played action videogames a minimum of 5 h a week for 10 weeks. Both groups returned
for a post-assessment on the MATB. We found the videogame treatment enhanced performance on
secondary tasks, without interfering with the primary tasks. Our results demonstrate action videogames
can increase peoples ability to take on additional tasks by increasing attentional capacity.
Ó2012 Elsevier Ltd and The Ergonomics Society. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
1.1. The need to train multi-tasking ability
Information technology has greatly increased system
complexity (Dekker, 2012;Karr-Wisniewski and Lu, 2010). Whether
the domain is nuclear power plants, unmanned aircraft systems
(UAS), military command, or air trafc control, operators need to
coordinate the performance of multiple tasks, but have limited
attentional resources to do so (Chiappe et al., 2012;Liu et al., 2009).
Indeed, cognitive demands on operators are most likely going to
increase. In the case of UAS, for example, future designs will
incorporate more high-resolution cameras and sensing equipment,
increased weapons delivery capabilities, and possibly a single
operator controlling many aircraft (Liu et al., 2009). Although
automation tools are being developed, their net effect will likely be
to reduce physical workload while increasing cognitive workload
(McCarley and Wickens, 2005;Dehais et al., 2012). Fortunately,
recent studies have found that capacity for attention can be
enhanced (Bavelier et al., 2012). The present study examines
whether videogames can serve as a cost-effective training tool to
increase operatorsprociency at coordinating multiple tasks in
high-stress situations.
Much research has been devoted to how to train performance of
complex cognitive tasks; ones requiring people to simultaneously
process multiple interacting information elements as they carry out
various subtasks (e.g., Healy et al., 2012;Lim et al., 2009;Merrill,
2002;Paas and Van Gog, 2009). In general, training is most effec-
tive when it involves practicing all subtasks simultaneously, in
environments that are as similar as possible to those the operator
will be engaged in (Paas and Van Gog, 2009;Van Merriënboer and
Kirschner, 2007). High-delity training environments, however, are
costly. As such, cost-effective techniques are being investigated,
including using off the shelf action videogames (Green and Bavelier,
2008;Wu et al., 2012).
Action videogames have the potential to serve as useful training
tools for improving multi-tasking because they can preserve the
cognitive authenticity associated with many complex tasks
(Herrington et al., 2003). Cognitive authenticity refers to when
training and transfer environments possess similar information-
processing demands, despite different supercial characteristics.
The benet of cognitive authenticity is illustrated by Cassavaugh
and Kramer (2009), who found that computer-based training of
attention demanding driving tasks improved driving performance
of older adults. The cognitive authenticity of many videogames
stems from the fact that they simulate the same mental demands
many system operators regularly employ; constantly shifting
attention across multiple tasks that include monitoring commu-
nications, fuel status, weapons, and the location of team members,
all while simultaneously navigating through a virtual system
(Spence and Feng, 2010;Wu et al., 2012).
Using action videogames to develop multi-tasking skills is also
supported by well-known training principles. This includes delib-
erate practice, well-timed feedback, and variability of training
*Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1 562 985 5024.
E-mail address: dan.chiappe@csulb.edu (D. Chiappe).
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Applied Ergonomics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/apergo
0003-6870/$ esee front matter Ó2012 Elsevier Ltd and The Ergonomics Society. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2012.08.002
Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284
(Healy et al., 2012). With respect to deliberate practice, action
videogames promote learning that is highly motivated, focused,
and effortful (Green et al., 2010). They have features motivating
players to keep improving their performance, including different
levels of difculty so novices can gradually develop their skills.
They also have engaging storylines and cinematic elements like
music and sound effects that increase emotional impact. In terms of
well-timed feedback, videogames often display scores at the end of
each level. This allows players to improve performance without
interfering with play. If games feature multiplayer modes, feedback
on how performance compares to others can also motivate players
to improve their skills (Spence and Feng, 2010). Regarding vari-
ability of training,with the great number of titles available, vid-
eogames offer a large variety of contexts where skills relevant to
multi-tasking can be exercised. Varying the practice stimuli
strengthens representations of key features that need to be atten-
ded to during task performance, increasing the likelihood that
individuals will be able to generate action schemata in novel situ-
ations (Green et al., 2010).
1.2. Theoretical background
While the preceding suggests that videogames may improve
multi-tasking, this is only going to be the case if capacity for
attention can be modied through training. Fortunately, many
studies have found that videogame play can increase attentional
resources (e.g., Bavelier et al., 2012;Boot et al., 2008;Green et al.,
2010;Hubert-Wallander et al., 2011;Rosser et al., 2007). The
greatest benets come from action videogames, particularly the
First Person Shooter (FPS) variety, as opposed to strategy games,
role playing games, or maze/puzzle games (Feng et al., 2007;Green
and Bavelier, 2003, 2006;Spence et al., 2009). This is not surprising
because as Spence and Feng (2010) point out, action videogames
place greater demands on spatial attention than other genres.
Playing FPS games requires that individuals have their attention
captured by unexpected objects in the periphery. They must quickly
recognize those objects, select relevant ones for further processing
and ignore those that are irrelevant, all while making split-second
decisions. In addition, FPS games require individuals to divide their
attention across different objects, as multiple threats often need to
be dealt with simultaneously.
Studies also suggest that the increased attentional resources
produced by action videogames lead to a broadening of the atten-
tional visual eld (Hubert-Wallander et al., 2011;Bavelier et al.,
2012). This is the area around xation an individual is able to
attend to, and process information from, without moving their
head or eyes. For example, Green and Bavelier (2003) used the
Useful Field of View task (UFOV) that required participants to
identify small targets presented radially at different eccentricities
(10
,20
and 30
). They found individuals that trained with action
videogames improved their ability to identify targets at all eccen-
tricities compared to those trained by playing non-action video-
games. Green and Bavelier (2006) added a primary task that
required processing information at xation, in addition to detecting
peripheral targets. They found action videogame and non-action
videogame groups did not differ in the primary task. However,
the former performed better on the peripheral task. This suggests
the improved ability to process peripheral information did not
come at the expense of primary task performance. Important to our
purpose, these results suggest playing action videogames can
improve peoples ability to take on additional tasks.
More recently, neuropsychological research has found evidence
that action videogames benet attentional processing. Bavelier
et al. (2012), for example, compared action videogame players to
non-gamers in a fMRI study that used a version of the UFOV task.
They found that non-gamers featured increased activation of
fronto-parietal regions (i.e., regions crucial to the control of atten-
tion) as attentional demands of the task increased, while gamers
did not. Evidently, the latter were able to manage the greater task
demands with less attentional effort. Likewise, Wu et al. (2012)
recorded ERP at parietal regions while people performed a task
similar to the UFOV. Two training groups were compared; those
that had played a FPS game for 10 h, and those that played a non-
action videogame for 10 h. They found large differences in late-
onset parietal activation for those who played the FPS game, but
not for the control group. Wu et al. (2012) state this reects an
enhanced allocation of attentional resources among those that
played the FPS game. More directly related to multi-tasking, Maclin
et al. (2011) examined parietal ERP changes for primary and
secondary task performance as a function of playing the Space
Fortress action videogame. In a pre-assessment, participants played
the game while also performing the oddballsecondary task. The
latter required them to silently count the rare tones presented in
a sequence of frequent tones. This was followed by 20 h of playing
the videogame. During a post-assessment, participants had to play
the game while carrying out the oddball task. They found that after
treatment, parietal ERP activity was lower during videogame
maneuvers, but higher during the oddball task. Thus, due to prac-
tice, participants were able to devote fewer attentional resources to
the videogame, and were able to devote more to the secondary
task.
1.3. The present study
This study also examined whether action videogames can
improve multi-tasking. It differs from previous studies in important
respects. First, people administered treatment in their own home.
This was done because for videogame training to be cost-effective
(e.g., as part of an effort to reduce the high cost of pilot training), it
should be implemented at home. It is thus necessary to show
benets even when treatment is administered outside the connes
of the laboratory. Second, we assessed multi-tasking using the
Multi-Attribute Task Battery (MATB), a task environment much
more complex than is typically used in videogame research. It
consists of two dwell tasks requiring constant monitoring and
action, as well as two secondary tasks performed intermittently,
simulating many of the tasks that aircrews regularly perform. It is
a validated measure that correlates with real-world performance
(Comstock and Arnegard, 1992). By using the MATB, we were able
to determine whether effects of videogame play scale up,and
provide benets in high-workload tasks more comparable to
realistic task environments. This is important because a recent
study by Donohue et al. (2012) calls this into question. It failed to
nd multi-tasking differences between action videogame players
and non-players using complex tasks. For example, videogame
players did not perform better in a driving task under dual task
conditions that required participants to simultaneously answer
trivia questions.
In our study, two groups with no experience playing action
videogames were pre-tested using the MATB. One group served as
a control, while the experimental group was treated with action
videogame play for 10 weeks. Both groups were then given a post-
assessment on the MATB. We hypothesized that if playing action
videogames increases attentional resources, those receiving the
treatment should perform better on the MATB post-assessment.
Consistent with claim that greater attentional resources leads
to a broadening of the attentional eld, we expected the biggest
improvement to be in the secondary MATB tasks, and that this
improvement should not come at the expense of the primary
tasks.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284 279
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Participants were undergraduates from California State Univer-
sity, Long Beach. They were recruited fromIntroductory Psychology
courses and word-of-mouth. Potential participants lled-out
questionnaires to be entered into a rafe for $50. This question-
naire asked students to provide information about whether they
are currently playing videogames, or whether they have played
them in the past, and if so, which types of games they played. Of the
600 questionnaires received, we selected 53 individuals who
indicated they had no prior or current experience playing action
videogames. Of these, 26 were randomly assigned to the video-
game group and 27 to the control group. Ages ranged from 18 to 36
with a mean of 22 years for both groups. Two videogame partici-
pants were dropped for not playing the required amount. Two
control participants were dropped because they did not attend all
assessment sessions. The remaining included 12 males and 12
females in the videogame group, and 12 males and 13 females in
the control. According to their responses to a questionnaire, all
participants had normal or corrected to normal vision, and 87%
reported being right-handed. All reported being in good general
health, with no physical disabilities. Furthermore, 76% reported
their occupation as being a full time student. The remainder re-
ported working part-time, with sales and service jobs as the most
frequent occupations.
2.2. Materials
The tool used to assess multi-tasking is the MATB (Comstock
and Arnegard, 1992). It consists of computerized tasks analogous
to activities aircrews perform while engaged in high workload
ight operations, yet is accessible to populations with no aviation
experience. It requires the performance of four tasks: systems
monitoring, fuel management, communications, and tracking tasks.
These are described in detail in Table 1.
Videogame participants were given a SONY PS3Ôconsole to use
at home during the 10 weeks of treatment. In addition, they were
given commercially-available FPS games. This means players have
the same point of view as the main character in the game, and the
primary task is to shoot at enemy targets. While designing our own
game would have afforded greater control over the stimuli that
participants were exposed to, we followed much of the research
literature in using off the shelf games. Indeed, many of the games
we used were ones used in other studies examining the cognitive
effects of action videogames. Although some control is relinquished
by not designing our own game, the virtue is it allows us to examine
whether these products can be used to enhance attentional
resources. This is important because for videogame training to be
cost effective, it will be necessary to use commercially available
products.
Furthermore, as Spence and Feng (2010) point out, despite
differing in some supercial details, commercially available FPS
games represent a coherent stimulus class, as the games share key
features that distinguish them from other types of games, including
strategy games, role playing games, and puzzle games. They feature
considerable sensory demands, as targets are often presented in
cluttered settings. In terms of attentional demands, all the games
feature abrupt onset of events, with the need to discriminate and
select important objects among distracters. The games also require
task switching and multi-tasking, as players have to monitor factors
such as ammunition and weapons systems, as well as other team
members, while at the same time carrying out the primary task of
shooting targets. In addition, multiple objects have to be attended
to, and often at the periphery of the visual eld. Working memory is
also taxed by these games as people have to make decisions on how
to allocate resources, and which weapons to use, often under
stressful conditions. Finally, all games place high demands on
spatial cognition as they require mental rotation and navigation
through complex visual domains. They also require visuo-motor
coordination through aiming and shooting and the operation of
complex virtual machinery. In short, games were chosen for the
high mental, visual, and attentional demands they exert. Our
participants were given two games to start: Ghost Recon Advanced
War Fighter 2Ôand Unreal Tournament 3Ô. Once they nished
these, they were given additional games including Medal of Hon-
orÔ,VanquishÔ, Bioshock 2Ô, and Resistance 2Ô.
2.3. Procedure
Selected participants in the control and videogame groups were
brought into our lab for a pre-assessment on the MATB. It was
administered in four high workload sessions, including one 10-min
(familiarization), one 20-min (warm-up), and two 30-min (practice
and test) sessions. The last 30-min session was used in the analyses
reported below.
Once they completed the pre-assessment, participants in the
videogame condition were trained on the PS3Ô. Experimenters
taught participants how to set up the console. They were then
assisted on the training modes of the games Ghost Recon 2Ôand
Unreal Tournament3Ô. This ensured they understood how to orient
their characters, the goals of the games, and basic maneuvers such
as how to use weapons and positioning. The participants were then
instructed on how to complete their weekly game-playing diaries.
Participants were asked to play a minimum of 5 h per week for
10 weeks, but were given some exibility for their weekly game
playing. All participants averaged a minimum of 5 h of play across
the 10 weeks. To keep track of gameplay, participants were emailed
a videogame diary each week. This required them to record perti-
nent information about their videogame activities, including time
of play, achievements made, how enjoyable the games were during
each session, and whether they experienced any difculties.
Participants were informed that they could be asked to come to the
Table 1
The Multi-Attribute Task battery.
a
Task Description of duties
Tracking task Requires the manual operation of a joystick to keep a target in the center of a tracking window.
Fuel management task Requires the activation of virtual pumps in order to ll two tanks, maintaining them as closely as possible at a pre-specied ideal level.
Systems monitoring task Requires monitoring lights and dials. For the lights, when one goes out, participants are required to press a button to turn it back on.
For the other designated light, participants have to press a button when it is lit to turn the light back off. For the dials, participants have
to reset a dial by pressing a button if it is found to be operating outside of a specied range.
Communications task Requires individuals to respond to auditory communication instructions to a particular call-sign (ignoring communications to
other call-signs). When their call-sign is mentioned, they have to change one of two communications settings, or one of two
navigational settings.
a
The MATB tool was presented to participants on a Dell Desktop computer using a 20
00
by 20
00
computer monitor.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284280
lab to demonstrate their reported level of videogame skills during
the course of the study. Because we captured the number of re-
ported hours of videogame playing and levels/games completed in
the weekly diaries, we were able to perform correlational analyses
to examine the relationship between number of games completed
and performance on the MATB.
Participants in the videogame and control conditions were
informed after the pre-assessment whether they would be coming
back for one more (post-assessment only) or two more (mid-term
1
and post-assessment) sessions. Following each session, the control
group was paid in cash, at a rate of $10/hour. In the post-
assessment, videogame participants were given the code to
unlock their PS3Ôconsoles, which they were allowed to keep.
3. Results
3.1. Description of analyses
To analyze performance on the MATB, we conducted a series of
2223 mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests, with
condition (videogame vs. control) and gender (male vs. female) as
between-subject factors, and session (pre-assessment vs. post-
assessment) and interval (rst 10 min, second 10 min, vs. third
10 min) as repeated measures factors. Interval was included
because workload is not evenly distributed throughout the MATB.
Workload starts light (e.g., no communications, and dials within the
normal range) and builds during the session. Including interval
therefore enabled us to examine whether there are any changes in
performance throughout the session, either because of fatigue, or
practice effects. The alpha level was set a priori to a p-value of .05
for statistical signicance, and to .10 for marginal effects.
While MATB yields data on 21 variables, separate ANOVAs were
carried out only for some of these. We only included variables not
suffering from range restriction and that have been reported in
other studies using the MATB (e.g., Caldwell and Ramspott, 1998;
Singh et al., 2010). We also selected variables in each MATB subtask.
For reasons of brevity, below we report only ndings pertaining to
main effects or interactions with the variable condition. Whenever
appropriate we also include correlations for the videogame group
between number of games completed and performance.
3.2. Communications
The analysis examining RT for correct responses to communi-
cation instructions (COMCRT) revealed a signicant interaction
between session and condition, F(1,45) ¼5.02, p<.05. In the
control, RT did not decrease between pre- and post-assessments,
F<1, but for the videogame condition, RT did decrease,
F(1,22) ¼10.18, p¼.004 (See Fig. 1). Although the two groups
started off at the same level, the videogame group was faster at
responding to communications during the post-assessment. There
were no other main effects or interactions. Correlation analyses
between number of videogames completed and post-assessment
COMCRT revealed that those who completed more games had
a faster RT (r¼.41, p<.05). There was no signicant correlation
between number of games completed and pre-assessment
COMCRT (p>.10). These results show playing videogames for 10
weeks improved performance on the communications task.
An analysis of the standard deviation of the RT to correctly
respond to communication instructions (COMCSD) revealed
a marginal interactionbetween session and condition, F(1,45)¼3.32,
p¼.075. For the control group, SD did not decrease between the pre-
and post-assessments, F<1. For the videogame condition, however,
there was a decrease in SD between the two assessments,
F(1,22) ¼4.47, p<.05 (See Fig. 2). Not only did the videogame
participants become faster; they also became more consistent in
how quickly they responded to communications. This is further
supported by a negative correlation between number of games
completed and their overall COMCSD performance for the post-
assessment (r¼.48, p¼.017). Number of videogames completed
was not related to performance during the pre-assessment.
3.3. Systems monitoring
With respect to the time out errors for the dials (DLSTO), there
was a main effect of condition, F(1,45) ¼6.84, p<.05. Videogame
participants had fewer time-out errors (M¼.33, SE ¼.10) than
controls (M¼.70, SE ¼.10). There was also a signicant interaction
between session, gender, and condition, F(1,45) ¼6.02, p¼.018.
Subsequent analyses revealed that for male participants there was
a signicant interaction between session and condition (See Fig. 3),
F(1,22) ¼5.95, p¼.02, but not for females F(1,23) ¼1.06, p¼.31. For
the males in the control condition, the time-out errors increased
between the pre-assessment (M¼.42, SE ¼.19) and the post-
assessment (M¼.83, SE ¼.21), while for the videogame group,
the time-out errors decreased between the pre-assessment
(M¼.50, SE ¼.19) and the post-assessment (M¼.17, SE ¼.21).
Reaction Time to Respond to
Communication Instructions
(COMCRT)
2.0
2.5
3.0
3.5
4.0
Videogame Group
Control Group
Pre-Assessment Post-Assessment
Condition
Session
Mean RT (in seconds)
Fig. 1. Reaction time to respond to communication instructions.
Standard Deviation of Reaction Time to
Respond to Communication Instructions
(COMCSD)
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
Videogame Group
Control Group
Pre-Assessment Post-Assessment
Condition
Session
Mean SD (in seconds)
Fig. 2. Standard deviation of reaction time to respond to communication instructions.
1
One-third of the control and videogame participants came back for a midterm
evaluation on the MATB. However, due to the small sample size, no signicant
effects were obtained when comparing the midterm performance to the pre-test
performance. Many of the trends evident in the midterm, though, were signi-
cant in the post-assessment comparisons.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284 281
Furthermore, there was an interaction between interval, session,
and condition, F(2,90) ¼5.39, p¼.006. Post hoc analyses revealed
that for the control group, the interaction between session and
interval was not signicant, F(2, 46) ¼2.95, p¼.10. For the video-
game condition, the interaction was marginally signicant,
F(2,44) ¼3.07, p¼.056. In the pre-assessment, videogame partici-
pants displayed an inverted U function across the three intervals.
They had fewer time-outerrors in the rst interval (M¼.29, SE ¼.14),
but these increased during the higher workload second interval
(M¼.92, SE ¼.30), and then decreased again in the nal interval
(M¼.21, SE ¼.10). However, in the post-assessment, time-out errors
did not increase due to increasing workload. Instead, they decreased
from the rst (M¼.25, SE ¼.09) and second interval (M¼.25,
SE ¼.11)to the third (M¼.08, SE ¼.06). Thus, in the post-assessment,
the videogame participants displayed a greater ability to manage an
increased workload, while the controlgroup did not display a similar
improvement. Furthermore, in the post-assessment, the more
games participants completed, the fewer their time-out errors inthe
second 10-min interval (r¼.39, p¼.056). In contrast, none of the
correlations between the number of games completed and DLSTO
errors for the pre-assessment were signicant.
For the RT to respond to the lights (LTSRT), there was a main
effect of condition, F(1,45) ¼5.34, p<.05. Overall, videogame
participants responded more quickly to the lights (M¼1.72 s ,
SE ¼.08) than controls (M¼1.9 7 s , SE ¼.08). There was also an
interaction between interval and condition, F(2,90) ¼5.59,
p¼.005. For the control group, RT remained constant throughout
the three 10-min intervals, F(2,46) ¼1.89, p¼.163, while for the
videogame group, RT decreased across the three 10-min intervals,
F(2,44) ¼7.15 , p¼.002, showing that the videogame participants
improved throughout the 30-min sessions while the control group
did not. However, the absence of an interaction between interval,
condition and session does not allow us to conclude that the
improvement is due to treatment.
With respect to the time-out errors to the lights (LTSTO), there
were no main effects or interactions with condition (F<1).
However, we found a negative correlation between the number of
games completed and the number of time-out errors to the lights in
the post-assessment (r¼.40, p¼.055), but not for the pre-
assessment (r¼.16, p¼.46), showing a benet of playing vid-
eogames for 10 weeks.
3.4. Tracking
For the tracking task, we examined the tracking RMS errors
(TRKRMS). We found a main effect of condition, with videogame
participants (M¼23.25, SE ¼1.09) overall performing better than
controls (M¼27.86, SE ¼1.07), F(1,45) ¼9.10, p¼.004. However,
the lack of an interaction between session and condition (F<1)
suggests the intervention is not responsible for this effect. None of
the correlations between number of videogames completed and
post-assessment TRKRMS approached signicance.
3.5. Fuel management
Prior to analyzing the mean deviation of fuel tanks from the
ideal of 2500 (TNKMAD), three participantsresponses were
dropped (two control, and one videogame) because their data was
greater than 2 standard deviations from the mean. With the
remaining participants, we found a signicant effect of condition,
F(1,42) ¼4.84, p¼.033. Participants in the videogame condition did
better (M¼279.24, SE ¼30.26) than the controls (M¼373.41,
SE ¼30.26). No other main effects or interactions reached signi-
cance, including the interaction between session and condition,
F(1,45) ¼1.02, p¼.32. The absence of an interaction between
session and condition does not allow us to claim the treatment had
any effect on this task. Furthermore, none of the correlations
between number of videogames completed and TNKMAD for the
post-assessment were signicant.
4. Discussion
We examined whether action videogames can enhance the
ability of novices to carry out multiple tasks. We found playing FPS
games improved performance on the communications and systems
monitoring tasks of the MATB. Specically, participants who played
FPS games became faster and more consistent in their time to
respond to communications, with those completing more games
showing the most improvement. In the systems monitoring tasks,
videogame participants got better at monitoring and responding to
the lights and dials compared to control participants. In particular,
male videogame players showed a greater overall improvement in
reacting to dials. Furthermore, videogame participants but not
control participants, improved in their ability to react to dials
needing their attention during the high workload second interval.
Indeed, the more games they completed, the fewer time out errors
they had during this interval. With respect to reacting to the lights,
participants that completed more videogames had fewer time out
errors. However, our results did not reveal any differences between
conditions in the tracking or fuel management tasks that could be
attributed to treatment.
Our results show the 10-week action videogame treatment
improved performance on the secondary tasks, and this benet did
not come at the expense of the primary tasks. The tracking and fuel
management tasks are primary because they appear in the center of
the eld of view, and are the most dynamic and demanding of
attentional resources. The fuel management task also requires
complex decision-making because participants have to develop
a strategy for efciently transferring fuel across the various tanks.
In contrast, the communications and system monitoring tasks,
although important, do not change as frequently, require inter-
mittent responding, and involve processing peripheral information.
Of course, because we only used action videogames and no other
types of videogames, on their own the results of our study do not
allow us to conclude that only action videogames benetmulti-
tasking. However, given the results of previous studies (e.g., Wu
et al., 2012;Spence and Feng, 2010), it is likely that the nature of
the FPS games produced the performance benets.
The present ndings are noteworthy because, unlike other
experiments examining the effects of videogames (e.g., Feng et al.,
2007;Green and Bavelier, 2006), we had participants administer
Time Out Errors to Dials for Males
(DLSTO)
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
Videogame Group
Control Group
Pre-Assessment Post-Assessment
Condition
Session
Mean Number of Errors
Fig. 3. Number of time out errors to respond to dials in systems monitoring task.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284282
the treatment in their own home rather than in a lab. Although we
required them to carefully document their videogame play, we did
not closely monitor their activity during treatment. We took this
approach for reasons of ecological validity eit is likely that any
training program that makes use of videogame play will require
individuals to administer the treatment on their own. Adding close
supervision would, after all, eliminate much of the cost effective-
ness. The fact that videogame play enhanced performance even
though the intervention took place at home demonstrates the
robustness of the treatment.
The results of the present study are consistent with those re-
ported by Green and Bavelier (2006). Their study employed the
UFOV task, where the primary task required participants to
discriminate between shapes presented centrally, while the
secondary task required them to identify the location of targets
presented peripherally. They found that action videogame training
improved performance on the peripheral task, without sacricing
performance on the primary task. A key difference between our
study and theirs, however, is that we employed a much more
complex task, one that has been shown to predict performance in
real life settings ethe MATB.
Our nding that action videogame play improved participants
performance in a complex task differs from that obtained by
Donohue et al. (2012). They found that participants classied as
action videogame players did not perform complex tasks under
dual task conditions better than non-gamers. One possible reason
for the discrepancy between the present results and theirs is that in
our study, participants played videogames for a longer period of
time (minimum of 5 h per week). In Donohue et al. (2012),
participants labeled action videogame playersreported currently
playing only an average of 3 h per week. Thus, it is possible they
were not playing enough to see multi-tasking benets in complex
task environments. Indeed, we saw that the more games people
completed in the 10 weeks of treatment, the more they improved in
their ability to multi-task. In addition, Donohue et al. (2012) used
a trivia task as a secondary task that is different from the visuo-
spatial or communication secondary tasks used in the present
study. Although the trivia task was prescreened to include ques-
tions that participants would answer with 50e90% accuracy, in
actuality it resulted in much lower accuracy rates, as participants
under dual task conditions averaged only slightly above 50%. Thus,
it is not clear whether the excessive difculty of their secondary
task washed out any differences between the action videogame
players and non-players, or whether the participants were even
taking the secondary tasks very seriously.
According to Hubert-Wallander et al. (2011), action videogame
play enhances multi-tasking by increasing attentional resources.
This allows for a broadening of the attentional visual eld (see also
Feng et al., 2007 and West et al., 2008). Playing such games thus
increases the ability to spatially distribute visual attention and to
resolve visual details. As a result, when task-relevant information
appears on the periphery, it is more likely to capture attention and
receive the required processing. Our nding that the videogame
participants showed improvements on the systems monitoring tasks
supports this theory. Although their attention was primarily focused
on the tracking and fuel management tasks, the videogame players
were more likely to notice deviations in the lights and dials and to be
able to process these stimuli during high-workload intervals.
The fact that we found videogame play also enhanced the ability
to process auditory communications suggests that the benets of
videogame play are not limited to the visual modality, but can spill
over to other modalities. While many studies have found increases
in visual attention (e.g., Spence and Feng, 2010;Green and Bavelier,
2008), ours is the rst to show behaviorally that the increased
attentional capacity occurs in both the visual and auditory
modalities. Maclin et al. (2011) found increased attentional
resources for an acoustic secondary task. However, the attentional
benets were only evident in the ERP data, and not in the behav-
ioral data. Finding behavioral benets that span modalities is
important, however, because complex systems often distribute
information across modalities, with a strong reliance on auditory
communications and alarms. In short, our results support the view
that attentional capacity is a domain-general resource that can be
increased through training (Liu et al., 2009;Shipstead et al., 2010).
The nding that action videogame play enhances the ability of
individuals to process peripheral information also suggests training
programs that incorporate this treatment will likely yield
improvements in the ability of operators to maintain situation
awareness (SA). SA refers to the understanding needed to operate
a system in a rapidly changing task environment (Chiappe et al., in
press;Durso et al., 2007). Maintaining SA requires perceiving task-
relevant information, comprehending this information in light of
current processing goals, and projecting the status of the system
into the near future (Endsley, 2006). Indeed, operators of complex
systems spend much of their time developing SA and ensuring that
it is up to date (Stanton et al., 2010).
As Endsley (2006) points out, most cases where human error is
identied as the cause of an aviation mishap, the problem lies with
an operators failure to maintain SA. Most of these failures stem
from failing to perceive information in the environment. For
example, Jones and Endsley (1996) examined 143 aviation inci-
dents involving pilots and air trafc controllers. They found that
76% of the SA errors involved perceptual failures. In contrast, 20% of
the errors involved failures in comprehension, with the remainder
being failures to properly project future states of a system. Relevant
to our ndings, the majority of the perception errors involved
a failure to perceive information that was clearly available in the
operators environment, including requests for communication.
Many of these failures arose because the pilots or controllers were
already engaged in other tasks, displaying attentional tunneling.
This is likely to happen under conditions of stress, which increases
the likelihood of people ignoring peripheral information, even if it
happens to be an otherwise salient alarm (Dehais et al., 2012).
In short, failing to maintain SA by ignoring communication
requests or failing to monitor the status of systems can have
disastrous consequences. Fortunately, it appears playing action
videogames can improve SA by minimizing the likelihood opera-
tors will succumb to a narrowing of attention when carrying out
primary tasks. Action videogames can therefore serve as training
tools for increasing attentional capacity. Furthermore, operators
will likely enjoy implementing this tool, yielding real improve-
ments at a relatively low-cost.
Acknowledgement
This research was funded by Northrop Grumman Corporation.
References
Bavelier, D., Achtman, R., Föcker, M., 2012. Neural bases of selective attention in
action video game players. Vision Res. 61, 132e143.
Boot, W.R., Kramer, A.F., Simons, D.J., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., 2008. The effects of
video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychol.
129, 387e398.
Caldwell, J.A., Ramspott, S., 1998. Effects of task duration on sensitivity to sleep
deprivation using the multi-attribute task battery. Behav. Res. Meth. Instr. 30,
651e660.
Cassavaugh, N., Kramer, A., 2009. Transfer or computer-based training to simulated
driving in older adults. Appl. Ergon. 40, 943e952.
Chiappe, D., Vu, K.-P.L., Strybel, T., 2012. Situation awareness in the NextGen air
trafc management system. Int. J. Hum-Comput. Interact. 28, 140e151.
Chiappe, D., Strybel, T., Vu, K-P.L. Mechanisms for the acquisition of situation
awareness in situated agents. Theor Issues Ergonomics Sci., in press.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284 283
Comstock, J., Arnegard, R., 1992. The Multi-attribute Task Battery for Human
Operator Workload and Strategic Behavior Research. Technical Report 104174.
NASA Langley Research Center, Hampton, VA.
Dehais, F., Causse, M., Vachon, F., Tremblay, S., 2012. Cognitive conict in
human-automation interactions: a psychophysiological study. Appl. Ergon.
43, 588e595.
Dekker, S., 2012. Complexity, signal detection, and the application of ergonomics:
reections on a healthcare case study. Appl. Ergon. 43, 468e472.
Donohue, S.E., James, B., Eslick, A., Mitroff, S., 2012. Cognitive pitfall! Videogame
players are not immune to dual-task costs. Attention Percept. Psychophys. 74,
803e809.
Durso, F., Rawson, K., Girotto, S., 2007. Comprehension and situation awareness. In:
Durso, F., Nickerson, R., Dumais, S., Lewandowsky, S., Perfect, T. (Eds.),
Handbook of Applied Cognition, second ed. Wiley, Hoboken, pp. 163e194.
Endsley, M., 2006. Situation awareness. In: Salvendy, G. (Ed.), Handbook of Human
Factors and Ergonomics, third ed. Wiley, New York, pp. 528e542.
Feng, J., Spence, I., Pratt, J., 2007. Playing an action video game reduces gender
difference in spatial cognition. Psychol. Sci. 18, 850e855.
Green, C., Bavelier, D., 2003. Action video game modies visual selective attention.
Nature 423, 534e537.
Green, C., Bavelier, D., 2006. Action video game experience alters the spatial reso-
lution of attention. Psychol. Sci. 18, 88e94.
Green, C., Bavelier, D., 2008. Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plas-
ticity and training-induced learning. Psychol. Aging 23, 692e701.
Green, C., Li, R., Bavelier, D., 2010. Perceptual learning during action video game
playing. Top. Cognit. Sci. 2, 202e216.
Healy, A.F., Schneider, V.I., Bourne Jr., L.E., 2012. Empirically valid principles of
training. In: Healy, A.F., Bourne Jr., L.E. (Eds.), Training Cognition: Optimizing
Efciency, Durability, and Generalizability. Psychology Press, New York.
Herrington, J., Oliver, R., Reeves, T.C., 2003. Patterns of engagement in authentic
online learning environments. Aust. J. Educ. Tech. 19, 59e71.
Hubert-Wallander, B., Green, C., Bavelier, D., 2011. Stretching the limits of visual
attention: the case of action video games. WIREs Cognit. Sci. 2, 222e230.
Jones, D., Endsley, M., 1996. Sources of situation awareness errors in aviation. Aviat.
Space Environ. Med. 67, 507e512.
Karr-Wisniewski, P., Lu, T., 2010. When more is too much: operationalizing tech-
nology overload and exploring its impact on knowledge worker productivity.
Comput. Human Behav. 26, 1061e1072.
Lim, J., Reiser, R.A., Olina, Z., 2009. The effects of part-task and whole-task
instructional approaches on acquisition and transfer of a complex cognitive
skill. Educ. Tech. Res. 51, 61e77.
Liu, D., Wasson, R., Vincenzi, D., 2009. Effects of system automation management
strategies and multi-mission operator-to-vehicle ratio on operator performance
in UAV systems. J. Intell. Robotic. Syst. 54, 795e810.
Maclin, E., Mathewson, K., Low, K., Boot, W., Kramer, A., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G.,
2011. Learning to multitask: effects of video game practice on electrophysio-
logical indices of attention and resource allocation. Psychophysiology 48,
1173 e1183.
McCarley, J.S., Wickens, C.D., 2005. Human Factors Implications of UAVs in the
National Airspace. University of Illinois Institute of Aviation Technical Report
(AHFD-05-5/FAA-05-1), Savoy, IL. Aviation Human Factors Division.
Merrill, M.D., 2002. First principles of instruction. Educ. Tech. Res. 50, 43e59.
Paas, F., Van Gog, T., 2009. Principles for designing effective and efcient training of
complex cognitive skills. In: Durso, F. (Ed.), Reviews of Human Factors and
Ergonomics, vol. 5. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica,
pp. 166e194.
Rosser, J.C., Lynch, P.J., Cuddihy, L., Gentile, D.A., Klonsky, J., Merrell, R., 2007. The
impact of video games on training surgeons in the 21
st
century. Arch. Surg. 142,
181 e186.
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T.S., Engle, R.W., 2010. Does working memory training
generalize? Psychol. Belg. 50, 245e276.
Singh, A., Tiwari, T., Singh, I., 2010. Performance feedback, mental workload and
monitoring efciency. J. Indian Acad. Appl. Psychol. 36, 151e158.
Spence, I., Feng, J., 2010. Video games and spatial cognition. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14,
92e104.
Spence, I., Yu, J.J., Feng, J., Marshman, J., 2009. Females match males when learning
a spatial skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. 35, 1097e1103.
Stanton, N., Salmon, P., Walker, G., Jenkins, D., 2010. Is situation awareness all in the
mind? Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 11, 29e40.
Van Merriënboer, J., Kirschner, P., 2007. Ten Steps to Complex Learning. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah.
West, G., Stevens, S., Pun, C., Pratt, J., 2008. Visuospatial experience modulates
attentional capture: evidence from action video game players. J. Vis. 8, 1e9.
Wu, S., Cheng, C., Feng, J., DAngelo, L., Alain, C., Spence, I., 2012. Playing a rst-
person shooter video game induces neuroplastic change. J. Cognit. Neurosci.
24, 1286e1293.
D. Chiappe et al. / Applied Ergonomics 44 (2013) 278e284284
... Providing cognitive tasks (excluding RESMAN subtask) as a measuring index of performance and predictor of flight performance in simulator 16 [19] Test the accuracy of cross-subject workload classifier based on hierarchical Bayes model Providing three levels of task difficulty (all subtasks) to be measured by physiological methods (EEG) and then sampled for classifier model training 17 [20] Examine the effects of action videogames training in improving multitasking performance in high workload environments ...
... The rest of the enlisted studies (around 74%) were conducted in non-aviation, more general context. Those studies took benefits from MATB ability to generate multitasking environment as a basis to assess drug effect [15], workload/task demands/task complexity [8], [9], [14], [21], [26], [30], multitasking strategies [20], [24], [25], learning improvement [22], and cognitive performance [12], [16]. Another context that is possibly one of the prominent topics in this context is modelling [6], [10], [10], [19], [24], [29], [32]- [34]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Multi-Attribute Task Battery (MATB) is a software that has been arguably utilized in many ergonomics/human factors studies, including in the topic of mental workload. However, the use of this well-known program in diverse investigations has not yet been systematically tracked. Furthermore, it may be argued that a critical appraisal of MATB is urgently needed so that future researchers and users can take several crucial factors into account when planning a study or experiment using MATB. The aim of this paper is to comprehensively identify and review the use of MATB software in published studies. This aim might be accomplished by achieving two goals: (1) systematic discovery of published papers in literature databases and (2) categorization of research according to pertinent topics. In this paper, thirty-one articles were included for analysis after carefully screening for their eligibility. Our scoping review finds that MATB is a beneficial program for creating multitasking environments in general, with aviation being the area where it has been used the most. The program was also extensively used for studies on mental workload, especially by producing various stimuli that ultimately result in varying degrees of task demand or difficulty. Moreover, to successfully use MATB, researchers must be aware of a few operational issues and criticisms.
... This test, in the form of a computerized game, requires the simultaneous performance of four unrelated hard skills tasks. Action video games can increase a person's ability to take on additional tasks by increasing attentional capacity, i.e., enhancing performance on secondary tasks without interfering with the primary tasks (Chiappe et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Optimizing selection processes is a multilayer beneficial achievement. Moreover, task-switching is a commonly required ability in a variety of jobs (e.g., programmers). The present study aims to develop a rapid task-switching assessment for the Portuguese-speaking population that integrates verbal content with nonverbal components. One hundred and sixty-four participants answered the task-switching test, and 78 of those were evaluated in terms of their overall job performance in 2020, 2021, and 2022 by their managers. Fifteen items were kept in the final version of the tool, across three tasks. Statistical analyses showed there was positive evidence for the acceptable psychometric properties of the assessment tool and how the task-switching performance of the test correlated to job performance.
... As studies have shown that video game time has an influence on the use of automation (Chen and Barnes, 2012;Chiappe et al., 2013;Cummings et al., 2010;McKinley et al., 2011;Wright et al., 2018), we categorically captured the average video game time per week (1 = not at all to 7 -13 h or more). ...
... AVGs are characterized by unique fast-paced gameplay with multiple events occurring unexpectedly in the central and peripheral visual field . High demands are placed on decision-making, task switching, visuospatial attention, and perceptual processing abilities in these environments, and previous work has shown that individuals who play AVGs display superior visuospatial attention (Feng et al., 2007), short-term memory capacity (Green & Bavelier, 2003), information processing speed (Dye et al., 2009), and task-switching abilities (Chiappe et al., 2013;Kowal et al., 2018) when compared to non-gamer (NG) populations . ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent work demonstrates that those who regularly play action video games (AVGs) consistently outperform non‐gamer (NG) controls on tests of various cognitive abilities. AVGs place high demands on several cognitive functions and are often engaged with for long periods of time (e.g., over 2 h), predisposing players to experiencing cognitive fatigue. The detrimental effects of cognitive fatigue have been widely studied in various contexts where accurate performance is crucial, including aviation, military, and sport. Even though AVG players may be prone to experiencing cognitive fatigue, this topic has received little research attention to date. In this study, we compared the effect of a cognitively fatiguing task on the subsequent cognitive performance of action video game players and NG control participants. Our results indicated AVGs showed superior spatial working memory and complex attention abilities while showing no difference from NGs on simple attention performance. Additionally, we found that our cognitive fatigue and control interventions did not differentially affect the cognitive performance of AVGs and NGs in this study. This pre‐registered study provides evidence that AVGs show superior cognitive abilities in comparison to a non‐gaming population, but do not appear more resilient to cognitive fatigue.
Article
Full-text available
Prior studies investigating the effects of routine action video game play have demonstrated improvements in a variety of cognitive processes, including improvements in attentional tasks. However, there is little evidence indicating that the cognitive benefits of playing action video games generalize from simplified unisensory stimuli to multisensory scenes — a fundamental characteristic of natural, everyday life environments. The present study addressed if video game experience has an impact on crossmodal congruency effects when searching through such multisensory scenes. We compared the performance of action video game players (AVGPs) and non-video game players (NVGPs) on a visual search task for objects embedded in video clips of realistic scenes. We conducted two identical online experiments with gender-balanced samples, for a total of . Overall, the data replicated previous findings reporting search benefits when visual targets were accompanied by semantically congruent auditory events, compared to neutral or incongruent ones. However, according to the results, AVGPs did not consistently outperform NVGPs in the overall search task, nor did they use multisensory cues more efficiently than NVGPs. Exploratory analyses with self-reported gender as a variable revealed a potential difference in response strategy between experienced male and female AVGPs when dealing with crossmodal cues. These findings suggest that the generalization of the advantage of AVG experience to realistic, crossmodal situations should be made with caution and considering gender-related issues.
Article
Full-text available
p style="text-align: justify;">This review is aimed at systematizing the results of empirical and meta-analytical work on the problem of the influence of video games on cognitive processes. Psychology has accumulated a lot of facts indicating that computer games have positive effects, but sometimes it is impossible to fix them. Analysis of the literature has shown that video games have a positive effect on cognitive processes, but the size of this effect depends on the genre of the game. There is a largest effect of puzzle-games and the “arcade” genre on spatial thinking. “Action”, “shooter”, and “arcade” games have small but significant effects on some aspects of perceptual processing, as well as “action” games have a strong positive effect on problem solving speed. The effects on executive functions were small. A review of modern trends in gamification has shown that video games can be effective in teaching specific professional skills, for example, in the field of medicine, and in the treatment of psychological disorders, in particular dementia. The effectiveness of videogames as a part of cognitive learning products has not been confirmed, this area needs to be studied in more details. At the same time, only moderate video gaming is safe for development. Studies show that excessive video game passion causes gaming addiction and negatively affects attention, academic performance and emotional state. An analysis of the literature has shown that video games potentially have a positive effect on cognitive processes, but the size of this effect depends on the genre of the game. There is a largest effects of puzzle-games and the «arcade» genre on spatial thinking. «Action», «shooter», and «arcade» games have small but significant effects on some aspects of perceptual processing, as well as «action» games have a strong positive effect on problem solving speed. The effects on executive functions were small. A review of modern trends in gamification has shown that video games can be effective in teaching specific professional skills, for example, in the field of medicine, and in the treatment of certain psychological disorders, in particular dementia. The effectiveness of videogames as part of cognitive learning products has not been confirmed, this area need more detailed research. At the same time, despite the some positive effects of computer games, only moderate gaming is safe for development, studies show that excessive video game passion causes the gambling addiction and negatively affects attention, academic performance and emotional state.</p
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the effect of success and failure performance feedback on perceived mental workload and monitoring performance in flight simulation task. The revised version of the multi-attribute task battery (MATB) was administered on 20 non-pilot participants. The performances were recorded as hit rates, false alarms and root mean square errors. Mental workload was assessed using NASA-TLX questionnaire. A 2(success-failure feedback) x 2(30-min sessions) x 3(10-min blocks) mixed factorial design with repeated measures on last two factors was used. The obtained results revealed that performance feedback did not have a significant effect on mental workload and malfunction detection. The findings support the notion that monitoring inefficiency (i.e., automation-induced complacency) is a robust phenomenon and it can be observed in multi-task environment with high static automation reliability. The technological revolution has gradually removed the human-operators of many complex systems from front-line levels of control and having their actions relayed via an intervening mass of computers and microprocessors. Instead of active controller of the system, the operator of an automated system has now become a passive observer. It may seem paradoxical, but automated systems can both reduce and increase mental workload. Therefore, mental workload is considered an important factor in the area of automation research. One of the fundamental reasons for introducing automation in complex systems is to reduce workload, and thereby to reduce human error. However, evidence shows that this is not necessarily true in all situations. Infact the automation merely changes how work is accomplished (Woods, 1994). Further, Reinartz and Gruppe (1993) argued that automated system present cognitive demands, which increases workload. The performance of the operator is hindered by the increase in processing load resulting from the additional task of collecting information about the system state. This is further complicated by the extent of the operator's knowledge about the system. In the event of manual takeover, the operator must either disable interlocks to other systems, or else match his/her actions to those of related process functions.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter contains section titled:
Article
Full-text available
Recently, attempts have been made to alter the capacity of working memory (WMC) through extensive practice on adaptive working memory tasks that adjust difficulty in response to user performance. We discuss the design criteria required to claim validity as well as generalizability and how recent studies do or do not satisfy those criteria. It is concluded that, as of yet, the results are inconsistent and this is likely driven by inadequate controls and ineffective measurement of the cognitive abilities of interest. The capacity of working memory varies among individuals. About 50% of the variance in general intelligence between individuals can be explained by differences in working memory capacity. Kids with attention problems often have working memory deficits. Working memory has been linked to academic success. Stroke victims often suffer from impaired working memory. Working memory is plastic. Like a muscle, it can be improved through exercise.
Article
Four sets of cognitive psychology experiments are aimed to identify and empirically support principles of training intended to increase training effectiveness. Each set supports a different training principle: strategic use of knowledge (learning and memory are facilitated whenever preexisting knowledge can be used as a mediator in the process of acquisition), training difficulty (conditions that cause desirable difficulties during learning might facilitate later retention and transfer), mental practice (mental practice can retard forgetting and promote transfer of training, sometimes to a larger extent than can physical practice), and cognitive antidote (adverse effects of prolonged work on routine tasks can be mitigated by adding cognitive complications). The first 2 principles are focused on knowledge acquisition and the last 2 on skill acquisition. Nevertheless, the principles converge on similar prescriptions: Contrary to intuition and common practice, the trainer is recommended to increase the complexity of the training situation rather than use the simplest methods available.
Article
This chapter describes principles for the design of training of complex cognitive tasks that pose relatively great challenges to the cognitive capacity of the trainee. Using the theoretical framework of cognitive load, we argue that the structures that constitute human cognitive architecture need to be taken into account in the design of effective and efficient training. For training complex cognitive tasks, this means that cognitive load needs to be controlled by optimizing the relationship between (a) the intrinsic load imposed by the complexity of the training task and (b) the effective (germane) and ineffective (extraneous) loads imposed by the design of the training tasks. Design principles for training tasks, for sequences of training tasks in fixed training programs (in which all trainees receive the same sequence), and for ways to create adaptive or personalized training programs (in which each trainee receives a personalized sequence) are discussed. Simple to complex sequencing of training tasks, decreasing support, increasing contextual interference, and spacing are discussed as effective principles for fostering learning and transfer in fixed training programs. System-controlled, shared-responsibility, and advisory models for task selection are discussed as possible models to be used to foster learning and transfer in adaptive training programs.
Article
This paper addresses the fundamental discipline theoretic question of whether situation awareness is a phenomenon best described by psychology, engineering or systems ergonomics. Each of these disciplines places a different emphasis on the notion of what situation awareness is and how it manifests itself. The approach from psychology places situation awareness as something that can only exist in the minds of people in a system. This means that the unit of analysis is the individual and that team situation awareness is the summation of individual situation awareness. The engineering perspective puts situation awareness in the world, represented in the artefacts and objects that people use. This means that the unit of analysis is the things that people interact with. Finally, the systems ergonomics perspective places emphasis on the interaction between people and their artefacts in the world, to propose that situation awareness functions like distributed cognition. This means that the unit of analysis is the whole socio-technical system. Each of these perspectives is presented and compared with reference to studies in aviation and other domains. It is concluded that the distributed cognition perspective of situation awareness offers the most comprehensive explanation of the phenomena observed in socio-technical systems. Socio-technical systems theory allows exploration of the social and technical sub-systems independently, which offers a theoretical framework for aligning the three views of situation awareness.
Article
Situation awareness (SA) is the understanding required to operate a complex system in a highly dynamic environment. We evaluate theories of individual SA and the processes by which individuals maintain their understanding of a situation. We support a situated approach, which holds that individual operators make use of limited internal representation, and rely extensively on interactions with external props and tools to achieve and maintain SA. We also propose a synthesis of Durso et al. (2007) [Durso, F., Rawson, K., and Girotto, S., 2007. Comprehension and situation awareness. In: F. Durso, et al., eds. Handbook of applied cognition. 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 163–194.] Construction-Integration model of scene analysis with Sperber and Wilson's (1995) [Sperber, D. and Wilson, D., 1995. Relevance: communication and cognition. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell.] Relevance Theory of comprehension. We show that the combination of the two theories can provide a more complete account of the mechanisms and processes underlying situation assessment, or sense-making, and is consistent with the situated approach to SA.
Chapter
IntroductionDeveloping Situation AwarenessSituation Awareness ChallengesLevels of Situation AwarenessSystem Design to Support Situation AwarenessConclusions References