ArticlePDF Available

A 61-Million-Person Experiment in Social Influence and Political Mobilization

Authors:

Abstract

Human behaviour is thought to spread through face-to-face social networks, but it is difficult to identify social influence effects in observational studies, and it is unknown whether online social networks operate in the same way. Here we report results from a randomized controlled trial of political mobilization messages delivered to 61 million Facebook users during the 2010 US congressional elections. The results show that the messages directly influenced political self-expression, information seeking and real-world voting behaviour of millions of people. Furthermore, the messages not only influenced the users who received them but also the users' friends, and friends of friends. The effect of social transmission on real-world voting was greater than the direct effect of the messages themselves, and nearly all the transmission occurred between 'close friends' who were more likely to have a face-to-face relationship. These results suggest that strong ties are instrumental for spreading both online and real-world behaviour in human social networks.
LETTER doi:10.1038/nature11421
A 61-million-person experiment in social influence
and political mobilization
Robert M. Bond
1
, Christopher J. Fariss
1
, Jason J. Jones
2
,AdamD.I.Kramer
3
, Cameron Marlow
3
, Jaime E. Settle
1
& James H. Fowler
1,4
Human behaviour is thought to spread through face-to-face social
networks, but it is difficult to identify social influence effects in
observational studies
9–13
, and it is unknown whether online social
networks operate in the same way
14–19
. Here we report results from
a randomized controlled trial of political mobilization messages
delivered to 61 million Facebook users during the 2010 US con-
gressional elections. The results show that the messages directly
influenced political self-expression, information seeking and real-
world voting behaviour of millions of people. Furthermore, the
messages not only influenced the users who received them but also
the users’ friends, and friends of friends. The effect of social trans-
mission on real-world voting was greater than the direct effect of
the messages themselves, and nearly all the transmission occurred
between ‘close friends’ who were more likely to have a face-to-face
relationship. These results suggest that strong ties are instrumental
for spreading both online and real-world behaviour in human
social networks.
Recent experimental studies
6,14–16
have attempted to measure the
causal effect of social influence online. At the same time, there is
increasing interest in the ability to use online social networks to study
and influence real-world behaviour
17–19
. However, online social networks
are also made up of many ‘weak-tie’ relationships
20
that may not
facilitate social influence
21
, and some studies suggest that online
communication may not be an effective medium for influence
22
.An
open question is whether online networks, which harness social
information from face-to-face networks, can be used effectively to
increase the likelihood of behaviour change and social contagion.
One behaviour that has been proposed to spread through networks
is the act of voting in national elections. Voter turnout is significantly
correlated among friends, family members and co-workers in obser-
vational studies
23,24
. Voter mobilization efforts are effective at increas-
ing turnout
25
, particularly those conducted face-to-face and those that
appeal to social pressure
26
and social identity
27
. There is also evidence
from one face-to-face field experiment that voting is ‘contagious’, in
the sense that mobilization can spread from person to person within
two-person households
28
. Although anecdotal accounts suggest that
online mobilization has made a big difference in recent elections
21
,a
meta-analysis of email experiments suggests that online appeals to vote
are ineffective
24
.
Voter mobilization experiments
26–28
have shown that most methods
of contacting potential voters have small effects (if any) on turnout
rates, ranging from 1% to 10%. However, the ability to reach large
populations online means that even small effects could yield behaviour
changes for millions of people. Furthermore, as many elections are
competitive, these changes could affect electoral outcomes. For
example, in the 2000 US presidential election, George Bush beat Al
Gore in Florida by 537 votes (less than 0.01% of votes cast in Florida).
Had Gore won Florida, he would have won the election.
To test the hypothesis that political behaviour can spread through
an online social network, we conducted a randomized controlled trial
with all users of at least 18 years of age in the United States who
accessed the Facebook website on 2 November 2010, the day of the
US congressional elections. Users were randomly assigned to a ‘social
message’ group, an ‘informational message’ group or a control group.
The social message group (n560,055,176) was shown a statement at
the top of their ‘News Feed’. This message encouraged the user to vote,
provided a link to find local polling places, showed a clickable button
reading ‘I Voted’, showed a counter indicating how many other
Facebook users had previously reported voting, and displayed up to
six small randomly selected ‘profile pictures’ of the user’s Facebook
friends who had already clicked the I Voted button (Fig. 1). The
informational message group (n5611,044) was shown the message,
poll information, counter and button, but they were not shown any
faces of friends. The control group (n5613,096) did not receive any
message at the top of their News Feed.
The design of the experiment allowed us to assess the impact that the
treatments had on three user actions; clicking the I Voted button,
clicking the polling-place link and voting in the election. Clicking
the I Voted button is similar to traditional measures of self-reported
voting, but here users reported their vote to their social community
rather than to a researcher. We therefore use this action to measure
political self-expression, as it is likely to be affected by the extent to
which a user desires to be seen as a voter by others. In contrast, social
desirability should not affect other user actions in the same way.
Clicking the polling-place link took users to a separate website that
helped them to find a polling location, and this action was not reported
to the user’s social community. We therefore use this action to measure
a user’s desire to seek information about the election. Finally, we used
a group-level process to study the validated voting behaviour of
6.3 million users matched to publicly available voter records (see
Supplementary Information).
We first analyse direct effects. We cannot compare the treatment
groups with the control group to assess the effect of the treatment on
self-expression and information seeking, because the control group did
not have the option to click an I Voted button or click on a polling-
place link. However, we can compare the proportion of users between
the two treatment groups to estimate the causal effect of seeing the
faces of friends who have identified themselves as voters (Fig. 1). Users
who received the social message were 2.08% (s.e.m., 0.05%; t-test,
P,0.01) more likely to click on the I Voted button than those who
received the informational message (20.04% in the social message
group versus 17.96% in the informational message group). Users
who received the social message were also 0.26% (s.e.m., 0.02%;
P,0.01) more likely to click the polling-place information link than
users who received the informational message (Fig. 1).
Although acts of political self-expression and information seeking
are important in their own right, they do not necessarily guarantee that
a particular user will actually vote. As such, we also measured the effect
that the experimental treatment had on validated voting, through
examination of public voting records. The results show that users
1
Political Science Department, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA.
2
Psychology Department, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA.
3
Data
Science, Facebook, Inc., Menlo Park, California 94025, USA.
4
Medical Genetics Division, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA.
13 SEPTEMBER 2012 | VOL 489 | NATURE | 295
Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved
©2012
who received the social message were 0.39% (s.e.m., 0.17%; t-test,
P50.02) more likely to vote than users who received no message at
all. Similarly, the difference in voting between those who received the
social message and those who received the informational messagewas
0.39% (s.e.m., 0.17%; t-test, P50.02), suggesting that seeing faces of
friends significantly contributed to the overall effect of the message on
real-world voting. In fact, turnout among those who received the
informational message was identical to turnout among those in the
control group (treatment effect 0.00%, s.e.m., 0.28%; P50.98), which
raises doubts about the effectiveness of information-only appeals to
vote in this context.
These results show that online political mobilization can have a
direct effect on political self-expression, information seeking and
real-world voting behaviour, and that messages including cues from
an individual’s social network are more effective than information-
only appeals. But what about indirect effects that spread from person
to person in the social network? Users in our sample had on average
149 Facebook friends, with whom they share social information,
although many of these relationships constitute ‘weak ties’. Past
research indicates that close friends have a stronger behavioural effect
on each other than do acquaintancesor strangers
9,11,13,21
. We therefore
expected mobilization to spread more effectively online through
‘strong ties’.
To distinguish users who are likely to have close relationships, we
used the degree to which Facebook friends interacted with each other on
the site (see Supplementary Information for more detail). Higher levels
of interaction indicate that friends are more likely to be physically
proximate and suggest a higher level of commitment to the friendship,
more positive affect between the friends, and a desire for the friendship
to be socially recognized
29
. We counted the number of interactions
between each pair of friends and categorized them by decile, ranking
them from the lowest to highest percentage of interactions. A validation
study (see SupplementaryInformation)shows that friends in the highest
decile are those most likely to be close friends in real life (Fig. 2a).
We then used these categories to estimate the effect of the mobil-
ization message on a user’s friends. Random assignment means that
any relationship between the message a user receives and a friend’s
behaviour is not due to shared attributes, as these attributes are not
correlated with the treatment (see Supplementary Information). To
measure a per-friend treatment effect, we compared behaviour in the
friends connected to a user who received the social message to beha-
viour in the friends connected to a user in the control group. To
account for dependencies in the network, we simulate the null distri-
bution using a network permutation method (see the Supplementary
Information). Monte Carlo simulations suggest that this method
minimizes the risk of false positives and recovers true causal effects
without bias (see Supplementary Information).
Figure 2 shows that the observed per-friend treatment effects increase
as tie-strength increases. All of the observed treatment effectsfall outside
the null distribution for expressed vote (Fig. 2b), suggesting that they are
significantly different from chance outcomes. For validated vote
(Fig. 2c), the observed treatment effect is near zero for weak ties, but
it spikes upwards and falls outside the null distribution for the top two
deciles. This suggests that strong ties are important for the spread of
real-world voting behaviour. Finally, the treatment effect for polling
place search gradually increases (Fig. 2d), with several of the effects
falling outside the 95% confidence interval of the null distribution.
To simplify the analysis and reporting of results, we arbitrarily
define ‘close friends’ as people who were in the eightieth percentile
or higher (decile 9) of frequency of interaction among all friendships in
the sample (see the Supplementary Information). ‘Friends’ are all other
Facebook friends who had less interaction. A total of 60,491,898 (98%)
users in our sample had at least 1 close friend, with the average user
having about 10 close friends (compared with an average of 139 friends
who were not close).
The results suggest that users were about 0.011% (95% confidence
interval (CI) of null distribution 20.009% to 0.010%) more likely to
engage in an act of political self-expression by clicking on the I Voted
button than they would have been had their friend seen no message.
Similarly, for each close friend who received the social message, an
individual was on average 0.099% (null 95% CI –0.042% to 0.048%)
more likely to express voting.
We also found an effect in the validated vote sample. For each close
friend who received thesocial message, a user was 0.224% (null 95% CI
–0.181% to 0.174%) more likely to vote than they would have been had
their close friend received no message. Similarly, for information-
seeking behaviour we found that for each close friend who received
the social message, a user was 0.012% (null 95% CI –0.012% to 0.012%)
more likely to click the link to find their polling place than they would
have been had their close friends received no message. In both cases
there was no evidence that other friends had an effect (see
Supplementary Information). Thus, ordinary Facebook friends may
affect online expressive behaviour, but they do not seem to affect
private or real-world political behaviours. In contrast, close friends
seem to have influenced all three.
The magnitude of these contagion effects are small per friend, but it
is important to remember that they resultfrom a single message, and in
many cases it was not possible to change the target’s behaviour. For
example, users may have already voted by absentee ballot before
Election Day, or they may have logged in to Facebook too late to vote
or to influence other users’ voting behaviour. In other words, all effects
measured here are intent-to-treat effects rather than treatment-on-
treated effects, which would be greater if we had better information
about who was eligible to receive the treatment.
ab
Informational message
Social message
friends have voted.
Today is Election Day What’s this?
People on Facebook Voted
Find your polling place on the U.S.
Politics Page and click the "I Voted"
button to tell your friends you voted.
close
VOTE l Voted
10 155376
Today is Election Day What’s this?
People on Facebook Voted
Find your polling place on the U.S.
Politics Page and click the "I Voted"
button to tell your friends you voted.
close
VOTE l Voted
10 155376
0
0.3
0.6
0.9
1.2
1.5
1.8
2.1
Direct effect of treatment
on own behaviour (%)
Self-
reported
voting
Search for
polling
place
Validated
voting
Validated
voting
Social
message
versus
control
Social
message
versus
informational
message
Jaime Settle, Jason Jones, and 18 other
Figure 1
|
The experiment and direct effects. a,b, Examples of the informational message and social message Facebook treatments (a) and their direct effect on
voting behaviour (b). Vertical lines indicate s.e.m. (they are too small to be seen for the first two bars).
RESEARCH LETTER
296 | NATURE | VOL 489 | 13 SEPTEMBER 2012
Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved
©2012
Moreover, the scale of the number of users, their friendship
connections and the potential voters in a given election is very large.
We estimated the per-user effect (the per-friend effect multiplied by
the average number of friends per user) and the total effect (the
per-user effect multiplied by the total number of users) on the
behaviour of everyone in the sample (see Supplementary Informa-
tion). The results suggest that friends generated an additional
886,000 expressed votes (11.4%, null 95% CI 21.1% to 1.1%),
and close friends generated a further 559,000 votes (10.9%, null
95% CI –0.3% to 0.3%). In the Supplementary Information we also
show that close friends of close friends (2 degrees of separation)
generated an additional 1 million expressed votes (11.7%, null 95%
CI –0.8% to 0.9%). Thus, the treatment clearly had a significant impact
on political self-expression and how it spread through the network,
and even weak ties seem to be relevant to its spread.
However, the effect of the social message on real-world validated
vote behaviour and polling-place search was more focused. The results
suggest that close friends generated an additional 282,000 validated
votes (11.8%, null 95% CI –1.3% to 1.2%) and an additional 74,000
polling-place searches (10.1%, null 95% CI –0.1% to 0.1%), but there
is no evidence that ordinary friends had any effect on either of these
two behaviours. In other words, close friendships accounted for all of
the significant contagion of these behaviours, in spite of the fact that
they make up only 7% of all friendships on Facebook.
To put these results in context, it is important to note that turnout
has been steadily increasing in recent US midterm elections, from
36.3% of the voting age population in 2002 to 37.2% in 2006, and to
37.8% in 2010. Our results suggest that the Facebook social message
increased turnout directly by about 60,000 voters and indirectly
through social contagion by another 280,000 voters, for a total of
340,000 additional votes. That represents about 0.14% of the voting
age population of about 236 million in 2010. However, this estimate
does not include the effect of the treatment on Facebook users who
were registered to vote but who we could not match because of
nicknames, typographical errors, and so on. It would be complex to
estimate the number of users on Facebook who are in the voter record
but unmatchable, and it is not clear whether treatment effects would be
of the same magnitude for these individuals, so we restrict our estimate
to the matched group that we were able to sample and observe. This
means it is possible that more of the 0.60% growth in turnout between
2006 and 2010 might have been caused by a single message on
Facebook.
The results of this study have many implications. First and foremost,
online political mobilization works. It induces political self-expression,
but it also induces information gathering and real, validated voter
turnout. Although previous research suggested that online messages
do not work
19
, it is possible that conventional sample sizes may not
be large enough to detect the modest effect sizes shown here. We
also show that social mobilization in online networks is significantly
more effective than informational mobilization alone. Showing
familiar faces to users can dramatically improve the effectiveness of
a mobilization message.
Beyond the direct effects of online mobilization, we show the
importance of social influence for effecting behaviour change. Our
0%
2%
4%
6%
8%
10%
Probability of being the closest friend
Decile of user–friend interactions
Decile of user–friend interactions Decile of user–friend interactions
Decile of user–friend interactions
24681013579
12345678910
–0.300
–0.200
–0.100
0
0.100
0.200
0.300
Increase in probability of vote (%)
Observed value
Simulated null
95% CI
12345678910
–0.100
–0.075
–0.050
–0.025
0
0.025
0.050
0.075
0.100
Increase in probability of expressed vote (%)
Observed value
Simulated null
95% CI
12345678910
–0.020
–0.010
0
0.010
0.020
Increase in probability of
polling-place search (%)
Observed value
Simulated null
95% CI
a b
c d
Figure 2
|
The effect of mobilization treatment that a friend received on a
user’s behaviour. ad, A validation study shows that at increasing levels of
interaction, Facebook friends are more likely to have a close real-world
relationship (a; see also the Supplementary Information). As the interaction
increases, so does the observed per-friend effect of friend’streatment on a user’s
expressed voting (b), validated voting (c) and polling-place search (d). Blue
diamonds indicatethe observed treatment effect. Horizontalgrey bars show the
null distribution derived from simulations of identical networks in which the
topology and incidence of the behaviour and treatment are the same but the
assignments of treatment are randomly reassigned.
LETTER RESEARCH
13 SEPTEMBER 2012 | VOL 489 | NATURE | 297
Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved
©2012
validation study shows that close friends exerted about four times
more influence on the total numberof validated voters mobilized than
the message itself. These results are similar to those from a prior
network simulation study based on observational data that suggested
each act of voting on average generates an additional three votes as this
behaviour spreads through the network
30
. Thus, efforts to influence
behaviour should pay close attention not only to the effect a message
will have on those who receive it but also to the likelihood that the
message and the behaviour it spurs will spread from person to person
through the social network. And, in contrast to the results for close
friends, we find that Facebook friends have less effect. Online
mobilization works because it primarily spreads through strong-tie
networks that probably exist offline but have an online representa-
tion. In fact, it is plausible that unobserved face-to-face interactions
account for at least some of the social influencethat we observed in this
experiment.
More broadly, the results suggest that online messages might
influence a variety of offline behaviours, and this has implications
for our understanding of the role of online social media in society.
Experiments are expensive and have limited external validity, but the
growing availability of cheap and large-scale online social network
data
17
means that these experiments can be easily conducted in the
field. If we want to truly understand—and improve—our society,
wellbeing and the world around us, it will be important to use these
methods to identify which real world behaviours are amenable to
online interventions.
Received 17 April; accepted 18 July 2012.
1. Berger, J. & Le Mens, G. How adoption speed affects the abandonment of cultural
tastes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 8146–8150 (2009).
2. Borgatti, S. P. et al. Network analysis in the social sciences. Science 323, 892–895
(2009).
3. Ohtsuki, H., Hauert, C., Lieberman, E. & Nowak, M. A. A simple rule for the
evolution of cooperation on graphs and social networks. Nature 441, 502–505
(2006).
4. Onnela, J. P. & Reed-Tsochas, F. Spontaneous emergence of social influence in
online systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 18375–18380 (2010).
5. Apicella, C. L., Marlowe, F. W., Fowler, J. H. & Christakis, N. A. Social networks and
cooperation in hunter-gatherers. Nature 481, 497–501 (2012).
6. Aral, S. & Walker, D. Identifying influential and susceptible members of social
networks. Science 337, 337–341 (2012).
7. Centola,D. An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of healthbehavior.
Science 334, 1269–1272 (2011).
8. Gonzalez-Bailon, S., Borge-Holthofer, J., Rivero, A. & Moreno, Y. The dynamics of
protest recruitment through an online network. Sci. Rep. 1, 197 (2011).
9. Christakis, N. A.& Fowler, J. H. Social contagion theory: examining dynamic social
networks and human behavior. Preprint at http://arXiv.org/abs/1109.5235v2
(2011).
10. Aral, S., Muchnik, L. & Sundararajan, A. Distinguishing influence-based contagion
from homophily-driven diffusion in dynamic networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
106, 21544–21549 (2009).
11. Christakis, N. A. & Fowler, J. H. Thespread of obesity in a large social network over
32 years. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 370–379 (2007).
12. Fowler, J. H. & Christakis, N. A. The dynamic spread of happiness in a large social
network. Br. Med. J. 337, a2338 (2008).
13. Christakis, N. A. & Fowler, J. H. The collectivedynamics of smoking in a large social
network. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 2249–2258 (2008).
14. Salganik, M. J., Dodds, P. S. & Watts, D. J. Experimental study of inequality and
unpredictability in an artificial cultural market. Science 311, 854–856 (2006).
15. Centola, D. The spreadof behavior in an online social networkexperiment. Science
329, 1194–1197 (2010).
16. Fowler, J. H. & Christakis, N. A. Cooperative behavior cascades in human social
networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 5334–5338 (2010).
17. Lazer, D. et al. Computational social science. Science 323, 721–723 (2009).
18. Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C. & Lampe, C. The benefits of facebook ‘‘friends:’’ Social
capital and college students’ use ofonline social network sites. J. Comput. Mediat.
Commun. 12, 1143–1168 (2007).
19. Traud, A. L., Kelsic, E. D., Mucha, P. J. & Porter, M. A. Comparing community
structure to characteristics in online collegiate social networks. SIAM Rev. 53,
526–543 (2011).
20. Granovetter, M. The strength of weak ties. Am. J. Sociol. 78, 1360–1380 (1973).
21. Christakis, N. A. & Fowler, J. H. Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social
Networks and How They Shape Our Lives (Little, Brown, and Company, 2009).
22. Nickerson, D. W. Does email boost turnout? Q. J. Polit. Sci. 2, 369–379 (2007).
23. Kenny, C. B. Political participation andeffects fromthe social environment. Am. J.
Pol. Sci. 36, 259–267 (1992).
24. Huckfeldt, R. & Sprague,J. Citizens, Politics, and Social Communication: Information
and Influence in an Election Campaign (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1995).
25. Gerber, A. S. & Green, D. P. Does canvassing increase voter turnout? A field
experiment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 10939–10942 (1999).
26. Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P. & Larimer, C. W. Social pressure and voter turnout:
evidence from a large-scale field experiment. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 102, 33–48
(2008).
27. Bryan, C. J., Walton, G. M., Rogers, T. & Dwecka, C. S. Motivating voter turnout by
invoking the self. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 12653–12656 (2011).
28. Nickerson, D. W. Is voting contagious? Evidence from two field experiments. Am.
Polit. Sci. Rev. 102, 49–57 (2008).
29. Vitak, J. et al. It’s complicated: Facebook users’ political participation in the 2008
election. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 107–114 (2011).
30. Fowler, J. H. in The Social Logic of Politics: Personal Networks as Contexts for Political
Behavior (ed. Zuckerman, A.) 269–287 (Temple Univ. Press, 2005).
Supplementary Information is available in the online version of the paper.
Acknowledgements We are grateful to S. Aral, J. Berger, M. Cebrian, D. Centola,
N. Christakis, C. Dawes, L. Gee, D. Green, C. Kam, P. Loewen, P. Mucha, J. P. Onnela,
M. Porter, O. Smirnov and C. Volden for comments on early drafts. This work was
supported in part by the James S. McDonnell Foundation, and the University of Notre
Dame and the John Templeton Foundation as part of the Science of Generosity
Initiative.
Author Contributions All authors contributed to study design, data collection, analysis
and preparation of the manuscript. J.H.F. secured funding.
Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at
www.nature.com/reprints. The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Readers are welcome to comment on theonline version of the paper. Correspondence
and requests for materials should be addressed to J.H.F. (jhfowler@ucsd.edu).
RESEARCH LETTER
298 | NATURE | VOL 489 | 13 SEPTEMBER 2012
Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved
©2012
... Thus, the consideration of social dynamics is highly important to explore the interrelation between climate policy and public support and design politically feasible policies. Similar to Lipari et al. (2024), we represent social norms by households' reference groups (Bicchieri, 2005) and include them as determinant of individual policy opinion (Cole et al., 2022;Bond et al., 2012). There is ample evidence that social networks are highly stratified by socioeconomic class. ...
... There is ample evidence that social networks are highly stratified by socioeconomic class. The existence of reference groups gives rise to homophily in social networks which describes the tendency of individuals to interact more closely with others who share similar attributes, resulting in stronger mutual influence among similar individuals than with those who differ significantly in their beliefs and backgrounds (McPherson et al., 2001;Boguná et al., 2004;Currarini et al., 2009). ...
... To analyze the perception of actors and optimization of Influence on social networks, it was developed and tested to the semantic model for the influence maximization analysis. The propagation of information In a social network has sparked intense research (Settle, Fowler, 2012;Anshelevich et al. 2013;Wang et al. 2016;Yang, et al. 2019a, b). ...
Article
Full-text available
The paper deals with the analysis of the communicative behavior of various types of actors, speech perception and optimization of influence based on social media data and is an extended version of the report presented at CSoNet 2020 and published based on the deliverables of the conference. The paper proposes an improved methodology that is tested on the new material of conflicts regarding urban planning. The research was conducted on the material of social media concerning the construction of the South-East Chord in Moscow (Russia). The study involved a cross-disciplinary approach using neural network technologies, complex networks analysis. The dataset included social networks, microblogs, forums, blogs, videos, reviews. This paper presents the semantic model for the influence maximization analysis in social networks using neural network technologies, also proposed a variant of analyzing the situation with individual and collective actors, multiple opinion leaders, with a dynamic transformation of the hierarchy and ratings according to various parameters.
... Multiplex networks provide a more realistic representation of social systems, capturing the complexity of how individuals interact across various contexts such as family, work, and social circles. The advent of online social networks has witnessed an unparalleled surge in their adoption, profoundly transforming the landscape of interpersonal communication and prompting extensive research into their impacts on offline user behavior [30], such as green behavior [31], voting [32], health behavior [33], and epidemic spreading [34]. Therefore, many studies focused on the coupled diffusion of information on online social networks and green behavior in the physical contact layer. ...
Article
Full-text available
How to comprehend the relationship between information spreading and individual behavior adoption is an essential problem in complex networks. To this end, a novel two-layer model to depict the diffusion of green behavior under the impact of positive and negative information is proposed. Positive information motivates people to adopt green behavior, while negative information reduces the adoption of green behavior. In the model, the physical contact layer describes the green behavior diffusion, and the information layer describes the positive and negative information spreading. Moreover, the social interactions of individuals in two layers change with time and are illustrated by an activity-driven model. Then, we develop the probability transition equations and derive the green behavior threshold. Next, experiments are carried out to confirm the preciseness and theoretical predictions of the new model. It reveals that the prevalence of green behavior can be promoted by restraining the negative information transmission rate and recovery rate of the green nodes while facilitating the positive information transmission rate and green behavior transmission rate. Additionally, reducing the positive information recovery rate and the recovery rate of the green nodes, and increasing the rates of forgetting negative information are beneficial for encouraging the outbreak of green behavior. Furthermore, in the physical contact layer, higher contact capacity and greater activity heterogeneity significantly facilitate green behavior spreading. In the information layer, smaller contact capacity and weaker activity heterogeneity promote diffusion when negative information dominates, whereas larger contact capacity and stronger activity heterogeneity are beneficial when positive information prevails.
... In addition, the advent of the social media era has led to a "bottom-up" model of political communication in American political culture. During the 2010 United States congressional elections, a randomized controlled trial of 61 million Facebook users showed that the online mobilization through social media was effective in promoting public political self-expression, information gathering, and voting behavior [3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The swift advancement of social media technologies has profoundly reshaped the political and economic landscape of the United States, with platforms such as Twitter and Facebook exerting substantial influence. Social media have not only transformed traditional modes of political communication, but have also restructured the structure of the economy through the monetization of advertising revenue and the commercialization of user data. However, this transformation has also introduced new challenges, such as the proliferation of disinformation and political polarization, both of which undermine the quality of political decision-making and negatively impact the stability of the economy. In this context, this paper examines the transformative role of social media in reshaping the dynamics of U.S. politics and economics, focusing on the shift from traditional one-way communication to multidirectional interaction. Through historical analysis and case studies, this study examines how social media affects political decision-making, election outcomes, and the structure of the economy. The results indicate that social media have democratized the dissemination of information, but they have also intensified challenges such as political polarization and the spread of disinformation, which undermine governance and economic stability. Furthermore, social media platforms have emerged as powerful economic entities, reshaping traditional business models via data commercialization and targeted advertising.
... Studies have demonstrated that prior ratings can influence online product ratings and reviews, resulting in herding effects [2]. Furthermore, online social networks have been found to exert significant influence on users' attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors [7]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Online ratings and reviews dominate consumer decision-making in the digital era. This research examines the social effect of online reviews and the individual and environmental elements that increase or decrease them. This study poses critical questions: How does social influence affect the credibility and conformity of online ratings and reviews? What roles do individual personality traits and product type play in moderating this influence? By conducting a comprehensive literature review, the study identifies critical factors and trends in this domain and elucidates the mechanisms of social influence in online environments and its varied impacts across different contexts. The literature review reveals that social influence greatly influences online evaluations, with less experienced reviewers and subjective categories more conformist. Experienced reviewers are more independent. According to the report, businesses must actively manage their online reputations, and platforms must incorporate tools to counteract herding effects and create a more credible review environment. These findings help explain digital consumer behavior and online social impact.
... In multiple domains of political science researchers have found experimental evidence of peer effects. Peer effects have been identified in voter turnout (Nickerson 2008;Bond et al. 2012), (fraudulent) voter registration (Ichino and Schündeln 2012), government employee performance (Keiser and Miller 2019), and the fulfillment of public records requests by government officials (ben Aaron et al. 2017). Recent research has documented peer effects among legislators. ...
Article
Full-text available
We report on a field experiment that has been designed to provide evidence of causal effects underlying the micro-foundations of public policy diffusion across the U.S. states. The aim of our study is to test how and if cross-state legislator level connections serve as a vector through which support for policies diffuses. We measure a novel cross-state legislative network dataset in which two legislators are connected through co-signing environmental policy statements organized by the National Caucus of Environmental Legislators. We survey legislators’ support for policies proposed in other states, and randomize the degree of information included in the policy description regarding support by other legislators in the network. We contribute to our understanding of state legislative politics, policy networks, and interest group politics. We focus on environmental policy due to the inherently nationalized consequences of state and local policy innovations.
... Therefore, we anticipate that the digital script of many negative messages will echo traditional, harmful stereotypes. Given that forms of social media are increasingly influential in society and play a notable part in shaping politics [8][9][10][11], the dissemination of stereotypes in these venues represents an essential topic of study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cyberbullying entails multiple, problematic consequences for its victims. However, little is known about the factors that influence the dispersion of these damaging messages. Drawing on theories of social interaction, we argue that perpetrators disseminate messages containing sexist and racist slurs that reinforce stereotypical, social norms to enhance their online visibility. We collected cross-sectional and longitudinal samples of tweets (N = 539,558 and 248,497, respectively) that included either gender or Asian slurs. We find that tweets containing gender or Asian slurs that were more negative in sentiment had a significantly higher number of retweets than more positive tweets, thereby heightening a user’s online presence and the reach of their content. Two historical events unfolded during our study—the onset of COVID-19 and the murder of George Floyd by a police officer. Tweet negativity increased following both events. Our findings demonstrate the capacity of aggressive tweets to generate wide-ranging networks, a process which is exacerbated further by public crises. Results suggest that the practice of sending such messages is strategic. Perpetrators likely engage in cyberbullying, consciously or not, to increase their online attention. Moreover, this strategy appears to be successful.
Chapter
This indispensable collection provides extensive, yet accessible, coverage of conceptual and practical issues in research design in personality and social psychology. Using numerous examples and clear guidelines, especially for conducting complex statistical analysis, leading experts address specific methods and areas of research to capture a definitive overview of contemporary practice. Updated and expanded, this third edition engages with the most important methodological innovations over the past decade, offering a timely perspective on research practice in the field. To reflect such rapid advances, this volume includes commentary on particularly timely areas of development such as social neuroscience, mobile sensing methods, and innovative statistical applications. Seasoned and early-career researchers alike will find a range of tools, methods, and practices that will help improve their research and develop new conceptual and methodological possibilities. Supplementary online materials are available on Cambridge Core.
Article
Full-text available
Members of the same household share similar voting behaviors on average, but how much of this correlation can be attributed to the behavior of the other person in the household? Disentangling and isolating the unique effects of peer behavior, selection processes, and congruent interests is a challenge for all studies of interpersonal influence. This study proposes and utilizes a carefully designed placebo-controlled experimental protocol to overcome this identification problem. During a face-to-face canvassing experiment targeting households with two registered voters, residents who answered the door were exposed to either a Get Out the Vote message (treatment) or a recycling pitch (placebo). The turnout of the person in the household not answering the door allows for contagion to be measured. Both experiments find that 60% of the propensity to vote is passed onto the other member of the household. This finding suggests a mechanism by which civic participation norms are adopted and couples grow more similar over time.
Article
The prevalence of obesity has increased substantially over the past 30 years. We performed a quantitative analysis of the nature and extent of the person-to-person spread of obesity as a possible factor contributing to the obesity epidemic. We evaluated a densely interconnected social network of 12,067 people assessed repeatedly from 1971 to 2003 as part of the Framingham Heart Study. The body-mass index was available for all subjects. We used longitudinal statistical models to examine whether weight gain in one person was associated with weight gain in his or her friends, siblings, spouse, and neighbors. Discernible clusters of obese persons (body-mass index [the weight in kilograms divided by the square of the height in meters], > or =30) were present in the network at all time points, and the clusters extended to three degrees of separation. These clusters did not appear to be solely attributable to the selective formation of social ties among obese persons. A person's chances of becoming obese increased by 57% (95% confidence interval [CI], 6 to 123) if he or she had a friend who became obese in a given interval. Among pairs of adult siblings, if one sibling became obese, the chance that the other would become obese increased by 40% (95% CI, 21 to 60). If one spouse became obese, the likelihood that the other spouse would become obese increased by 37% (95% CI, 7 to 73). These effects were not seen among neighbors in the immediate geographic location. Persons of the same sex had relatively greater influence on each other than those of the opposite sex. The spread of smoking cessation did not account for the spread of obesity in the network. Network phenomena appear to be relevant to the biologic and behavioral trait of obesity, and obesity appears to spread through social ties. These findings have implications for clinical and public health interventions.
Article
This paper examines the distinction made by Huckfeldt (1979, 1986) and Giles and Dantico (1982) between individually and socially based forms of participation as affected by social environment. Using survey responses from the 1984 South Bend study, the relationship between political discussion partners is explicitly modeled and estimated for several forms of individually and socially based participatory acts. The evidence indicates that certain types of both individually based and socially based participation are affected by those in the immediate social environment, suggesting that a modification of this distinction is in order.
Article
Political campaigns are just now learning how to put the Internet to best use. Low transaction costs and huge economies of scale tempt campaigns to move traditional activities online, but the effectiveness of virtual campaigns is unknown. This paper conducts 13 field experiments on 232,716 subjects to test whether email campaigns are effective for voter registration and mobilization. Both registration and turnout were unaffected, suggesting that email, while inexpensive, is not cost-effective. Learning to use television as a campaign medium took politicians years, and candidates are now beginning to figure out how to use the Internet. An intuitive place to begin is by using the Internet to accomplish work previously done with older technology such as mail, phones, or face-to-face canvassing. The Internet's low transaction costs and massive economies of scale could alter the strategies parties employ in every facet of campaigning. The same economics that push businesses to move online are also present in the political realm. Unfortunately, studies of Internet usage during campaigns generally report on the content of websites (Farmer and Fender 2003, Norris 2003, Ward and Gibson 2003, Farmer and Fender 2005, Xenos and Foot 2005) and do not measure how voters respond to the online campaign activities.1 This paper evaluates the effectiveness of email as a voter mobilization tool by conduct- ing 13 field experiments. Direct mail has been shown to be an effective, albeit expensive, means of increasing voter turnout (Gerber et al. 2003). Examining all known randomized
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Democratic politics is a collective enterprise, not simply because individual votes are counted to determine winners, but more fundamentally because the individual exercise of citizenship is an interdependent undertaking. Citizens argue with one another, they inform one another, and they generally arrive at political decisions through processes of social interaction and deliberation. This book is dedicated to investigating the political implications of interdependent citizens within the context of the 1984 presidential election campaign as it was experienced in the metropolitan area of South Bend, Indiana. Hence, this is a community study in the fullest sense of the term. National politics is experienced locally through a series of filters unique to a particular setting. And this study is concerned with understanding that setting and its consequences for the exercise of democratic citizenship. Several different themes structure the undertaking: the dynamic implications of social communication among citizens, the importance of communication networks for citizen decision making, the exercise of citizen purpose in locating sources of information, the constraints on individual choice that arise as a function of contexts and environments, and the institutional and organizational effects that operate on the flow of information within particular settings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The Social Logic of Politics: Personal Networks as Contexts for Political Behavior. Edited by Alan S. Zuckerman. Philadelphia: Temple University Press, 2005. 368p. 72.50cloth,72.50 cloth, 25.95 paper. The central premise of this edited collection is set out with admirable clarity on the first page of the opening chapter: “It is both obvious and well-known that the immediate social circumstances of people's lives influence what they believe and do about politics. Even so, relatively few political scientists incorporate these principles into their analysis” (p. 3). Alan Zuckerman tackles this problem with a selection of chapters, written by authors with a range of intellectual pedigrees, that set out to show how what is “both obvious and well-known” can be incorporated into rigorous political science. The individual chapters are too numerous and diverse to review in detail here. What is perhaps more useful is to consider the extent to which, taken together, they map out a potentially fruitful line of future development for the discipline.