ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Does music make you smarter? Music listening and music lessons have been claimed to confer intellectual advantages. Any association between music and intellec-tual functioning would be notable only if the benefits apply reliably to nonmusical abilities and if music is unique in producing the effects. The available evidence indicates that music listening leads to enhanced performance on a variety of cognitive tests, but that such effects are short-term and stem from the impact of music on arousal level and mood, which, in turn, affect cognitive performance; experiences other than music listening have similar effects. Music lessons in childhood tell a different story. They are associated with small but general and long-lasting intel-lectual benefits that cannot be attributed to obvious con-founding variables such as family income and parents' education. The mechanisms underlying this association have yet to be determined.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Music and Cognitive Abilities
E. Glenn Schellenberg
University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada
ABSTRACT—Does music make you smarter? Music listening
and music lessons have been claimed to confer intellectual
advantages. Any association between music and intellec-
tual functioning would be notable only if the benefits apply
reliably to nonmusical abilities and if music is unique in
producing the effects. The available evidence indicates
that music listening leads to enhanced performance on a
variety of cognitive tests, but that such effects are short-
term and stem from the impact of music on arousal level
and mood, which, in turn, affect cognitive performance;
experiences other than music listening have similar effects.
Music lessons in childhood tell a different story. They are
associated with small but general and long-lasting intel-
lectual benefits that cannot be attributed to obvious con-
founding variables such as family income and parents’
education. The mechanisms underlying this association
have yet to be determined.
KEYWORDS—music cognition; intelligence; cognitive develop-
ment
People’s eagerness for quick fixes can be seen in the seemingly
irrational appeal of crash diets, get-rich-quick schemes, and get-
smart-fast programs. Is the claim of music as a route to enhanced
intelligence yet another self-improvement fantasy? In the pages
that follow, I outline the origins of the claim and evaluate the
available evidence.
Intellectual benefits of exposure to music would be noteworthy
if (a) they extend to nonmusical abilities, (b) they are systematic
and reliable, and (c) they are more likely to result from hearing or
playing music than from other activities. Unlike in other cul-
tures, where virtually everyone participates in music making,
musical experiences in Western society typically involve lis-
tening and, only in some cases, lessons and performing. Music
listening is ubiquitous, both purposefully (e.g., listening to the
radio) and incidentally (e.g., background music in stores and
restaurants). By contrast, relatively few individuals take music
lessons for several years. The consequences of music listening
are likely to differ quantitatively and qualitatively from those of
music lessons.
MUSIC LISTENING
Widespread interest in the potential benefits of music listening
was sparked by the publication of an article (Rauscher, Shaw, &
Ky, 1993) that reported superior spatial abilities for participants
who listened to a recording of music composed by Mozart com-
pared to those who sat in silence or listened to relaxation in-
structions. The finding, known as the ‘‘Mozart effect,’’ was
publicized widely in the popular media. Although the effect was
found to be brief (lasting 10–15 minutes) and the participants
were undergraduates, the news captured the public imagination
and led to social-policy changes. These included the distribu-
tion of a CD of Mozart’s music to every baby born in Georgia and
the formation of a cottage industry of music recordings designed
to make infants smarter. Presumably, the underlying rationale
was that if the short-term effect is reliable, long-term exposure to
music in infancy—when brain plasticity is greatest—might fa-
cilitate neural connections that could have long-term impact.
Subsequent replication attempts met with mixed success
(Chabris et al., 1999), however, which could be attributable to the
weakness of the effect or the reliance on group testing
(Schellenberg, in press). The original authors proposed that passive
listening to music composed by Mozart primed spatial abilities in
particular, and they attributed the replication failures to the wrong
music or the wrong task. But their proposal of cross-modal priming
between two unrelated domains is at odds with the available re-
search. Priming is a relatively robust psychological phenomenon
that occurs between stimuli with an obvious link. In language, for
example, cross-modal priming effects are evident for subsequent
presentations of the same word, a related word, a homonym, and the
sentential structure implied by the word (i.e., repetition,semantic,
phonological,andsyntactic priming, respectively).
The meta-analysis in Chabris et al. (1999) motivated specu-
lation that the Mozart effect, when evident, could be explained
as an artifact of arousal. Optimal levels of arousal (i.e., physical
and mental activation) have widespread, facilitative effects on
performance. In line with this view, a colleague and I (Nantais
and Schellenberg, 1999) replicated the Mozart effect (compared
to sitting in silence) but we also found a ‘‘Schubert effect’’ of
Address correspondence to Glenn Schellenberg, Department of Psy-
chology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario,
Canada, L5L 1C6; e-mail: g.schellenberg@utoronto.ca.
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Volume 14—Number 6 317Copyright r2005 American Psychological Society
identical magnitude. When listening to Mozart was contrasted
with listening to a narrated story, the effect disappeared (see
Fig. 1) but performance interacted with preferences. Listeners
who preferred Mozart performed better after listening to Mozart
than to the story. Listeners who preferred the story showed the
opposite pattern (a ‘‘Stephen King effect’’).
Thompson, Husain, and I (Thompson, Schellenberg, & Hu-
sain, 2001) subsequently formulated the arousal-and-mood
hypothesis: Listening to Mozart is one example of a stimulus that
influences the perceiver’s arousal level and mood, which can
affect performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. Our partici-
pants heard a fast-tempo piece by Mozart in a major (happy-
sounding) key, or a slow-tempo piece by Albinoni in a minor
(sad-sounding) key. As predicted, we observed a Mozart effect
(compared to silence) but no ‘‘Albinoni effect’’ on a test of spatial
abilities. We also found that arousal and mood were higher and
more positive after listening to Mozart than after listening to
Albinoni. The effect size of the Mozart advantage on the spatial
test was virtually identical in magnitude to the Mozart advantage
in arousal and mood. When changes in arousal or mood were
held constant by statistical means, the Mozart advantage on the
spatial test disappeared. In another study (Husain, Thompson, &
Schellenberg, 2002), the tempo (fast or slow) and mode (major
or minor) of the same Mozart piece were manipulated before
listeners completed a spatial task. The manipulations led to
different arousal levels and moods across conditions, which, in
turn, accounted for the majority of the variance in spatial abil-
ities. Yet another study tested nonspatial abilities after under-
graduates listened to Mozart or to Albinoni (Schellenberg,
Nakata, Hunter, & Tamoto, in press). When the listening expe-
rience resulted in differences in arousal and mood, performance
on a test of processing speed was also better following Mozart
than it was following Albinoni.
Paper-and-pencil measures of arousal and mood are not
available for children, but the available findings reveal en-
hanced cognitive performance after listening to music that is
thought to be arousing and pleasant for the age group under
investigation. For example, Hallam and I (Schellenberg and
Hallam, in press) reported a ‘‘Blur effect’’ for 10- and 11-year-
olds, who performed better on a spatial test after listening to pop
music (by Blur and other bands) compared to music composed by
Mozart or a scientific discussion. In a test of creativity among
younger children (Schellenberg et al., in press), 5-year-olds
drew with crayons after listening to Mozart, Albinoni, or familiar
children’s songs, or after singing familiar songs. Drawing times
were longer, and the drawings were judged to be more creative,
for the children exposed to familiar songs (a ‘‘children’s play
song effect’’). The effects did not differ between the listening and
singing groups (see Fig. 2).
In sum, music listening (or singing) can lead to enhanced
performance on a variety of tests of cognitive ability. These ef-
fects are mediated by arousal and mood and are unlikely to differ
from those that arise as a consequence of exposure to nonmusical
stimuli that have similar emotional impact (e.g., giving partici-
pants a cup of coffee or a small bag of candy; see Isen, 2000;
Smith, Osborne, Mann, Jones, & White, 2004). Listening to
music composed by Mozart does not have unique or special
consequences for spatial abilities. Rather, upbeat, age-appro-
priate music can improve listeners’ arousal level and mood, at
least for short periods. In turn, effects of arousal and mood ex-
tend beyond measures of spatial ability to tests of processing
speed and creativity. In principle, similar short-term cognitive
benefits might be evident among infants, whose arousal level is
altered by exposure to maternal singing (Shenfield, Trehub, &
Nakata, 2003). It is well established that infants perform best in
the laboratory when they are alert and content.
Mozart-Silence Schubert-Silence Mozart-Story
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
Mean Paper-Folding-and-Cutting Score
Experimental Comparison
Music
Control
Fig. 1. Mean scores on the paper-folding-and-cutting spatial test in the
music-listening and control conditions (Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999).
The figure shows a Mozart effect (left), a Schubert effect (middle), and,
when the control condition involved listening to a story, no effect (right).
Mozart Albinoni Familiar-
Listening
Familiar-
Singing
2.5
3
3.5
4
4.5
Mean Rating of Children's Drawings
Musical Experience
Fig. 2. Mean adult ratings of children’s drawings (Schellenberg et al.,
2005). Children drew after one of four musical experiences: listening to
Mozart, listening to Albinoni, listening to familiar children’s songs, or
singing familiar songs. Adult raters (blind to group membership) rated
the drawings. Higher ratings indicate more favorable appraisals of the
drawings relative to a baseline (no music) drawing.
318 Volume 14—Number 6
Music and Cognitive Abilities
MUSIC LESSONS
We turn now to the issue of whether music lessons confer non-
musical benefits. I (Schellenberg, 2004) conducted the only
controlled experiment to date that included random assignment
of individual children to music lessons or comparison condi-
tions. One hundred and forty-four 6-year-olds were administered
an entire standardized IQ test (the Wechsler Intelligence Scale
for Children-III, or WISC-III) before entering first grade (at age
6) and again between first and second grade (at age 7). In the
interim, two groups of children received 36 weeks of keyboard or
vocal instruction. Two control groups received drama lessons or
no lessons. All four groups had reliable increases in full-scale IQ
from the first to the second testing session. Such increases are a
known consequence of attending school. The two music groups
did not differ in this regard, nor did the two control groups, but
the increase in IQ was greater for the music groups than for the
control groups (see Fig. 3). This difference was not a conse-
quence of elevated performance on a specific subset of intel-
lectual abilities (e.g., verbal or spatial). Compared to the control
groups, the music groups had larger increases across the four
main areas of intellectual ability measured by the WISC-III (i.e.,
the four index scores, see Fig. 3). An incidental finding was that
the drama group had increases in adaptive social skills that were
larger than those in the other three groups.
This experiment provided evidence that music lessons cause
improvements in intellectual ability. My finding of broad intel-
lectual benefits of music lessons is also consistent with the lit-
erature as a whole (Schellenberg, in press), which includes
reports of positive associations between music lessons and
reading, mathematical, verbal, and spatial abilities. Would
music lessons of longer duration be accompanied by larger in-
tellectual benefits? Because longer-term experimental studies
would likely suffer from differential attrition across conditions,
I (Schellenberg, 2005) tested this question correlationally. In
contrast to previous research, I also measured confounding
variables such as family income and parents’ education, which
were held constant in the statistical analyses.
The participants were 147 children and 150 undergraduates.
For the children, outcome measures included the entire WISC-
III as well as a standardized test of educational achievement and
grades in school. The number of months of music lessons had a
modest but positive association with each outcome variable that
remained reliable when family income, parents’ education, and
involvement in nonmusical activities were held constant. Once
again, the effects were broad, extending across the four index
scores and the different areas of academic achievement (math,
spelling, reading) but not to social skills. Involvement in non-
musical out-of-school activities was not predictive of IQ, aca-
demic achievement, or social behavior. For the undergraduates,
the outcome measure was an entire adult IQ test (the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale-III, or WAIS-III). The association be-
tween years of playing music regularly and IQ was smaller than
the one observed in childhood but it was statistically reliable
even after accounting for individual differences in family in-
come and parents’ education. These correlational findings ex-
tend those of the experimental study by showing that real-world
effects of musical training on intellectual abilities are (a) larger
with longer periods of training, (b) long lasting, (c) not attrib-
utable to obvious confounding variables, and (d) distinct from
those of nonmusical out-of-school activities.
What are the underlying mechanisms driving the association
between music lessons and intellectual ability? One possibility
is that because music lessons are school-like, the intellectual
benefits of attending school are exaggerated by the positive
impact of additional schooling on IQ. From this perspective,
drama lessons might be insufficiently school-like (e.g., they
include pretending and dressing up), although other out-of-
school activities such as reading or chess lessons ought to have
benefits similar to music. Music lessons would be special only
because they represent a school-like activity that many children
enjoy and choose to do on a regular basis.
A second possibility is that the association stems from the con-
stellation of abilities that music lessons train and improve—abil-
ities including focused attention and concentration, memorization,
reading music, fine-motor skills, expressing emotions, and so on.
Although the association could be a consequence of improvement
in one of these abilities or a particular subset, the diffuse nature of
the association implicates the contribution of multiple factors. The
particular type of music lessons (e.g., the instrument or teaching
method) might have more specific effects on outcome measures
other than IQ. For example, keyboard lessons are as good as drama
lessons in improving children’s ability to decode the emotions
conveyed by prosody in speech, but voice lessons have no bene-
ficial effect (Thompson, Schellenberg, & Husain, 2004).
FSIQ VC PO FD PS
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
20
Mean Difference Score
WISC-III Outcome
Keyboard
Vocal
Drama
No Lessons
Fig. 3. Mean difference scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children-III (WISC-III) outcome measures for groups of children re-
ceiving keyboard lessons, vocal lessons, drama lessons, or no lessons
(Schellenberg, 2004). The music groups had larger increases than the
drama-lessons and no-lessons (control) groups. (FSIQ 5Full-Scale IQ, VC
5Verbal Comprehension Index, PO 5Perceptual Organization Index,
FD 5Freedom from Distractibility Index, PS 5Processing Speed Index.)
Volume 14—Number 6 319
E. Glenn Schellenberg
A third possibility is that music promotes intellectual develop-
ment because of its inherently abstract nature. For example, a tune
is defined solely by relational information. A listener can identify a
particular tune (e.g., ‘‘Yankee Doodle’’) when it is played fast or
slow, at a high or a low pitch level, on a piano or a guitar, and so on.
In other words, tunes are abstractions. Listeners’ representations
must generalize even farther to patterns that have similar but not
identical relational information (e.g., variations on a theme). To
illustrate, the opening bars of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony have a
repeated motive that varies in absolute pitch (different starting
tone) and in relative pitch (major third then minor third), yet lis-
teners hear the second motive as a repetition because of its iden-
tical rhythm and melodic contour. Learning to think abstractly and
to recognize musical similarities across contexts could facilitate
intellectual development more generally.
Finally, learning a musical language could have cognitive
benefits similar to those evident in bilingual children (Craik &
Bialystok, 2005). Although this view has intuitive appeal be-
cause music and language are both auditory communication
systems, the positive effects of bilingualism are evident for fluid
intelligence (i.e., executive control) but not for crystallized in-
telligence (e.g., knowledge acquired through experience, such
as vocabulary), whereas the effects of music lessons appear to
extend to both domains.
CONCLUSION
Does music make you smarter? The answer is a qualified yes.
Music listening and music lessons can lead to short-term and
long-term cognitive benefits, respectively. This positive answer
is qualified because the short-term benefits of music listening do
not appear to differ from those associated with other stimuli that
optimize arousal level or elicit mild positive affect. The mech-
anisms driving the intellectual benefits of music lessons remain
more obscure. Similar benefits could be derived from other out-
of-school activities that are similarly school-like. Nonetheless,
music lessons might be special in this regard because (a) they are
a school-like activity that many children enjoy, (b) multiple
skills are trained in music lessons, (c) music is a domain that
improves abstract reasoning, or (d) acquiring musical knowledge
is similar to acquiring a second language. Future research could
identify the underlying mechanisms more clearly.
Is music a quick fix to the problem of intelligence? The answer
is most definitely no. Short-term positive benefits of music lis-
tening dissipate rapidly as listeners’ arousal level and mood
fluctuate with time and experience. Although music lessons have
relatively long-term benefits, the effort involved (weekly lessons,
daily practice) can hardly be construed as a quick fix. Depending
on the perspective of the child and his or her parents, the benefits
of a few IQ points may or may not be worth the costs in time,
money, and effort. Moreover, different extracurricular activities
(e.g., drama lessons) have salutary effects in other domains (e.g.,
social skills) that could be as valuable as the modest increase in
IQ attributable to music lessons. The simplest take-home mes-
sage is that extracurricular activities benefit child development.
Recommended Reading
Isen, A. (2000). (See References)
Schellenberg, E.G. (2004). (See References)
Schellenberg, E.G. (in press). (See References)
Acknowledgments—Funded by the Natural Sciences and
Engineering Research Council of Canada and the International
Foundation for Music Research. Craig Chambers, Kate McLean,
and Sandra Trehub provided helpful comments on earlier ver-
sions of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Chabris, C.F., Steele, K.M., Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., Dunlop, T., Dawe,
L.A., Humphrey, G.K., Shannon, R.A., Kirby, J.L. Jr., Olmstead,
C.G., & Rauscher, F.H. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart
Effect’? Nature,400, 826–828.
Craik, F., & Bialystok, E. (2005). Intelligence and executive control:
Evidence from aging and bilingualism. Cortex,41, 222–224.
Husain, G., Thompson, W.F., & Schellenberg, E.G. (2002). Effects of
musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities.
Music Perception,20, 151–171.
Isen, A.M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis &
J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (2nd ed., pp.
417–435). New York: Guilford.
Nantais, K.M., & Schellenberg, E.G. (1999). The Mozart effect: An
artifact of preference. Psychological Science,10, 370–373.
Rauscher, F.H., Shaw, G.L., & Ky, K.N. (1993). Music and spatial task
performance. Nature,365, 611.
Schellenberg, E.G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological
Science,15, 511–514.
Schellenberg, E.G. (2005). Long-term positive associations between
music lessons and IQ. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Schellenberg, E.G. (in press) Exposure to music: The truth about the
consequences. In G.E. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A
handbook of musical development. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University
Press.
Schellenberg, E.G., & Hallam, S. (in press). Music listening and cog-
nitive abilities in 10 and 11 year olds: The Blur effect. Annals of the
New York Academy of Sciences.
Schellenberg, E.G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P.G., & Tamoto, S. (in press).
Exposure to music and cognitive performance: Tests of children
and adults. Psychology of Music.
Shenfield, T., Trehub, S.E., & Nakata, T. (2003). Maternal singing
modulates infant arousal. Psychology of Music,31, 365–375.
Smith, B.D., Osborne, A., Mann, M., Jones, H., & White, T. (2004).
Arousal and behavior: Biopsychological effects of caffeine. In A.
Nehlig (Ed.), Coffee, tea, chocolate, and the brain: Nutrition, brain,
and behavior (pp. 35–52). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Thompson, W.F., Schellenberg, E.G., & Husain, G. (2001). Arousal,
mood and the Mozart effect. Psychological Science,12, 248–251.
Thompson, W.F., Schellenberg, E.G., & Husain, G. (2004). Decoding
speech prosody: Do music lessons help? Emotion,4, 46–64.
320 Volume 14—Number 6
Music and Cognitive Abilities
... Mittal et al. further examined temporal perception, uncovering nuanced modalityspecific differences between musically trained and untrained individuals. These findings contribute to the always growing body of evidence supporting music's transferable benefits (Moreno and Bidelman, 2014), in cognitive and motor skills (Schellenberg, 2005), educational outcomes (Jaschke et al., 2018) and in second language acquisition (Marie et al., 2011;Delogu and Zheng, 2020;Zhang et al., 2024). ...
... Similar to other sensory experiences, acoustic stimuli, especially music, engage a person and help to induce and perceive different emotions. [1][2][3][4][5] Music not only entertains an individual but also affects cognition [6,7] and reduces stress. [8][9][10][11] These responses can be recorded using subjective (questionnaires) [12] and objective measures [electrocardiogram (ECG), electroencephalogram (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives There is variation in the level of engagement among individuals during any sensory experience, including listening to music. This current randomised controlled triple-blind study employed Indian music to understand electroencephalogram (EEG) based inter-brain synchronisation. Materials and Methods Four groups (three music interventions: mode or raga Miyan ki Todi, Malkauns and Puriya and one control group) were compared, with 35 participants in each group. EEG power spectrums before intervention (BI), during intervention (DI) and after intervention (AI) acoustic were analysed for correlated components (C1, C2 and C3), and scores of Inter-subject correlation (ISC). Results Left frontal gamma power increased with raga Malkauns DI. Raga Miyan ki Todi showed a frontal increase in beta1 power, and raga Puriya showed a decrease in right frontoparietal delta power AI. The raga Malkauns and Miyan ki Todi groups showed decreased C1 (globally distributed low-frequency activity) and increased C2 (posteriorly dominant alpha-beta1 activity) power. Raga Puriya showed a weak decrease in C1 and ISC scores and a marginal drop in C3 (peripherally dominant broad-band activity) AI. Conclusion The findings demonstrate specific mode-dependent correlated EEG components that persist after the listening period. The short-term effects were postulated due to default-mode network activity and autobiographical memory. Overall, this study adds to our understanding of the effects of Indian music on the brain.
... 2024; 3:.463 2 exposición temprana a la música mejora la memoria, la atención y las habilidades matemáticas en los niños. (3,4,5) La práctica musical activa, como el canto y la exploración de instrumentos, fortalece las conexiones neuronales y estimula áreas del cerebro relacionadas con la creatividad y la resolución de problemas. (6) Desarrollo Emocional y Social El aprendizaje musical también favorece el desarrollo emocional y social, ayudando a los niños a reconocer y expresar emociones de manera saludable. ...
Article
Full-text available
Musical education in early childhood is a crucial component for holistic development, promoting cognitive, emotional, and social skills. This systematic review explores the impact of musical education on preschool children, emphasizing the Latin American and Argentine contexts.Methods: A systematic review was conducted following PRISMA guidelines. Studies from 2000 to 2023 focusing on the effects of musical education in children aged 3-6 years were included. Databases like Scopus, PubMed, ERIC, and Scielo were searched using keywords such as "musical education," "preschool," and "child development." Data extraction covered demographics, study design, interventions, and outcomes. Study quality was assessed using the CASP checklist.Results: The review highlights that musical education significantly enhances cognitive development, improving memory, attention, and mathematical skills. Emotional and social benefits include better emotional expression, empathy, and cooperation. Active musical practices, such as singing and using simple instruments, foster creativity and self-esteem. In Latin America and Argentina, musical education reinforces cultural identity and promotes inclusion, albeit with challenges like resource disparities and teacher training limitations.Conclusions: Musical education in preschool is a powerful tool for holistic development, with well-documented cognitive, emotional, and social benefits. In Latin America and Argentina, it also serves as a cultural and inclusive practice. Efforts should focus on reducing inequalities in access to resources and strengthening teacher training programs.
... We cannot rule out that the quiz performance, in general, was disturbed by other factors, as we did not use a laboratory setting but a test-person platform. Possible cognitive binding elements such as music (Schellenberg, 2005) or a cell phone (Ward et al., 2017) could have had an influence that we could not measure. The taking of notes during the experiment could also not be verified. ...
Article
Full-text available
The real-time availability of information and the intelligence of information systems have changed the way we deal with information. Current research is primarily concerned with the interplay between internal and external memory, i.e., how much and which forms of cognitively demanding processes we handle internally and when we use external storage media (analog on paper, digital on the computer). This interplay influences how and what we memorize and learn consequently. This study was motivated by the finding that people perform significantly worse in a quiz setting when they obtain content from external sources instead of reading the same content directly in the question system. In our experiments, we wanted to investigate whether interruption by the user interface (ethical appeal or forced time delay) can improve performance in the quiz when users obtain external content. We evaluated the results of 262 valid participants to complete one of three topics at random and then randomly assigned them to one of three possible conditions (ethical appeal, forced time delay, no interruption). The calculated one-way ANOVA shows a statically significant F-value (3.25,p<0.053.25,\,p < 0.05), and the separate t-tests show that an ethical appeal (t=2.29,p<0.05t = 2.29,\,p < 0.05) and an enforced delay (t=2.08,p<0.05t = 2.08,\,p < 0.05) lead to a significantly higher mean of quiz scores across all three topics. The mean values of the quiz scores of the ethical appeal and the forced time delay are not significantly different. The present results should contribute to further studies of proactive learning interfaces.
... The results of this study align with previous findings that showed no impact from passive listening (19). However, they differ from other research indicating that even passive music listening can provide short-term cognitive performance benefits (27). ...
Article
Mental health disorders, especially those related to stress and anxiety, are some of the most critical issues facing teenagers today. While previous studies have focused on music’s effect on brain development and rehabilitation, there is a significant gap in understanding music’s effect on teen mental health. As one of the most avid consumers of music, teenagers’ frequent exposure to music can be utilized to combat the growing teen mental health crisis. We hypothesized that participation in music activities by teenagers would alleviate their stress levels and improve their performance. This research investigated how exposure to active versus passive music affects a teenager’s ability to perform a challenging task, namely a Sudoku puzzle, under stressful conditions. The study tested 75 high school teenagers in a Bay Area high school, split into 2 groups: students singing in a choir (active music) and students listening to low-fidelity music (passive music). Our results showed that active participation in music activities like choir yields a significantly greater improvement in performance compared to passive listening for brief time periods. Additionally, the more a participant listened to music, the better they appeared to manage stress levels and improve their performance on everyday tasks. Most participants reported listening to music while doing everyday tasks to relieve stress, boost mood, and improve focus. These results reinforce the importance of music in helping teens cope with stress and combat the growing mental health crisis. Schools and music teachers can use these findings to advocate for universal access to music programs.
Article
Okul öncesi eğitime önem vermek ve geliştirmek toplumların ilerleyişine önemli katkılar sağlar. Yapılan araştırmalar erken yaşta kaliteli bir eğitim almanın, çocukların sosyal ve bilişsel gelişimini olumlu yönde etkilediğini ortaya koymaktadır. Kaliteli bir eğitim sayesinde özgüvenli ve yaratıcı çocuklar yetiştirmek okul öncesi eğitimin hedeflerinden biridir. Bütün bunlar göz önüne alındığında, okul öncesi eğitim döneminde müzik dersi doğru yapılandırılmalı ve çocuklar için eğitici-öğretici bir şekilde işlenmelidir. Bu araştırma okul öncesi dönemde müzik eğitiminin yeri ve öneminin değerlendirilerek farklı bir bakış açısı sunulmasını amaçlamıştır. Bu çalışma, derleme yöntemi ile hazırlanmış ve mevcut literatür sistematik bir biçimde analiz edilmiştir. Makalede yapılan literatür taraması, inceleme ve araştırmadan elde edilen bulgular, okul öncesi dönemde müzik eğitiminin çocukların çok yönlü gelişimine katkı sağladığını ortaya koymaktadır. Müzik eğitimi, çocukların bilişsel, sosyal-duygusal ve motor becerilerini desteklerken aynı zamanda etkili öğrenme süreçlerini güçlendiren önemli bir araç olarak öne çıkmaktadır.
Article
In today's demanding world, achieving both productivity and mental well-being is paramount. Lo-fi music has been shown to boost motivation and creativity, enhancing mood and mindset. With its soothing and repetitive nature, lo-fi music is believed to trigger dopamine release, enhancing focus and cognitive function. Productivity tools streamline tasks, reducing stress and ensuring important responsibilities are met. ZenFi offers a unified platform where users can seamlessly transition between focused work and relaxation, integrating lo-fi music and productivity tools. By empowering users to cultivate environments that balance focus and relaxation, ZenFi transforms daily routines for enhanced mental wellbeing and productivity.
Chapter
The present study investigates, effects of music intervention on sleep quality and mental health in young adult women with PMS. With the help of the Pearson Correlation method, the study analyzes the 83 PMS participants’ responses in the age range 20–26 to better understand the link between sleep quality, depression, anxiety, and stress. After the music intervention, the data was analyzed using the Paired t-test method to find out the differences, the findings revealed increased sleep quality, and a decrease in depression, anxiety, and stress.
Article
Full-text available
Psycholinguistics, the bridge between language and cognition, has evolved through a rich history of theories and debates, shaping our comprehension of the human mind. But what role does language play in cognition? Language is far beyond just mere communication, as it molds cognition, perception, memory, and problem-solving skills. At its core, the study of language acquisition stands as a foundation for the study of psycholinguistics, spanning diverse theoretical landscapes from behaviorist theories to nativist perspectives. This paper aims to offer insights from a myriad of disciplines, offering a holistic understanding of how language is intertwined with cognitive development. Drawing upon the fields of psychology, linguistics, neuroscience, and anthropology, it unveils the profound impact language has in shaping the trajectory of cognitive development. This interdisciplinary approach illuminates the mechanisms by which language influences cognitive processes, underscoring the symbiotic relationship between language and the development of the human mind. It indicates the importance of interdisciplinary research in understanding the marriage between language and cognition, ultimately revealing the magic behind the human mind.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the effect of maternal singing on the arousal levels of healthy, non-distressed infants. Mothers sang to their 6-month-old infants for 10 minutes, after which they continued interacting for another 10 minutes. To estimate infant arousal, we gathered saliva samples from infants immediately before the mothers began singing and 20 minutes later. Laboratory analyses of the saliva samples revealed that salivary cortisol levels converged from baseline to post-test periods. Specifically, infants with lower baseline levels exhibited modest cortisol increases in response to maternal singing; those with higher baseline levels exhibited modest reductions. This convergence of arousal levels was confirmed by reduced variability in cortisol values from baseline to post-test. These findings are consistent with the view that maternal singing modulates the arousal of prelinguistic infants. Copyright
Article
Full-text available
The “Mozart effect” refers to claims that people perform better on tests of spatial abilities after listening to music composed by Mozart. We examined whether the Mozart effect is a consequence of between-condition differences in arousal and mood. Participants completed a test of spatial abilities after listening to music or sitting in silence. The music was a Mozart sonata (a pleasant and energetic piece) for some participants and an Albinoni adagio (a slow, sad piece) for others. We also measured enjoyment, arousal, and mood. Performance on the spatial task was better following the music than the silence condition, but only for participants who heard Mozart. The two music selections also induced differential responding on the enjoyment, arousal, and mood measures. Moreover, when such differences were held constant by statistical means, the Mozart effect disappeared. These findings provide compelling evidence that the Mozart effect is an artifact of arousal and mood.
Article
Full-text available
We examined effects of tempo and mode on spatial ability, arousal, and mood. A Mozart sonata was performed by a skilled pianist and recorded as a MIDI file. The file was edited to produce four versions that varied in tempo (fast or slow) and mode (major or minor). Participants listened to a single version and completed measures of spatial ability, arousal, and mood. Performance on the spatial task was superior after listening to music at a fast rather than a slow tempo, and when the music was pre-sented in major rather than minor mode. Tempo manipulations affected arousal but not mood, whereas mode manipulations affected mood but not arousal. Changes in arousal and mood paralleled variation on the spatial task. The findings are consistent with the view that the "Mozart effect" is a consequence of changes in arousal and mood.
Article
Full-text available
This article reports on two experiments of exposure to music and cognitive performance. In Experiment 1, Canadian undergraduates performed better on an IQ subtest (Symbol Search) after listening to an up-tempo piece of music composed by Mozart in comparison to a slow piece by Albinoni. The effect was evident, however, only when the two pieces also induced reliable differences in arousal and mood. In Experiment 2, Japanese 5-year-olds drew for longer periods of time after singing or hearing familiar children’s songs than after hearing Mozart or Albinoni, and their drawings were judged by adults to be more creative, energetic, and technically proficient. These results indicate that (1) exposure to different types of music can enhance performance on a variety of cognitive tests, (2) these effects are mediated by changes in emotional state, and (3) the effects generalize across cultures and age groups.
Article
Full-text available
In Study 1 (N = 147), duration of music lessons was correlated positively with IQ and with academic ability among 6-to 11-year-olds, even when potential confounding variables (i.e., family income, parents' education, involvement in nonmusical activities) were held constant. In Study 2 (N = 150), similar but weaker associations between playing music in childhood and intellectual functioning were evident among undergraduates. In both studies, there was no evidence that musical involvement had stronger associations with some aspects of cognitive ability (e.g., mathematical, spatial–temporal, verbal) than with others. These results indicate that formal exposure to music in childhood is associated positively with IQ and with academic performance and that such associations are small but general and long lasting.
Article
Full-text available
The “Mozart effect” reported by Rauscher, Shaw, and Ky (1993, 1995) indicates that spatial-temporal abilities are enhanced after listening to music composed by Mozart. We replicated and extended the effect in Experiment 1: Performance on a spatial-temporal task was better after participants listened to a piece composed by Mozart or by Schubert than after they sat in silence. In Experiment 2, the advantage for the music condition disappeared when the control condition consisted of a narrated story instead of silence. Rather, performance was a function of listeners' preference (music or story), with better performance following the preferred condition.
Article
Resenha de The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (New York, Oxford University Press, 2006, 501 páginas) editado por Gary E. McPherson Grupo de Pesquisa PROFCEM/UFPR/CNPq
Chapter
The effects of caffeine on several aspects of psychological and related physiological functioning, such as cognition, mood state, anxiety disorders, caffeine intoxication, psychiatric disorders, psychopharmacological treatment and pain perception, are discussed. The paper begins with a discussion on the genetic and neural mechanisms that underlie the behavioural effects of caffeine. Also, the beneficial and detrimental effects of acute and chronic caffeine ingestion are considered.
Article
presents some of the findings regarding the impact of mild positive affect on thinking and motivation / explores the processes underlying them and the circumstances under which they are likely to be observed / focus is on decision making, but in order to understand affect's influence on decisions, it is helpful to consider its impact on cognitive organization (or the way material is thought about and related to other material) and on motivation (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)