Access to this full-text is provided by SAGE Publications Inc.
Content available from Evolutionary Psychology
This content is subject to copyright.
Evolutionary Psychology
www.epjournal.net – 2010. 8(3): 405-419
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯
Original Article
Sleeping in Safe Places: An Experimental Investigation of Human Sleeping
Place Preferences from an Evolutionary Perspective
Matthias Spörrle, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, General Psychology II, Leopoldstraße
13, 80802 München, Germany. Email: spoerrle@psy.uni-muenchen.de (corresponding author).
Jennifer Stich, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, General Psychology II, Leopoldstraße 13,
80802 München, Germany.
Abstract: Although humans spend a third of their life asleep, their choice of sleeping
places has so far been little investigated both theoretically and empirically. We address this
issue from the perspective of evolutionary psychology. Our basic assumption is that
humans have an evolved preference for safe sleeping places, that is, those that promise
protection against potential aggressors and nighttime predation. Several testable predictions
were derived from this assumption concerning the preferred location of the bed in a
sleeping room. Specifically, we predicted that people prefer sleeping places that allow them
to view the entrances to the sleeping room (doors and windows) from a distance while
remaining concealed from the entrances themselves. To test these hypotheses, 138
participants were asked to arrange a bed and other pieces of furniture on floor plans that
were experimentally manipulated with respect to the direction in which the door opened
and the presence of a window. In agreement with predictions, participants predominantly
positioned the bed in a way that (a) allowed them to see the door, (b) was as distant as
possible from the door, and (c) was on the side of the room toward which the door opened.
In addition, the positioning of the bed was influenced as predicted by the presence of a
window.
Keywords: Sleep, sleeping place, sleeping site, bedroom, theory of evolution, housing,
predation risk
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯
Introduction
On average, humans spend a third of their lives asleep. Newborns sleep about 16
hours, grown-ups 7-8 hours, and elderly people approximately 6 hours (Zimbardo and
Gerrig, 2004). Thus, the bedroom is one of the rooms in which humans spend most of their
time, and the bed is probably the piece of furniture they use most.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -406-
Although numerous sleep-related phenomena have been empirically investigated,
such as sleep disorders (e.g., sleep apnea, Aloia, Arnedt, Davis, Riggs, and Byrd, 2004),
sleeping phases (Gulyani, Majumdar, and Mallick, 2000) and sleep-related social practices
such as bed-sharing (Ball, 2002), surprisingly few studies have addressed people’s
preferences for the furnishing and layout of bedrooms and beds. Parsons (1972, 1981)
summarized early research on the topic providing several recommendations concerning the
size, lighting, airing and furnishing of bedrooms and the size, height and bedding of beds
(but not their positioning). One study addressed the more specific question of how to
position the bed in relation to the door, using a structured interview technique (Famos,
1989, cited in Schulze and Richter, 2004; see also: Richter, 2004). Flade (1987)
emphasized humans’ general need for secure habitats on a theoretical basis without,
however, formulating concrete predictions concerning furnishing.
The small number of experimental psychological studies on the topic of beds and
bedrooms is not theory-based and has focused on questions such as color preferences and
the comfort of different mattresses (Bader and Engdal, 2000; Christl and Richter, 2004;
Slatter and Whitfield, 1977). Apart from a few further studies of a similar kind, there are no
psychological experiments concerned with the topic of bedrooms and beds. Moreover,
although traditional research in architectural psychology is often based on Gestalt
psychology (cf. Schramm and Richter, 2004) or field theory (cf. Ehmig and Richter, 2004),
these theories have not been applied to the prediction of human sleeping site preferences.
In sum, whereas many other sleep-related phenomena have been extensively
investigated, bedrooms and beds have inspired only very little experimental and theory-
based psychological research, despite their essential meaning. Indeed, as far as we know,
the topic addressed in this article—the preferred position of the bed in a sleeping room—
has never been experimentally investigated before. This is surprising, since the question of
where the bed (i.e., the sleeping place) is located in relation to the entrance to the bedroom
(i.e., the sleeping site) seems to be of fundamental importance for the understanding of
human sleeping behavior. Furthermore, it is only through the systematic study of sleeping
site and place preferences that we can hope to arrive at scientifically founded practical
recommendations concerning the architectural design of rooms and the positioning of beds
within these rooms (e.g., in hotels).
In marked contrast to the scarce empirical research on human sleeping site and
place preferences as well as its atheoretical nature, the corresponding preferences of
mammalians in general (Allison and Cicchetti, 1976), as well as nonhuman primates and
monkeys, have been investigated extensively from the perspective of the theory of
evolution (for a theoretical transfer of research on apes’ nesting behavior to human housing
see: James, 2009). For example, Di Bitetti, Vidal, Baldovino, and Benesovsky (2000)
studied how capuchin monkeys choose their sleeping places and found clear indications
that this choice is mainly motivated by the concern of finding safety from aggressors
(rather than, for example, safety from parasites). A whole body of literature on the choice
of sleeping places in baboons (e.g., Hamilton, 1982) likewise shows that one of the most
important criteria for the choice of a sleeping site is that it is inaccessible to aggressors (for
gibbons see: Reichard, 1998; for langurs and macaques see: Ramakrishnan and Coss,
2001a, 2001b; for tamarins see: Day and Elwood, 1999). Anderson (1998, 2000) concludes
that not only is the choice of sleeping sites in primates strongly influenced by the level of
safety from aggressors that it provides (cf. Isbell, 1994), but also that the primates’
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -407-
behavior at their sleeping places is directed at minimizing the risk of being attacked. In
fact, research indicates that for monkeys sleeping site selections ensuring safety by
reducing predation risk might be even more important than foraging (Smith, Knogge,
Huck, Löttker, Buchanan-Smith, and Heyman, 2007). Taken together, research of
nonhuman primates and other mammalians documents that evolutionary considerations
(more specifically: predation risk) are remarkably well able to explain the choice of
sleeping locations: mammals prefer sleeping places that maximize their chances of survival
during sleep and thereby increase their inclusive fitness.
During recent years, the theory of evolution has become increasingly accepted in
psychology as providing plausible and parsimonious explanations of certain phenomena,
such as sex differences in mate preferences (e.g., Buss, 2004). However, in the field of
architectural psychology, the theory of evolution seems to have been applied only rarely
(e.g., it has been used to explain distorted perceptions of architecture; Rump, 2004) and has
not inspired much empirical research. We aim at connecting architectural psychology more
closely to evolutionary psychology, by studying human sleeping preferences—specifically
the location of the sleeping place (bed) within the sleeping site (bedroom) in relation to
possible entrances—from an evolutionary perspective.
In keeping with the general evolutionary approach and the primate studies reviewed
above, the focus is on the fitness costs and benefits of different sleeping site preferences.
The general hypothesis tested is that humans tend to position their bed within a sleeping
room in a way that maximizes safety from possible aggressors. Even though sleeping
rooms and doors were not part of our environment of evolutionary adaptedness, it seems
reasonable to assume that we perceive them in a similar way as our ancestors might have
perceived, for instance, caves and their entrances (cf. Brain, 1981, p. 271). Further
substantiating the legitimacy of an evolutionary approach to current living environments, it
seems justified to assume that modifications of our environment (such as doors) are
actually based on our evolutionary needs: “The world has been selectively changed to suit
the needs of people. […] In seeking to tailor the world to human needs and desires, people
have altered what the world has to offer” (Michaels and Carello, 1981, p. 54f).
Based on the results of primate and anthropological research (Groves and Sabater
Pi, 1985; Sabater Pi, Veà, and Serrallonga, 1997; Sept, 1992; Worthman and Melby, 2002),
we assume that during the evolutionary development of humans, attacks during sleep came
from nonhuman predators, that is, hunters belonging to other species (Hankerson and
Caine, 2004; Peetz, Norconk, and Kinzey, 1992) as well as from conspecific aggressors (cf.
Hamilton, 1982). Even though in modern urban surroundings attacks by predators no
longer pose a significant threat (but see for rural areas: Packer, Ikanda, Kissui, and Kushnir,
2005; Peterhans and Gnoske, 2001; Treves and Naughton-Treves, 1999) attacks by human
aggressors do remain an issue. Indeed, empirical research suggests that aggression-caused
attacks of humans on other humans occur preferably at night (Cohn and Rotton, 1997), that
is, the time predominantly used for sleeping. In line with this, the most frequently reported
content of children and adolescent nighttime fears is fear of intruders (Gordon, King,
Gullone, Muris, and Ollendick, 2007; Muris, Merckelbach, Ollendick, King, and Bogie,
2001).
Safety from predators can be maximized by choosing a sleeping place that (a)
allows one to detect a potential aggressor as early as possible (cf. Nasar, Julian, Buchman,
Humphreys, and Mrohaly, 1983), (b) allows one to remain hidden from the aggressor as
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -408-
long as possible, and (c) allows for maximum reaction time in the case of an attack (at this
point, we want to note that not only this choice of sleeping places but also characteristics of
sleep itself are assumed to be influenced by predation pressure, see: Lesku, Roth, Amlaner,
and Lima, 2006; Lima, Rattenborg, Lesku, and Amlaner, 2005).
The evolved preference of humans concerning sleeping sites may be regarded as
forming the core of a set of evolved psychological processes. We assume that these
processes solved a specific problem of survival: sleep is a biological necessity for humans.
While asleep, sensory (especially visual) input is reduced and the person is in a state of
diminished consciousness. For these reasons, a sleeping person is easy to attack, a fact of
which aggressors certainly have tried to take advantage of during evolution. Evolved
psychological processes that motivate humans to find a safe sleeping site will foster
survival and will for this reason be selected for.
Similar to the concept of the evolved psychological mechanism (Buss, 2004) we
propose that the information that these processes use to determine the adaptationally best
location of the sleeping place is limited to information relevant for protecting against
possible attacks by aggressors. Other information, such as for example air humidity, is not
considered relevant in this context. This does not mean, of course, that the ultimate
decision for a sleeping place cannot be influenced by nonevolutionary considerations, or
environmental aspects (e.g., temperature, cf. Videan, 2006), as well.
Furthermore, we think that these evolved psychological processes (perhaps by
triggering affective states) result in behavioral output. For example, the feeling of
discomfort caused by an adaptationally inappropriate sleeping place (e.g., with the head
positioned in direction and close to the entrance) may lead to feelings of discomfort causing
the individual to change or modify the sleep setting (e.g., by moving the bed farther away
from the door and placing the body with the feet in direction to the entrance) thus helping
the individual to optimize its sleeping place in order to reduce potential risks e.g., of being
killed by an intruder. From a Gibsonian perspective of affordances (cf. Michaels and
Carello, 1981, p. 41f), the bed and the entrance might be perceived by the individual
through their affordances (i.e., their meaning). Therefore, while the bed is perceived as a
place to sleep, the entrance is perceived as an access possibility for potential aggressors.
We assume that evolved psychological processes enable the individual to meaningfully
relate these interpretations of the environment with each other thus enabling optimizing
modifications of the environment when necessary.
In sum, we postulate a set of evolved psychological processes underlying the choice
of sleeping places in humans, which is accessible to empirical research. In this article, we
focused on but a few, but central predictions that can be derived from this set of processes.
Specifically, using prearranged floor plans, we empirically tested five hypotheses
concerning the positioning of the bed in a sleeping room. Hypotheses 1a and 1b concern the
direction in which the bed is placed in relation to the door and window; hypotheses 2a and
2b concern the distance at which the bed is placed from the door and the window; and
hypothesis 3 concerns the location of the bed in the room in relation to the door.
Hypothesis 1a. If possible, people prefer to place their beds in a room in a way that
allows them to see the door from a resting position without having to move their heads
much, rather than to have the door in their back. Reason: a room surrounded by walls is
primarily entered through the door. To detect an aggressor as soon as possible, it is
therefore best to place the bed in a way that allows one to have a view of the door.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -409-
Hypothesis 1b. If the bedroom has a window, and if it is not possible to position
oneself in a way that allows one to see the door and the window at the same time, the
preference for the location of the bed (hypothesis 1a) is reduced. Reason: if a window is
present, it poses another possibility of entering the sleeping place that needs to be taken
into account; hence two behavioral tendencies that cannot be enacted simultaneously (to the
same degree) are present. However, as the door still remains the main entrance, most
people are expected to stick with the preference for keeping the door in view, although
(depending on interindividual differences in how the threat posed by the door and window
are assessed) some may regard keeping the window in view as more important.
Hypothesis 2a. Other factors constant, people prefer sleeping places farther away
from the entrance to the room. Reason: greater distance allows more time to react to the
entry of an aggressor; therefore, other factors constant, sleeping places that are farther away
from the entrance are safer and should thus be preferred.
Hypothesis 2b. If the sleeping room also has a window in the wall opposite the door,
the distance preference is modified as follows: the bed is placed closer to the door than if
the room has no window. Reason: this placement simultaneously maximizes distance to
both potential entry points of an aggressor.
Hypothesis 3. Other factors constant, people prefer to sleep on that side of the room
to which the door opens. Reason: should an aggressor enter the room, it is of advantage to
be on the side of the room to which the door opens, as this allows one to remain unseen
longer, but to quickly see the door being opened.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Participating were 138 volunteers (50% female) aged from 14 to 73 years, M = 29.6,
SD = 12.1. A total of 61% of the participants were students, 20% were employees, and 19%
had other jobs. More than 80% of participants had a general qualification for university
entrance, at least.
Materials and Design.
Depending on condition, participants were presented with one of four different floor
plans showing a bedroom (Figure 1). The floor plans differed with respect to (a) the
presence versus absence of a window in the wall opposite the door, and (b) the direction in
which the door opened (to the right versus to the left). These two variables were combined
into a 2 (window present vs. absent) × 2 (door opens to the left vs. to the right) factorial
design. Male and female participants were separately assigned at random to the four
experimental conditions (34-36 participants per condition). Participants were tested
individually. At the beginning, a floor plan was placed horizontally on a board. The floor
plan was 12 cm x 22 cm in size and showed a non-furnished room. On an instruction sheet,
the participant was asked to furnish the bedroom in the way he or she liked it best, keeping
in mind that a separate bathroom (not shown on the plan) was available in the apartment.
Figure 1. Example of a floor plan showing an unfurnished bedroom with a window; door
opens to the right.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -410-
Next to the floor plan, four true-to-scale moveable and widely used pictorial
symbols of pieces of furniture were positioned in random order on the board. They
included a bed (12.7 x 5.0 cm), a closet (6.2 cm x 2.2 cm), a table (2.8 cm x 3.6 cm), and a
chair (2.2 cm x 2.4 cm). The meanings of these pictorial symbols were explained on the
instruction sheet. Although our interest was on the positioning of the bed, we added other
pieces of furniture to enhance the ecological validity of the participants’ task (furnishing a
room), and to direct their attention away from the bed. The bed was deliberately made
longer (12.7 cm) than the width of the floor plan (12.0 cm), so that it could only be
positioned at the longitudinal side of the bedroom or diagonally, but not at the narrower
side of the room. This was done to simplify the interpretation of the participants’ responses
by restricting the possible locations of the bed. For similar reasons, the window (when
present) was placed in the wall opposite the door. This position of the window made it
impossible to watch the window and the door at the same time from a resting position
(Figure 2), thus allowing us to separate the direction and strength of the influences of the
door and the window on the positioning of the bed.
Figure 2. Example of a floor plan showing a furnished bedroom with a window; door
opens to the left. Another bed is shown outside the bedroom next to its smaller side to
illustrate graphically that the bed was too large to be placed on this side of the wall.
After the participants had arranged the furniture, they provided some demographic
information. Furnishing the room and completing the questionnaire took about 15 minutes.
After each session, the furniture arrangement of the participant was copied by the
experimenter onto a sheet of paper for further analysis.
Dependent variables.
Dependent variables included the following: (a) the direction of the bed (i.e., head
side towards the door vs. towards the wall opposite the door) chosen by the participant; (b)
the distance between the wall opposite the door and the bed (measured in cm); and (c) the
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -411-
side of the room where the bed was positioned (i.e., the side to which the door opened
versus the opposite side).
Results
Preliminary data analyses did not reveal any reliable differences for sex or
educational level on the dependent variables.
Hypothesis 1a. In each of the four experimental conditions, the majority of the
participants (76-94%) positioned the bed in such a way that it offered a view of the door
from the resting position. Across conditions, 83% preferred this bed position. This is
significantly more than would be expected in the absence of a preference for bed position
(i.e., 50%), binomial test, p < .001, effect size for dichotomous data g = .33. According to
Cohen (1988), this is a large effect. Hence, supporting hypothesis 1a, the preferred bed
position was strongly influenced by the presence of the door.
Hypothesis 1b. In line with hypothesis 1b, fewer participants (79%) selected a bed
position with a view of the door if the room had a window than if it had no window (87%).
Thus the percentage of persons who directed their view away from the door increased if the
room had a window. However, the effect size was small (crosstab data effect size w = 0.10;
cf. Cohen, 1988), and the difference between conditions did not reach significance, χ² (1, N
= 138) = 1.5, p = .11 (one-tailed).
Hypothesis 2a. Within experimental groups 50-83% of the participants (overall:
70%) positioned the bed to the wall opposite the door without leaving space between bed
and wall. Hence, a significant majority (binomial test, p < .001; g = .20) of the participants
chose the maximum possible distance between bed and door.
Theoretically, the maximum possible distance between the bed and the wall
opposite the door was 9.3 cm on the floor map. However, if the bed was positioned at this
distance, it was no longer possible to (fully) open the door since it was blocked by the bed.
The practically feasible maximum distance between bed and wall was therefore only 4.3
cm on the map (5 cm minus the width of the maximally opened door; see Figure 3; note
that choosing the practically rather than the theoretically feasible maximum distance results
in a conservative test of the hypothesis at stake). Assuming that the participants had no
preference for any of the practically feasible distances between bed and wall from 0.00 cm
to 4.30 cm, they should have chosen each distance with equal probability, and the expected
distance is then 2.15 cm. A one-sample t-test comparing this theoretically expected mean
distance between bed and wall in the case of no preference with the observed mean
distance of 0.81 cm (SD = 1.68) was significant, t(137) = 9.33, p < .001, d = 0.79
(according to Cohen, 1988, d ≥ 0.80 indicates a large effect), thus further confirming a
preference for sleeping places farther away from the entrance to the room.
Figure 3. Example of a floor plan without a window showing the bed positioned with
maximum distance (4.3 cm on the plan) from the wall opposite the door.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -412-
In sum, the participants (a) preferred a bed position maximally away from the door
with a probability significantly beyond chance and (b) chose on average a distance
significantly greater than expected on the assumption that all distances were equally
preferred.
Hypothesis 2b. Overall, 70% of the respondents did not leave any space between the
bed and the wall opposite the door, meaning that they placed the bed at the maximally
possible distance to the door. However, if the experimental conditions with and without a
window are considered separately, the respective percentages are different: 81% of the
participants in the condition without a window, but only 59% in the condition with a
window positioned the bed at the maximally possible distance to the door, χ²(1, N = 138) =
8.4, p < .005 (one-tailed), w = 0.25 denoting a medium effect (Cohen, 1988). Thus, in both
conditions, a majority of the participants positioned the bed maximally away from the door
to the opposite wall; however, if a window was present in this wall, a noteworthy
proportion of the participants decided to move the bed away from the wall.
On average, participants left a distance of 0.81 cm between bed and wall. A
Welch’s t-test comparing the chosen distance in the condition with a window (M = 1.2 cm,
SD = 2.0) and without a window (M = 0.4 cm, SD = 1.3) revealed a significant difference,
t(114) = 2.9, p < .005, d = 0.55, indicating a medium effect (Cohen, 1988). Hence, the
chosen distance between bed and wall was markedly influenced by the presence vs.
absence of a window in this wall.
Hypothesis 3. According to hypothesis 3, participants should prefer to sleep on the
side of the room to which the door opens. In the test of this hypothesis, we had to exclude
two participants because their positioning of the bed (diagonally and in the middle of the
room, respectively) did not allow us to unambiguously assign the bed position to one side
of the room. Analyses of the remaining data supported hypothesis 3: A total of 74% of the
participants positioned the bed on the left side of the room if the door opened to the left,
and 64% positioned the bed on the right side of the room if the door opened to the right.
The difference between the two conditions was statistically significant, χ²(1, N = 136) =
20.3, p < .001 (one-tailed), w = 0.39 indicating a medium effect (Cohen, 1988). Hence, the
direction in which the door opens is also important for the positioning of the bed:
Participants preferably positioned their bed to the side of the room to which the door
opened; presumably because from this location, they can see when the door is opened
without being immediately visible to the person entering the room.
Discussion
Although previous psychological studies have investigated diverse aspects of sleep
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -413-
and sleep-related behavior, only very few of them have specifically looked at the preferred
arrangement of the sleeping site (i.e., bedroom) thereby pursuing a non-experimental and
atheoretical approach.
In contrast, the present study used experimental methodology to test predictions
derived from an evolutionary-psychological theory of sleeping place preferences
concerning the preferred positioning of the bed in the sleeping room. Starting from the
consideration that sleeping constitutes a state of increased vulnerability, and the assumption
that protection during sleep was a selection factor in human’s evolutionary history, we
postulated a set of evolved psychological processes whose function is to minimize the risk
of fitness costs resulting from attacks during sleep. Our hypotheses concerned two aspects
of bed position that are of crucial importance for reducing the fitness costs resulting from
possible attacks: the easy visibility of possible room entrances (door and windows) to the
person lying in the bed, and the establishment of space between the sleeping place and the
possible entrances. We predicted that people should prefer a bed position that (a) allows
them to keep the entrance in view, ideally without being immediately visible themselves to
a possible attacker (as is the case if the bed is positioned at the wall to which the door
opens); and (b) allows them to prepare against an attacker entering the room, which is
achieved most easily by keeping a distance to the room entrances. We tested these
hypotheses by asking participants to furnish hypothetical bedrooms that were
experimentally varied in a way that allowed us to study the effects of different entrance
configurations.
In line with predictions, participants predominantly positioned the bed in a way that
allowed them to see the door. This preference was found both when the door was the only
entrance to the room, and when a window was present at the opposite side of the room. In
the latter case, however, there was a tendency (although it failed to reach statistical
significance) towards a reduced preference for the “visible door” bed position. This
orientation towards the (only) entrance is in line with the assumed selection pressure of our
ancestors during roosting: “Pliocene hominids that anticipated a particular direction of
attack might have been more successful at evading predation” (Coss and Goldthwaite,
1995, p. 129). In addition to this orientation towards the entrance, also in line with
predictions, the majority of the participants positioned their beds on that side of the room to
which the door opened. As said, evolutionary theory explains this finding by assuming that
this bed position allows the person lying in the bed to quickly perceive the opening of the
door while remaining hidden from the intruder. This positioning closely corresponds to the
concept of prospect refuge (Appleton, 1975), a place providing the possibility “to look out
and not be seen” (Owens, 1988, p. 21). Finally, we found strong evidence for a tendency to
maximize the distance between the bed and the entrance. This maximization is primarily
assumed to provide the sleeping person with increased time to react on approaching
intruders. In sum, fundamental aspects of bed positioning were systematically influenced
by the entrance options to the bedroom in a way that was predicted by, and can be
parsimoniously and coherently explained by evolutionary-psychological considerations that
center on an evolved desire for a safe sleeping place.
Our results are in line with existing findings. For instance, the distance
maximization between bed and dominant entrance option corresponds to research
indicating that even young children without climbing experience maximize their distance
between themselves and a ground predator (e.g., a lion) by preferring higher trees as
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -414-
antipredator refuge sites (Coss and Moore, 2002). Moreover, our results closely correspond
to findings of a controlled primate study on sleeping place preferences (Caine, Potter, and
Mayer, 1992) which found that captive tamarins chose as sleeping places boxes that were
maximally distant to the door of the room.
In addition to being supportive of the evolutionary approach to sleeping site
preferences in humans, our findings are somewhat inconsistent with alternative non-
evolutionary approaches. For example, they are difficult to explain purely in terms of
considerations of functionality. If the positioning of the bed were fully determined by
considerations of functionality, people should not prefer the maximum practically feasible
distance between bed and entrance, because this distance has to be walked whenever one
moves into or out of the room from the bed (e.g., when going to the restroom at night).
Finally, choosing to sleep at the side of the room to which the door opens also is counter to
convenience, as it means a longer way to walk to open the door (or, if it is already partly
open, to circumvent it) to leave the room.
Still, some aspects of our findings are amenable to an ergonomic-functional
explanation. For example, some participants may have positioned the head side of the bed
to the wall because they imagined a bed without a headrest, and wanted to compensate for
its lack by placing the bed to the wall (however, this explanation cannot explain the
tendency to keep the window in view by turning the head position to the room). Moreover,
positioning of the bed in relation to the door might also be influenced by intentions to avoid
a draft coming from this entrance. These alternative explanations can be tested in future
studies in which the relevant variables are systematically varied. For example, participants
could be explicitly informed that the bed has a headrest versus no headrest or that draft is
present versus absent.
A different alternative explanation of our findings that could be attempted is to
explain them in terms of culturally transmitted norms. Specifically, it could be argued that
the preferred bed position is the traditional and culturally prescribed way of how to position
one’s bed. However, the mere existence of such cultural norms does not contradict the
evolutionary explanation, because these norms could themselves be based on evolutionary
preferences. Furthermore, it can be questioned whether such explicit norms—at least norms
that are behaviorally relevant, as opposed to mere folk traditions—do indeed exist. If the
evolutionary story is correct, there may be no need for such norms, because the
evolutionary preferences are strong enough to make people prefer particular bed positions.
To our knowledge, there is at present no systematic empirical evidence concerning the
existence of such norms. However, this question is highly amenable to future (cross
cultural) research (cf. Tooby and Cosmides, 1992).
According to the postulated processes, safe bed positions might elicit feelings of
comfort and unsafe bed positions feelings of discomfort. These feelings were not measured
in our study. However, this could easily be done in future research by asking participants to
imagine safe and unsafe bed positions, and to indicate the experienced degree of comfort
versus discomfort. We predict that participants experience discomfort when they imagine
an unsafe bed position (i.e., one counter to their evolutionary preferences), and therefore
avoid such configurations. Conversely, they should experience comfort if they imagine safe
bed positions.
A methodological limitation of the present study is that the participants did not
furnish a real room nor spend a real night in it, but did both only in their imagination.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -415-
However, participants were apparently able to imagine the room quite well; for example,
they spontaneously added flowerpots to the floor map or commented on what they would
store in the closet. Furthermore, asking participants to imagine places and situations is a
well-established method of architectural psychology (e.g., Slatter and Whitfield, 1977) and
of evolutionary psychology (e.g., Buss, Larsen, Westen, and Semmelroth, 1992).
Moreover, we believe that the task (furnishing a room) has prima facie ecological validity,
which was further increased by the requirement to actually arrange the icons on the floor
plan.
Another limitation of the present study is that the pre-specified floor plans used
permitted only a restricted set of possibilities to arrange the furniture, specifically the bed.
In particular, there was not enough room to position the bed at a right angle to the door or
on the same wall as the door. As mentioned, this restriction was deliberately chosen to limit
the degrees of freedom available to the participants for positioning the bed to those
locations that were particularly diagnostic (in particular, this restriction forced the
participants to move the bed either towards or away from the door). Nevertheless, future
studies should also use floor plans with higher degrees of freedom.
Another limitation of our research is that our floor plans and the symbolic
representations of the furniture were only two-dimensional in nature. Therefore, it was not
possible to modify the height of the bed which might be influenced by evolutionary
mechanisms, as well (cf. Hunt, 1997, p. 65). Thus, future research should try to replicate
our findings by using three-dimensional representations of the room (e.g., by means of
virtual environments).
To summarize, the present study seeks to contribute to the literature on sleeping
behavior in three ways. First, by examining the effects of the position of the door and the
window on bed positioning, we investigated a central aspect of human sleeping
environments that, surprisingly, has not been experimentally investigated so far. Second,
we presented a theoretical framework for investigating the topic of sleeping place
preference that allows concrete predictions that can be experimentally tested. Thereby,
evolutionary psychology is put to concrete use in the field of architectural psychology.
Third, the present study also contributes to evolutionary psychology, by extending its
domain of application to the domain of sleeping behavior in humans. We postulated the
existence of evolved psychological processes for sleeping that are of central importance for
human survival, comparable to the evolutionary mechanisms for e.g., food selection.
Thereby, we hope to have opened up a new field of inquiry to evolutionary psychology that
has not been attended to so far.
The present results encourage further investigation of the topic. Three possible lines
of future inquiry were already mentioned: the investigation of the universality versus
possible cultural specificity of sleeping place preferences; the investigation of possible
mediating emotional processes (comfort and discomfort); and the systematic test of
possible alternative explanations of (certain aspects of) our findings, such as the hypotheses
that sleeping-place preferences are based on functional-ergonomic considerations. Further
studies could also address individual differences in bed positioning, as well as the effect of
various contextual conditions on bed positioning (e.g., knowledge of where the sleeping
room is situated in the house, for example the ground floor versus the second floor) and the
interplay with other security-related mechanisms (e.g., increased group cohesion at night,
cf. Vessey, 1973). More generally, it would be interesting to extend the present
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -416-
evolutionary model to other types of rooms and their protective function.
Perhaps characteristic of modern achievement societies, too little attention has been
paid to sleeping as a central human need. The present work seeks to contribute to changing
this state of affairs, at least on an empirical level.
Acknowledgements
This paper is dedicated to the memory of Friedrich Försterling (1953-2007). Thanks are
due to Friedrich Försterling, Jon K. Maner and Rainer Reisenzein for their helpful
comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Parts of this research have been presented
on the 47th Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP; Conference of
Experimentally Working Psychologists; Spörrle and Stich, 2005).
Received 23 February 2009; revision submitted 16 May 2010; accepted 17 May 2010
References
Aloia, M. S., Arnedt, J. T., Davis, J. D., Riggs, R. L., and Byrd, D. (2004).
Neuropsychological sequelae of obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome: A
critical review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 772–
785.
Allison, T., and Cicchetti, D. V. (1976). Sleep in mammals: Ecological and constitutional
correlates. Science, 194, 732–734.
Anderson, J. R. (1998). Sleep, sleeping sites, and sleep-related activities: Awakening to
their significance. American Journal of Primatology, 46, 63–75.
Anderson, J. R. (2000). Sleep-related behavioural adaptations in free-ranging anthropoid
primates. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 355–373.
Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of the landscape. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Bader, G. G., and Engdal, S. (2000). The influence of bed firmness on sleep quality.
Applied Ergonomics, 31, 487–497.
Ball, H. L. (2002). Reasons to bed-share: Why parents sleep with their infants. Journal of
Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 20, 207–221.
Brain, C. K. (1981). The hunters or the hunted? An introduction to African cave
taphonomy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R., Westen, D., and Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in
jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychological Science, 3,
251–255.
Caine, N. G., Potter, M. P., and Mayer, K. E. (1992). Sleeping site selection by captive
tamarins (saguinus labiatus). Ethology, 90, 63–71.
Christl, B., and Richter, P. G. (2004). Territorialität und Privatheit [Territoriality and
Privacy]. In P. G. Richter (Ed.), Architekturpsychologie [Architectural psychology]
(pp. 193–209). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cohn, E. G., and Rotton, J. (1997). Assault as a function of time and temperature: A
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -417-
moderator-variable time-series analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 72, 1322–1334.
Coss, R. G., and Goldthwaite, R. O. (1995). The persistence of old designs for perception.
In N. S. Thompson (Ed.), Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 11: Behavioral Design (pp.
83–148). New York: Plenum Press.
Coss, R. G., and Moore, M. (2002). Precocious knowledge of trees as antipredator refuge in
preschool children. Ecological Psychology, 14, 181–222.
Day, R. T., and Elwood, R. W. (1999). Sleeping site selection by the golden-handed
tamarin sanguinus midas midas: The role of predation risk, proximity to feeding
sites, and territorial defence. Ethology, 105, 1035–1051.
Di Bitetti, M. S., Vidal, E. M. L. Baldovino, M. C., and Benesovsky, V. (2000). Sleeping
site preferences in tufted capuchin monkeys (cebus apella nigritus). American
Journal of Primatology, 50, 257–274.
Ehmig, J., and Richter, P. G. (2004). Die Feldtheorie (Kurt Lewin) [The Field Theory (Kurt
Lewin)]. In P. G. Richter (Ed.), Architekturpsychologie [Architectural psychology]
(pp. 29–36). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.
Flade, A. (1987). Wohnen psychologisch betrachtet [Housing from a psychological
perspective]. Bern, Switzerland: Verlag Hans Huber.
Gordon, J., King, N., Gullone, E., Muris, P., and Ollendick, T. H. (2007). Nighttime fears
of children and adolescents: Frequency, content, severity, harm expectations,
disclosure, and coping behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2464–
2472.
Groves, C. P., and Sabater Pi, J. (1985). From ape’s nest to human fix-point. Man (New
Series), 20, 22–47.
Gulyani, S., Majumdar, S., and Mallick, B. N. (2000). Rapid eye movement sleep and
significance of its deprivation studies: A review. Sleep and Hypnosis, 2, 49–68.
Hamilton, W. J., III. (1982). Baboon sleeping site preferences and relationships to primate
grouping patterns. American Journal of Primatology, 3, 41–53.
Hankerson, S. J., and Caine, N. G. (2004). Pre-retirement predator encounters alter the
morning behavior of captive marmosets (callithrix geoffroyi). American Journal of
Primatology, 63, 75–85.
Hunt, C. (1997). A cross-cultural examination of daytime and nighttime fears as evolved
adaptive mechanisms. Unpublished master’s thesis, California State University,
Sacramento.
Isbell, L. A. (1994). Predation on primates: Ecological patterns and evolutionary
consequences. Evolutionary Anthropology, 3, 61–71.
James, R. N., III. (2009). The origin of spaces: Understanding residential satisfaction from
ape nests, human cultures and the hierarchy of natural housing functions. Housing,
Theory and Society, iFirst Article, 1–17.
Lesku, J. A., Roth, T. C., II., Amlaner, C. J., and Lima, S. L. (2006). A phylogenetic
analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: The integration of anatomy, physiology,
and ecology. The American Naturalist, 168, 441–453.
Lima, S. L., Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A., and Amlaner, C. J. (2005). Sleeping under the
risk of predation. Animal Behaviour, 70, 723–736.
Michaels, C. F., and Carello, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -418-
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., and Bogie, N. (2001). Children’s
nighttime fears: Parent–child ratings of frequency, content, origins, coping
behaviors and severity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 13–28.
Nasar, J. L., Julian, D., Buchman, S., Humphreys, D., and Mrohaly, M. (1983). The
emotional quality of scenes and observation points: A look at prospect and refuge.
Landscape Planning, 10, 355–361.
Owens, P. E. (1988). Natural landscapes, gathering places, and prospect refuges:
Characteristics of outdoor places valued by teens. Children’s Environments
Quarterly, 5, 17–24.
Packer, C., Ikanda, D., Kissui, B., and Kushnir, H. (2005). Lion attacks on humans in
Tanzania. Nature, 436, 927–928.
Parsons, H. M. (1972). The bedroom. Human Factors, 14, 421–450.
Parsons, H. M. (1981). Residential design for the aging (for example, the bedroom).
Human Factors, 23, 39–58.
Peetz, A., Norconk, M. A., and Kinzey, W. G. (1992). Predation by jaguar on howler
monkeys (alouatta seniculus) in Venezuela. American Journal of Primatology, 28,
223–228.
Peterhans, J. C. K., and Gnoske, T. P. (2001). The science of ‘man-eating*’ among lions
panthera leo with a reconstruction of the natural history of the ‘man-eaters of
Tsavo’. Journal of East African Natural History, 90, 1–40.
Ramakrishnan, U., and Coss, R. G. (2001a). A comparison of the sleeping behavior of three
sympatric primates: A preliminary report. Folia Primatologica, 72, 51–53.
Ramakrishnan, U., and Coss, R. G. (2001b). Strategies used by bonnet macaques (Macaca
radiata) to reduce predation risk while sleeping. Primates, 42, 193–206.
Reichard, U. (1998). Sleeping sites, sleeping places, and presleep behavior of gibbons
(hylobates lar). American Journal of Primatology, 46, 35–62.
Richter, P. G. (Ed.) (2004). Architekturpsychologie [Architectural psychology]. Lengerich,
Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.
Rump, R. (2004). Aneignung von Raum. Die Bewegung im Raum und dessen Aneignung
[Acquisition of room. The movement in the room and its acquisition]. In P. G.
Richter (Ed.), Architekturpsychologie [Architectural psychology] (pp. 243–265).
Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.
Sabater Pi, J., Veà, J. J., and Serrallonga, J. (1997). Did the first hominids build nests?
Current Anthropology, 38, 914–916.
Schramm, A., and Richter, P. G. (2004). Prinzipien und Phänomene der Wahrnehmung
[Principles and phenomena of perception]. In P. G. Richter (Ed.),
Architekturpsychologie [Architectural psychology] (pp. 71–91). Lengerich,
Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.
Schulze, B., and Richter, P. G. (2004). Das Drei-Ebenen-Konzept der Mensch-Umwelt-
Regulation (Alfred Lang) [The three-level approach of human-environmental
regulation (Alfred Lang)]. In P. G. Richter (Ed.), Architekturpsychologie
[Architectural psychology] (pp. 49–70). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science
Publishers.
Sept, J. M. (1992). Was there no place like home? A new perspective on early hominid
archaeological sites from the mapping of chimpanzee nests. Current Anthropology,
33, 187–207.
Sleeping in safe places
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 8(3): 2010. -419-
Slatter, P. E., and Whitfield, T. W. (1977). Room function and appropriateness judgments
of color. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45, 1068–1070.
Smith, A. C., Knogge, C., Huck, M., Löttker, P., Buchanan-Smith, H. M., and Heyman, E.
W. (2007). Long-term patterns of sleeping site use in wild saddleback (saguinus
fuscicollis) and mustached tamarins (s. mystax): Effects of foraging,
thermoregulation, predation, and resource defense constraints. American Journal of
Physical Anthropology, 134, 340–353.
Spörrle, M. and Stich, J. (2005). Experimentelle Überprüfung einer evolutionstheoretischen
Erklärung von Schlafplatzpräferenzen des Menschen [Experimental examination of
an evolutionary explanation of human sleeping place preferences] [Abstract]. In K.
W. Lange, K.-H. Bäuml, M. W. Greenlee, M. Hammerl and A. Zimmer (Eds.),
Experimentelle Psychologie. Beiträge zur 47. Tagung experimentell arbeitender
Psychologen [Experimental psychology. Proceedings of the 47th Conference of
Experimentally Working Psychologists] (p. 195). Lengerich: Pabst Science
Publishers.
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1992). Psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L.
Cosmides and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Treves, A., and Naughton-Treves, L. (1999). Risk and opportunity for humans coexisting
with large carnivores. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 275–282.
Vessey, S. H. (1973). Night observation of free-ranging rhesus monkeys. American Journal
of Physical Anthropology, 38, 613–620.
Videan, E. N. (2006). Sleep in captive chimpanzee (pan troglodytes): The effects of
individual and environmental factors on sleep duration and quality. Behavioural
Brain Research, 169, 187–192.
Worthman, C. M., and Melby, M. K. (2002). Toward a comparative developmental ecology
of human sleep. In M. A. Carskadon (Ed.), Adolescent sleep patterns (pp. 69–104).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zimbardo, P. G., and Gerrig, R. J. (2004). Psychologie [Psychology] (16th rev. ed.).
Munich Germany: Pearson Studium.
Available via license: CC BY-NC 3.0
Content may be subject to copyright.