ArticlePDF Available

Finding pearls: Psychometric reevaluation of the Simpson–Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ)

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The Simpson–Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ) was developed as part of a study to assess adolescent commitment to and achievement in science. For this psychometric reappraisal of the 57-item STAQ, data were analyzed from a convenience sample of 1,754 secondary students. Confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses were applied, and results suggested that the STAQ can be shortened from 57 to 22 items spanning five revised dimensions. These five dimensions were arranged into an exploratory structural equation model, which showed some grade and gender differences and strong associations among classroom environment, self-directed effort, and science affect. Findings raise the potential for teacher professional development to improve science classroom activities, and influence student self-directed effort and science affect. © 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Sci Ed92:1076–1095, 2008
Content may be subject to copyright.
Finding Pearls: Psychometric
Reevaluation of the
Simpson–Troost Attitude
Questionnaire (STAQ)
STEVEN V. OWEN
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, The University of Texas Health Science
Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX 78229-3900, USA
MARY ANNE TOEPPERWEIN, CAROLYN E. MARSHALL
Barshop Institute for Longevity and Aging Studies and Frederic C. Bartter General
Clinical Research Center, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio,
San Antonio, TX 78229-3900, USA
MICHAEL J. LICHTENSTEIN
Barshop Institute for Longevity and Aging Studies; Division of Geriatrics and
Gerontology, Department of Medicine; and Frederic C. Bartter General Clinical Research
Center, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX
78229-3900, USA
CHERYL L. BLALOCK, YAN LIU, LINDA A. PRUSKI, KANDI GRIMES
Barshop Institute for Longevity and Aging Studies and Frederic C. Bartter General
Clinical Research Center, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio,
San Antonio, TX 78229-3900, USA
Received 24 October 2007; revised 27 March 2008, 9 May 2008; accepted 19 May 2008
DOI 10.1002/sce.20296
Published online 7 October 2008 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).
ABSTRACT: The Simpson–Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ) was developed as part
of a study to assess adolescent commitment to and achievement in science. For this psycho-
metric reappraisal of the 57-item STAQ, data were analyzed from a convenience sample
of 1,754 secondary students. Confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses were applied,
and results suggested that the STAQ can be shortened from 57 to 22 items spanning five
Correspondence to: Mary Anne Toepperwein; e-mail: Toepperwein@uthscsa.edu
The contents of this report are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent
the official views of the National Center for Research Resources, National Institute on Aging, National
Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, or National Institutes of Health.
C
2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1077
revised dimensions. These five dimensions were arranged into an exploratory structural
equation model, which showed some grade and gender differences and strong associations
among classroom environment, self-directed effort, and science affect. Findings raise the
potential for teacher professional development to improve science classroom activities, and
influence student self-directed effort and science affect. C
2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Sci
Ed 92:1076 1095, 2008
INTRODUCTION
From a global perspective, scientific and technological development is occurring at a
rapid pace in many countries, while in the United States such development is slowing
and perhaps even eroding (National Academy of Sciences, 2006). In their report Rising
Above the Gathering Storm: Energizing and Employing America for a Brighter Economic
Future, the National Academy of Sciences issued their prioritized recommendations for
preserving the “science and technology enterprise so that the United States can successfully
compete, prosper, and be secure in the global community” (p. ES-1). Significantly, the top
priority was substantive improvement of K-12 science and mathematics education in the
United States. With global scientific and technological growth occurring rapidly, declining
student interest in science courses and careers is a worldwide concern that has prompted
science education reform efforts on an international scale (e.g., InterAcademy Panel on
International Issues, 2008; National Science Teachers Association, 1996). Since student
attitudes toward science affect course and career choices, measuring the impact of reform
efforts on student attitudes is important (e.g., Koballa, 1988; Rennie & Punch, 1991; Wyer,
2003) and will require measurement tools with robust psychometric properties. Reviews
of science attitude instruments have found that few have sufficient psychometric data and
called for more rigorous analyses of existing tools (Blalock et al., 2008; P. L. Gardner, 1975;
Munby, 1980; Osborne, Simon, & Collins, 2003). Applying modern psychometric analyses
to historically accepted and used instruments can result in shorter, more compact scales
with stronger psychometric properties, thus setting the stage for further study (Marshall et
al., 2007; Owen et al., 2007). Alternately, such an approach may reveal that a scale does
not have sufficient psychometric properties to warrant recommendation for continued use
(Blalock et al., 2008; Lichtenstein et al., 2008).
In the 1970s and 1980s, significant work was done internationally to develop instruments
that measure attitudes toward science (Fraser, 1977; C. Gardner, 1975; Khalili, 1987; Lin &
Crawley, 1987; Osborne et al., 2003; Schibeci & McGraw, 1981; Simpson, Koballa, Oliver,
& Crawley, 1994). However, statistical and psychometric methods have improved sharply
since that time. This improved methodology creates a timely opportunity to reevaluate
the factor structure and reliability of existing scales. With this goal in mind, and because
the Simpson–Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ) continues to be used in contemporary
science education research, we sought to reevaluate its psychometric properties.
Development and Use of the STAQ
Concerns about scientific literacy and the loss of American leadership in science and
technology were major issues in the early 1980s. It was in this climate that Simpson
and Troost (1982) developed the STAQ as the centerpiece of a longitudinal, multimethod,
and multidimensional study to examine adolescent commitment to and achievement in
science. Simpson and Troost’s study was funded by the National Science Foundation,
spanned a decade, and aimed to provide longitudinal data for a model that could effectively
predict how students develop lifelong learning habits related to science. Their plan was to
Science Education
1078 OWEN ET AL.
use this information to inform science education policy in the United States (Simpson &
Troost, 1982; Simpson & Oliver, 1990).
Simpson and Troost (1982) described the theoretical framework for their multistage study.
One of the stages involved creation of instruments that would, over a number of years, be
given to students and teachers to provide baseline information and to assess changes. The
overall study was designed to determine whether affective factors related to self,home and
family, and school and classroom, influenced student commitment to and achievement in
science. The Simpson and Troost definition of commitment to science included “attitudes,
interests, values, and other affective behaviors identified in the study” (p. 765). Existing
instruments that might be used to measure the variables in the study were evaluated and
deemed insufficient because of “readability, item complexity, and low internal consistency”
(p. 771). Therefore, a major initiative of their study became the development of instruments
that could measure relevant variables.
The STAQ was developed and pilot tested with secondary students. Results from several
factor analyses created a final pool of 58 Likert-type items comprising 14 subscales (Oliver
& Simpson, 1988; Simpson & Troost, 1982). Simpson and Troost (1982) used factor
analysis to confirm that factors could be replicated. Content validity was evaluated by an
expert panel and item analysis, although no data were provided in their 1982 publication.
Exploratory factor analysis was mentioned, but no detail was provided, and confirmatory
factor analysis was not used. In fairness, confirmatory factor analysis was just emerging
in psychometric research at the time the STAQ was developed, so it is not surprising that
factor analysis results are incomplete.
Reliability and Validity Evidence for the STAQ
Our comprehensive review of studies using the STAQ is summarized in Appendices A
(peer-reviewed literature) and B (non-peer-reviewed literature, e.g., dissertations), both of
which are available at http://teachhealthk-12.uthscsa.edu/STAQ-App.pdf. In their validation
of the STAQ, Simpson and Troost (1982) modified the initial instrument based upon factor
analysis of data from 4,508 students.In the original study, no reliability estimates were
reported for the 14 subscales. Later peer-reviewed studies provided insight into reliability
evidence by reporting subscale KR20 values or Cronbach’s alpha with coefficients ranging
from .33 to .95 (Atwater, Wiggins, & Gardner, 1995; Cannon & Simpson, 1985; Hill,
Atwater, & Wiggins, 1995; Oliver & Simpson, 1988; Simpson & Oliver, 1985, 1990; Talton
& Simpson, 1985, 1986, 1987). Dissertation studies using the STAQ reported Cronbach’s
alpha values for the subscales ranging from .27 to .95 (Cannon, 1983; C. Gardner, 1992;
Maidon, 2001; Oliver, 1986; Spellman & Oliver, 2001). Information provided by Dr. Oliver
(personal communication, September 30, 2004) reported subscale reliability coefficients
ranging from .35 to .90 on ten subscales; information on four subscales was not included.
By today’s standards, the reliability evidence indicates that some subscales do not provide
a dependable measure of their construct.
Content validity information about the STAQ is limited, including the use of judgments
from expert panels (Cannon, 1983; Simpson & Troost, 1982) and professional test writers
(Cannon, 1983). With modern psychometric methods available, a more quantitative index
of content validity would help researchers evaluate the value of the STAQ as a research
tool (Polit, Beck, & Owen, 2007, in press). Exploratory factor analysis was mentioned
(Greenfield, 1997; Simpson & Oliver, 1985; Simpson & Troost, 1982), but neither summary
data nor full explanations of procedures were provided. No published study has reported
confirmatory factor analysis on the STAQ. In short, scant psychometric data have been
reported since Simpson and Troost’s (1982) original study. It is important to note that, with
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1079
the exception of Greenfield (1997), all studies reviewed here used the original Simpson and
Troost (1982) data set.
In summary, STAQ content validation was done by a team of expert judges and teachers,
reports of internal consistency have been highly variable, item analysis was undefined,
and factor analysis results were poorly described. However, because of the theoretical
underpinnings, large data set, and longitudinal design, Simpson and Troost (1982) set the
stage for a long history of research and validation. Our aim in this paper was to reassess the
STAQ with a new and large data set. Using current statistical techniques, we reevaluated
the psychometric properties and internal structure of the STAQ, and then applied structural
equation modeling to explore how STAQ variables interact.
METHODS
The instrument, obtained from Drs. Simpson and Oliver, consisted of 59 items with
five-point Likert-type responses, plus two supplementary biographic items that we did not
use (living arrangements of the respondent and whether the respondent was right-handed
or left-handed). In addition, one item was duplicated, which according to Dr. Simpson was
a typing error, so we deleted one of these items. One school district requested that one
sensitive item be removed (I argue a lot with my family). Thus in the current study, the
administered instrument consisted of 57 items. Permission to use the instrument was given
by Dr. Simpson (personal communication, March 18, 2004). The project was approved by
the sponsoring university’s institutional review board.
Sample
The STAQ was administered to a geographically constrained convenience sample of
1,812 secondary students from 23 teachers in 8 schools, and within 6 districts in the Bexar
County (Texas) area. All but one school are majority-minority schools, i.e., schools whose
students are mostly from racial or ethnic backgrounds (African American, Hispanic, and
Native American) that are traditionally underrepresented in science in the United States.
We recognize that the unique population and sample in South Texas may limit the external
validity and generalization of our findings. The instruments were collected at the beginning
of the 20042005 school year, and the distribution of boys and girls from each grade level
is shown in Table 1. Reflecting the population of teachers participating in our program, the
preponderance of our sample of students was seventh graders (about 12– 13 years old).
Lengthy scales that span several pages are bound to have some missing item responses.
Initial inspection of the data showed an obvious increase in missing data after item 37. That
happened to be the division between the second and third pages of the survey. We decided
that the purposeful omission of the last 20 items could not be regarded as missing-at-random
data, and those 58 cases were removed from all remaining analyses. Of the remaining 1,754
students, 65.3% responded to all questions on the STAQ, 26.9% were missing 1– 2 items,
and 7.8% were missing between 3 and 21 items.
Analyses
Additional preliminary analyses addressed whether data were missing at random. Later
analyses focused on studying the internal structure (i.e., dimensionality) of the STAQ.
Subsequently, we estimated subscale reliabilities, tested a causal model among STAQ
dimensions, and evaluated whether the model worked equally well for boys and girls and
across grades 68. A repeatability test was conducted using the revised 22-item instrument.
Science Education
1080 OWEN ET AL.
TABLE 1
Distribution of Boys and Girls (Number and Column Percent) Completing
the STAQ
Gender
Boys Girls Total Missing
a
Grade N (%) N (%) N (%)
6 172 (9.8) 196 (11.2) 370 (21.1) 2
7 537 (30.6) 602 (34.3) 1141 (65.1) 2
8 112 (6.4) 128 (7.3) 240 (13.7) 0
Missing 0 0 0 3
b
Total 821 (46.8) 926 (52.8) 1754 (100) 7
a
Gender and/or grade level were missing for seven students.
b
Gender
and
grade level were missing for three students.
AMOS 7.0 was used for confirmatory factor models and the structural equation modeling
(SEM). SPSS 15.0 was used for all other analyses.
Our data were multilevel in nature, with students nested in classes, and classes nested
in schools. Such multilevel data often violate the statistical assumption of uncorrelated
errors, which in turn raises the alpha level (e.g., the Type I error rate). The usual approach
to nested data is to use specialized multilevel analyses. The multilevel correction to alpha
levels, however, depends on the intraclass correlation (ICC). With an ICC of .00, there is
no advantage to a multilevel model. We computed ICCs for each of the composite variables
created during later analyses. They were all very small (mean ICC =.005), suggesting that
the added complexity of a multilevel structural model would not have significantly altered
the results.
RESULTS
Preliminary Study of Missing Data
After removing those 58 respondents who did not answer the final page, we studied
whether the remaining data were missing at random. The data set was partitioned into
two groups: those missing one or more responses on any of the 57 items and those with
complete data. Next, a discriminant function analysis was arranged to predict missingness.
Predictor variables were the only other two variables in the data set (grade level and
gender). The discriminant model was significant (p=.01). However, Wilkes’ lambda was
0.991 (that is, the effect size was R2=.009), which we judged trivial. Inspection of the
predictor means confirmed this: For the missing group, the average grade level was 6.88;
for the nonmissing group, 6.95. And for the missing group, the average gender value
(boys =0, girls =1) was 0.64; for the nonmissing group, 0.56. The essential randomness
of missing data was important for later analyses, because different analyses treat missing
data differently. Exploratory factor analyses and reliability estimations, for example, use
casewise deletion, where every case with any missing data is automatically discarded.
By contrast, confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation models operate with full
information maximum likelihood, which estimates the covariance matrix with all available
data, and no cases are thrown out. If data are missing at random, then these two approaches
to missing data should not produce results specific to their sample.
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1081
Dimensionality Evidence for the STAQ
For an initial test of the full Simpson–Troost 14-factor model, a confirmatory factor
analysis (CFA) was arranged with the complete data set. The model was not supported,
with χ2=4,986.8, df =1,234, p< .0001 (should be nonsignificant; Hu & Bentler, 1995)
and a substandard comparative fit index (CFI)=0.86 (should be at least 0.90; Hu & Bentler,
1995). The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), however, was acceptable
at 0.040 (0.90 CI =0.039 0.041; p=1.0; should be less than .08 and nonsignificant; Hu
& Bentler, 1995).
We then moved to an exploratory mode, splitting the overall sample into two approximate
random halves. One sample was named validation (n=848) and the other, cross-validation
(n=906). With the validation data, we staged a series of principal axis factor models with
oblique (direct oblimin) rotation, beginning with a 13-factor model, and descending to a
1-factor model. (The rationale for beginning with the 13-factor model was that the 14-factor
model had already been disconfirmed.) We used a loading criterion of 0.35 to identify
salient items. Several of the models contained uninterpretable factors whose content was
not coherent. The research team, seven members who have been studying student attitudes
toward science for the past 2 years, found the 10-factor solution, shown in Table 2, most
interpretable. All subscales had at least three items, and there was minimal cross-loading
on factors. One of the 10 factors was essentially nil (i.e., all item loadings were less than
0.35). We also reviewed and ultimately deleted item 21, which appeared markedly different
from its companion items.
We then applied a reliability test to each of the dimensions: Any subscale data whose co-
efficient alpha was below .70 would beeliminated from further consideration. This criterion
was based on the minimum value suggested for research that compares groups (Nunnally &
Bernstein, 1994). This is a much more stringent standard than used in earlier publications
investigating the STAQ, which reported subscale reliabilities as low as the .30s. However,
we argue that measurement error worms its way through statistical analyses—especially
multivariate methods—and poor measurements in turn weaken a study’s conclusions and
recommendations. By using the item loading criterion of 0.35 and setting a minimum alpha
coefficient of .70 for each subscale, five revised factors were retained from the exploratory
factor analysis (Factors 1, 2, 3, 4, and 7 in Table 2). We labeled these subscales “Motivating
Science Class” (Factor 1), “Self-Directed Effort” (Factor 2), “Family Models” (Factor 3),
“Science Is Fun for Me” (Factor 4), and “Peer Models” (Factor 7) as shown in Table 3.
Next, we used the cross-validation sample (n=906) for a more stringent confirmatory
factor analysis of the remaining five dimensions. Except for the exact fit test, χ2(203) =
853.9, p<.001, the remaining fit indices were good, with CFI =0.94 and RMSEA =0.043
(0.90 CI =0.0400.046). Table 3 gives the standardized loadings for this confirmatory
model. Figure 1 is a visual synopsis of how items on the original STAQ were either dropped
or rearranged to coalesce into the revised shortened structure presented in this paper. Within
the larger original STAQ, we were able to define a more concise, cohesive set of internally
consistent constructs (“the pearls”) that may be more useful for evaluating curricular and
pedagogical interventions in the classroom.
To explore grade level and gender differences on the revised STAQ dimensions, a
2 (gender) ×3 (grade) MANOVA was arranged with the cross-validation data set (Table 4).
The five dependent variables were scale scores of the revised STAQ dimensions, created by
averaging across the relevant items. The main effect of gender was significant (F5/890 =6.99,
p<.01), but a univariate probe of the effect showed that boys and girls differed only on the
“Self-Directed Effort” subscale (respective means 4.04 and 4.28, p<.01, with a small effect
size, η2=.03). The main effect of grade was also significant (F10/1780 =10.25, p<.01).
Science Education
1082 OWEN ET AL.
TABLE 2
Ten-Factor Pattern Matrix for the STAQ from Exploratory Factor Analysis of
the Validation Set (
n
= 848)
Factor
STAQItem1 2345678910
07 66
17 66
05 62
04 46
25 43
27 41
13 57
57 56
28 52
29 42
47 66
43 64
41 55
55 54
01 65
54 58
08 51
15
a
36 49
31 45
49 38
52 51
16 49
53 44
32 41
23 37
34 37
39 57
20 41
35 40
26 79
50 69
02 55
51 44
33 47
09 44
12 38
37 69
14 52
06 43
Alpha .75 .72 .71 .84 .60 .62 .71 .52 .50
SSL 12.25 2.77 2.28 1.27 1.08 0.76 0.64 0.61 0.53 0.40
Notes. Decimals omitted, loadings below 0.35 omitted. SSL =Sum of squared loadings.
Bold numbers indicate items or factors that met the criteria of having item loadings of 0.35
or better and alpha coefficients of .70 or better.
a
Item deleted because of dual loading.
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1083
TABLE 3
Final Five-Factor Revised STAQ Model from Confirmatory Factor Analysis:
Item Stems and Standardized Loadings in the Cross-Validation Set (
n
= 906)
a
Loading
“Motivating Science Class” Subscale (Factor 1)
STAQ 17. We do a lot of fun activities in science class. 0.65
STAQ 7. We cover interesting topics in science class. 0.67
STAQ 25. I consider our science classroom attractive and comfortable. 0.59
STAQ 27. My science teacher makes good plans for us. 0.59
STAQ 4. We learn about important things in science class. 0.58
STAQ 5. Our science classroom contains a lot of interesting equipment. 0.56
“Self-Directed Effort” Subscale (Factor 2)
STAQ 57. I always try hard, no matter how difficult the work. 0.73
STAQ 28. I try hard to do well in science. 0.69
STAQ 13. I always try to do my best in school. 0.62
STAQ 29. When I fail that makes me try that much harder. 0.57
“Family Models” Subscale (Factor 3)
STAQ 43. My mother likes science. 0.67
STAQ 47. My father likes science. 0.66
STAQ 55. My brothers and sisters like science. 0.59
STAQ 41. My family watches science programs on TV. 0.54
“Science Is Fun for Me” Subscale (Factor 4)
STAQ 54. I really like science. 0.78
STAQ 31. I have good feelings toward science. 0.73
STAQ 8. I enjoy science courses. 0.73
STAQ 1. I would enjoy being a scientist. 0.58
“Peer Models” Subscale (Factor 7)
STAQ 26. My best friend likes science. 0.74
STAQ 50. My best friend in this class likes science. 0.67
STAQ 2. My friends like science 0.60
STAQ 51. Most of my friends do well in science. 0.47
a
Items listed in parallel to Table 2 (rank order).
Univariate ANOVAs showed significant grade level variation on each of the five STAQ
subscores, with η2ranging from .02 (“Family Models”) to .08 (“Peer Models”). Tukey
pairwise comparisons showed that each grade was different from each other grade on the
subscales “Motivating Science Class, “Science Is Fun for Me,” and “Peer Models.” For
the subscales “Family Models” and “Self-Directed Effort, eighth graders split off from the
other two grades. From Table 4, it can be seen that scores on each subscale were reliably
lower as grade level increased. The average effect size across all five subscales was 0.048.
Structural Model With the Remaining STAQ Dimensions
The factor intercorrelations from the exploratory factor analysis ranged from .02 to
.51. It occurred to us that the intercorrelations might represent more than simple bivariate
relations—they might represent underlying causal arrangements among the STAQ dimen-
sions. We set about linking the five remaining dimensions in a hypothesized causal arrange-
ment, depicted in Figure 2. For this work, the cross-validation sample (n=906) was used.
Latent variables are shown in elliptical shapes, and observed variables (the item responses)
Science Education
1084 OWEN ET AL.
Figure 1. STAQ subscales created through factor analyses. Note that STAQ 21 was removed because its content
was out of alignment with companion items.
are shown in rectangles. The straight arrows represent hypothesized causal influences. The
coefficients between a latent variable and its indicators are standardized factor loadings.
Values sitting between the latent variables are standardized path coefficients, akin to beta
weights in a regression model. These depict direct influences of one latent variable on
another. For example, assuming the causal effect is true, the path coefficient of 0.66 from
“Motivating Science Class” to “Self-Directed Effort” suggests that increasing the latent
variable “Motivating Science Class” by 1 standard deviation would result in about two
thirds of a standard deviation improvement in “Self-Directed Effort.”
Two latent variables—“Family Models” and “Motivating Science Class”—were exoge-
nous, that is, they are beginning points with no specified causes within the model. We
could not justify a causal relationship between those two, so they were agnostically linked
with the curved, double-headed arrow that simply allows them to be correlated. Each of
the two exogenous variables was hypothesized to influence other variables in the model, as
shown in Figure 2. Importantly, the three latent variables on the left portion of the model
(“Family Models, “Motivating Science Class, and “Peer Models”) may be regarded as
antecedent variables, which influence the two right side variables (“Science Is Fun for Me”
and “Self-Directed Effort”). In other words, the attitude-like “Science Is Fun for Me” and
the attribution variable of “Self-Directed Effort” were hypothesized to be affected by the
important relationships in a student’s life, plus the teacher’s arrangement and structure of
the science class. The items comprising “Science Is Fun for Me” refer to student perceptions
of positive affect (e.g., I really like science; I have good feelings toward science). Some
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1085
TABLE 4
Descriptive Summary by Gender and Grade, Cross-Validation Data
Gender
Boys Girls ANOVA Effect
Revised Subscale
n
Mean Standard Deviation
n
Mean Standard Deviation
p
η2
“Motivating Science Class” Grade
6 97 4.03 0.76 95 3.93 0.72 Gender .16 .00
7 286 3.75 0.62 300 3.83 0.59 Grade <.01 .05
8 63 3.37 0.74 59 3.60 0.69 Interaction .08 .01
Total 446 3.76 0.70 454 3.82 0.64
“Self-Directed Effort” Grade
6 97 4.16 0.81 95 4.42 0.62 Gender <.01 .03
7 286 4.10 0.76 300 4.29 0.64 Grade <.01 .04
8 63 3.60 0.87 59 4.06 0.72 Interaction .18 .00
Total 446 4.04 0.81 454 4.28 0.66
“Family Models” Grade
6 97 2.99 0.93 95 2.77 0.98 Gender .09 .00
7 286 2.78 0.85 300 2.66 0.80 Grade <.01 .02
8 63 2.47 0.94 59 2.45 0.74 Interaction .62 .00
Total 446 2.78 0.89 454 2.66 0.84
“Science Is Fun for Me” Grade
6 97 3.66 0.93 95 3.58 0.90 Gender .45 .00
7 286 3.30 0.89 300 3.28 0.86 Grade <.01 .06
8 63 2.68 0.84 59 2.95 0.88 Interaction .19 .00
Total 446 3.30 0.93 454 3.30 0.89
“Peer Models” Grade
6 97 3.14 0.85 95 3.41 0.83 Gender .20 .00
7 286 2.91 0.78 286 2.90 0.72 Grade <.01 .08
8 63 2.53 0.81 59 2.51 0.68 Interaction .08 .01
Total 446 2.90 0.82 454 2.96 0.78
Note. Subgroup sizes do not sum to data set total of 906 because of several cases of missing data on gender or grade.
Science Education
1086 OWEN ET AL.
Family
Models
Peer
Models
Self-Directed
Effort
Motivating
Science
Class
Science
Is Fun for Me
Simpson27
Simpson25
Simpson04
.58
Simpson05
Simpson17
Simpson07
Simpson29
Simpson28
Simpson57
Simpson13
Simpson01
Simpson54
Simpson08
Simpson31
Simpson47
.66
Simpson43
.67
Simpson41
.54
Simpson55
.59
Simpson51Simpson02Simpson50Simpson26
.56
.67
.65
.59
.59
.67
.74 .47
.60
.45
.66
.57
.62
.58
.78
.29
.73
.56
.73
.69
.73
.51
.21
Figure 2. Initial structural model of STAQ components.
items within “Motivating Science Class” also refer to positive affect, but with an important
addition: They refer specifically to teacher-determined lessons (e.g., We do a lot of fun
activities in science class; We cover interesting topics in science class). The logic of the
structural model is that the teacher’s instructional arrangements should strongly influence
student affect. The opposite causal route—student affect influencing teacher lessons—
should be less pronounced. Teachers are not oblivious to student affect, but the classroom’s
affective and motivational climate is largely a function of the teacher’s lesson planning and
execution, and management style (Cheng, 1994; Meece, Andermann, & Andermann, 2006;
Turner, Meyer, & Schweinle, 2003).
This structural model was evaluated with the cross-validation sample (n=906). The
test of exact fit for the structural model gave χ2(221) =927.9, p<.001. Indices of close
fit gave more encouragement, thus CFI =0.94 and (RMSEA) =0.043 (0.90 CI =0.040
0.046). Three hypothesized causal paths, removed from Figure 2, were nonsignificant at
p=.05, and were deleted from later analyses. A rerun of the model with the three deleted
paths showed that the trimming did not worsen the overall model (χ2[224] =930.6,
p<.001; CFI =0.94; RMSEA =0.042 [0.90 CI =0.0400.045]). Removal of the three
paths changed none of the factor loadings or the remaining path coefficients.
The factor loadings were all strong, with the lowest at 0.47. This implies that the
latent variables had acceptable indicators, and it confirmed the five strongest factors from
the exploratory factor analysis with the validation data set. Interestingly, in this trimmed
model, three variables (especially “Motivating Science Class”) had important influence on
“Science Is Fun for Me.” But “Science Is Fun for Me” was disconnected from self-reported
behavior, represented by “Self-Directed Effort.” And although “Family” and “Peer Models”
affect “Science Is Fun for Me, they had no influence on “Self-Directed Effort.
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1087
Further Evaluation of Gender Differences
Substantial earlier work (e.g., American Association of University Women Education
Foundation, 1992; National Coalition for Women and Girls in Education, 2002; U.S. De-
partment of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, 2004) has reported gender
differences in attitudes toward science and many other school variables. We created a multi-
group analysis model, using the cross-validation sample, to test gender differences in the
trimmed structural model. The multigroup approach is typically a series of increasing con-
straints on the model. In the first stage, boys’ and girls’ unconstrained models are analyzed
simultaneously, the only requirement being that the factors have the same indicators (but not
the same loadings), and the causal arrows linking the latent variables are the same (but not
the same path coefficients). In the next stage, an additional requirement is added that factor
loadings are equivalent for boys and girls. Each successive constraining condition is nested
in the previous one, so changes in successive models may be tested with a simple χ2test.
The first of the multigroup series showed the unconstrained comparison to fit closely,
although the exact fit test was significant (χ2[406] =805.8, p<.001; CFI =0.93;
RMSEA =0.030 [0.90 CI =0.029 0.035]). When the next model forced the factor load-
ings to be equal, there was an insignificant change in exact fit (χ2[17] =20.5, p=.25).
Thus, the measurement of the five revised STAQ latent variables was invariant across
gender. However, each additional constrained model was significantly different from the
immediately preceding model. Of most interest were the differences in “structural weights,
i.e., the path coefficients. The variations are shown in Figure 3 (girls) and Figure 4 (boys);
these are the models in which the path coefficients were free to vary across gender. One
notable difference in magnitude appears between “Family Models” and “Science Is Fun for
Me” (standardized coefficient =.34 and .20 for girls and boys, respectively). This implies
Family
Models
Peer
Models
Self-Directed
Effort
Motivating
Science
Class
Science
Is Fun for Me
Simpson27
Simpson25
Simpson04
.54
Simpson05
Simpson17
Simpson07
Simpson29
Simpson28
Simpson57
Simpson13
Simpson01
Simpson54
Simpson08
Simpson31
Simpson47
.64
Simpson43
.75
Simpson41
.52
Simpson55
.60
Simpson51Simpson02Simpson50Simpson26
.54
.66
.65
.60
.57
.74 .49.60
.35
.56
.58
.63
.73
.64
.60
.79
.34
.16
.75
.73
.47
.59
.80
Figure 3. Girls’ structural model with factor loadings constrained.
Science Education
1088 OWEN ET AL.
Family
Models
Peer
Models
Self-Directed
Effort
Motivating
Science
Class
Science
Is Fun for Me
Simpson27
Simpson25
Simpson04
.53
Simpson05
Simpson17
Simpson07
Simpson29
Simpson28
Simpson57
Simpson13
Simpson01
Simpson54
Simpson08
Simpson31
Simpson47
.66
Simpson43
.70
Simpson41
.52
Simpson55
.59
Simpson51Simpson02Simpson50Simpson26
.52
.64
.64
.58
.61
.63 .48.59
.54
.69
.63
.63
.75
.67
.57
.76
.20
.38
.71
.67
.60
.45
.68
Figure 4. Boys’ structural model with factor loadings constrained.
that, compared to boys, family members of girls play a stronger role in affecting girls’
beliefs about whether science is fun. However, this gender difference should not obscure
the far larger effect of “Motivating Science Class” on “Science Is Fun for Me.”
A second obvious difference shows up between “Motivating Science Class” and “Self-
Directed Effort” (.56 and .69 for girls and boys, respectively). This suggests that the
teachers’ efforts to arrange the science class environment have a larger impact on boys’
effort than on girls’ effort. But as before, we emphasize that the gender difference is
fairly small in view of the larger principle: Among the various motivational aspects of this
structural model, it is the environment of the science class, arranged by the science teacher,
that delivers a large effect on self-reported student effort. Significantly, the science teacher
wields considerable control over class environment, choice of activities, and instructional
delivery.
An additional impact of “Motivating Science Class” is not immediately obvious from
the girls’ and boys’ structural models of Figures 3 and 4. The total standardized effect of
“Motivating Science Class” on “Science Is Fun for Me” was nearly the same for girls (0.66)
and for boys (0.68). The total effect is the sum of the direct link between these two latent
variables, plus the indirect route mediated through “Peer Models. In the girls’ model, the
total effect is decomposed into 0.07 indirect and 0.59 direct effects; in the boys’ model it
is decomposed into 0.23 indirect and 0.45 direct. In other words, “Peer Models” was a far
more important mediator for boys than for girls.
Further Evaluation of Grade-Level Differences
In the earlier MANOVA, conspicuous grade-level differences were seen for each of
the STAQ subscales. For another perspective on this, we used the cross-validation data to
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1089
perform a multigroup analysis across the three grade levels. The arrangement was similar to
the gender multigroup model, with a series of increasing constraints following a completely
unconstrained model. However, even the baseline unconstrained model showed substandard
fit, with (χ2[609] =1188.0, p<.001; CFI =0.89; RMSEA =0.033 [0.90 CI =0.030
0.035]). Given a poor start and the fact that approximately 65% of the sample was composed
of seventh graders, there is not much merit in assessing the sequential constraints (although
each was significantly different from each preceding model). Evidently, the systematic
and widespread grade-level differences seen in the MANOVA reverberated through the
multigroup structural model as well.
Reliability Evidence for the Revised STAQ Subscales
In general, reliability asks about score consistency. Because consistency has various
definitions (e.g., consistency over time; consistency among items), it is helpful to calculate
more than one reliability estimate to see how consistent the coefficients themselves are. The
classic reliability coefficient is calculated as Cronbach’s alpha, which evaluates consistency
among items. However, Hancock and Mueller (2001) have argued that alpha is really an
estimate for a composite of observed variables (e.g., item responses). They pointed out that
in structural equation modeling, one might instead be interested in “construct reliability,
which may be calculated as coefficient H, a direct reliability estimate of the latent variable.
Following Hancock and Mueller’s formulas for coefficient H, Table 5 shows coefficient H
values for each of the five Simpson–Troost latent variables, as well as classic Cronbach
alphas and stability coefficients.
The stability data came from an independent data set of an additional 186 youngsters,
which included 68 ninth and tenth graders (approximately 1416 years old). We wanted
to see whether the middle school reliability evidence generalized to high schoolers. These
students completed the STAQ on two occasions 1 week apart.
Several summary results from the reliability data are clear. First, the reliability estimates
are adequate, running mostly in the .70s. The “Science Is Fun for Me” dimension was de-
pendably more consistent, and this is also supported by higher 1-week stability coefficients.
Second, there was very little difference between coefficients H and alpha. And third, differ-
ences between boy and girl reliabilities were negligible. However, when stability data were
TABLE 5
Reliability Estimates for Revised Simpson–Troost Subscales
Stability
a
Construct (H) Classic (alpha) Stability
a
Middle High
Variable Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls School School
“Motivating Science .77 .79 .78 .79 .82 .77 .74 .77
Class”
“Self-Directed Effort” .75 .71 .75 .71 .78 .82 .81 .79
“Family Models” .72 .71 .74 .70 .77 .84 .77 .87
“Science Is Fun for Me” .82 .84 .79 .81 .85 .87 .84 .90
“Peer Models” .71 .77 .70 .73 .77 .79 .72 .90
a
For 1-week stability estimates, boys
n
=86, girls
n
=100, middle school =118, high
school =68.
Science Education
1090 OWEN ET AL.
examined by school level, high school student data generally showed more consistency
over time, compared to middle school data.
DISCUSSION
We accomplished three aims in this paper. The first was to conduct a psychometric review
and reappraisal of the STAQ using an independently collected data set from a sample of
middle school students (about 1114 years old). In our analyses, the original 14 subscales
did not fit a cohesive structure. Instead, using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses,
requiring a factor loading of 0.35 per item, and a subscale Cronbach’s alpha of .70, we
were able to derive a five-factor revised STAQ. This model reduced the number of items
from 57 to 22. Reducing the items on any scale while retaining or improving data reliability
and validity has two practical virtues. Shortened instruments (a) require less class time to
administer and (b) reduce the problem of missing data.
The second aim was to determine whether the five-factor revised STAQ would support a
putative structural equation model of influences on science affect and self-directed effort.
The model fit the data and demonstrated that “Motivating Science Class” has strong asso-
ciations with science affect (“Science Is Fun for Me”) and “Self-Directed Effort. Family
models and peer models were associated with science affect, but not effort. The model fit
equally well for boys and girls with moderate differences between two of the specified
paths. However, there were sizeable grade-level differences among the five revised STAQ
dimensions.
Although many variables related to science education are under only partial control
of educators, the models generated in this study suggest that creating a motivating, fun
science class has greater impact on student affect (“Science Is Fun for Me”) and effort
(“Self-Directed Effort”) than family or peer variables. This is a compelling affirmation
(and responsibility) for teachers to know that their effectiveness in the classroom exerts
considerable influence. The subscale “Motivating Science Class” consists of items related
to the physical environment of the classroom, the teacher, and the curriculum. This subscale
has strong associations with both student perceptions of science and student effort, affirming
the importance of the classroom teacher. “Family Models” have modest associations with
student perception that science is fun, while “Peer Models” have about the same effect.
Neither “Family Models” nor “Peer Models” exerted significant effect on “Self-Directed
Effort. Interestingly, the causal relationships hypothesized in the model affirm Simpson
and Oliver’s finding (1990) that classroom variables had the strongest impact on student
affect. Simpson and Oliver (1990) discovered that gender differences were not as significant
as hypothesized, which was also true of our data.
The third aim was to assess the stability of the five-factor STAQ. This was accomplished
in a separate sample of 186 students and showed repeatability estimates ranging from 0.70
to 0.90. While acceptable, there is still room for improvement (see Table 5). Thus, the
groundwork is laid for additional research into the ability of this model to predict student
achievement over time and perhaps student participation in science over time.
Although we noted that structural equation modeling accounts for measurement error in
calculating parameter estimates, that does not itself justify the continued use of psychome-
trically substandard measures. Classic parametric procedures (e.g., ANOVA, regression)
are bound by the impossible assumption that measurements have zero measurement error.
To the extent measurement error exists, those sorts of analyses will deliver biased results.
In simple bivariate analyses, such as a correlation, there is an exactly known downward
bias. But in a multivariate approach, measurement error ripples through the entire set of
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1091
measures, and results are unpredictably biased upwardly or downwardly (Owen & Froman,
2005). Such biases can readily distort researchers’ conclusions.
Theoretical Basis for Hypothesized Structural Model
By its nature, science education is a dynamic interaction of variables related to learners,
educators, schools, science, and society. The literature generally assumes that childhood
experiences greatly influence both academic interest and achievement (e.g., Simpson &
Oliver, 1990). However, some relevant influences, such as personal motivation, and home
and peer variables, are only under partial control of educators (Kremer & Walberg, 1981).
Children and adolescents imitate those they respect and admire and with whom they identify.
Typically, those are models with whom they have the most contact, such as parents, siblings,
peers, and perhaps older friends (Owen, Froman, & Moscow, 1981). The norms displayed by
such groups help to steer students’ behavior and attitudes (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). There
is thus some theoretical and empirical support for family and peer influences on student
perceptions about science (“Science Is Fun for Me”) as shown in the hypothesized structural
model.
Although individual and home influences strongly affect attitudes toward science, their
influence is mediated by the science classroom (Simpson & Oliver, 1990). Talton and
Simpson (1986) found that classroom environmental variables were the “strongest predic-
tors of attitudes toward science in grade levels 610” (p. 366), and teachers had the largest
single impact on classroom variables (Simpson & Oliver, 1990). When a teacher is ani-
mated, enthusiastic, and excited about the subject at hand, students experience emotional
arousal that can change behavior (Bandura, 1986). In contrast, a facile, disengaged, and
critical teacher can crucially blunt student attitudes and interest. Thus, teachers help to
determine student behavior and attitudes toward school science. This important role of the
classroom teacher is reflected in the model as “Motivating Science Class” (arranged by the
teacher) and is positioned as an antecedent to the attitudinal “Science Is Fun for Me” and
the behavioral “Self-Directed Effort.
Whether attitudes influence behavior, or the other way around, has been notably difficult
to establish, in some part because of imprecise definition of terms (Simpson et al., 1994).
Behavior may be influenced by unmeasured attitudes that exert a stronger effect than
measured ones, and behavior may seem inconsistent with a given attitude if it is due
to a stronger, hidden motivation, or modified by expected and unexpected consequences
(Osborne et al., 2003). It is also important to make a distinction between attitudes toward
science in general and attitudes toward school science; the latter may be a better predictor
of student behavior (Osborne et al., 2003). Thus the items contained in the “Science Is Fun
for Me” scale may be revised in future studies to more clearly define the construct and may
result in a more clearly defined interaction with student “Self-Directed Effort.
Programs directed at increasing achievement motivation have not been generally suc-
cessful (Elliott & Bempechat, 2002; Elliott & Huffton, 2003) and yet motivating students
toward classroom goals is a major responsibility of teachers (Owen et al., 1981). Simpson
and Oliver (1990) found that attitudes toward science, science self-concept, and achieve-
ment motivation, all self-related factors, were strong predictors of student achievement
in science. The revised STAQ subscales “Science Is Fun for Me” and “Self-Directed
Effort” consist of items from these original subscales, as shown in Figure 1. The theo-
retical relationship among these factors may point the way toward further revision of the
STAQ.
Science Education
1092 OWEN ET AL.
Limitations of the Study
The original STAQ was intended to measure student attitudes toward science. In our data,
we discovered that a subset of STAQ items seems to assess additional constructs related to,
but different from, attitudes. For example, such constructs as “Peer Models” and “Family
Models” plainly focus on perceptions about peer and family behaviors rather than personal
attitudes. It is the task of additional research to study whether these restructured subscales
are better than, or redundant with, existing measures. In short, our dimensionality analyses,
and the subsequent elimination of certain items, have changed the nature of the STAQ. This
is an obvious limitation, in that our results cannot readily be compared to earlier STAQ
research. But it is also a psychometric enhancement, in that the original STAQ produced
doubtful data.
It is risky to assert causal relationships with cross-sectional data, especially when claimed
causes and consequences are measured at the same time. We acknowledge that our causal
links are simply hypotheses. As such, SEM allows them to be disconfirmed but not con-
firmed. Unfortunately, there is little hope of creating a true experimental design with such
data, because student perceptions cannot be randomly assigned. We recommend future
work with a longitudinal approach, where constructs are assessed on at least two occasions.
This sort of research design offers a stronger approach to evaluating causal claims.
In our data, we found increasingly lower self-reported scores as grade level increased
from sixth through eighth. Our finding roughly parallels that of Reid and Skryabina (2003),
who used a large national data set from Scotland. They suggested that schools and teachers
think about how to make science—and the teaching of science—more interesting and
engaging.
With cross-sectional data, it is incorrect to describe grade-level differences as showing
“declining” attitudes. However, George (2000) used a national longitudinal database that
authentically documented declines over time in student attitudes toward science, confirming
the work of Simpson and Oliver (1990) and Kaplan (2000). George also found that boys’
attitudes declined more sharply over time than those of girls. But because those attitude
measures were linked to specific science courses, there was no way to tell whether attitudes
declined generally, or whether taking specific courses (e.g., physics) was the influence.
We also point out that the sophistication and power of SEM is no substitute for logical
thinking and convincing arguments about causes and consequences. It is well known,
even with statistical evidence favoring a given model, that there are alternate (and usually
untested) models that fit the data just as well. Of course, researchers cannot evaluate
the entire universe of alternate models, and must eventually summarize a tentative but
plausible one. Additional research—with new data—is necessary to replicate or refine a
model of student attitudes toward science. And because all of our data refer to various
student perceptions, future research should include some objective measure of student
achievement, which is widely viewed as the most important outcome of schooling.
Finally, we remind readers that our data came from a restricted geographical and cultural
area, in south Texas and with schools whose populations are about 80% Mexican American.
Whether our findings generalize to other ethnicities or areas (including countries) remains
a topic for further research.
Recommendations
Our restructuring of the STAQ into five dimensions did not produce a conclusive instru-
ment. The multigroup structural model for gender was promising in that the factor loadings
could be forced to equality without harming the overall model fit. But the multigroup model
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1093
for grade level, even with no constraints imposed, did not show satisfactory fit. There are
thus additional questions about how much tinkering with items and subscales is needed to
show measurement invariance across grades 6, 7, and 8. We suggest caution to researchers
who use the revised STAQ with samples that span two or more grade levels.
We urge researchers who intend to use selected revised Simpson–Troost subscales to con-
sider improving their internal consistency reliabilities. One way to do this is by lengthening
the scales (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). This leads to an important research decision, be-
cause scale revision demands not only writing additional items but also pilot testing prior to
the main study. Researchers need to consider the trade-offs between instrument length, fea-
sibility of administering the instrument, and data completeness. Investigators must choose
instruments that adequately assess the study variables and defend their choices. If more
researchers chose such rigor, collectively, they would contribute to the development of more
dependable instruments for all (Blalock et al., 2008). In an environment that increasingly
stresses best-evidence practices and therefore effective evaluation strategies, it is imperative
that the research community take time to collect, analyze, and share extant scale data that
will ultimately lead to better assessment tools.
Structural equation analyses provided evidence for strong associations among the class-
room environment, student effort, and science affect. Classrooms are generally under the
management and control of the teacher and school leadership. Our findings raise the
potential for teacher professional development to improve the classroom and activities,
enhance student “Self-Directed Effort, and presumably improve science achievement. To
test this will require longitudinal data collection in controlled studies. The revised STAQ
provides a feasible instrument to evaluate the dynamic relationships of these variables over
time.
This work was supported by Science Education Partnership Award R25-RR-18549 (National Center
for Research Resources [NCRR] and the National Institute on Aging [NIA]), a Minority K-12
Initiative for Teachers and Students Grant R25-HL-075777 (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute
[NHLBI]) and MO1-RR-01346 for the Frederic C. Bartter General Clinical Research Center. The
NCRR, NIA, and NHLBI are all part of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
Wethank the administration, teachers, and students from Edgewood ISD, Northside ISD, NorthEast
ISD, San Antonio ISD, and South San Antonio ISD in San Antonio, Texas, and Hondo ISD in Hondo,
Texas. Our special thanks go to Linda Bononcini, assistant superintendent Edgewood ISD; George
Colon, principal and Teresa Gatell, language arts teacher at Truman MS; From Northside ISD thanks
to, Dr. Phil Linerode, evaluation specialist; Alice Fiedler, science curriculum specialist; John Folks,
superintendent; Priscilla Shaver, gifted/talented coordinator; Javier Martinez, principal and Lorenda
Segura, science teacher at Zachry MS; Lynn Pierson, principal, and Jose Luna, special education
teacher at Luna MS; From NorthEast ISD, we thank Richard Middleton, superintendent; Alicia
Thomas, associate superintendent; Mark Sheffler, associate superintendent; Don Dalton, executive
director, curriculum & instruction; Pattie Castellano, science curriculum coordinator; Francene Tharp,
health services; Randy Hoyer, principal and Tamisine Neal and Melissa Moody, science teachers
from Bush MS. From San Antonio ISD, our thanks go to Ruben Olivares, superintendent; Bill Vinal,
science director; Anita Chavera, principal, Josephine Rose, language arts teacher, A. Ahrenholtz,
M. Dovalina, J. Garza, E. Jordan, Carla Maldonado, C. Smith, John Stauffer, Adah Stock, and L.
Thomas, science teachers from Irving MS; Armando Gutierrez, principal, Elizabeth Aguilar-Cruz,
Deborah Friesenhan, and Francisco Lara, science teachers from Lowell MS. From South San Antonio
ISD, our thanks go to Evelyn Trinidad, director for mathematics and science; Thomas Fonseca,
principal and Elsa Gonzalez and Mary Ramos, science teachers at Dwight MS. From Hondo ISD, our
thanks go to Clyde Parsons, superintendent; Charles Carlson, assistant superintendent; Larry Carroll,
principal and Lee Ann Young and Cora Rothe, science teachers at Hondo HS.
We thank Olivia Lemelle for her graphic design assistance.
Science Education
1094 OWEN ET AL.
REFERENCES
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (1992). How schools short-change girls:
Executive summary. Washington, DC: Author.
Atwater, M. M., Wiggins, J., & Gardner, C. M. (1995). A study of urban middle school students with high and
low attitudes toward science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 665 677.
Bandura, A. (1986). Observational learning. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory (pp. 47– 105). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Blalock, C. L., Lichtenstein, M. J., Owen, S. V., Pruski, L. A., Marshall, C. E., & Toepperwein, M. A. (2008). In
pursuit of validity: A comprehensive review of science attitude instruments 1935 2005. International Journal
of Science Education, 30, 961 977.
Cannon, R. K. (1983). Relationships among attitude, motivation, and achievement of ability grouped, seventh-
grade, life science students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 1408. (UMI No. 8320073)
Cannon, R. K., & Simpson, R. D. (1985). Relationships among attitude, motivation, and achievement of ability
grouped, seventh-grade life science students. Science Education, 69, 121 138.
Cheng, Y. C. (1994). Classroom environment and student affective performance: An effective profile. Journal of
Experimental Education, 62, 221 238.
Elliott, J. G., & Bempechat, J. (2002). The culture and contexts of achievement motivation. New Directions for
Child and Adolescent Development, 96, 7 26.
Elliott, J. G., & Huffton, N. (2003). Achievement motivation in real contexts. British Journal of Educational
Psychology Monograph Series II, Number 2—Development and Motivation, 1, 155 172.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fraser, B. J. (1977). Selection and validation of attitude scales for curriculum evaluation. Science Education, 61,
317 330.
Gardner, C. (1992). Influences on attitudes toward science of nontraditional students. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 53, 3161. (UMI No. 9301195)
Gardner, P. L. (1975). Attitude measurement: A critique of some recent research. Educational Research, 17,
101 109.
George, R. (2000). Measuring change in students’ attitudes toward science over time: An application of latent
variable growth modeling. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 9, 213– 225.
Greenfield, T. A. (1997). Gender and grade level differences in science interest and participation. Science Educa-
tion, 81, 259 275.
Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2001). Rethinking construct reliability within latent variable systems. In
R. Cudeck, S. du Toit, & D. S¨
orbom (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: Present and future—A Festschrift
in honor of Karl J¨
oreskog (pp. 195 216). Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.
Hill, G. D., Atwater, M. M., & Wiggins,J. (1995). Attitudes toward science of urban 7th grade life-science students
over time, and the relationship to future plans, family, teacher, curriculum and school. Urban Education, 30,
71 92.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling. Concepts,
issues, and applications (pp. 76 99). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
InterAcademy Panel on International Issues. (2008). Science education. Retrieved February 12, 2008 from
http://www.interacademies.net/CMS/Programmes/3123.aspx
Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling: Foundations and extensions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Khalili, K. Y. (1987). A crosscultural validation of a test of science related attitudes. Journal of Research in
Science Teaching, 24, 127 136.
Koballa, T. R. (1988). Attitude and related concepts in science education. Science Education, 72, 115 126.
Kremer, B. K., & Walberg, H. J. (1981). A synthesis of social and psychological influences on science learning.
Science Education, 65, 11 23.
Lichtenstein, M. J., Owen, S. V., Blalock, C. L., Liu, Y., Ramirez, K. A., Pruski, L. A. et al. (2008). Psychometric
re-evaluation of the Scientific Attitude Inventory-Revised (SAI-II). Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
45, 600 616.
Lin, B., & Crawley, F. E. (1987). Classroom climate and science-related attitudes of junior high school students
in Taiwan. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24, 579 –591.
Maidon, C. H. (2001). A comparison of a fifth-grade elementary school science research-based curriculum
and an activity-centered traditional curriculum: effects on conceptual knowledge, process skills and attitude.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 63, 2497. (UMI No. 3059912)
Marshall, C. E., Blalock, C. L., Liu, Y., Pruski, L. A., Toepperwein, M. A., Owen, S. V., et al. (2007). Psychometric
re-evaluation of the Image of Science and Scientists Scale (ISSS). School Science and Mathematics, 107, 149–
160.
Science Education
REAPPRAISAL OF THE SIMPSON–TROOST 1095
Meece, J. L., Andermann, E. M., & Andermann, L. H. (2006). Classroom goal structure, student motivation, and
academic achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 487 –503.
Munby, H. (1980). An evaluation of instruments which measure attitudes to science. In C. P. McFadden (Ed.),
Worldtrends in science education (pp. 226 275). Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Atlantic Institute of Education.
National Academy of Sciences. (2006). Rising above the gathering storm: Energizing and employing America for
a brighter economic future. Washington, D.C.: The National Academies Press.
National Coalition for Women and Girls in Education. (2002). Title IX at 30: Report card on gender equity.
Washington DC: Author.
National Science Teachers Association. (1996). Position statement on international science education. Retrieved
on February 12, 2008, from http://www.nsta.org/about/positions/international.aspx
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Oliver, J. S. (1986). A longitudinal study of attitude, motivation and self-concept as predictors of achievement in
and commitment to science among adolescent students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 2983. (UMI
No. 8628904)
Oliver, J. S., & Simpson, R. D. (1988). Influences of attitude toward science, achievement motivation, and science
concept on achievement in science: A longitudinal study. Science Education, 72, 143 155.
Osborne, J., Simon, S., & Collins, S. (2003). Attitudes towards science: A review of the literature and its
implications. International Journal of Science Education, 25, 1049 1079.
Owen, S. V., & Froman, R. D. (2005). Why carve up your continuous variables? Research in Nursing & Health,
28, 496 503.
Owen, S. V., Froman, R. D., & Moscow, H. (1981). Educational psychology (2nd ed.). Boston: Little, Brown &
Co.
Owen, S. V., Toepperwein, M. A., Pruski, L. A., Blalock, C. L., Liu, Y., Marshall, C. E., et al. (2007). Psychometric
reevaluation of the Women in Science Scale (WiSS). Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44, 14611478.
Polit, D. F., Beck, C. T., & Owen, S. V. (2007). Is the CVI an acceptable indicator of content validity? Appraisal
and recommendations. Research in Nursing & Health, 30, 459 467.
Polit, D. F., Beck, C. T., & Owen, S. V. (in press). Item and scale content validity: Clarification and recommen-
dations. Research in Nursing & Health.
Reid, N., & Skryabina, E. (2003). Gender and physics. International Journal of Science Education, 5, 509 536.
Rennie, L. J., & Punch, K. F. (1991). The relationship between affect and achievement in science. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 28, 193 209.
Schibeci, R. A., & McGraw, B. (1981). Empirical validation of the conceptual structure of a test of science-related
attitudes. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 41, 1195 1201.
Simpson, R. D., Koballa, T. R., Oliver, J. S., & Crawley, F. E. (1994). Research on the affective dimension of
science learning. In D. Gabel (Ed.), Handbook of research in science teaching and learning (pp. 211 234).
New York: Macmillan.
Simpson, R. D., & Oliver, J. S. (1985). Attitude toward science and achievement motivation profiles of male and
female students in grades six through twelve. Science Education, 69, 511526.
Simpson, R. D., & Oliver, J. S. (1990). A summary of major influences on attitude toward and achievement in
science among adolescent students. Science Education, 74, 1 18.
Simpson, R. D., & Troost, K. M. (1982). Influences on commitment to and learning of science among adolescent
students. Science Education, 66, 763 781.
Spellman, J. E., & Oliver, J. S. (2001). The relationship between attitude toward science with enrollment in a 4 ×
4 block schedule. Proceedings of Annual Meeting of the Association for the Education of Teachers in Science
(pp. 3 30), Costa Mesa, CA. 3 30.
Talton, E. L., & Simpson, R. D. (1985). Relationships between peer and individual attitudes toward science among
adolescent students. Science Education, 69, 19 24.
Talton, E. L., & Simpson, R. D. (1986). Relationships of attitudes toward self, family, and school with attitude
toward science among adolescents. Science Education, 70, 365– 374.
Talton, E. L., & Simpson, R. D. (1987). Relationships of attitude toward classroom environment with attitude
toward achievement in science among adolescent tenth grade students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,
24, 507 525.
Turner, J. C., Meyer, D .K., & Schweinle, A. (2003). The importance of emotion in theories of motivation:
Empirical, methodological, and theoretical considerations from a goal theory perspective. International Journal
of Educational Research, 39, 375 393.
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2004). Trends in educational equity of
girls & women: 2004 (NCES 2005-016). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Wyer, M. (2003). Intending to stay: Images of scientists, attitudes toward women and gender as influences
on persistence among science and engineering majors. Journal of Women and Minorities in Science and
Engineering, 9, 1 16.
Science Education
... Attitude Questionnaire (Owen et al., 2008;Simpson & Troost, 1982) • Science self-concept ...
... • Perceived usefulness of school science for the student's future career. Blalock et al. (2008) conducted a review of 66 existing science attitude instruments and showed that many of these were lacking appropriate psychometric data to warrant generalised use. The majority of the studies that we considered ( for their use of their instrument and that the instrument has been used in replication studies. ...
... The warnings of Blalock et al. (2008), the wide range of instruments from which items have been drawn, and the range of response formats available, caused the adoption of a wide range of analyses and treatments-including some from outside the normal scope of educational research-in the quest to ensure methodological validity for the SSAS. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis investigates the interplay between students attitudes towards the science, technology, engineering and mathematics learning areas—the STEM subjects—as taught in Australian high-schools and their intentions to enrol in these subject areas in post-compulsory education. Falling enrolment rates in these areas have attracted much attention in recent years and there have been numerous strategies implemented in an attempt to reduce the declines. A common approach to addressing the problem of declining participation has been to devise strategies that target students’ attitudes towards the STEM subjects with the implication that positive attitudes increases the likelihood of continued student enrolments. Research findings have provided different insights into students’ intentions or enrolments, though there has been little conclusive evidence of an association between intentions and enrolments. Given the lack of clarity surrounding some of these issues apparent in the research corpus, this thesis takes a novel approach to address a number of methodological impediments to further progress in this field. In studying changes in students’ attitudes towards individual STEM subjects this study also considered students’ changes in attitudes towards the academic aspects of school more generally. This approach enables a measure of student attitudes to school subjects overall, to be used as a point of reference against which to assess attitudes specifically in relation to the STEM subjects. Further, while the majority of attitude studies have been cross-sectional—either surveying a single age cohort at a point in time, or surveying different age cohorts and drawing conclusions about differences—this thesis adopted a longitudinal approach to the research design. Finally, in contrast to common media rhetoric, enrolment trends and attitudinal patterns across the wide range of STEM subject areas were investigated and were not limited to just science and mathematics. This thesis explores the nexus between enrolments and attitudes through the format of two introductory chapters, four published and one unpublished journal articles, and a general discussion combining the conclusions of each article with each other and with the existing literature. The first two papers illustrate how patterns of student participation in the STEM subject areas in Australian high-schools have changed since 1992 onward. The data were then interrogated in search of common enrolment themes and correlations. It was found that the enrolment patterns and the scale of the changes in them were noticeably different for each of the STEM subjects indicating that they were each subject to separate underlying influences. The third paper presents the development, validation and use of the School Science Attitude Survey (SSAS). This digital survey instrument captures students’ self-measures of attitudes towards school subjects, across seven attitudinal constructs and enables changes in those attitudes to be investigated using Attitude Profiles and Attitude trajectories. The fourth and fifth papers make use of the SSAS data to investigate changes in attitudes for Year 7 students across science, mathematics and technologies, and to analyse differences in attitude trajectories for students in science as they proceed through Year 7 and 8. The SSAS also obtained qualitative data around changes to student attitudes that were analysed though the use of network maps. These revealed that among many explanatory factors offered by students, there were a small number of key explanations that were particularly central to the formation and refinement of attitudes towards STEM areas. Following this approach, this thesis shows that while participation rates in some STEM subject areas continue to decline, albeit slowly, other areas are either stable or experiencing mild growth. It is also shown that students’ attitudes towards STEM subjects are strongly interrelated with the nature of the attitudes that students hold for those subjects.
... These are termed subscales of attitudes on the attitude questionnaire used. These factors are, namely; interest of students ('science is fun to me' subscale), the role of teachers and classroom activities (motivating science class subscale), self-directed efforts by students, the influence of the student's family as well as the student's peer models (Owen et al., 2008). The interest of students, here, relates to the desire of learners to study science in school. ...
... The Revised Simpson-Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ-R) was used in collecting data for the study. This is a 22-item questionnaire that measures various aspects of the attitude of middle school students (the equivalent of junior high school students) (Owen et al., 2008). It is the shortened and revised form of the original 58-item Likert-type Simpson-Troost Attitude Questionnaire. ...
... The STAQ-R is a standardised instrument that is both reliable and valid. The Cronbach reliability coefficient, alpha, has been reported for the five variables of the STAQ-R to span between 0.70 and 0.81 (Owen et al., 2008). The authors reported a high content validity of the instrument. ...
Article
Attitude of students towards studying science is essential in their pursuit of academic success and development of interest to pursue science-related professions. The study investigated factors that affect attitudes of first-year Junior High School students toward the study of science in the Cape Coast Metropolis of the Central Region of Ghana. The standardised Revised Simpson-Troost Attitude Questionnaire (STAQ-R) was used to measure the attitudes of 162 students in seven basic schools. The results showed significant differences exist among ages of students in the same class (mixed-age enrolment) on overall attitudes toward the study of science. Also, teachers and classroom activities (motivating science class), self-directed efforts, family background, students’ interest and peer influence significantly contribute toward students’ attitudes toward the study of science. Gender does not significantly affect students’ attitudes toward the study of science. Overall, the participants possessed, on average, a positive attitude towards the study of science. Public education on gender equality in the study of science and the need for change in some socio-cultural practices that affect education might be yielding good results. School authorities and parents should continue to create the enabling environment that will help develop appropriate attitudes of students (wards) toward the study of science.
... With global scientific and technological growth occurring rapidly, declining student interest in science courses and careers is a worldwide concern that has prompted science education reform efforts on an international scale. Since student attitudes toward science effect course and career choices, measuring the impact of reform efforts on student attitudes is important and will require measurement tools with robust psychometric properties (Owen et al 2008). ...
... A questionnaire containing 30-item was used for this research which was based on Simpson-Troost Attitude Questionnaire-revised (STAQ-R). STAQ-revised is a revised form of Simpson-Troost attitude questionnaire that was developed by Owen et al (2008) to assess adolescent commitment to and achievement in science. STAQ has 22 Likert items that examines attitude towards science but we added 8 items to it regarding cultural and ethical properties of our country Iran. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This paper presents the results of a research concerning the application of educational software in classroom and its’ possible correlations with pedagogical considerations and teaching behavior towards the software. It’s been presented the application of the technical and computer environment features of the Verb Train software as well as some remarks concerning pedagogical perspectives during its’ application in classroom. More specifically, Verb Train is educational software that facilitates teaching and learning the conjugation of Ancient Greek verbs at secondary school level. The rationale stems from the need to codify verb conjugations in alternative ways and is based on contemporary teaching methods that can be used in the classroom and as home study. The structure and organization of the curriculum is based on three functions:LEARN & GRASP, PRACTISE & APPLY 1, and PRACTISE & APPLY 2. The program is accompanied by a CD-ROM Teacher’s Handbook, which contains teaching tips and instructions, an extensive bibliography and worksheets with educational activities that cover the whole range of the curriculum, along with three Student’s Books. School practice of this software showed a significant difference in the performance of students using it in order to understand and learn items of a “dead”, classic language compared to those who didn’t belong to a technology-oriented classroom. Important correlation between pupils’ scores in learning to conjugate ancient Greek verbs and their teachers’ perspective of ICT teaching has also been notified.
... As an important part of scientific literacy, attitudes towards science have been considered to not only influence students' science learning and performance at school but also their future career choice and attitudes and values towards science and technology in their adulthood, thus affecting their lifelong development and relevant social decisions T. Pell & Jarvis, 2001;Wyer, 2003). Therefore, cultivating students' positive attitudes towards science has been a longstanding concern in science education (Gardner, 1975;Oon & Subramaniam, 2018;Osborne et al., 2003;Potvin & Hasni, 2014), especially as the gap between a larger requirement of highly educated scientific talents and a decreasing number of students choosing to pursue science-related careers was internationally recognised (George, 2006;Hillman et al., 2016;Kind et al., 2007;Osborne et al., 2003;Owen et al., 2008). While many studies have been conducted on attitudes towards science in various cultural contexts, research at the secondary and postsecondary levels have drawn more attention as, seemingly, 'an immediate response' to the declining trend of students' enrolment in science courses and pursuit of related careers (Chang et al., 2009;T. ...
Article
The aim of this study was to develop an instrument to measure upper-level Chinese elementary students’ attitudes towards science by addressing problems with existing ones, as highlighted in the literature. The theoretical framework was drawn from the ABC model, which defines attitudes towards science as consisting of affective, behavioural, and cognitive domains in the study. Instrument development was performed through expert review, small-scale pilot tests and student interviews, large-scale field tests and validation. Factor analysis and Rasch analysis provided reliability and validity evidence. According to the results of the first round of the field test, changes were made to the items and the response category in the second round of the development procedure. The final instrument demonstrated good psychometric properties and consisted of four dimensions: enjoyment of science learning, self-efficacy in science learning, value of science learning and intention to learn and do science.
... Besides drawing upon the framework, the BRAINS instrument construct defined based on theoretical and empirical research on students' attitude toward science, and the development of items also derived from extant instruments through the process of review and modification (Summers & Abd-El-Khalick, 2018). Compared with other recent surveys such as My Attitude Toward Science by Hillman et al. (2016) and the Simpson-Troost Attitude Questionnaire revised by Owen et al. (2008), BRAINS was the more appropriate instrument to measure students' attitude toward science based on the strong theoretical framework employed, and it also could cover the students' attitudes across a broader range from fifth until tenth grade. The Global Korean Science Program (GKSP) we investigated in this study mainly aims to promote positive student attitudes toward science, and thus, because we aimed to measure changes in a group of students' science attitudes, BRAINS instrument with the framework of theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour were the most suitable theory for our goal. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the globalisation era, engaging youth in science and technology is a joint responsibility across nations. This study examined the effects of Global Korean Science Program (GKSP) on 608 Indonesian middle school students’ attitudes towards science. Their science attitudes were assessed before and after the programme with Behaviour, Related Attitudes, and Intentions towards Science (BRAINS) survey. We also collected students’ responses to an open-ended programme evaluation. The repeated-measures ANOVA was performed to compare students’ attitudes before and after the programme by including gender effect, followed by clustering analysis and magnitude coding to analyse open-ended programme evaluation data. Results showed that GKSP improved students’ behavioural and normative beliefs, suggesting that the programme had positive impacts on students’ beliefs about the importance of science in daily life. The decline in students’ control belief towards science after the programme indicates the possibility of science contents delivered in programme being more difficult than in the typical Indonesian science curriculum. Additionally, we found a significant gender effect on intention and normative dimensions. Furthermore, clustering analysis classified students into three groups; small changes (62%), increase trend (29%), and decrease trend (9%) indicating that most students participating in the study experienced a small science attitudes shift.
... interest in a career in the field of Natural Sciences, which explains the desire of students to continue their education to a higher level and apply the knowledge obtained in the field of Natural Sciences. [31] students' attitudes toward Natural Science affect the future of students and their careers. such as teaching in the field of Natural Sciences, working at the laboratory, becoming a scientist of Natural Sciences, and so forth. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this research is to know how the attitude of junior high school students to science mathematics with three indicators adopted from TOSRA are: 1) pleasure in learning science, 2) interest to increase study time of science, and 3) interest in a career in the science field. This research uses survey type research with design research is quantitative design. This research was conducted in several junior high schools in Muaro Jambi district with total 2815 of total students. The instruments used are attitude questionnaires and interview sheets. The data analysis used for quantitative data is the descriptive statistical type and reinforced by interview analysis. The result of the research shows that students with the indicator of pleasure in learning science, and interest in increasing the study time dominant IPA has a good attitude category, while the indicator of career interest in the field of IPA dominant has a fairly good attitude category.
Article
Full-text available
In the last decades, secondary school students have indicated a low interest in science and a lack of awareness of authentic science careers that may impede their aspiration to work in science-related fields in future. To raise students’ aspirations, several studies used context-based approaches, but few integrated career aspects into the school curricula. Accordingly, this study aimed to promote lower secondary school students’ interest in and awareness of science careers by introducing science career-related scenarios reflected in a real-world context as embedded careers education in science lessons. In this study, we explored the effect of the interventions on students’ interest, awareness, and aspirations towards science careers using Estonian, Finnish, and German datasets. According to the results, the students participating in the project indicated a higher interest in science, aspiration towards science careers, and awareness of future careers than those who did not experience the embedded science career interventions. Also, the results showed that when students got more information about science careers at school, their interest in science was more easily transferred to their aspirations in science studies and careers. Thus, this result emphasizes the importance of fostering awareness of science careers at lower secondary schools in order to inspire young learners to engage in science studies and works in future.
Article
Prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, many high school students were unaware of careers in public health—that there are many options available for those interested in improving health and preventing disease beyond being a doctor or a nurse. The Rutgers School of Public Health (R-SPH) developed PHocus (Public Health: Outbreaks, Communities, and Urban Studies) to introduce high school students to the interdisciplinary field of public health as well as to promote population and individual health. The PHocus Summer Experience was designed for high school students to explore population health and learn about the fundamentals of epidemiology, the breadth of topics addressed by public health, and public health careers. R-SPH hosted PHocus as 1-week sessions in Summer 2018 and 2019, available at both the School’s Piscataway, New Jersey, and Newark, New Jersey, locations. The Newark sessions targeted recruitment from high schools with underserved, minority, and/or economically disadvantaged students, and tuition for these students was supported through external funding. Across 2018 and 2019 PHocus Summer Experiences, 130 students participated, representing 63 high schools, in four 1-week sessions. The experience was rated very highly by participants; on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree), the median score for the program being a worthwhile experience was 4.0. R-SPH faculty, staff, and students, as well as external stakeholders, eagerly volunteered to be part of the program, suggesting that this educational program can be reproduced at other schools and programs of public health.
Chapter
Full-text available
El presente estudio examina el marco conceptual de 15 instrumentos de actitudes hacia la ciencia identificados en las bases de datos Web of Science y Science Direct, empleando los criterios PRISMA para el desarrollo de revisiones sistemáticas de la literatura. En conjunto, los resultados sugieren una escasez en la bibliografía de instrumentos conceptualmente válidos. La gran mayoría de los instrumentos examinados carecen de fundamento teórico, y presentan un amplio espectro de dimensiones que se emplean como reflejo de las actitudes de los individuos hacia la ciencia, aun cuando no se ha definido el concepto objeto de estudio ni justificado la inclusión de estas dimensiones. Estos hallazgos destacan la necesidad de avanzar hacia una conceptualización común sobre qué aspectos constituyen las actitudes hacia la ciencia.
Article
Full-text available
Se realiza una revisión sistemática de las propiedades psicométricas que poseen 15 instrumentos de actitudes hacia la ciencia publicados entre los años 2004 y 2016, identificados en las bases de datos Web of Science Core Collection y Science Direct siguiendo los criterios PRISMA. En conjunto, los resultados revelan que la calidad de la mayoría de los instrumentos actuales sigue siendo preocupante dada la ausencia casi generalizada de adopción de un marco teórico para el desarrollo de los ítems y debido a un deficiente e inadecuado análisis de sus propiedades psicométricas. Estos hallazgos cuestionan la validez y fiabilidad de los instrumentos analizados y ponen de manifiesto la necesidad de adoptar diseños metodológicos más rigurosos para el desarrollo de instrumentos de actitudes hacia la ciencia.
Article
Full-text available
This article offers a review of the major literature about attitudes to science and its implications over the past 20 years. It argues that the continuing decline in numbers choosing to study science at the point of choice requires a research focus on students' attitudes to science if the nature of the problem is to be understood and remediated. Starting from a consideration of what is meant by attitudes to science, it considers the problems inherent to their measurement, what is known about students' attitudes towards science and the many factors of influence such as gender, teachers, curricula, cultural and other variables. The literature itself points to the crucial importance of gender and the quality of teaching. Given the importance of the latter we argue that there is a greater need for research to identify those aspects of science teaching that make school science engaging for pupils. In particular, a growing body of research on motivation offers important pointers to the kind of classroom environment and activities that might raise pupils' interest in studying school science and a focus for future research.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this review is to present a comprehensive evaluation of science attitude instruments based on published psychometric evidence. A multitude of instruments have been used through the years and some have been linked to career choice and school performance. Substantiating such associations is of paramount importance if researchers wish to influence educational policy. However, associations are reduced, or hard to discover, if instruments have weak psychometric properties. Several databases were searched for peer‐reviewed articles that discussed the development and use of science attitude instruments. Instruments were grouped into the following categories: attitudes toward science, scientific attitudes, nature of science, scientific career interests, and other. A data abstraction and scoring rubric was used to summarize and evaluate 150 published articles that spanned 66 instruments. Most instruments had single study usage and showed an absence of psychometric evidence. This review demonstrated that there are few instruments available with the necessary psychometric data to merit recommendation. The review quantifies the current state of the research regarding the measurement of science attitude in students; the results should elicit further discussion and encourage more rigorous analyses of instruments. The findings may assist other researchers to select an instrument and alert them to its strengths and weaknesses. This review points the way forward for research in this field. Instruments already in existence should be used in repeat studies, and reliability and validity evidence should be collected and shared.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary research on gender and persistence in undergraduate education in science and engineering has routinely focused on why students leave their majors rather than asking why students stay. This study compared three common ways of measuring persistence-commitment to major, degree aspirations, and commitment to a science or engineering career-and emphasized factors that would encourage students to persist, including positive images of scientists and engineers, positive attitudes toward gender equity in science and engineering, and positive classroom experiences. A survey was administered in classrooms to a total of 285 female and male students enrolled in two required courses for majors. The results indicate that the different measures of persistence were sensitive to different influences but that students' gender did not interact with their images, attitudes, and experiences in predicted ways. The study concludes that an individual student's gender may be a more important factor in explaining why some female students leave their science and engineering majors than in explaining why others stay.
Article
The coalescence of oil drops in water are studied using acoustic levitation and stimulated with acoustic cavitation. The time required for two drops to coalesce, once they have been brought into proximity of one another, is one factor that affects emulsion stability. Unlike most earlier studies, which investigate the coalescence of a single drop with an initially planar interface, the use of acoustic radiation forces allows two drops to be brought into contact and allowed to coalesce. This acoustic technique has the advantage over other drop-drop coalescence systems in that the drops remain in contact until they coalesce without the use of solid supports to control them. Additionally, acoustic cavitation is observed to deposit sufficient energy in the oil-water interface to trigger the coalescence of a pair of 2 mm diameter drops. Some of the factors that affect spontaneous and stimulated coalescence are investigated.
Article
In this study, the relationship between student affective performance and classroom physical environment, social climate, and management style were investigated in a sample of classes in Hong Kong primary schools. The results of Pearson and canonical correlation analyses indicated that among the measures of classroom environment, perceived quality of physical environment and class master's expert power, personal power, and coercive power were the strongest predictors of affective performance. This finding supports the importance of class master's management style in the classroom environment. Students' attitudes toward school and teachers appeared to be most sensitive to variation in the classroom environment, and self-concept was the least sensitive among the seven student affective measures. Students' self-efficacy of learning and intention to drop out were moderately sensitive to classroom environment. Profiles of effective and ineffective classroom environments were also mapped. In effective classrooms, class masters care for students, pay attention to teaching, do not use force or punishment but do create a good classroom climate with their professional knowledge, personal morality, and personality. Physical environment and psychological environment are both important; a good classroom environment is highly correlated with student affective performance.
Article
The 70 items of the Test of Science Related Attitudes (TOSRA) were given to 1041 students who responded directly to them as attitude test items and to 39 teachers who were asked to assign the items to categories, without directions about either the number of categories to be used or the criteria to be employed in establishing them. Factor analysis of the item correlation matrix obtained from the student responses suggested that the seven subscales of the test were not distinct. Factor analysis of a joint proportions matrix for the items, obtained from the teachers' categorizations of the items, suggested that there are clear conceptual distinctions among the seven subscales even if not operational distinctions.
Article
Unlike many other countries, physics is highly popular in secondary education in Scotland, with large numbers opting for study at the Higher Grade on which entry to higher education is based. This paper reports a project that explored the attitudes and perceptions of Scottish girls and boys towards physics over the age range of 10-18 years old. Towards the end of primary school, attitudes towards science are very positive and both boys and girls are looking forward to studying more science in secondary school, although there is no evidence that the introduction of primary science has been a factor. By the end of the second year of secondary school, these positive attitudes have declined quite markedly and a significant decline of girls' attitudes towards science relative to boys' attitudes was clearly observed. The success of the Standard Grade physics course (a 2-year course taken in third and fourth year, ages 14-16 approximately) is easy to observe in terms of the restoration of positive attitudes of boys and girls again as the pupils move through third and fourth year. This process is especially clearly marked among girls. Surprisingly, over 90 per cent of the observed fourth year pupils wanted to continue studies in physics but a marked decline in attitude is observed during the Higher Grade course (a 1-year course which follows Standard Grade), this being more marked for boys. If the number of girls in physics is an issue for concern within the structure of Scottish system, then the focus of attention should be the structure and nature of the science course in the early secondary school.
Article
This study was conducted to determine changes in attitude toward science over time from the end of the first semester to the end of the second semester among seventh-grade life science students in a southeastern urban school system. The average attitude toward science of the 299 seventh graders changed from an undecided to a positive attitude toward science: 54 percent of the student sample remained the same in their attitude toward science, 46 percent had a change in attitude, 36 percent maintained a positive attitude toward science, and 20 percent of the student sample changed from a negative or undecided attitude to a positive attitude. Forty-four percent of the sample ended with a negative or undecided attitude toward science. Attitudinal subscales for this study included attitude toward the science teacher; science curriculum, and school. Significant relationships were found among the attitudinal subscales.