ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Purpose. P300 memory detection test is a neuroscientific procedure to assess memories stored in the brain. P300 memory detection can and is currently applied to assess criminal suspects on recognition of critical crime information. Contrasting memory detection with lie detection, researchers have argued that P300 memory detection does not involve deception. We empirically investigated this argument by manipulating deception between groups. Methods. Thirty-four community volunteers participated in a P300 memory detection test, answering either deceptively (deceptive condition) or truthfully (truth condition) to their own name. Results. P300 memory detection was significant in the truth condition, indicating that deceptive responding is not a prerequisite for valid P300 memory detection. However, there were clear indications that deceptive responding improved memory detection. Conclusions. Deception seems involved in the P300 memory detection test; and deceptive responding may add to test accuracy.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The role of deception in P300 memorydetection
Bruno Verschuere
1
*, J. Peter Rosenfeld
2
,Michael R. Winograd
2
,
Elena Labkovsky
2
and Roeljan Wiersema
1
1
Department of Psychology,Ghent University,Henri Dunantlaan, Ghent, Belgium
2
Department of Psychology,Northwestern University,Evanston, IL, USA
Purpose.P300 memorydetection test is aneuroscientific procedure to assess
memories stored in the brain. P300 memorydetection can and is currently applied to
assess criminal suspects on recognition of critical crime information. Contrasting
memorydetection with lie detection, researchers have argued that P300 memory
detection does not involvedeception. We empirically investigated this argument by
manipulating deception between groups.
Methods. Thirty-four community volunteers participated in aP300 memory
detection test, answering either deceptively (deceptivecondition) or truthfully (truth
condition) to their own name.
Results. P300 memorydetection was significant in the truth condition, indicating
that deceptiveresponding is not aprerequisite for valid P300 memorydetection.
However, there wereclear indications that deceptiveresponding improvedmemory
detection.
Conclusions. Deception seems involved in the P300 memorydetection test; and
deceptiveresponding mayadd to test accuracy.
The P300 memory detection test is aneuroscientific procedure to assess memories
stored in the brain. Memory detection has promising forensic potential. Amurderer can
be assessed on recognition of the murderweapon. Veracity of memoryfailure canbe
assessedin-patients whoare suspected of malingering.Recognitionofsecret
membership details can be assessed in terrorism suspects. Several laboratorystudies
found P300 memorydetection to be highlyaccurate in detecting crime memoryand
malingering (for areview see Rosenfeld, 2002). Memorydetection has often been
contrasted with lie detection. Agreat disadvantageoflie detection is that one cannot
measure lies directly.Insteadone measures the presumed physiological arousal that
accompanies stressoranxiety whenlying. Memorydetection, on the other hand, is
based upon recognition rather than deception. Pushing the lie detection versus memory
detection dichotomytothe limit, its was argued that deception is not involved in
*Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Bruno Verschuere,Department of Psychology,Ghent University,H.Dunantlaan 2,
B-9000 Ghent, Belgium (e-mail: bruno.verschuere@ugent.be).
The
British
Psychological
Society
253
Legal and Criminological Psychology(2009), 14, 253–262
q2009 The British Psychological Society
www.bpsjournals.co.uk
DOI:10.1348/135532508X384184
memorydetectionand that it ‘can (and should)bemeasured withoutdishonest
responding (Meegan, 2008, p. 18)’. Instead, we argue that deception may be involved in
memorydetection, and perhaps that it should be involved to obtain high accuracy.
P300 memory detection is based uponthe Concealed Information Te st (CIT;Lykken,
1959).The concealed Information Te st assesses memorybypresenting the examinee
with aseries of questions, each having one correct and several incorrect alternatives.
Autonomic nervous system (ANS) activitytothe alternatives is measured. Stronger
physiological responding to the correct items than to the incorrect items indicates
recognition. Amurder suspect, forexample, could be tested on recognition of crime
details such as the murderweapon: ‘Was it :::Agun? Abaseballbat? Arope? Aplastic
bag? Aknife?’. The Concealed Information Test relies uponasound theory and
demonstratedaccuracy (Ben-Shakhar &Elaad, 2003; Ve rschuere, Crombez, De Clercq, &
Koster,2004).Inthe eighties, neuroscientists have modified the Concealed Information
Test to allow examination of event-related potential (ERP). ERP studies use an oddball
paradigm, asking participants to classify stimuli in two categories: Target (1/6 of the
stimuli) versus non-target (5/6 of the stimuli). The target items are defined prior to the
task, during which theyrequire auniqueresponse. Compared to the non-target items,
the target items are infrequent and salient. Theyare expected to evoke aP300, apositive
ERP that peaks about400–900 ms after the onset of an infrequent and/orsalient
stimulus. The crucial manipulation is that to-be-assessed (‘probe’) items are embedded
within the non-target category. AP300 memory detection test in the murdercase above,
forexample, could involveasking the participant to press one keyfor ‘scissors’ and
another keyfor all non-target items (e.g.gun, baseballbat, rope, plastic bag, and knife).
Amongthe non-target items is the actual murderweapon (e.g.plastic bag).For the
innocent, the plastic bag will be anon-targetitem amongstthe others. The guilty,
however,will recognize the plastic bag as the actual murder weapon. Forhim/her,this
probe item is infrequent and salient, and thus will elicit alarger P300 amplitude and
longer reaction times (RTs) compared to the non-target items. At the individual level,
P300 memorydetectionoften reaches accuracy levels around 85–95% (Allen, Iacono, &
Danielson, 1992; Farwell &Donchin, 1991; Rosenfeld, Biroschak, &Furedy,2006;
Rosenfeld, Shue, &Singer,2007).
P300 memorydetection is based upon recognition rather than deception. There is,
however,noconsensus on the precise role of deception in P300memorydetection.
Reviewing the literature, we found three differenttheoretical positions on the role of
deception.
The first possibility is that deception is irrelevant and does not affect P300memory
detection. In their Psychological Bulletin review article, Bashore and Rapp (1993) take
such aposition whenstating that ‘it will not matter :::if the subject is told to be
deceptive or not (1993; pp. 12)’. Farwell and Smith (2001; pp. 8), who commercialized
P300 memory detection as ‘brainfingerprinting’, also argued that ‘the subject neither
lies nor tells the truth during the procedure, and in fact the results (:::)are exactly the
same whether the subject lies or tells the truth at any time’. However,itmay be
premature to conclude that deception is not involved. Classifying probes as irrelevant
items may be considered alie. Moreover,the main task of the participant is concealing
probe recognition, clearly involving deception. Thisanalysis suggests that deception is
involved in P300 memory detection.
The second possibility is that deception increases task difficulty,thereby decreasing
P300 probe amplitude. Indeed, anumber of studies have shown that P300 amplitude in
asecondarytask decreases with increasing workload of the primarytask (for areview
254 B. Verschuere et al.
see Kok, 2001).Toillustrate, Allison and Polich (2008) presented participants with
1000 Hz tones while playing afirstpersonshooter computer game. Comparedto
viewing the game setup screen, P300 amplitude to the tones was decreased when
activelyengaged in the game. Common interpretation of such finding is that the more
attention is allocated to the (primary) screen, the less attentionisallocated to the
(secondary) tones. Similarly, deception may increase workload, diverging resources
away from the oddball task and reducingthe oddball evoked P300. Thus, researchers
have arguedthat deception is detrimentalfor P300memorydetection (Meegan, 2008;
Rosenfeld, Biroschak, &Furedy,2006). Although deception may increase workload,it
may also increase attention to the probes therebyincreasing probe P300 amplitude
(Polich &Kok, 1995).
The third possibility is that deception may increase P300amplitudebyincreasing
probe saliency(see e.g. Allen, Iacono, &Danielson, 1992). One should keep in mind
that the primarytask of the participant is to classify stimuli as target versus non-target.
Whether stimuli are probes is irrelevant forthis task.Thus, the investigator assumes that
‘subjectsare sensitive to such seemingly irrelevant items if these items are distinctive in
some dimension that is important to the subject, even though it is not relevant to the
task the subjectisperforming (Farwell &Donchin, 1991; pp. 545)’. The more salient the
probesare, the greater the chance that the participant will attend to them and display a
P300. Some studies support the idea that deception may improve P300 memory
detection. First, ameta-analysis on 80 ANS memory detectionstudies explored factors
that moderated test accuracy (Ben-Shakhar &Elaad, 2003). It was found that overtly
answering ‘no’ to the stimuli (thereby lying to the probes) increased accuracy compared
to silentanswer tests. These data indicate that deceptive responding adds to the ANS-
based Concealed Information Test, and one could assume that this maygeneralize to the
P300-based modification of the Concealed Information Te st. Second, arecent study by
Meijer and colleagues (2007) is of relevance here. Theseauthorsperformed two studies
using facesasprobes. In afirstexperiment, ahit rate of 92% wasobtained when
participants wererequired to give deceptive responses to faces of friends/siblings.Ina
secondexperiment, participants were not explicitly instructed to give deceptive
responses. Ahit rate of only 17% was found forfaces of teachers. Results suggest that
deceptive responding adds to P300memorydetection accuracy.However,because the
two studies also differ in probe saliency (highly salient friend/sibling faces versus low
salientteacher faces) no firmconclusions can be drawn with regard to the role of
deceptive responding.
In sum, there are three differentpositions with regard to the role of deception in
P300 memorydetection. The first positionisthat deception is irrelevant and it is
predicted that deception does not affect P300 memorydetection(Bashore &Rapp,
1993; Farwell &Smith, 2001). The second position is that by increasing task difficulty
deception will decrease P3 amplitude, predicting worse P300 memory detection
performance when answering deceptivelycompared to answering truthfully (Meegan,
2008; Rosenfeld, Biroschak, &Furedy,2006). However,note that the latter prediction
is not that straightforward. Increasing workload may reduce P300 amplitude to
all stimuli, but does notnecessarily reducethe crucial probe-irrelevant difference.
The third position is that deception will increase probe saliency thereby increasing P300
amplitudetothe probes. Compared to truthfulresponding, deceptive responding
is predictedtoresult in better P300memorydetection performance by this view.
In the present study,weexaminethe role of deception by instructing half of the
participants to answer truthfully (truth condition), and half to answer deceptively
Deception and P300 255
(deceptive condition). This manipulationallows us to investigate whether and how
deceptive responding affects P300 memorydetection accuracy.Wepredicted better
probe versus irrelevant differentiationinthe deceptive condition than in the truth
condition.
Method
Participants
Forty-fourparticipants were recruitedthroughanagencyand also through
advertisements and flyers.
Apparatus
The master PC, running WINEEG(MitsarCorp.), recorded the EEG and RTs, and
triggered aslave PC running PSYTASK software (MitsarCorp.), to present stimuli to the
participants on the monitor.
Procedure
Participants were askedtotake partina‘Brain Wa ves and Deception’ study.Written
informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants werepresented with
the irrelevant items,which were common North American names consisting of 1to3
syllables. Items that resembled names that were of high relevance to the participant
(e.g. name of relative) were changed. Participants wereinformedthat theywere
required to watch names on the computer screen and to execute atask while doingso.
Participants were asked to press the right mouse button with the right middle finger
whenever theysaw the target name (CASEY) and to press the left mouse button with the
right indexfinger forall other items.
The crucial manipulationisthe way that the meaning of the buttons was explained.
Participants in the deceptive conditionwere told that: ‘You have been randomly
assigned to the deceptive condition. Yo uhave to trytoconcealrecognition of
meaningful information,inthis case your own first name. Yo uhave to pretend that
there’snothing special about this name. Yo ualso have to execute atask: Wheneveryou
see the target name, CASEY,you press the right buttonasfast as possible. Pressingthis
button means YES, Irecognize this name.For all other names, you press the left button
as fast as possible. Pressingthis buttonmeans NO,Idon’t recognize this name.You can
press YES only to the target name, CASEY.Thatmeans that you are obliged to press NO
to your own name. Clearly,this is alie, since youpress abutton that says youdon’t
recognize the name when in fact, youdo’. Participants in the truth condition were told
that: ‘You have been randomly assigned to the control condition. While watching the
computer screenyou will see several names.One of these will be the target name,
CASEY.Whenever you recognize the target name, CASEY,you press the right button as
fast as possible. Pressingthis buttonmeans YES,Irecognize this as the target name.For
all othernames,you will press the leftbutton as fast as possible. Pressingthis button
means NO,this is not the targetname.Weare using the names of all the participants in
this study,soyou may also see you own name, occasionally.Itisnot the target name, so
you also press NO to your name along with the other non-target names’.
The oddball task consistedof360 trials, with 60 repetitions of the participants’first
name, 60 repetitions of the target name, and 60 repetitions of each of 4irrelevant item
256 B. Verschuere et al.
(totaling 240 irrelevant names). Presentation was random with the exception that there
could be no consecutive trials of one name. The stimuli, about 1cmtall, were presented
as words in white capital lettersinArial font on ablack display monitor about 1mfrom
the subject’seyes. Stimuli were presented for300 ms, with the next stimulus appearing
2700 ms after offset of the previous stimulus. After the test, participants were paid and
debriefed.
Recording and scoring of the data
EEG was recorded with silver electrodes filled with adhesive paste attached to sites
Fz, Cz, and Pz. The scalp electrodes werereferenced to linked mastoids. EOG was
recorded with silver electrodes above and below the right eye, but laterally offset
from one another by 1cm. Theywere placed diagonallytopick up both vertical and
horizontal eye-movements. Impedance was kept below 10 k Vforall electrodes. The
EEG electrodes were referentially recordedbut the EOG electrodes were differentially
amplified. The forehead was grounded. Amplifier output was passed to an A/D
converter sampling at 500 Hz. Signals first passed an on line hardware filter (0.3–
30 Hz),and later an additional offline filter (0–6 Hz). The artifact rejectioncriterion
forEOG was 50mV. Trials were averaged per stimulus type. We used the peak-to-peak
method to calculateP300 amplitude. The algorithm first searches forthe positive
peak, which is the maximum 100 ms mean positivity in the 350–900 ms period post
stimulusonset. Next, the negative peak (or NEG) is determined, which is the the
maximum 100 ms mean negativity in the 1300 ms following the positive peak. P300
amplitudeiscalculated by subtracting the amplitudeofthe negative peak from that
of the positive peak.Ithas been repeatedly shown that this peak-to-peak method
is more sensitive forindividual classification than the baseline to peak method
(e.g. Soskins et al.,2001).
Results
Data from participants with too high anumber of behavioral errorsorEEG artifacts,
were excluded from further analyses ( n¼10).The final sample consisted of 16
participants (8 women) in the deceptive condition, and 18 participants (10 women) in
the truth condition. There werenosignificant group differences in gender,ethnicity,
handedness, time of last meal, length of name or medication use. The deceptive group
had alower mean agethan the truthgroup (Deceptive: Mage¼21 years [SD ¼3];
Truth: Mage¼27 years [SD ¼9]), t(32) ¼2.33,p,.05. Becauseage did not correlate
with any of the dependent variables,wedid not control forthis factor.
Separate 2(Condition: deceptive versus truth) by 2(Stimulus: probe
versus irrelevant) analyses of variance (ANOVA)were runonthe mean scorefor
each dependent variable (RTs, error rates, and the P300). We reportpartial eta squared
( h
p2
¼SS
effect
/(SS
effect
þSS
error
)) as an estimate of effect size.
To determine how manyparticipants showed areliable probe .irrelevant P300
difference, we usedthe bootstrap method(Wasserman &Bockenholt, 1989).For each
individual one has available only one averageresponsefor each stimulus type. To allow
statistical testingofprobe versus irrelevant differences within an individual, a
distribution is generated foreach stimulus type. First, aset of responses is drawn at
random,with replacementfrom the probe items.Mean P300 amplitude is calculated
from the drawn sweeps using the peak-to-peak method.The sameisdone forthe
Deception and P300 257
irrelevant items.The number of responses is the actual number of accepted trials for
that stimulus type forthat individual. Next, adifference score is obtained by subtracting
the irrelevant P300 from the probe P300. This process is repeated 500 times,resulting in
adistribution of 500 differences scores. If the mean difference score minus 1.29 times
the standard deviation is greater than zero, it can be concluded with 90% confidence
that the probe P300isgreater than the irrelevant P300. Asimilar procedure was used for
reactiontimes.
Errors
The 2(condition: deceptive versus truthful) by 2(stimulus: probeversus irrelevant)
ANOVA revealed asignificant main effect of stimulus, F (1, 32) ¼8.45, p,.01, h
p2
¼.21,
indicating ahigher error percentageonprobe compared to the irrelevant items,see
Table 1. No other effect reached significance, F’s ,1.7.
RTs
Error trials were excluded from RT analyses, as were RTs smaller than 150 ms or
greater than 800ms(,2% of all trials). The 2(condition: deceptive versus truthful) by
2(stimulus: probeversus irrelevant) ANOVA revealed amain effect of stimulus,
F(1, 32) ¼33.53, p,.001,h
p2
¼.51, indicating greater RTs to probe compared to
irrelevant items,see Table 1. There was amain effect of condition, F (1, 32) ¼9.93,
p,.01, h
p2
¼.24, indicating that participants in the deceptive conditionwere slower
than participants in the truth condition. Finally, there wasatrend towards stronger
reactiontime slowing on probe versus irrelevant items stimuli in the deceptive
condition compared to the truth condition, F(1, 32) ¼2.92, p¼.10, h
p2
¼.08. Hit rates
were similar in the deceptive (9 out of 16 participants or 56%) and the truth condition
(9 out of 18 participants or 50%),x
2
(1,34) ,1.
P300 amplitude
The 2(condition: deceptive versus truthful) by 2(stimulus: probeversus irrelevant)
ANOVA revealed amain effect of stimulus, F(1, 32) ¼21.38, p,.001, h
p2
¼.40,
indicating larger P300amplitudes to probe compared to irrelevant items, see Ta ble1
and Figure 1. The main effect of condition wasnot significant, F,1. Although, the
stimulus£condition effect was not significant in the ANOVA ,F,1.7, h
p2
¼.05, there
was aclear trend towardsbetter detection in the deceptive (12 out of 16 participants or
75%) than in the truth condition (8 out of 18 participants or 44%), x
2
(1,34) ¼3.26,
p¼.07.
Discussion
Exploring the role of deception in P300 memorydetection, participants either
answered truthfully or deceptivelytotheir own name. Twoimportant findings result
from this study: (1) That P300 memorydetection is valid in the truth condition, and (2)
that deceptive responding seems to add to P300 memory detection accuracy.
First, given that P300 memory detection was significant when participants answered
truthfully; deceptive responding seems not aprerequisitefor P300memorydetection.
At first, this finding seems to contrast with Meijer et al. (2007; Experiment 2) who found
258 B. Verschuere et al.
Ta ble 1. Mean percentage errors, reaction times and P300 amplitude for target, probe, and irrelevant itemsinthe deceptiveand the truthful condition
DeceptiveTruthful
Ta rget M(SD)Probe M(SD)
Irrelevant
M(SD)
Probe-irrelevant
difference
Ta rget
M(SD)
Probe
M(SD)
Irrelevant
M(SD)
Probe-irrelevant
difference
Errors (%) 7.08 (6.28) 1.04 (1.91) 0.03 (0.10) p¼.05 9.54 (10.41) 0.46 (0.96) 0.07 (0.21) p¼.05
RTs(ms) 541 (52) 483 (69) 451 (55) p,.001 473 (61) 418 (51) 401 (44) p,.004
P300 (m V) 17.69 (9.78) 14.72 (9.73) 8.64 (4.43) p,.008 16.55 (5.67) 12.64 (5.24) 9.44 (5.07) p,.002
Note.The probe-irrelevant difference arethe result from planned paired sample ttests.
Deception and P300 259
no significant memorydetection in students fortheir teacher’sfaces. An important
difference betweenthe present study and Meijer et al is that, we used highlysalient
probes. Basic P300research(Polich &Kok, 1995) and applied P300 memory detection
research(Rosenfeld, Biroschak, &Furedy,2006) has shown that probe saliency
increases P300amplitude. Ta ken together,our data indicate that recognitionofan
infrequent significantevent is necessaryand sufficient fordifferential responding in
P300 memory detection. Thesefindings are informative on when and in whom P300
memorydetection will work. Let’sconsider the Harrington case, whereP300 memory
detection was used to tryand exonerate aconvicted murderer (http://www.
brainwavescience.com). More than 20 years after the crime, Harrington was tested on
recognition of crime details such as ‘weeds and grasses’ on the escaperoute. The
examiner, Larry Farwell, concluded that ‘with a99.9% statistical confidencelevel that
the recordstoredinHarrington’sbrain does not match the crime scene’. In terms of
signal detection theory,Farwell concluded that lack of significant probe –irrelevant
discrimination was atruenegative test result. However,one could question how likely it
is that someone would remember apparenttrivial crime details formorethan 20 years.
The validityofthe P300memorydetection test is questionable because Harrington may
have forgotten crime details after 20 years. Moreover,probe items such as ‘weeds and
grasses’ may lack the saliency needed to elicit areliable P300. Stated differently, our
analysis of the necessaryconditions forsuccessful P300 memorydetectionindicates
that Harrington’stest outcome may have been afalse negative test result.
Second, our data indicate that deceptive responding may add to P300 memory
detection accuracy: the probe –irrelevant difference forRTs tendedtobegreater
with deceptive responding (32 ms) compared to truthful responding (17 ms). For
P300, there was aclear tendency towards higher hit rates with deceptive responding
(75%) compared to truthfulresponding (44%). Caution is warranted because statistical
tests didnot reach conventional levels of statistical significance.Wethink that our
findings are reliable because of three reasons.First, effects were found fortwo
different measures: RTs and P300 amplitude. Second, the currentfindings are in line
Figure1.P300 amplitude grand averages (expressed in m V) for target, probe,and irrelevant items in
the truthful condition (left) and the deceptivecondition (right). The first vertical line (at 100 ms)
indicates stimulus onset, the second vertical line (at 400 ms) indicates stimulus offset.
260 B. Verschuere et al.
with findings from ANS-based memorydetection where it was found that deceptive
responding adds to memorydetection accuracy (Ben-Shakhar &Elaad, 2003).Third, a
recent study by Nittono and Kubo (2008) also indicates that deception enhances P300
memorydetectionvalidity: P300amplitude was enhanced whenparticipants were
asked to hide recognition of the probes (deception) compared to acontrol condition
withoutdeception.
Taken together,our data indicate that P300 memorydetectionbenefits from
deceptive responding. Instructions in P300 memorydetectiontests should stress
deception. Thiscan be easily done by instructing the participant that pressing NO
means non-recognition, hence deception if one does recognize aparticular stimulus.
From an applied perspective, it is sufficient to know that explicit deception instructions
enhance differential probe-irrelevant responding. From atheoretical perspective it is
important to understand why deception affects P300 memorydetection. The workload
hypothesis predicted that P300 amplitude would be decreased by the greater task
difficulty when responding deceptively. Participants in the deceptive conditionwere
generally slower than participants in the truth condition, indicatingincreased task
difficulty.Still, P300was increased rather than decreased. We hypothesized that
deceptive responding increases probe saliency,hence, P300 amplitude. Future research
should investigate whether deceptive responding affectsP300 memorydetection
troughprobe saliencyorother (e.g.emotional or motivational) factors.
Acknowledgements
This researchwas supported by agrant from the ResearchFoundation –Flandersawarded to the
first author (Grant number K.2.144.07.N.01) and by agrant from the US Department of Defense
Polygraph Institute awarded to the second author (Grant Number DODP198-P-0001). We thank
Gershon Ben-Shakhar forhis valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
References
Allen, J. J.,Iacono, W. G.,&Danielson, K. D. (1992). The identification of concealed memories
using the event-related potential and implicit behavioral measures –amethodology for
prediction in the face of individual-differences. Psychophysiology,29(5), 504–522.
Allison, B. Z., &Polich, J. (2008). Workload assessment of computer gaming using asingle-stimulus
event-related potential paradigm. Biological Psychology ,77(3), 277–283.
Bashore, T. R., &Rapp, P. E. (1993). Are there alternatives to traditional polygraph procedures.
Psychological Bulletin,113(1), 3–22.
Ben-Shakhar,G., &Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psycho physiological detection of information
with the Guilty knowledgetest: Ameta-analytic review.Journal of Applied Psychology ,88,
131–151.
Farwell, L. A., &Donchin, E. (1991). The truth will out –interrogative polygraphy(lie detection)
with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology,28(5), 531–547.
Farwell, L. A., &Smith, S. S. (2001). Using brain MERMER testing to detect knowledgedespite
effortstoconceal. Journal of Forensic Sciences,46(1), 135–143.
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as ameasureofprocessing capacity.
Psychophysiology,38,557–577.
Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology ,43,
385–388.
Meegan, D. V. (2008). Neuroimaging techniques formemorydetection: Scientific, ethical, and
legal issues. TheAmerican Journal of Bioethics,8(1), 9–20.
Deception and P300 261
Meijer,E.H., Smulders, F. T. Y. ,Merckelbach, H. L. G. J.,&Wo lf, A. G. (2007). The P300 is sensative
to concealed face recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology ,66,231–237.
Nittono, H., &Kubo, K. (2008). The effect of intentional concealment on the event-related
potentials in aconcealed information test. International Journal of Psychophysiology ,69(3),
149–150.
Polich, J.,&Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300 –anintegrativereview.
Biological Psychology ,41(2), 103–146.
Rosenfeld, J. P. (2002). Event-related potentials in the detection of deception, malignering, and
false memories. In K. Murray (Ed.), Handbook of Polygrahtesting (pp. 265–286). UK:
Academic Press.
Rosenfeld, J. P. ,Biroschak, J. R., &Furedy, J. J. (2006). P300-based detection of concealed
autobiographical versus incidentallyacquired information in target and non-target paradigms.
International Journal of Psychophysiology,60,251–259.
Rosenfeld, J. P. ,Shue, E., &Singer,E.(2007). Single versus multiple probe blocks of P300-based
concealed information tests forself-referring versus incidentallyobtained information.
Biological Psychology ,74(3), 396–404.
Soskins, M., Rosenfeld, J. P. ,&Niendam, T. (2001). The case forpeak-to-peak measurement of P300
recorded at .3 hz high pass filter settings in detection of deception. International Journal of
Psychophysiology,40,173–180.
Verschuere, B., Crombez, G.,DeClercq, A., &Koster,E.H.W.(2004). Autonomic and behavioral
responding to concealed information: Differentiating orienting and defensive responses.
Psychophysiology,41,461–466.
Wasserman, S., &Bockenholt, U. (1989). Bootstrapping: applications to psychophysiology.
Psychophysiology,26,208–221.
Received 14 August 2008; revised version received 8October 2008
262 B. Verschuere et al.
... And, in forensic contexts, the oddball paradigm can be modified such that crime-related objects only known to the culprit (e.g., a murder weapon) are intermixed with unfamiliar objects. In this way, by hypothesis, only the culprit should evince stronger P300 when a crime-related object appears (50). In short, this approach is well-suited to investigation of both veracity and conscious perception. ...
Article
Full-text available
Even when brain scans fail to detect a striate lesion, functional evidence for blindsight can be adduced. In the aftermath of an automobile accident, JK became blind. Results of ophthalmic exams indicated that the blindness must be cortical. Nevertheless, multiple MRI scans failed to detect structural damage to the striate cortex. Prior to the accident JK had been an athlete; after the accident he retained some athletic abilities, arousing suspicions that he might be engaged in fraud. His residual athletic abilities—e.g., hitting a handball or baseball, or catching a Frisbee—coupled with his experienced blindness, suggested blindsight. But due to the apparent absence of striate lesions, we designed a series of tasks for temporal and spatial dimensions in an attempt to detect functional evidence of his disability. Indeed, test results revealed compelling neural evidence that comport with his subjective reports. This spatiotemporal task-related method that includes contrasts with healthy controls, and detailed understanding of the patient's conscious experience, can be generalized for clinical, scientific and forensic investigations of blindsight.
... And, in forensic contexts, the oddball paradigm can be modified such that crime-related objects only known to the culprit (e.g., a murder weapon) are intermixed with unfamiliar objects. In this way, by hypothesis, only the culprit should evince stronger P300 when a crime-related object appears (50). In short, this approach is well-suited to investigation of both veracity and conscious perception. ...
Article
Full-text available
Even when brain scans fail to detect a striate lesion, functional evidence for blindsight can be adduced. In the aftermath of an automobile accident, JK became blind. Results of ophthalmic exams indicated that the blindness must be cortical. Nevertheless, multiple MRI scans failed to detect structural damage to the striate cortex. Prior to the accident JK had been an athlete; after the accident he retained some athletic abilities, arousing suspicions that he might be engaged in fraud. His residual athletic abilities—e.g., hitting a handball or baseball, or catching a Frisbee—coupled with his experienced blindness, suggested blindsight. But due to the apparent absence of striate lesions, we designed a series of tasks for temporal and spatial dimensions in an attempt to detect functional evidence of his disability. Indeed, test results revealed compelling neural evidence that comport with his subjective reports. This spatiotemporal task-related method that includes contrasts with healthy controls, and detailed understanding of the patient's conscious experience, can be generalized for clinical, scientific and forensic investigations of blindsight.
... Over the past few years, the frontal negative slow wave (FNSW) has been discussed in the Concealed Information Test (CIT) as well as consumer neuroscience research. The CIT is used for memory detection research (Verschuere et al., 2011) and is mainly done to investigate whether a perpetrator knows crime-relevant information by using physiological responses. During the CIT, examinees are presented a crime-relevant item and several crime-irrelevant items. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is a connection between the frontal negative slow wave (FNSW) and the arousal inhibition in the hedonic purchase context. To calculate the FNSW (400–800 ms), event-related potentials (ERPs) method was applied to depict the neural substrates on prudent and impulsive consumers’ behaviors within various states of promotion. Promotion types include the pure price promotion and the mixed promotion (a mixture of a charitable donation and a discount). Behaviorally, consumers response more quickly in the pure price promotion condition and they express a preference for the mixed promotion. More importantly, a larger FNSW emerged in the impulsive consumers than the prudent, suggesting that the former might tend to control their eagerness to consume hedonic items. Compared with the price promotion as the worse option, the mixed promotion as the better option caused more perceptual conflict, leading to an increase in N2 amplitude. It suggests that consumers incline to reject the worse offers. These results also reveal that people primarily have to search negative promotion information by their insight and subsequently impulsive consumers inhibit the responses to the promotion information. The method of ERPs and FNSW should be helpful for marketing researchers and professionals on hedonic consumption and sales promotion.
Article
Deception often occurs in response to a preceding cue (e.g., a precarious question) alerting us about the need to subsequently lie. Here, we simulate this process by adapting a previously established paradigm of intentionally false responding, now instructing participants about the need for deception (vs. truthful responses) by means of a simple cue occurring before each response‐relevant target. We analyzed event‐related brain potentials (ERPs) as well as cortical oscillations recorded from the scalp. In an experimental study ( N = 44), we show that a cue signaling the need for deception involves increased attentional selection (P2, P3a, P3b). Moreover, in the period following the cue and leading up to the target, ERP and oscillatory signatures of anticipation and preparation (Contingent Negative Variation, alpha suppression) were found to be increased during trials requiring a deceptive as compared to a truthful response. Additionally, we replicated earlier findings that target processing involves enhanced motivated attention toward words requiring a deceptive response (LPC). Moreover, a signature of integration effort and semantic inhibition (N400) was observed to be larger for words to which responses have to be intentionally false as compared to those to which responses must be truthful. Our findings support the view of the involvement of a series of basic cognitive processes (especially attention and cognitive control) when responses are deliberately wrong instead of right. Moreover, preceding cues signaling the subsequent need for lying already elicit attentional and preparatory mechanisms facilitating the cognitive operations necessary for later successful lying.
Article
Full-text available
The present work is one of the conclusions of the ongoing work on Biological Signal Processing (BSP), started in 2003 in collaboration with Bhabha Atomic Research Center (BARC), Mumbai that could evince the underlying primal qualia of the Working Human Mind in Real-Time, Chaos, a seeming order within a disorder, the prelude and premise of Universe (published in 2009), through signal processing of electroencephalographic (EEG) time-series. Subsequently, in the year 2012, the trend-setting discovery of the Dysfunctional Mirror Neurone System (MNS), also known as Broken Mirrors of Professor V. S Ramachandran, in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) was invited to be presented at Nature Medicine Translational Neurosciences Symposium (TNS) at Lucerne, Switzerland. The processing of EEG signals along non-linear domain in varied disease forms of Migraine, Alzheimer's, Epilepsies, Deception, Dyslexia, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and in normal young adults during the use of diverse paraphernalia of language inclusive of vowels, consonants and phonemes evinced a qualified distinctive pattern that could be of potential use both in therapeutics and prognostication.
Article
Full-text available
Unlabelled: We conducted (I) 18 event-related potential (ERP) field tests to detect concealed information regarding major terrorist crimes and other real-world crimes and (II) 5 ERP tests regarding participation in a classified counterterrorism operation. This study is a test of the brain fingerprinting scientific standards hypothesis: that a specific set of methods for event-related potential (ERP) concealed information tests (CIT) known as the brain fingerprinting scientific standards provide the sufficient conditions to produce less than 1% error rate and greater than 95% median statistical confidence for individual determinations of whether the tested information is stored in each subject's brain. All previous published results in all laboratories are compatible with this hypothesis. We recorded P300 and P300-MERMER ERP responses to visual text stimuli of three types: targets contain known information, irrelevants contain unknown/irrelevant information, and probes contain the situation-relevant information to be tested, known only to the perpetrator and investigators. Classification CIT produced significantly better results than comparison CIT, independent of classification criteria. Classification CIT had 0% error rate; comparison CIT had 6% error rate. As in previous studies, classification-CIT median statistical confidences were approximately 99%, whereas comparison CIT statistical confidences were no better than chance for information-absent (IA) subjects (who did not know the tested information). Over half of the comparison-CIT IA determinations were invalid due to a less-than-chance computed probability of being correct. Experiment (I) results for median statistical confidence: Classification CIT, IA subjects: 98.6%; information-present (IP) subjects (who know the tested information): 99.9%; comparison CIT, IA subjects: 48.7%; IP subjects: 99.5%. Experiment (II) results (Classification CIT): error rate 0%, median statistical confidence 96.6%. Countermeasures had no effect on the classification CIT. These results, like all previous results in our laboratory and all others, support the brain fingerprinting scientific standards hypothesis and indicate that the classification CIT is a necessary condition for a reliable, accurate, and valid brainwave-based CIT. The comparison CIT, by contrast, produces high error rates and IA statistical confidences no better than chance. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11571-022-09795-1.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The Response Time Concealed Information Test (RT‐CIT) can reveal when a person recognizes a relevant item among other irrelevant items, based on comparatively slower responses. Therefore, if a person is concealing knowledge about the relevance of this item (e.g., recognizing it as a murder weapon), this deception can be revealed. A recent study introduced additional ‘familiarity‐related fillers’, and these items substantially enhanced diagnostic efficiency in detecting autobiographical data. However, the generalizability of the efficiency of fillers to other scenarios remains an open question. We empirically investigated whether new importance‐related fillers enhanced diagnostic efficiency in an imaginary crime scenario. Methods Two hundred and thirty‐nine volunteers participated in an independent samples experiment. Participants were asked to imagine either committing a crime (‘guilty’ group) or to imagine visiting a museum (‘innocent’ group). Then, all participants underwent RT‐CIT testing using either a standard single probe or an enhanced single probe (with importance‐related fillers) protocol. Results The enhanced RT‐CIT (with importance‐related fillers) showed high diagnostic efficiency (AUC = .810), and significantly outperformed the standard version (AUC = .562). Neither dropout rates nor exclusion criteria influenced this enhancement. Conclusions Importance‐related fillers improve diagnostic efficiency when detecting episodic information using the RT‐CIT and seem to be useful in detecting knowledge in a wide range of scenarios.
Article
Many studies on concealed knowledge involve mock-thefts. The present study compares ERPs of participants concealing knowledge of a morally negative behavior (mock-theft) with ERPs of participants concealing knowledge of a morally positive behavior. Some participants (n= 33) stole a candy box out of an office, whereas others (n= 28) put the candy box into an office as a present. During a concealed information test, participants concealed knowledge of the candy box and a key they had seen in the office (probe stimuli) and honestly indicated not knowing similar irrelevant stimuli. P300 s were enlarged for probe, compared to irrelevant stimuli in both conditions, revealing that probe stimuli were more salient than irrelevant stimuli regardless of their moral valence. Likewise, medial frontal negativities were enlarged for probe versus irrelevant stimuli in both conditions, indicating response conflicts when answering deceptively to probe items in both situations.
Article
Research on deception focused on the investigation of event-related potentials (ERP) to facilitate our knowledge on cognitive-motivational processes underlying deception. We investigated the reliability of the frontal N2 amplitude in a modified Concealed Information Test (CIT) (N = 64 participants). Variations of Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients were examined for number of epochs per stimulus type, task modality and types of ERP quantification. Cronbach's Alpha coefficients of the N2 increased with the number of epochs across task modality, stimulus types and electrode positions. In a pictorial CIT, the frontal N2 indicated excellent Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients (≥ .90) across electrode positions and quantification methods for 25 epochs per stimulus type. At least 15 epochs for an excellent Cronbach’s Alpha coefficients (≥ .90) of the peak-to-peak N2 in a modified CIT across electrode positions and stimulus types. We provide best-practice advises for the necessity to assess N2 in a modified CIT with excellent reliability.
Article
Full-text available
Kindergarten, Grade 2, Grade 4, and adult subjects viewed a brief video of two children arguing about the use of a bicycle. One week later subjects were asked for their free recall of the events in the video followed by sets of hierarchically arranged, increasingly suggestive questions that suggested a correct (positive-leading), an incorrect (misleading), or no specific (unbiased-leading) answer, with the final level of questioning for each item being a three-alternative multiple-choice question. Correct free recall varied with age, with the kindergarten and Grade 2 children generally following the lead of the first-level questions more so than the older subjects. Older children were as accurate as adults in responding to questions about the central items, but not so for noncentral items. Developmental differences were found in responses to repeated suggestive questioning, with kindergarten children following misleading questions and changing answers more often than older subjects. On the final, multiple-choice questions, kindergarten children were able to provide the correct answer as often as they had to the initial questions, despite intervening errors. Findings are discussed in terms of the type of questions presented, the repetition factors, and the opportunities for subjects changing their answers in response to subsequent questions about the same item.
Article
Full-text available
In the present experiment, we analysed court transcripts in which children aged 5 to 13 years provided the key evidence in sexual abuse trials. We developed two separate coding schemes for lawyers' questions and children's responses. Consistent with past research, defence lawyers conducting cross-examination asked a higher proportion of complex, grammatically confusing, credibility-challenging, leading, and closed questions than prosecution lawyers. In responding to defence lawyers' questions, child witnesses rarely asked for clarification and often attempted to answer questions that were ambiguous or did not make sense. Furthermore, over 75% of children changed at least one aspect of their testimony during the cross-examination process. These findings have important implications for the way in which children are examined in court.
Article
Full-text available
Eyewitness researchers have long argued that children's testimony is unreliable because of their excessive suggestibility. The present study is an examination of two factors thought to explain some of the apparent suggestibility of children: (1) the emphasis placed on giving a classifiable answer to every question asked; (2) the effects of repeated questioning within an interview. A total of 72 children, from three age groups (6, 8 and 10 years) was questioned about a staged event they had witnessed earlier. It was found that telling children that they could say 'Don't know' to questions increased the number of such responses, but with no effect on the overall proportion of correct or incorrect responses. Even subjects who had not been instructed in the use of this response were prepared to say 'Don't know' to the experimenter's questions. Repeated questioning resulted in a decrease in the number of correct responses given, but had no effect on the number of incorrect re sponses.
Article
One can discern two parallel trends in the law and the psychology of child witnesses. In the law, appellate courts are beginning to stem the once powerful movement to increase the acceptance of children's testimony and the admissibility of children's out-of-court statements. Lyon analyzes particular strands of each trend.
Article
This paper presents the statistical technique known as the bootstrap to the general audience of psychophysiologists. The bootstrap, introduced by Efron (1979), allows data analysts to study the distribution of sample statistics that might otherwise be too complicated to consider. The technique, which requires simple calculations, involves drawing repeated samples (with replacement) from the empirical--or the actual--data distribution and then building a distribution for a statistic by calculating a value of the statistic for each sample. The bootstrap can be used to obtain confidence intervals, standard errors, and even higher moments for the statistic. It is similar to the well-known jackknife of Quenouille and Tukey. After discussing the history and theory of both the bootstrap and the jackknife, we illustrate the use of the bootstrap in the statistical analysis of correlation coefficients and the general linear model.