ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Laypeople and many social scientists assume that superior reasoning abilities lead to greater well-being. However, previous research has been inconclusive. This may be because prior investigators used operationalizations of reasoning that favored analytic as opposed to wise thinking. We assessed wisdom in terms of the degree to which people use various pragmatic schemas to deal with social conflicts. With a random sample of Americans, we found that wise reasoning is associated with greater life satisfaction, less negative affect, better social relationships, less depressive rumination, more positive versus negative words used in speech, and greater longevity. The relationship between wise reasoning and well-being held even when controlling for socioeconomic factors, verbal abilities, and several personality traits. As in prior work, there was no association between intelligence and well-being. Further, wise reasoning mediated age-related differences in well-being, particularly among middle-aged and older adults. Implications for research on reasoning, well-being, and aging are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
A Route to Well-Being: Intelligence Versus Wise Reasoning
Igor Grossmann
University of Waterloo
Jinkyung Na
University of Texas at Dallas
Michael E. W. Varnum
Peking University
Shinobu Kitayama and Richard E. Nisbett
University of Michigan
Laypeople and many social scientists assume that superior reasoning abilities lead to greater well-being.
However, previous research has been inconclusive. This may be because prior investigators used
operationalizations of reasoning that favored analytic as opposed to wise thinking. We assessed wisdom
in terms of the degree to which people use various pragmatic schemas to deal with social conflicts. With
a random sample of Americans, we found that wise reasoning is associated with greater life satisfaction,
less negative affect, better social relationships, less depressive rumination, more positive versus negative
words used in speech, and greater longevity. The relationship between wise reasoning and well-being
held even when controlling for socioeconomic factors, verbal abilities, and several personality traits. As
in prior work, there was no association between intelligence and well-being. Further, wise reasoning
mediated age-related differences in well-being, particularly among middle-aged and older adults. Impli-
cations for research on reasoning, well-being, and aging are discussed.
Keywords: affect, aging, intelligence, reasoning, well-being, wisdom
The happiness of your life depends upon the quality of your thoughts.
—Marcus Antonius
Scholars since at least the time of Aristotle have speculated that
superior reasoning leads to greater well-being. This is consistent
with laypeople’s intuitions and beliefs about themselves—those
people who report greater well-being believe that they have supe-
rior reasoning abilities (Campbell, Converse, & Rogers, 1976,
Study 3; Diener & Fujita, 1995). However, various large-scale
studies have shown no relationship between standard measures of
intelligence and well-being (e.g., Sigelman, 1981;Watten, Sy-
versen, & Myhrer, 1995;Wirthwein & Rost, 2011). Furthermore,
abstract reasoning abilities and other types of fluid intelligence
decline over adulthood (Salthouse, 2004), yet older adults report
greater well-being than their younger counterparts (e.g.,
Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000;Mroczek &
Kolarz, 1998). Moreover, standard intelligence tests do not do a
good job of capturing people’s ability to think about social rela-
tions (e.g., Sternberg, 1999) or real-world decision making (e.g.,
Stanovich, 2009).
We propose that superior reasoning may, in fact, be related to
well-being but that this is true for pragmatic (as opposed to
abstract) reasoning. By pragmatic reasoning, we mean reasoning
that is influenced by life experiences and situated in a social
context. Such reasoning strategies have been described as wise by
both philosophers and psychologists. Although wisdom has been
defined in many ways (Sternberg & Jordan, 2005), there is some
consensus that wisdom involves the use of certain types of prag-
matic reasoning that are prosocial and that help to navigate im-
portant challenges in social life. For instance, Baltes and col-
leagues (Baltes & Smith, 2008)—who developed the Berlin
wisdom paradigm— have defined wisdom as knowledge useful for
dealing with life problems, including an awareness of the varied
contexts of life and how they change over time, recognition that
values and goals differ among people, and acknowledgment of the
uncertainties of life (together with ways to manage these uncer-
tainties). Similarly, Basseches (1980) and Kramer (1983)—repre-
senting the neo-Piagetian view of reasoning—formulated a set of
cognitive schemas they believed to be involved in wise thinking,
including acknowledgment of others’ points of view, appreciation
of contexts broader than the issue at hand, sensitivity to the
possibility of change in social relations, acknowledgment of the
likelihood of multiple outcomes of a conflict, concern with conflict
resolution, and preference for compromise in resolving opposing
viewpoints.
This article was published Online First August 6, 2012.
Igor Grossmann, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo,
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada; Jinkyung Na, Center for Vital Longevity,
University of Texas at Dallas; Michael E. W. Varnum, Department of
Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, People’s Republic of China; Shi-
nobu Kitayama and Richard E. Nisbett, Department of Psychology, Uni-
versity of Michigan.
The work presented in this article was completed as part of Igor Gross-
mann’s dissertation research. It supported by the International Max Plank
Research LIFE Fellowship and the Rackham Predoctoral Dissertation
Fellowship awarded to Igor Grossmann and National Institute on Aging
Grant 5RO129509-02 awarded to Richard E. Nisbett and Shinobu Ki-
tayama. We thank Robert J. Sternberg and Jacqui Smith for thoughtful
comments on a previous version of the article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Igor
Grossmann, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo,
Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada. E-mail: igrossma@uwaterloo.ca
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General © 2012 American Psychological Association
2013, Vol. 142, No. 3, 944–953 0096-3445/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029560
944
Little work has directly tested the relationship between wise
reasoning and well-being. The only two studies we are aware of
that have examined this question used the Berlin wisdom para-
digm. These studies found inconclusive results. In one study, wise
reasoning was unrelated to negative affect,
1
but it was weakly
positively related to some aspects of positive affect (e.g., feeling
interested or inspired; Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003). In another
study, wise reasoning was unrelated to people’s positive or nega-
tive emotional responses but negatively related to global judgment
of life satisfaction (particularly among the top 15% on wise rea-
soning; Mickler & Staudinger, 2008). It is important to consider
that the materials in these studies included rather abstract de-
scriptions of personal problems. For instance, participants were
asked to read and respond to such scenarios as “A 14-year-old girl
wants to move away from home right away” or “Somebody gets a
phone call from a good friend. The friend says that she or he
cannot go on anymore and that she or he has decided to commit
suicide” (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000). Such briefly described sce-
narios provided little information about social context, which may
be a critical factor in the assessment of wise reasoning (Sternberg,
2004). Thus, it remained unclear if wisdom-related forms of rea-
soning are linked with well-being. With the present work, we
aimed to fill this gap in the literature by using a novel measure of
wise reasoning to systematically investigate its relationship to a
large number of well-being indicators.
We built on the idea that people acquire wisdom through expe-
rience and through successful mastery of various challenging life
experiences (Pascual-Leone, 1990;Rowley & Slack, 2009;Stern-
berg, 1998). Such experiences are heterogeneous in nature and
may result in idiosyncratic ways of thinking about conflict. There-
fore, and consistent with the process-oriented view of wisdom
(Kramer, 2000;Sternberg, 1998), we conceptualized wisdom as a
set of reasoning strategies that may be applicable and beneficial
across a large number of social conflicts. In other words, we
defined wisdom not through the availability of static knowledge
about a particular conflict and its solution but rather through the
use of dynamic reasoning strategies that can be applied in various
domains. Building on earlier theoretical work conceptualizing
wisdom as an inherently social construct (Rowley, 2006;Stern-
berg, 2007), we measured wisdom using naturalistic, context-rich
materials concerning social conflicts and by examining reasoning
in a structured interview with a researcher.
We measured six broad strategies of wise reasoning in our
content analyses of participants’ responses to social dilemmas
(Baltes & Staudinger, 2000;Basseches, 1980;Kramer, 1983;
Staudinger & Glück, 2011). These components were (a) consider-
ing the perspectives of people involved in the conflict, (b) recog-
nizing the likelihood of change, (c) recognizing multiple ways in
which the conflict might unfold, (d) recognizing uncertainty and
the limits of knowledge, (e) recognizing the importance of or
searching for a compromise between opposing viewpoints, and (f)
recognizing the importance of or predicting conflict resolution.
The validity of these dimensions as measures of wise reasoning
has been demonstrated in a recent study, which surveyed a large
pool of wisdom researchers and counseling practitioners. The
results from this study indicated that wisdom researchers and
practitioners rated responses that are high on these dimensions as
wiser than those that were low on these dimensions (Grossmann et
al., 2010, Study 3).
Overview of the Present Research
To address the relationship between wise reasoning and well-
being, we measured wise reasoning about real-world intergroup
and interpersonal dilemmas and various indicators of well-being.
To maximize the possibility of detecting conditions under which
wise reasoning may be related to well-being, we tested a diverse
set of socioemotional tasks. In addition to exploring the relation-
ship between wise reasoning and well-being, we also addressed the
question of how aging impacts this relationship. Specifically, we
predicted that older adults would show greater well-being, partly
because they are wiser than younger adults when reasoning about
social conflicts (Grossmann et al., 2010; Studies 1–2; Worthy,
Gorlick, Pacheco, Schnyer, & Maddox, 2011).
Method
Participants
We recruited a stratified random sample of 241 Americans in
Washtenaw County, Michigan, with an approximately equal num-
ber of participants of both genders and of each of three age groups
(25– 40 years, 41–59 years, 60 –90 years) and an adequate number
of adults from lower educated strata (see Table 1). Participants
were informed that we were interested in human reasoning and
they were compensated with $70 for taking part in each of the two
2-hr individual experimental sessions.
Procedure and Materials
Wise reasoning. Part of the data on wise reasoning came from
the Michigan Wisdom Study (Grossmann et al., 2010). The qual-
itative part of the study was conducted in a face-to-face interview
setting of the Robert B. Zajonc Experimental Laboratories at the
Institute for Social Research, Ann Arbor, Michigan. Participants
were informed that we were interested in people’s reasoning about
various future events. Care was taken to ensure that the setting did
not induce fear of cognitive ability testing and possible ageism-
related stereotype threat (Hess & Blanchard-Fields, 1999). Specif-
ically, the test setting was designed to provide a social atmosphere
(e.g., use of a meeting room instead of a laboratory cubicle, neutral
art pictures on the walls of the room), and the wisdom-related
stimulus materials were formatted as regular newspaper articles.
Further, the interview was conducted using a semistructured pro-
cedure and involved additional probes to each question. In Session
1, participants read three newspaper articles describing intergroup
tensions (over ethnic differences, politics, and natural resources) in
unfamiliar countries (Grossmann et al., in press;Grossmann et al.,
2010). After each story, the interviewer instructed participants to
talk about the future development of the conflict, guided by three
questions in the following order: “What do you think will happen
after that?” “Anything else?” and “Why do you think it will
happen this way?” Participants’ responses were audiorecorded. In
Session 2, participants read three letters describing interpersonal
dilemmas (between friends, spouses, and neighbors) selected from
1
This was particularly true in analyses with age as a covariate. Zero-
order correlations in this study showed a negative relationship between
wise reasoning and both forms of affect (positive or negative).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
945
REASONING AND WELL-BEING
the Dear Abby advice column. Participants then answered ques-
tions similar to those used in Session 1. These tasks lasted 30 min
per session on average.
Coding procedure. Participants’ transcripts were masked,
and age-related information was removed. Two hypothesis-blind
coders were trained on sample materials until they reached high
interrater reliability. These coders then scored transcripts of each
response on the six aspects of wise reasoning: (a) considering the
perspective of the parties involved, (b) recognizing the likelihood
of change, (c) recognizing multiple possibilities regarding how a
conflict might unfold, (d) recognizing limits of one’s own knowl-
edge and acknowledging uncertainty, (e) searching for compro-
mise, and (f) predicting conflict resolution. Raters coded partici-
pants’ transcripts on a scale from 1 not at all to 3 a great deal;
see Table 2 for example responses. To increase the external va-
lidity of these ratings, we used separate groups of raters for
intergroup and interpersonal scenarios. Agreement between two
respective raters was good (interrater rs.85; coder discrepancies
were resolved in group discussion between the coders and Igor
Grossmann). Considering the diverse nature of the scenarios,
scores across various scenarios showed acceptable reliability as
indicated by internal consistency and intraclass correlation mea-
sures (for Session 1, Cronbach’s ␣⫽.61; For Session 2, Cron-
bach’s ␣⫽.77; across both sessions, Cronbach’s ␣⫽.78).
Well-Being and Longevity
Participants completed a set of well-being indicators and asso-
ciated measures of emotion regulation tendencies.
Positive versus negative affect. Participants were asked to
recall 10 situations (Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, &
Uskul, 2009). Some of these episodes involved social relations
(e.g., having a positive interaction with friends), some were
related to study and work (e.g., being overloaded with work),
Table 1
Sample Characteristics
Demographic
category % or M(SD)
Gender 50.9% women
Age in years 49.48 (16.65)
Age group
25–40 years 38.2%
41–59 years 28.9%
60–90 years 32.9%
African American 12.3%
Asian American 3.5%
European American 80.26%
Latino 3.9%
Education
High school or less 27.90%
Some college 11.50%
College 30.30%
Postsecondary 58.20%
Family income/year
Under $40,000 29.6%
$40,000–$60,000 19.7%
$60,000–$80,000 20.2%
Over $80,000 30.5%
Table 2
Example Responses for the Immigration Story, Indicating Low and High Wise Reasoning
Low High
Search for a compromise
I’m sure that each, each culture will keep their original customs.
It’s not likely that someone that’s lived a certain way is going
to change just because they moved to a new area. . . . People
are pretty true to their nature and they’re not really big on
change so I’m sure that it won’t be an easy thing for them to
change their culture.
They might want to let them continue with their ways and maybe at the same
time maybe try to do some kind of promotion to encourage them to better
assimilate into the culture though, not throw away their own culture, but to
try to make the country more unified, maybe bring customs together that
might be similar for both cultures, in order to unify the country.
Recognizing multiple ways the conflict might unfold
Most likely there is going to be very similar things as going on
in the United States . . . the economic drivers are going to
want to keep the immigration going and more traditionalists
are going to want to stem it and make laws like only
speaking Tajik, instead of both. . . . It seems like that’s
happened throughout the world history when you get a large
number of immigration coming in.
It’s hard to predict in a short period of time if the Kyrgyz will be assimilated
with the Tajiks or whether there will begin to be unrest and civil disunity
because of this immigration. . . . I don’t think it will necessary happen
either way. You have several possibilities. Either taking historically into
account what has happened: One you will have assimilation and happiness
or two you will have constant bickering and at least social disunity and
social warfare.
Considering perspectives of people involved in the conflict
I think eventually it’ll be like what’s happening in the U.S. is
that all the different cultures will merge. If it’s really
productive for the country I think the cultures will merge and
there’ll be more peace within them. . . . I think that they’ll let
them come in, because they need the labor force. . . . There’s
going to be some conflict of social interests and their
cultures. . . . There’s going to be a little bit of culture
difference. It sounds like that’s the main issue here, it’s just
the culture. I think usually that ends up working out.
I think there’ll be friction between those two ideas. People do assimilate
eventually but it often takes a couple generations to do that. . . . There’ll be
influences both ways but people who are in particular countries that
receive immigrants, they always see it from their point of view, namely
that these immigrants are changing the country. They don’t necessarily see
it from the other point of view. Also, immigrants might be upset because
their children are not the way they would be if they were back in their
homeland.
Note. The examples were adapted from Grossmann et al., 2010.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
946 GROSSMANN, NA, VARNUM, KITAYAMA, AND NISBETT
and some others concerned daily hassles and bodily conditions
of the self (e.g., being caught in a traffic jam). Participants were
asked to remember the latest occasion when each of the 10
situations happened to them. They were asked to report the
extent to which they experienced a series of emotions in these
situations. The list of emotion terms contained seven positive
(e.g., feeling of closeness, pride, elated, happy) and five nega-
tive (e.g., ashamed, frustrated, angry, unhappy) emotions. Six-
point scales that ranged from 1 not at all to6very strongly
were used in rating emotional experience. To minimize possible
age-related positivity effect in memory (Carstensen & Mikels,
2005), we followed the recommendations by Kahneman and
colleagues (Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone,
2004) and performed our analyses on the episodes from the
preceding 2 days.
2
We averaged the scores for positive (Cron-
bach’s ␣⫽.88) versus negative emotions (Cronbach’s ␣⫽
.68).
Relationship satisfaction. We asked participants to consider
their personal network including people “who are important in
your life right now.” Participants reported the initials of such
people in three concentric circles, ranging from “people to whom
you feel so close that it is difficult to imagine life without them”
(innermost circle) to “people who are close enough and important
enough in your life that they should be placed in your personal
network” (outer circle). Network members included relatives
(44%, M12.74, SD 8.71), nonkin friends (42%, M14.86,
SD 14.07), and acquaintances (14%, M5.11, SD 6.20).
Next, participants indicated initials of those network members who
“have given you advice and social support during the last month.”
We counted the number of such individuals as an index of positive
relations (M5.39, SD 4.50). Finally, we asked them to
indicate initials of those member who “have caused annoyances
and troubles during the last month,” which we counted as an index
of negative relations (M1.85, SD 2.11). Following previous
research (Fiori, Antonucci, & Akiyama, 2008), we obtained the
index of relationship satisfaction by examining the relative pro-
portion of distinctly positive versus negative relationships. To
normalize the skewed distribution, we log-transformed the result-
ing scores.
Rumination. Participants next completed the five-item
Brooding subscale of the Ruminative Response Scale, which ex-
amines an emotion regulation tendency to respond to distress by
repeatedly reflecting on past negative experiences (Treynor, Gon-
zalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). For instance, one of the ques-
tions asked participants to indicate how often they think “What am
I doing to deserve this?” from 1 almost never to 4 almost
always. In the present study, this scale has showed acceptable
reliability, which was comparable to previous research (Cron-
bach’s ␣⫽.69).
Life satisfaction. Participants further answered a life satisfac-
tion question (a common technique in well-being research; Kah-
neman, Diener, & Schwarz, 1999)—“All things considered, how
satisfied are you with your life as a whole these days?”— on a
10-point scale ranging from 1 not at all to 10 very much
satisfied. This measure was administered via a survey distributed
a year after completion of the experimental sessions, resulting in a
smaller sample size (N141).
Longevity. Finally, because subjective well-being is a strong
predictor of longevity (Chida & Steptoe, 2008), we explored how
wise reasoning relates to longevity 5 years after completion of the
first session of our study by examining publicly available death
records.
Emotional discourse. In an attempt to supplement the main
well-being measures reported in this study with implicit measures
that are less susceptible to demand effects, we explored emotional
discourse patterns. We reanalyzed participants’ narratives about
the social conflicts using the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count
program (Pennebaker, Francis, & Booth, 2001). This program
analyzes texts on a probabilistic basis by comparing files on a
word-by-word basis with a dictionary of 2,290 words and word
stems that are organized into several different language categories.
The analysis computes the percentage of total words found in the
text that belong to these language categories. We took the relative
percentage of positive to negative affect words contained in the
narratives as an index of positive versus negative thought acces-
sibility. We focused on this index, because previous research
indicated that counting positive versus negative words in verbal
and written discourse about oneself and others is positively asso-
ciated with greater self-report positive affect (Pennebaker, Mehl,
& Niederhoffer, 2003) and lower neuroticism and greater agree-
ableness (Pennebaker & King, 1999) and predicts longevity (Dan-
ner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001).
Covariate Measures: Speed of Processing, Cognitive
Abilities, and Personality
Given the novelty of our measure of wise reasoning, we also
tested its relationships to cognitive abilities and personality traits.
Participants completed two tests of processing speed (Hedden et
al., 2002) and the digit span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intel-
ligence Scale (WAIS; r
forward-backward
.49, p.001). Speed-
of-processing scores were significantly correlated (r.69, p
.001) and thus were standardized and collapsed into a single index.
Participants also completed comprehension and vocabulary sub-
tests of WAIS (r.52, p.001).
The Big Five personality dimensions were measured using the
10-item personality measure, which has shown high convergent
validity with other Big Five personality measures in past research
(Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003) and the highest convergence
with the 60-item NEO Five-Factor Inventory in comparison to
other short Big Five measures (Furnham, 2008). The two items per
each dimension were significantly correlated (r
Extraversion
.53,
p.001; r
Agreeableness
.19, p.05; r
Conscientiousness
.37, p
.001; r
Neuroticism
.48, p.001; r
Openness
.40, p.001).
Following an established procedure (Gosling et al., 2003), we
collapsed the pairs of scores into a single index for each of the five
personality dimensions.
Control Variables: Perceived Health and Social Class
Perceived health was measured with a three-item health ques-
tionnaire (e.g., “Compared to other people your own age, how
2
Participants reported at least one positive and one negative episode
from the past 2 days. The main results looked very similar when examining
all episodes; for example, wise reasoning was not related to positive affect
(r⫽⫺.02), but wise reasoning was significantly negatively related to
negative affect (r⫽⫺.16, p.02).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
947
REASONING AND WELL-BEING
would you rate your physical health?”; Hedden et al., 2002;
Cronbach’s ␣⫽.78). Participants also provided demographic
information including their age, education, family income, and
occupation. We coded participants’ occupations using the Interna-
tional Socioeconomic Index of Occupational Status (Ganzeboom
& Treiman, 1996).
Results
Preliminary Analyses: Interitem Correlations and
Factor Extraction
Results from exploratory analyses are summarized in Table 3.
All pairwise correlations of wise reasoning dimensions were sig-
nificant except for limits of knowledge being unrelated to change
and conflict resolution (mean r.25). Principal component anal-
yses and a scree plot provided evidence for a single factor, which
accounted for over 38% of the variance. An alternative two-factor
solution yielded highly correlated factors (r.67), with the
second factor explaining a comparable amount of variance as each
of the six items by itself (17%). In the interest of parsimony and to
enhance measurement reliability, we collapsed the scores into a
single index.
3
Relation to Cognitive Abilities and Personality
As shown in Table 4, speed of processing was negatively
correlated with wise reasoning about intergroup conflicts (an
artifact of differential age effects on processing speed and wise
reasoning, r
partial
.05, ns), whereas verbal cognitive abilities
were positively associated with wise reasoning. Specifically,
WAIS–Comprehension was positively associated with wise rea-
soning about interpersonal scenarios, and WAIS–Vocabulary
was positively associated with wise reasoning about both inter-
group and interpersonal conflicts. Among the Big Five factors,
only Agreeableness was positively related to wise reasoning.
Relation to Well-Being
Table 4 indicates a significant association between wise reason-
ing and a wide range of well-being indicators. Participants who
scored high on wise reasoning reported less negative affect in daily
life, better relationship quality, greater life satisfaction, less ten-
dency to brood, and a more positive way of talking about social
conflicts but not more positive affect in daily life. This pattern of
results was highly consistent across both intergroup and interper-
sonal scenarios.
In contrast to wise reasoning, cognitive abilities showed no
systematic relationship to the well-being indicators. Faster speed
of processing was negatively related to positive affect (r⫽⫺.13,
p.06) and relationship quality (r⫽⫺.15, p.05) and positively
related to brooding (r.12, p.10). Greater crystallized abilities were
correlated with less positive affect (r
WAIS–Comprehension
⫽⫺.19, p
.006; r
WAIS–Vocabulary
⫽⫺.26, p.001) and less negative affect
(r
WAIS–Vocabulary
⫽⫺.14, p.07). However, higher scores on the
WAIS–Comprehension task were positively related to frequency
of positive versus negative words in the discourse (r.13, p
3
In Session 1 (intergroup conflicts), we explored wise reasoning
about fictional scenarios, which had similar narrative structure. We
attempted to increase the external validity in Session 2 (interpersonal
conflicts) by selecting scenarios from the real newspaper column. The
greater diversity likely resulted in somewhat lower internal reliability of
wisdom scores in Session 2 (Cronbach’s ␣⫽.50) than in Session 1
(Cronbach’s ␣⫽.71). We attempted to increase measurement reliabil-
ity by performing the main analyses on the composite index across
intergroup and interpersonal scenarios (Cronbach’s on six wisdom
dimensions across both sessions .71). Results were comparable
across both types of scenarios (see Table 4).
Table 3
Factor Loadings for the Six Dimensions and
Dimension Intercorrelations
Dimension
Factor
loading 1 23456
1. Change .56 .23
ⴱⴱⴱ
.23
ⴱⴱⴱ
.26
ⴱⴱⴱ
.38
ⴱⴱⴱ
.03
2. Compromise .70 .44
ⴱⴱⴱ
.35
ⴱⴱⴱ
.27
ⴱⴱⴱ
.14
3. Flexibility .76 .35
ⴱⴱⴱ
.25
ⴱⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱⴱ
4. Perspective .64 .17
ⴱⴱ
.17
ⴱⴱ
5. Resolution .55 .03
6. Limits of
knowledge .44
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
Table 4
Relationship Between Wise Reasoning, Well-Being, and Analytic
Abilities and Personality
Variable
Wise reasoning
Intergroup
(Session 1)
Interpersonal
(Session 2) Total
Relation to well-being
Life satisfaction (n144) .16
.07 17
Positive affect (n222) .03 .04 .01
Negative affect (reverse scored;
n222) .18
ⴱⴱ
.23
ⴱⴱ
.27
ⴱⴱⴱ
Relationship quality (log; n
166) .23
ⴱⴱ
.15
.25
ⴱⴱⴱ
Depressive brooding (reverse
scored; n185) .33
ⴱⴱⴱ
.18
ⴱⴱ
.33
ⴱⴱⴱ
Positive vs. negative words in
speech (n221) .16
.16
.19
ⴱⴱ
Relation to cognitive abilities and personality
Analytic abilities
Speed of processing .25
ⴱⴱⴱ
.05 .25
ⴱⴱⴱ
WAIS Digit Span .01 .02 .01
WAIS–Comprehension .03 .22
ⴱⴱ
.08
WAIS–Vocabulary .15
.31
ⴱⴱⴱ
.25
ⴱⴱⴱ
Big Five personality dimensions
Neuroticism .13 .11 .15
Extraversion .06 .07 .06
Openness .05 .01 .06
Agreeableness .24
.20
.27
ⴱⴱ
Conscientiousness .09 .16 .12
Note. The values reported are zero-order correlation coefficients. Person-
ality measures were assessed via a mailed questionnaire 2 years after
completion of the laboratory sessions, leading to an attrition (n104).
WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
p.10.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
948 GROSSMANN, NA, VARNUM, KITAYAMA, AND NISBETT
.06). The remaining correlations between cognitive abilities and
well-being indicators were negligible (|rs| .09).
Replicating previous work, personality factors showed a
number of significant correlations with well-being (Diener,
Oishi, & Lucas, 2003). Neuroticism was related to less life
satisfaction (r⫽⫺.52, p.001), less positive affect (r
.25, p.01), more negative affect (r.22, p.03), and
more brooding (r.51, p.001). Agreeableness was related
to more life satisfaction (r.23, p.03), more positive affect
(r.23, p.02), less negative affect (r⫽⫺.19, p.05),
greater relationship quality (r.36, p.001), less brooding
(r.25, p.02), and more positive versus negative words in
discourse (r.22, p.02). Extraversion was related to lower
life satisfaction (r⫽⫺.22, p.04) but also less negative affect
(r⫽⫺.18, p.07). Finally, conscientiousness was linked to
more positive and less negative affect (r.24, p.02, and r
.21, p.03, respectively).
Wisdom and Well-Being: Control Analyses With
Cognitive Abilities, Personality, and Sociodemographic
Factors
We next ran a series of multivariate regression models, in
which we simultaneously included wise reasoning and a set of
covariates as predictors of well-being. Specifically, we tested
how wise reasoning predicts well-being when controlling for a
set of crystallized cognitive abilities (WAIS comprehension and
vocabulary tests), Agreeableness, or length of elaboration
(quantified as the number of words in the narrative). As Table
5indicates, wise reasoning remained a significant predictor of
the majority of well-being indicators when simultaneously con-
trolling for verbal abilities, Agreeableness, and response length.
There were two exceptions. First and similar to analyses with-
out covariates, wise reasoning did not predict more positive
affect. Second, wise reasoning did not significantly predict
more life satisfaction when controlling for Agreeableness (␤⫽
.14, t1.51, p.13). Moreover, structural equation analyses
in which all well-being indicators were modeled as part of a
latent well-being construct regressed on wise reasoning and
covariates indicated a significant overall effect of wise reason-
ing in each of the three models. In addition, Table 6 shows that
including wise reasoning in the model with socioeconomic
factors and perceived health predicting well-being improves the
fit of regression models for each well-being indicator except for
positive affect, as indicated by the Model II R
2
significance
level.
Longevity
Because mortality is quite rare among younger adults, lon-
gevity analyses focused on participants above 45 years of age.
The results were comparable when exploring the impact of wise
reasoning on longevity on the full sample. The results of a
probit model regression with wise reasoning and age as predic-
tors and longevity (1 dead, 0 alive; 13 deaths out of 127
participants) as the dependent variable showed a significant
effect of age (B0.048, SE 0.019, |t|2.58, p.01), and
a marginally significant effect of wisdom on longevity (B
1.325, SE 0.782, |t|1.70, p.09).
4
The effect of wise
reasoning on longevity remained significant in analyses with
such covariates as sociodemographic factors (gender, educa-
tion, occupational status, income), perceived health (B
1.389, SE 0.806, |t|1.72, p.09), or verbal cognitive
abilities (B⫽⫺1.325, SE 0.782, |t|1.70, p.09).
Age, Wisdom, and Well-Being
We subsequently examined whether wise reasoning mediates
the relationship between age and well-being by performing a
structural equation analysis with well-being indices as indicators
of a latent well-being construct. Each well-being indicator showed
a significant contribution to the latent well-being construct, and the
model showed an acceptable fit,
2
(19) 27.13, root-mean-square
error of approximation .04, comparative fit index 96). Indi-
cators such as life satisfaction (␤⫽.549, t6.41, p.001),
negative affect (reverse coded; ␤⫽.516, t7.16, p.001),
relationship quality (␤⫽.345, t4.00, p.001), and brooding
(reverse coded; ␤⫽.718, t10.37, p.001) showed substantial
contributions, whereas the contributions of positive affect (␤⫽
.184, t2.24, p.025) and positive speech (␤⫽.238, t2.83,
p.005) were comparably smaller.
As Figure 1 illustrates, age was positively related to wise
reasoning and well-being, and the effect of wise reasoning on
well-being was significant when controlling for age. Moreover,
the indirect effect of age on well-being via wise reasoning was
significant (see Figure 1 for 95% confidence intervals [CIs]
from a nonparametric bootstrap test with 2,000 random replace-
ments, the technique of choice for assessing indirect effect in
smaller samples; Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007). Follow-up
analyses indicated that the indirect effect was significant for
middle-aged (at M
age
; Sobel Z1.90, p.057; 90% CI [.001,
.004]) and older adults (1 standard deviation above the mean
age, Sobel Z2.43, p.015, 90% CI [.001, .004]) but not for
younger adults (1 standard deviation below the mean age, Sobel
Z1.24, ns, 90% CI [0, .003]), suggesting that the indirect
effect of age on well-being via wise reasoning was moderated
by age (Preacher et al., 2007).
Further analyses with wise reasoning, age, and their interac-
tions predicting well-being showed a significant effect of wise
reasoning (␤⫽.220, t2.19, p.028) and age (␤⫽.367, t
4.15, p.001) and a marginal interaction (␤⫽.158, t1.77,
p.077). Comparable analyses with gender and wise reason-
ing predicting well-being showed a significant effect of wise
reasoning on well-being (␤⫽.483, t6.28, p.001) but no
significant effects of gender (␤⫽.055, t0.69, ns), nor was
there a Gender Wise Reasoning interaction (␤⫽.007, t
4
The association between wise reasoning and longevity/mortality was
further moderated by participants’ age (B0.186, SE 0.065, |t|2.87,
p.004). Simple slopes analyses (Aiken et al., 1996) indicated that
middle-aged participants who scored low (10th quantile) on wise reasoning
showed a mortality probability as high as that of their older counterparts
(B0.022, SE 0.037, |t|1, ns.). However, middle-aged participants
who scored above average (50th quantile) on wise reasoning showed
significantly lower mortality probability than their older counterparts (B
1.208, SE 0.036, |t|3.33, p.001). In addition, wise reasoning did
not significantly contribute to greater longevity among participants above
67 years of age (|B|2.299, SE 1.644, |t|1.40, p.16). Note,
however, that the death numbers in the present study were fairly small, thus
interaction results have to be interpreted with caution.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
949
REASONING AND WELL-BEING
0.08, ns). Subsequent simple slope analyses (Aiken, Reno, &
West, 1996) are illustrated in Figure 2. The relationship be-
tween wise reasoning and well-being was marginally significant
among the middle-aged participants (at the mean age, B
0.297, SE 0.159, t1.86, p.06) and significant among the
older adults (1 standard deviation above the mean age; B
0.518, SE 0.208, t2.49, p.01) but not significant among
the younger adults (1 standard deviation below the mean age,
B0.076, SE 0.218, t1, ns).
Summary
Consistent with prior research, cognitive abilities such as crys-
tallized intelligence, processing speed, and working memory
Table 5
Wisdom and Well-Being: Control Analyses With Cognitive Abilities and Personality
Variable LS POS NEG REL BROOD SPEECH WB
Model I
Wise reasoning .165/1.896
.064/0.961 .275/4.316
ⴱⴱⴱ
.251/3.138
ⴱⴱ
.343/4.891
ⴱⴱⴱ
.201/2.970
ⴱⴱ
.497/6.243
ⴱⴱⴱ
WAIS–Comprehension .057/0.599 .083/1.103 .060/0.797 .012/0.134 .079/0.992 .173/2.201
.093/0.983
WAIS–Vocabulary .012/0.121 .218/2.748
ⴱⴱ
.023/0.279 .087/0.959 0.077/0.957 .123/1.279 .099/0.974
R
2
.031/1.014 .076/2.027
.081/2.331
.070/1.576 .124/2.476
.065/1.749
.240/3.195
ⴱⴱⴱ
Model II
Wise reasoning .138/1.509 .007/0.103 .268/4.216
ⴱⴱⴱ
.183/2.229
.309/4.264
ⴱⴱⴱ
.174/2.542
.359/3.981
ⴱⴱⴱ
Agreeableness .160/1.322 .214/2.116
.158/1.600 .320/3.493
ⴱⴱⴱ
.183/1.749
.150/1.420 .433/3.683
ⴱⴱⴱ
R
2
.044/1.138 .046/1.056 .097/2.346
.136/2.352
.129/2.585
ⴱⴱ
.052/1.482 .406/3.648
ⴱⴱⴱ
Model III
Wise reasoning .166/1.743
.001/0.015 .271/4.044
ⴱⴱⴱ
.214/2.493
.355/4.993
ⴱⴱⴱ
.212/3.044
ⴱⴱ
.491/5.998
ⴱⴱⴱ
Response length .011/0.124 .048/0.661 .040/0.569 .051/0.637 .065/0.928 .064/0.925 .030/0.355
R
2
.028/0.853 .002/0.328
.075/2.161
.048/1.396 .130/2.421
ⴱⴱ
.049/1.471 .232/3.125
ⴱⴱ
Note. The values to the left of the slash are standardized beta coefficients; values to the right of the slash are tvalues. LS life satisfaction; POS
positive affect; NEG less negative affect; REL relationship quality; BROOD less rumination; SPEECH positive versus negative words when
talking about social conflicts; WB latent well-being construct with contribution from each well-being indicator; WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale. Missing cases were estimated with the Mplus 6.1 full-maximum likelihood procedure. Model fit indices (root-mean-square errors of approximation)
were, for Model I, .06; for Model II, .04; and for Model III, .04.
p.10.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
Table 6
Model Comparison: Sociodemographic Factors Versus Sociodemographic Factors and Wise Reasoning
Variable LS POS NEG REL BROOD SPEECH WB
Model IV
Occupation .065/0.680 .221/3.018
ⴱⴱ
.039/0.503 .103/1.194 .009/0.113 .070/0.893 .061/0.623
Education (years) .062/0.665 .006/0.076 .045/0.595 .103/1.163 .034/0.421 .041/0.533 .076/0.807
Income (log) .002/0.023 .089/1.318 .050/0.735 .090/1.179 .141/1.990
.122/1.787
.133/1.541
Perceived health .198/2.543
ⴱⴱ
.282/4.520
ⴱⴱⴱ
.191/2.887
ⴱⴱ
.160/2.145
.285/4.259
ⴱⴱⴱ
.070/1.033 .417/5.264
ⴱⴱⴱ
1
2
/⫽⫺
1
2
.039/0.491 .043/0.682 .044/0.671 .170/2.309
.053/0.766 .072/1.080 .034/0.412
R
2
.043/1.384 .116/2.822
ⴱⴱ
.052/1.783
.072/1.846
.108/2.527
ⴱⴱ
.027/1.227 .198/2.934
ⴱⴱ
Model V
Occupation .084/0.887 .225/3.052
ⴱⴱ
.007/0.089 .124/1.462 .047/0.606 .093/1.189 .111/1.216
Education (years) .052/0.564 .007/0.098 .030/0.407 .083/0.945 .006/0.081 .028/0.377 .047/0.540
Income (log) .002/0.029 .089/1.324 .048/0.729 .086/1.156 .142/2.110
.120/1.801
.131/1.637
Perceived health .192/2.460
.280/4.487
ⴱⴱⴱ
.178/2.755
ⴱⴱ
.144/1.944
.267/4.111
ⴱⴱⴱ
.058/0.869 .387/5.143
ⴱⴱⴱ
1
2
/⫽⫺
1
2
.051/0.641 .046/0.721 .065/1.021 .152/2.067
.078/1.189 .057/0.874 .064/0.847
Wise reasoning .152/1.716
.029/0.452 .269/4.272
ⴱⴱⴱ
.208/2.573
ⴱⴱ
.323/4.695
ⴱⴱⴱ
.191/2.834
ⴱⴱ
.454/5.984
ⴱⴱⴱ
R
2
.063/1.687
.118/2.835
.112/2.915
.108/2.358
.197/3.782
ⴱⴱⴱ
.063/1.913
.396/4.799
ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. The values to the left of the slash are standardized beta coefficients; values to the right of the slash are tvalues. LS life satisfaction; POS
positive affect; NEG less negative affect; REL relationship quality; BROOD less rumination; SPEECH positive versus negative words
when talking about social conflicts; WB latent well-being construct with contribution from each well-being indicator. Missing cases were
estimated with the Mplus 6.1 full-maximum likelihood procedure. Model fit indices (root-mean-square errors of approximation) were, for Model IV,
.05, and, for Model V, .05.
p.10.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
950 GROSSMANN, NA, VARNUM, KITAYAMA, AND NISBETT
showed no systematic relationship to well-being. In contrast, the
ability to reason wisely about social conflicts was associated with
greater global life satisfaction, greater satisfaction with social
relationships, less negative affect in daily life, a lower propensity
to brood, relatively positive discussion social conflicts, and greater
longevity. This positive association between wise reasoning and
well-being was consistent when looking separately at wise reason-
ing about intergroup and interpersonal conflicts and when control-
ling for crystallized cognitive abilities or individual differences in
personality. It is important to note that wise reasoning was not
related to greater positive affect in daily life. The latter finding
suggests that people who reason wisely are more content without
being chronically more positive.
5
Moreover, wise reasoning explained a greater amount of vari-
ance in individual well-being than did gender or various socioeco-
nomic indicators. We also observed two age-related patterns. First,
the effect of age on well-being was partially mediated by wise
reasoning. Second, the link between wise reasoning and well-being
was stronger among older adults and absent among younger adults.
Discussion
Psychologists have long sought to identify strategies that are
reliably associated with greater well-being (Seligman, 2002). The
observation that wise reasoning improves into old age (Grossmann
et al., 2010;Worthy et al., 2011) in conjunction with experimental
work on the malleability of wise reasoning (Kross & Grossmann,
2012) suggests that it may be possible to train people to reason
wisely. Our findings further suggest that wise reasoning is a
potential psychological mechanism that may explain age-related
differences in well-being. This finding dovetails with work by
Carstensen and colleagues demonstrating that as people age, they
shift their priorities toward interpersonal issues and develop
greater emotional competence (Charles & Carstensen, 2010). Ex-
tending this work, the present research demonstrated that older
adults show greater ability to reason wisely about social conflicts
than younger adults and that among middle-aged and older adults,
such reasoning is positively linked to socioemotional benefits.
In interpreting the results of our research, one important con-
sideration deals with the directionality of the relationship between
wise reasoning and well-being, as well as between wise reasoning
and longevity. In the present article, we suggested that wiser
reasoning about social conflicts leads to greater well-being and not
vice versa. A reversal would suggest that positive affect facilitates
a desire to explore the world (e.g., Fredrickson, 1998), which, in
turn, might lead to more experience and stimulate the development
of wise reasoning. But positive affect was not related to well-
being; thus, it seems unlikely that wisdom is the product of chronic
positive affect. At the same time, it is plausible that some core
emotional and social intelligence may contribute toward both
greater wise reasoning and good mental health. Further, well-being
has been linked to greater longevity on its own right (Carstensen et
al., 2011), suggesting that some common underlying mechanism
for wise reasoning and well-being has an effect on longevity.
Therefore, additional longitudinal research with the simultaneous
assessment of wise reasoning, emotion regulation strategies (Gross
& John, 2003), and social intelligence (Cantor & Kilhlstrom, 1987)
would be useful to gain deeper insight into the directionality of the
relationship between wise reasoning and well-being and the pro-
cesses that may underlie this relationship.
Another important question deals with the divergence of the
present findings and those of previous research that has explored
the relationship between wisdom-related knowledge and well-
being (Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003;Mickler & Staudinger, 2008).
First of all, in contrast to the previous studies, we provided
meaningful contexts, which would help older participants reason in
a wise way. Also, because of the lack of contextual information,
participants in their studies should have relied on their own knowl-
edge about specific life events (e.g., suicide attempt). We believe
that such focus on specific factual knowledge may be problematic.
A large body of research in cognitive psychology has demon-
strated that knowledge acquired through experiences in one situ-
ation often fails to transfer to another (Singley & Anderson, 1989).
Therefore, when measuring knowledge about specific life events,
it may be important to sample from a heterogeneous set of life
5
The null relationship between wise reasoning and positive affect dove-
tails with some developmental work, suggesting that the greater well-being
in older age is mainly accounted by steep declines in negative affect and
only a modest increase in positive affect (Stone, Schwartz, Broderick, &
Deaton, 2010).
Figure 1. Wise reasoning mediating the effect of age on well-being in a
structural equation model. Standardized coefficients are shown. The value
under the age ¡well-being path reveals the relationship between age and
well-being, after controlling for wise reasoning. The values in the square
brackets correspond to the 95% confidence interval from a bootstrap test
performed to assess the significance of the indirect effect. The mediation is
significant if the confidence interval does not include zero.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.005.
Figure 2. The association between wise reasoning and well-being as
moderated by participants’ age in a multivariate regression analysis. Sim-
ple slopes for the minimum (min), mean, and maximum (max) ages in the
sample are shown.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
951
REASONING AND WELL-BEING
domains to make accurate estimations about one’s declarative
knowledge about such events. Conflating wise reasoning with
specific knowledge about life dilemmas may also be problematic
for some other reasons. It is plausible that wiser people may show
a lower likelihood of encountering challenging life dilemmas and
therefore have less specific knowledge about them. This may be
because wiser individuals are more likely to recognize and poten-
tially preempt some of these dramatic life events. Further, scoring
individuals with great specific knowledge about life tragedies as
wiser could introduce confounds with regard to the link between
wisdom and individual well-being: People who have greater
knowledge about very difficult situations presumably learned
about them from personal experiences; thus, their psychological
well-being might be lower. An important avenue for future re-
search would be to test the extent to which the development of
wisdom-related reasoning depends on personal or vicarious life
experiences and if such experiences strengthen or weaken the link
between wise reasoning and well-being.
Conclusion
Our results suggest that lay beliefs about the relationship be-
tween reasoning abilities and well-being are correct, with one
caveat. Whereas wise reasoning about social conflicts contributes
to well-being, abstract cognitive abilities (as measured by intelli-
gence tests) do not. On the practical side, the present work sug-
gests that despite the cognitive declines often associated with older
age, the increasing number of older adults may be of great value
for the social and emotional well-being of future communities.
References
Aiken, L. S., Reno, R. R., & West, S. G. (1996). Multiple regression:
Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2008). The fascination of wisdom: Its nature,
ontogeny, and function. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 56 –
64. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00062.x
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic
(pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American
Psychologist, 55, 122–136. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122
Basseches, M. (1980). Dialectical schemata: A framework for the empirical
study of the development of dialectical thinking. Human Development,
23, 400 – 421. doi:10.1159/000272600
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rogers, W. L. (1976). The quality of
American life: Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. New York,
NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Cantor, N., & Kilhlstrom, J. (1987). Social intelligence. New York, NY:
Prentice Hall.
Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion
and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 14, 117–121. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005
.00348.x
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2000).
Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644 – 655. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.79.4.644
Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Ersner-Hershfield, H.,
Samanez-Larkin, G. R.,...Nesselroade, J. R. (2011). Emotional
experience improves with age: Evidence based on over 10 years of
experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26, 21–33. doi:10.1037/
a0021285
Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging.
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383– 409. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych
.093008.100448
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and
mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 741–756. doi:10.1097/PSY
.0b013e31818105ba
Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive
emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804 – 813. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.804
Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjec-
tive well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926 –935. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.68.5.926
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and
subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. An-
nual Review of Psychology, 54, 403– 425. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54
.101601.145056
Fiori, K. L., Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (2008). Profiles of social
relations among older adults: A cross-cultural approach. Ageing and
Society, 28, 203–231. doi:10.1017/S0144686X07006472
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of
General Psychology, 2, 300 –319. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300
Furnham, A. (2008). Relationship among four Big Five measures of
different length. Psychological Reports, 102, 312–316. doi:10.2466/pr0
.102.1.312-316
Ganzeboom, H. B. G., & Treiman, D. J. (1996). Internationally comparable
measures of occupational status for the 1988 International Standard
Classification of Occupations. Social Science Research, 25, 201–239.
doi:10.1006/ssre.1996.0010
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief
measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in
Personality, 37, 504 –528. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Grossmann, I., Karasawa, M., Izumi, S., Na, J., Varnum, M. E. W.,
Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (in press). Aging and wisdom: Culture
matters. Psychological Science.
Grossmann, I., Na, J., Varnum, M. E., Park, D. C., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett,
R. E. (2010). Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age.
PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 107, 7246 –7250. doi:10.1073/pnas.1001715107
Hedden, T., Park, D. C., Nisbett, R. E., Ji, L., Jing, Q., & Jiao, S. (2002).
Cultural variation in verbal versus spatial neuropsychological function
across the lifespan. Neuropsychology, 16, 65–73. doi:10.1037/0894-
4105.16.1.65
Hess, T. M., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1999). Social cognition and aging.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-being: The foun-
dations of hedonic psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage Founda-
tion.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone,
A. A. (2004, December 3). A survey method for characterizing daily life
experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 1776 –1780.
doi:10.1126/science.1103572
Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K.
(2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing
North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 97, 236 –255. doi:10.1037/a0015999
Kramer, D. (1983). Post-formal operations? A need for further conceptu-
alization. Human Development, 26, 91–105. doi:10.1159/000272873
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
952 GROSSMANN, NA, VARNUM, KITAYAMA, AND NISBETT
Kramer, D. (2000). Wisdom as a classical source of human strength:
Conceptualization and empirical inquiry. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 19, 83–101. doi:10.1521/jscp.2000.19.1.83
Kross, E., & Grossmann, I. (2012). Boosting wisdom: Distance from the
self enhances wise reasoning, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: General, 141, 43– 48. doi:10.1037/a0024158
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Wisdom-related knowledge: Af-
fective, motivational, and interpersonal correlates. Personality and So-
cial Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1104 –1119. doi:10.1177/
0146167203254506
Mickler, C., & Staudinger, U. M. (2008). Personal wisdom: Validation and
age-related differences of a performance measure. Psychology and Ag-
ing, 23, 787–799. doi:10.1037/a0013928
Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and
negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1333–1349. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.75.5.1333
Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). An essay on wisdom: Toward organismic pro-
cesses that make it possible. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature,
origins, and development (pp. 244 –278). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139173704.013
Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic Inquiry
and Word Count (LIWC): LIWC2001 [Computer software]. Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles: Language use
as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 77, 1296 –1312. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1296
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psycho-
logical aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual
Review of Psychology, 54, 547–577. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54
.101601.145041
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing mod-
erated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Mul-
tivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227. doi:10.1080/
00273170701341316
Rowley, J. (2006). What do we need to know about wisdom? Management
Decision, 44, 1246 –1257. doi:10.1108/00251740610707712
Rowley, J., & Slack, F. (2009). Conceptions of wisdom. Journal of
Information Science, 35, 110 –119. doi:10.1177/0165551508092269
Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 140 –144. doi:10.1111/j.0963-
7214.2004.00293.x
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive
psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York,
NY: Free Press.
Sigelman, L. (1981). Is ignorance bliss? A reconsideration of the folk
wisdom. Human Relations, 34, 965–974. doi:10.1177/
001872678103401104
Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Stanovich, K. E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of
rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Staudinger, U. M., & Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research.
Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 215–241. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych
.121208.131659
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General
Psychology, 2, 347–365. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.4.347
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). The theory of successful intelligence. Review of
General Psychology, 3, 292–316. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.4.292
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Words to the wise about wisdom? A commentary
on Ardelt’s critique of Baltes. Human Development, 47, 286 –289.
doi:10.1159/000079155
Sternberg, R. J. (2007). Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized.
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., & Jordan, J. (Eds.). (2005). A handbook of wisdom:
Psychological perspectives. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511610486
Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A
snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the
United States. PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 107, 9985–9990. doi:10.1073/pnas
.1003744107
Watten, R. G., Syversen, J. L., & Myhrer, T. (1995). Quality of life,
intelligence and mood. Social Indicators Research, 36, 287–299. doi:
10.1007/BF01078818
Wirthwein, L., & Rost, D. H. (2011). Giftedness and subjective well-being:
A study with adults. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 182–186.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.001
Worthy, D. A., Gorlick, M. A., Pacheco, J. L., Schnyer, D. M., & Maddox,
W. T. (2011). With age comes wisdom: Decision making in younger and
older adults. Psychological Science, 22, 1375–1380. doi:10.1177/
0956797611420301
Received April 6, 2012
Revision received July 4, 2012
Accepted July 4, 2012
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
953
REASONING AND WELL-BEING
... It transcends mere self-interest, enabling individuals to adopt a more nuanced, objective, and balanced perspective, thereby offering a pragmatic approach to addressing contemporary challenges and leading to solutions surpassing biased conclusions to promote amicable outcomes (Brienza et al., 2021;Glück & Weststrate, 2022). Wise reasoning about social conflicts has been associated with enhanced life satisfaction, better social relationships, and improved emotional well-being, even when socioeconomic factors and personality traits are considered (Grossmann et al., 2013). Crucially, it also plays a vital role in crucial mental health interventions, such as suicide prevention (C. S. Hu et al., 2021). ...
... Additionally, many researchers have explored the relationship between general emotions and wise reasoning. They found that wise reasoning is associated with lower levels of general negative affect (Grossmann et al., 2013;Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003), while the associations between general positive emotions and wise reasoning vary, with some studies noting strong positive correlations (Grossmann et al., 2016;, and others finding minimal or negative correlations (Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003;Mickler & Staudinger, 2008). The mixed findings may stem from methodologies that did not distinguish between emotions experienced in general (e.g. ...
... The mixed findings may stem from methodologies that did not distinguish between emotions experienced in general (e.g. Grossmann et al., 2013;Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003;Mickler & Staudinger, 2008) and those elicited by specific challenging situations (e.g. Grossmann et al., 2016. ...
Article
Full-text available
Awe, a self-transcendent emotion, has been theoretically posited as a precursor to wise reasoning. However, direct empirical evidence supporting this relationship and the underlying mechanism has been limited. In four studies (N = 3700), we examined the relationship between awe and wise reasoning, as well as the mediating effect of self-transcendence, employing cross-sectional, longitudinal , and experimental designs. We consistently found that awe had a lagged effect on (Study 1), enhanced (Studies 2 & 3), and was associated with (Study 4) wise reasoning. Furthermore, self-transcendence mediated this relationship (Studies 3 & 4). The impact of awe on wise reasoning and mediating effect of self-transcendence could not solely be attributed to awe's predominantly positive nature, and the mediation model was established beyond the influence of self-smallness (Studies 3-4). These findings contribute to understanding the emotional trigger of wise reasoning, the cognitive implications of awe, and its role in promoting wise conflict resolution.
... Regarding the former, some studies have found that wisdom is positively associated with older age (Ardelt, 2010;Grossmann et al., 2010), while others have found no effect (Grossmann et al., 2012;Grossmann & Kross, 2014;Smith & Baltes, 1990), a negative association (Jeste et al., 2021;Thomas & Kunzmann, 2014), or even an inverted U-shape relationship (Brienza et al., 2018). As for the wisdom-as-a-resource perspective, some studies have found a positive association between wisdom and well-being or mental health (e.g., Ardelt, 1997;Bergsma & Ardelt, 2012;Grossmann et al., 2013), while others have found no effect (e.g., Mickler & Staudinger, 2008;Wink & Helson, 1997;Zacher & Staudinger, 2018, for review) . Some speculate that a certain degree of ignorance and positive illusions-contrary to wisdom-may benefit one's subjective and relational well-being (Murray et al., 1996). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
We reflect on the concept of wisdom from a social psychological lens, with particular focus on the discrepancy between dynamic theories of wisdom and the largely atemporal nature of existing empirical evidence. Upon reviewing theoretical models of the key components of wisdom such as intellectual humility, open-mindedness, and empathy/perspective-taking, we outline how these characteristics differ across situational contexts. Next, we posit that the temporal dimension—essential to virtually all theories of wisdom—is paradoxically neglected or misinterpreted in empirical studies. Our findings are supported by a systematic review of literature, demonstrating that many conclusions about the development and effects of wisdom are based on insufficient, atemporal data. We outline several steps to address these challenges in wisdom and social psychological research more broadly, including enhanced sampling and modelling practices, to foster a temporally precise, social psychological understanding of wisdom-related characteristics.
... Being "smart" (as measured by standard IQ tests) also does not lead to or correlate with well-being. But higherorder thinking abilities do strongly correlate with a number of factors of well-being (Grossmann et al., 2013). ...
... They encompass intellectual humility , open-mindedness to how events may unfold, perspective-taking, and compromise-seeking. Psychometrically distinct from other personality traits and intelligence (Brienza et al., 2018;Grossmann et al., 2013), they correlate with prosocial behavior (Brienza et al., 2021;Grossmann et al., 2017) and lower misinformation susceptibility (e.g., Koetke et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated large language models' (LLMs) efficacy in classifying complex psychological constructs like intellectual humility, perspective-taking, open-mindedness, and search for a compromise in narratives of 347 Canadian and American adults reflecting on a workplace conflict. Using state-of-the-art models like GPT-4 across few-shot and zero-shot paradigms and RoB-ELoC (RoBERTa -fine-tuned-on-Emotion-with-Logistic-Regression-Classifier), we compared their performance with expert human coders. Results showed robust classification by LLMs, with over 80% agreement and F1 scores above 0.85, and high human-model reliability (Cohen’s κ Md across top models = .80). RoB-ELoC and few-shot GPT-4 were standout classifiers, although somewhat less effective in categorizing intellectual humility. We offer example workflows for easy integration into research. Our proof-of-concept findings indicate the viability of both open-source and commercial LLMs in automating the coding of complex constructs, potentially transforming social science research.
Article
The longstanding view of doctors as scientists has been an emphasis in the MCAT and medical school training. However, the AAMC recommended recognizing the importance of social and behavioral science for medicine. There is also a growing realization that being a smart problem solver and the physician as scientist model emphasizes a cold cognitive problem-solving paradigm that overlooks other human capacities that may be critical to medical reasoning and decision-making. Considering a smart physician versus a wise physician, intelligence and problem-solving are important, but a wise physician can use other important capacities beyond intelligence and rationality. This could benefit patients by introducing patient and family perspective taking, as well as compassion in doctor-patient interaction. By reconceptualizing professions from the perspective of practical wisdom, this may increase resilience to problems such as burnout. I outline some psychological capacities viewed as important in wise reasoning that are not about traditional views of intelligence. I argue that wise reasoning is not a native talent but a skill that can be developed. I argue that different kinds of experiences can increase aspects of empathy, epistemic humility, perspective taking, and wise reasoning and I examine evidence that wise reasoning may increase resilience.
Article
Full-text available
The first part of the paper attempts to understand the phenomenon of wisdom better and offer a view of the pedagogy of wisdom through three forms of teaching: teaching about wisdom, teaching wisdom, and wise teaching. Starting from the assumption that the goals of teaching wisdom (making correct judgments, having broader insights, distinguishing internal qualities and relationships, accepting, appreciating, and integrating different perspectives) can be achieved through intentional pedagogical action, the paper sought to investigate whether the components of wise reasoning are integrated into the madrasah curriculum. The results showed that the need to enrich curricula with elements of wisdom in Islamic subjects was recognised. However, this dimension is absent in general education subjects. The paper calls for raising awareness of the importance of enriching curricula to develop wisdom. Developing wisdom should be an important goal of education, and it requires appropriate visibility throughout the curriculum.
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Wisdom is universally perceived as the pinnacle of human development across various religions, cultures, and literary traditions. However, research on wisdom has primarily been centered in Western countries. The Eastern understanding of wisdom draws heavily from research conducted in, or involving participants from, Taiwan, China, and Japan. This study endeavours to explore the implicit conception of wisdom among Indian scholars. Methods Forty scholars from various disciplines, including Science, Fine Arts, Humanities, and spiritual organizations (Sages), responded to questions regarding their perspectives on wisdom. Interview were transcribed and thematically analyzed. Wisdom characteristics provided by the Indian scholars were further validated by a sample of 353 adults. The findings were analyzed in comparison to both Western and Eastern (Taiwan, China, and Japan) research on wisdom. Results Indian scholars have conceptualized wisdom under six major themes: integration of various human abilities, gained through experience, inclusion of prosocial values, depth of understanding, spiritual/transcendental elements, and rarity but achievability by all. The characteristics of wisdom identified by these scholars show strong agreement among other Indians. Conclusion Indian scholars’ understanding of wisdom mirrors that of their counterparts worldwide. Their multifaceted view emphasizes wisdom’s role in ethical decision-making, spiritual evolution, and societal advancement. The insights gathered from this study are expected to enrich the global discourse on wisdom, offering a deeper understanding of Indian perspectives.
Article
Full-text available
We explored how individuals’ mental representations of complex and uncertain situations impact their ability to reason wisely. To this end, we introduce situated methods to capture abstract and concrete mental representations and the switching between them when reflecting on social challenges. Using these methods, we evaluated the alignment of abstractness and concreteness with four integral facets of wisdom: intellectual humility, open-mindedness, perspective-taking, and compromise-seeking. Data from North American and UK participants (N = 1,151) revealed that both abstract and concrete construals significantly contribute to wise reasoning, even when controlling for a host of relevant covariates and potential response bias. Natural language processing of unstructured texts among high (top 25%) and low (bottom 25%) wisdom participants corroborated these results: semantic networks of the high wisdom group reveal greater use of both abstract and concrete themes compared to the low wisdom group. Finally, employing a repeated strategy-choice method as an additional measure, our findings demonstrated that individuals who showed a greater balance and switching between these construal types exhibited higher wisdom. Our findings advance understanding of individual differences in mental representations and how construals shape reasoning across contexts in everyday life.
Article
Full-text available
In many contemporary societies, misinformation, epistemic arrogance, and intergroup conflict pose serious threats to social cohesion and well-being. Wisdom may offer a potential antidote to these problems, with a recently identified Common Wisdom Model (CWM) suggesting that wisdom involves epistemic virtues such as intellectual humility, openness to change, and perspective-taking. However, it is unclear whether these virtues are central for folk concepts of wisdom in non-Western contexts. We explored this question by conducting focus group discussions with 174 participants from the Philippines and Sri Lanka, two countries facing socio-political and economic challenges. We found that epistemic themes were common in both countries, but more so when participants were asked to define wisdom in general terms rather than to describe how it is acquired or expressed in daily lives. Moreover, epistemic themes were more prevalent among Filipino than Sri Lankan participants, especially when the questions posed were abstract rather than concrete. We discuss how these findings relate to the CWM and the socio-cultural contexts of the two countries, and suggest that a question format should be considered in cross-cultural research on wisdom.
Article
Full-text available
Established culture-invariant measures are needed for cross-cultural assessment of verbal and visuospatial speed of processing and working memory across the life span. In this study, 32 younger and 32 older adults from China and from the United States were administered numerically based and spatially based measures of speed of processing and working memory. Chinese superiority on the numerically based tasks was found for younger adults. Age and increasing task demands diminished this cultural effect, as predicted by the framework proposed by D. C. Park, R. Nisbett, and T. Hedden (1999). However, the visuospatial measures of both working memory and speed of processing did not differ cross-culturally for either age group. The authors concluded that these visuospatial measures provide culture-invariant estimates of cognitive processes in East Asian and Western cultures, but that numerically based tasks show evidence of cultural and linguistic biases in performance levels.
Article
Full-text available
Age differences in emotional experience over the adult life span were explored, focusing on the frequency, intensity, complexity, and consistency of emotional experience in everyday life. One hundred eighty-four people, age 18 to 94 years, participated in an experience-sampling procedure in which emotions were recorded across a 1-week period. Age was unrelated to frequency of positive emotional experience. A curvilinear relationship best characterized negative emotional experience. Negative emotions declined in frequency until approximately age 60, at which point the decline ceased. Individual factor analyses computed for each participant revealed that age was associated with more differentiated emotional experience. In addition, periods of highly positive emotional experience were more likely to endure among older people and periods of highly negative emotional experience were less stable. Findings are interpreted within the theoretical framework of socioemotional selectivity theory.
Article
Full-text available
Can language use reflect personality style? Studies examined the reliability, factor structure, and validity of written language using a word-based, computerized text analysis program. Daily diaries from 15 substance abuse inpatients, daily writing assignments from 35 students, and journal abstracts from 40 social psychologists demonstrated good internal consistency for over 36 language dimensions. Analyses of the best 15 language dimensions from essays by 838 students yielded 4 factors that replicated across written samples from another 381 students. Finally, linguistic profiles from writing samples were compared with Thematic Apperception Test coding, self-reports, and behavioral measures from 79 students and with self-reports of a 5-factor measure and health markers from more than 1,200 students. Despite modest effect sizes, the data suggest that linguistic style is an independent and meaningful way of exploring personality.
Article
Full-text available
The effect of age on happiness, as defined by positive and negative affect, was examined in a survey of 2,727 persons of a broad age range (25–74) conducted by the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development. The age–affect association was examined, controlling for a host of sociodemographic, personality, and contextual influences. Among women, age was related to positive affect nonlinearly but was unrelated to negative affect. Among men, age interacted with 2 key variables in predicting affect: extraversion and marital status. These findings lend support to recent life span theories of emotion and indicate that personality, contextual, and sociodemographic variables, as well as their interactions, are all needed to fully understand the age–affect relationship.
Article
Full-text available
The covariation of resources such as money, family support, social skills, and intelligence with subjective well-being (SWB) was assessed in 195 college students. Informant ratings provided an index of resources. Self-reports, daily experience sampling, and informant reports were used to measure SWB. The authors concluded that resources taken together are moderately strong predictors of SWB. This conclusion, however, was qualified by the fact that life satisfaction was more closely related to resources than was affective well-being and that social and personal resources were in general more strongly related to SWB than were material resources. The findings also supported the hypothesis that resources correlate more strongly with SWB when they are relevant to an individual's idiographic personal strivings. A tendency was found for people to choose personal strivings for which they have relevant resources, and the degree of congruence of individuals' goals with resources was predictive of SWB.
Article
Full-text available
Handwritten autobiographies from 180 Catholic nuns, composed when participants were a mean age of 22 years, were scored for emotional content and related to survival during ages 75 to 95. A strong inverse association was found between positive emotional content in these writings and risk of mortality in late life (p < .001). As the quartile ranking of positive emotion in early life increased, there was a stepwise decrease in risk of mortality resulting in a 2.5-fold difference between the lowest and highest quartiles. Positive emotional content in early-life autobiographies was strongly associated with longevity 6 decades later. Underlying mechanisms of balanced emotional states are discussed.
Chapter
Full-text available
Wisdom is such an elusive psychological construct that few people have considered it a viable field, though many are fascinated by the topic. Well-known psychologist Robert J. Sternberg of Yale University, perceiving the growth of interest in wisdom as a field, saw a need to document the progress that has been made in the field since the early '80s and to point the way for future theory and research. The resulting comprehensive and authoritative book, Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins and Development, is a well-rounded collection of psychological views on wisdom. It introduces this concept of wisdom, considers philosophical issues and developmental approaches, and covers as well folk conceptions of the topic. In the final section, Professor Sternberg provides an integration of the fascinating and comprehensive material.
Article
Full-text available
This paper provides operational procedures for coding internationally comparable measures of occupational status from the recently published International Standard Classification of Occupation 1988 (ISCO88) of the International Labor Office (ILO, 1990). We first discuss the nature of the ISCO88 classification and its relationship to national classifications used around the world and also to its predecessor, ISCO68 (ILO, 1969), which has been widely utilized in comparative research. We argue that comparative research would gain much from adopting ISCO88 as the standard tool of classification and provide guidance on how to do this. We then outline the procedures we have used to generate new standard recodes for three internationally comparable measures of occupational status: Treiman's Standard International Occupational Prestige Scale (SIOPS), Ganzeboom et al.'s International Socio-Economic Index of Occupational Status (ISEI), and Erikson and Goldthorpe's class categories (EGP). To update the SIOPS prestige scores we have directly matched the occupational titles in the SIOPS scale to the categories of the ISCO88 classification. For ISEI scores we have replicated the procedure used to create scores for the ISCO68 categories, employing the same data but using newly developed matches between the underlying national occupational classifications and ISCO88. To construct the EGP class codes we have mapped the ISCO88 occupation categories into a 10-category classification developed by the CASMIN project for a 12-country analysis. To validate these scales, we estimated parameters of a basic status-attainment model from an independent source of data: the pooled file from the International Social Justice Project (a large international data file that combines data from sample surveys in 14 countries). Estimates based on occupational status scales derived from ISCO88 and ISCO68 are highly similar.
Article
Critics of intelligence tests-writers such as Robert Sternberg, Howard Gardner, and Daniel Goleman-have argued in recent years that these tests neglect important qualities such as emotion, empathy, and interpersonal skills. However, such critiques imply that though intelligence tests may miss certain key noncognitive areas, they encompass most of what is important in the cognitive domain. In this book, Keith E. Stanovich challenges this widely held assumption. Stanovich shows that IQ tests (or their proxies, such as the SAT) are radically incomplete as measures of cognitive functioning. They fail to assess traits that most people associate with "good thinking," skills such as judgment and decision making. Such cognitive skills are crucial to real-world behavior, affecting the way we plan, evaluate critical evidence, judge risks and probabilities, and make effective decisions. IQ tests fail to assess these skills of rational thought, even though they are measurable cognitive processes. Rational thought is just as important as intelligence, Stanovich argues, and it should be valued as highly as the abilities currently measured on intelligence tests.