ArticlePDF Available

Perceived Symbols of Authority and Their Influence on Compliance1

Authors:

Abstract

There are many variables that influence compliance. With regard to individuals making requests of others, Bickman (1974) found that the apparel of the person making the request significantly influenced whether another person complied with the request. This study evaluates other factors such as sex, age, and altruism in compliance. Subjects were involved in a replication of Bickman's dime and parking meter study. Results showed that the dress of the perceived authority not only affected the number of subjects who complied but also the type of compliance, the type of noncompliance, and the latency between request and compliance. Also, older subjects complied significantly more often than younger subjects in the role authority condition.
Perceived Symbols of Authority and Their Influence
on Compliance’
BRAD
J.
BUSHMAN’
Weber
State
College
There are many variables that influence compliance. With regard to individuals
making requests of others, Bickman
(1974)
found that the apparel of the person
making the request significantly influenced whether another person complied
with the request. This study evaluates other factors such as
sex,
age, and altruism
in compliance. Subjects were involved in a replication of Bickman’s dime and
parking meter study. Results showed that
the
dress of the perceived authority
not only affected the number
of
subjects who complied but
also
the type
of
com-
pliance, the type of noncompliance, and the latency between request and compli-
ance.
Also,
older subjects complied significantly more often than younger sub-
jects in
the
role authority condition.
Our initial perceptions of an individual’s authority may be largely determined
by apparel. This is especially true when an individual is wearing a uniform be-
cause uniforms make the wearer’s status much more visible (Joseph
&
Alex,
1972). Uniforms have been found to influence honesty (Bickman, 1971), help-
ing behavior (Emswiller, Deaux,
&
Willits, 1971; Raymond
&
Unger, 1971),
political behavior (Suedfeld, Bochner,
&
Matas, 1971
;
Zimbardo, 1971), aggres-
sion (Borden, 1975), and compliance (Bickman, 1974). Bickman (1974) con-
ducted several field studies on the influence
of
uniforms on compliance. Bick-
man’s studies involved individuals with three levels of perceived authority: a
civilian, a milkman, and a guard. Bickman found that when requests were made
from an individual who was perceived as an authority, compliance was indeed
higher.
Bickman’s (1974) study dealt with the variable of perceived symbols of au-
thority. In determining the reasons for compliance one may also wish to con-
sider other variables, besides perceived symbols of authority. In this study, age,
altruism, and how apparel affected compliance, noncompliance, and the latency
to comply were investigated.
‘The author would like to thank Merrill May for his helpful comments on earlier drafts
’Requests for reprints should be sent to Brad
J.
Bushman,
3986
North
900
West, Ogden,
of this article and for providing assistance with the statistical analysis of the data.
Utah
84404.
501
Journal
of Applied
Social
Psychology,
1984,
14,
6.
pp.
501 -508.
Copyright
0
1984.
V.H.
Winston,
&
Sons, Inc.
All
rights reserved.
502
BRAD
J.
BUSHMAN
Method
Subjects
Subjects were 150 adult pedestrians on a major street in downtown Salt Lake
City, Utah. The study was done on a warm, clear Saturday in May to increase
the likelihood of
a
representative sample of the population being available. Sev-
eral pedestrians were present
for
the duration of the experiment. The subject
pool was limited to pedestrians between the ages of
16
and
70.
Because of the
heterogeneity
of
subjects at any given time, a quasi-random stratified sampling
procedure was used. Selection was based on the demographic characteristics
of
age, sex, race, and dress.
Between the selection of one subject and the next there was a delay, such
that the following subject could not have observed the interaction between the
confederate and the previous subject. Overall, 45%
of
the subjects were female
and 55% were male. Seventy-one percent were white,
9%
black, and the race
of
the remainder
(20%)
could not be determined. Most subjects were judged, by
their apparel, as middle-class. Postexperiment analysis of subjects’ demographics
showed no significant differences between subjects within each of the three con-
ditions (no authority, status authority, and role authority).
Design
This experiment was a field study, functional design with three levels
of
the
independent variable
:
no authority, status authority, and role authority. In the
no authority condition the confederate was dressed as a bum, was unshaven, and
wore an old pair
of
greasy coveralls, an old baseball type hat, and old workshoes.
In the status authority condition, the confederate dressed as a business execu-
tive, was shaven, wore a conservative two-piece business suit, white shirt, a con-
servative tie, and dress shoes. In the role authority condition, the confederate
was dressed as a fire fighter, and wore a fire fighter’s uniform that included a
medium blue shirt, dark blue pants, and a black hat. The shirt had a patch on
the sleeve designating the fire department (Ogden City) and a silver fire fighter
badge
on
the pocket. The hat also had
a
silver badge in the center.
The confederate was male,
47
years old, 5ft 11 in. tall
(1.8
m)
and weighed
210 lb (95.45 kg). Bickman’s (1974) experiment used four different confeder-
ates
of
similar physique. This experiment used the same confederate for all three
conditions to control for variables associated with the person making the request.
Bickman’s confederates were between the ages
of
18 and
20.
In this experiment
an older confederate was used to increase ecological validity to be consistent
with the assumption that authority figures are rarely young.
The person in need of a dime was the experimenter, a 23-year-old college
PERCEIVED SYMBOLS
OF
AUTHORITY
AND
COMPLIANCE
503
male student, 5 ft 10 in. tall
(1.78
m) and 135 lb (61.36 kg), who was dressed in
blue jeans and a casual shirt during the data collection.
The dependent variable, compliance, was defined as the subject giving the ex-
perimenter a dime
(or
other change if the subject did not have a dime). The
type of compliance was determined by a posttest interview conducted by the
experimenter. The behavior of those who complied was divided into four cate-
gories: altruism, compliance, unquestioned obedience,
or
ambiguous.
1.
AZtruism.
The subject complied because he or she wanted to help some-
one in need.
2.
Compliance.
The subject complied because he or she hoped to achieve a
favorable reaction from the experimenter, the confederate, or both. The sub-
ject’s response could have been dual in nature; that is, the subject wanted to
comply to the confederate’s request and help someone at the same time.
3.
Unquestioned obedience.
The subject complied because “He [the confed-
erate] told me to.”
4.
Ambiguous.
The experimenter could not determine why the subject com-
plied because his or her response was vague.
The reasons for noncompliance were divided, by the confederate, into four
categories: no change, questioned perceived authority, silent, and hostile.
1.
No
change.
The subject said he
or
she did not have any change.
2.
Questioned perceived authority.
The subject asked the confederate such
3.
Silent.
The subject did no* rcply to the confederate’s request.
4.
Hostile.
The subject responded to the confederate’s request in a hostile
manner (e.g., “Are you kidding? There’s no way I’m going to give him any
change
!
”)
questions as, “Why don’t you give him a dime?”
Procedure
The general procedure used was similar to Bickman’s
(1974)
study. The con-
federate stopped the chosen subject and pointed to the experimenter who was
standing beside a car, parked at an expired parking meter, searching in his pock-
ets for change. After pointing at the experimenter, the confederate said, “This
fellow is overparked at the meter but doesn’t have any change. Give him a
dime!” If the subject did not immediately comply, the confederate added that
he had no change either.
If
the subject did not comply after the explanation,
the confederate left.
To ensure an accurate and reliable recording of the data, the confederate re-
corded specific information about each subject after he
or
she left the vicinity.
This was accomplished by using the checklist in Figure
1.
If the subject did comply, the experimenter debriefed
him
or
her. The de-
briefing procedure went as follows: The experimenter asked the subject “Why
N-r
PQ,
NQ)
-
~lnl
Gender
Subject
Number
Estimated
Aae
t
II
No Change
White-W
Unknown-U
Race
(
Black-B
g2
Work
Clothes
Casual
Clothes
Semiformal
Clothes
Formal
Clothes
t
t
Color of Ss
Blouse or
Shirt
Questioned
Perceived Authority
Silent
Hostile
Latency (Fast-F,
Medium-M,
Slow-S)
t
Complied
A
Q)
Q)
n
s
1
11
Altruistic
I
Subject
Number
Estimated
~Zi?
5
zz
Age
20
43
Gender
g7J
n
White-W
Race
(
Black-B
Unknown-U
W
0
Casual
2.
D
CW
~
(DW
Work
Clothes
4
Semiformal
a4
Clothes
9'0
3
Formal
Clothes
v
Clothes
C
cn
Compliance
Unauestioned
Obedience
Ambiguous
Color
of
Ss
Blouse or
Shirt
506
BRAD
J.
BUSHMAN
would you just come over here and give me a. dime?” If the subject did not
respond clearly, the experimenter attempted to clarify the response. The experi-
menter then returned the subject’s dime and briefly explained the nature of the
experiment. After the subject left, the experimenter completed the checklist in
Figure 2.
After collecting the data for each condition, the experimenter and the con-
federate compared descriptions
of
the subjects in terms of estimated age, race,
and status
(as
indicated by apparel).
Results
The results indicate that compliance significantly increased as perceived au-
thority increased, x2(2,
N
=
150)
=
17.10,
p
<
.001.
Forty-five percent of the
subjects obeyed the bum,
50%
the business executive, and
82%
the fire fighter.
As
judged by the subject’s verbal responses, altruistic reasons given for com-
plying were significantly less as perceived authority increased,
xz
(6,
N
=
88)
=
26.60,
p
<
.001.
Although
50%
of
the reasons given for obeying were altruistic
in the
no
authority condition, 16% were altruistic in the status authority condi-
tion, and
10%
were altruistic in the role authority condition. Furthermore, 64%
of the reasons given for compliance were classified as “unquestioned obedience”
in the role authority condition,
48%
were classified as unquestioned obedience
in the status authority condition, and
23%
were classified as unquestioned obedi-
ence in the
no
authority condition.
Noncomplying subjects offered significantly fewer hostile reasons for
non-
compliance as perceived authority increased, ~’(4,
N
=
62)
=
14.66,
p
<
.05.
Twenty-nine percent of the subjects gave hostile responses in the
no
authority
condition, whereas
11%
gave hostile responses in the role authority condition.
Thirty-two percent of the subjects said they would have given the experimenter
a dime if they had change
in
the
no
authority condition, whereas
89%
of the
Table
1
Percentage
of
Subjects Complying
in
Each
Condition
Condition Compliance
N
%
Noncompliance
N
%
No authority
22
44
28 56
Status authority
25
50 25 50
Role authority 41
82
9
18
PERCEIVED SYMBOLS
OF
AUTHORITY AND COMPLIANCE
507
subjects said they would have given the experimenter
a
dime if they had change
in the role authority condition. In addition, not one subject questioned the con-
federate in the role authority condition.
The latency between request and compliance was significantly affected by the
apparel of the perceived authority, ~’(4,
N=
150)
=
44.37,
p
<
.05.
In the no
authority and status authority conditions, 23%-24% of the subjects complied
quickly (under
30
s),
whereas in the role authority condition,
85%
of the sub-
jects complied quickly. Thirty-six percent complied moderately quickly
(30
s
to
1
min) in the no authority condition, 64% in the status authority condition, and
15%
in the role authority condition, Fortyane percent complied slowly (over 1
min) in the no authority condition, 12% complied slowly in the status authority
condition, and not one subject complied slowly in the role authority condition.
Older subjects (over
30
years) complied significantly more than younger sub-
jects (16-30 yrs) in the role authority condition. One hundred percent of older
subjects and
57%
of
the younger subjects complied in the role authority condi-
tion. Significant age differences were not found
in
either the status authority or
no authority conditions.
There was no significant gender difference with regard to female/male com-
pliance rates. In addition, no significant difference was found between the sub-
ject’s apparel and the subject’s willingness to comply.
Discussion
As
did Bickman’s
(1
974) experiment, this experiment showed a significant
relation between the apparel the confederate wore and the number of subjects
who complied to the confederate’s request. In addition, several other interesting
differences were noticed between the subjects’ responses and the confederate’s
request. For example, in the role authority condition, the confederate noted
that the subjects responded quite differently. The confederate would say, “Give
him a dime!” and the majority of subjects would look at his badge and say,
“sure.”
Altruism, as defined
by
the subjects’ verbal reason given for complying, was
also significantly affected by the presence of a perceived authority. It seems
that compliance, when requested by an authority, may be less charitable. Dur-
ing the experiment, only one person gave the experimenter a dime without the
confederate requesting them to do
so
(a nun).
The confederate was of the opinion that the nature of noncompliance was
also different in the role authority condition. While in the fire fighter’s uniform,
the confederate felt eight
of
the nine subjects who did not comply would have
complied if they would have had change. For example, one woman said, “I’m
really sorry that
I
can’t, but
I
only have one dime and
I
need to make an impor-
tant phone call.” The confederate stated that subjects’ responses in the role
508
BRAD
J.
BUSHMAN
authority condition sounded more sincere.
Also,
none of the subjects ques-
tioned the confederate in the role authority condition, even though fire fighters
have nothing to do with parking meter violations.
Perceived authority is apparently an important variable influencing compli-
ance. These findings suggest that those holding authoritative positions have a
great responsibility, especially when making requests of others.
References
Bickman,
L.
(1971). The effects of social status on the honesty of others.
Bickman,
L.
(1974). The social power
of
a uniform.
Journal
of
Applied Social
Borden, R.J. (1975). Witnessed aggression: Influence
of
an observer’s sex and
values on aggressive responding.
Journal
of
Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,
31,567-573.
Emswiller, T., Deaux,
K.,
&
Willits, J.E. (1971). Similarity, sex, and requests
for small favors.
Journal
of
Applied Social Psychology,
1,284-291.
Joseph,
N.,
&
Alex,
N.
(1972). The uniform:
A
sociological perspective.
Amer-
ican Journal
of
Sociology,
77,7 19-730.
Raymond, B.J.,
&
Unger, R.K. (1971). Effect
of
deviant and conventional
attire on cooperation [Summary]
.
Proceedings
of
the 79th Annual Conven-
tion
o
f
the American Psychological Association,
6,357-358.
Suedfeld, P., Bochner,
S.,
&
Matas,
C.
(1971). Petitioner’s attire and petition
signing by peace demonstrators: A field experiment,
Journal
of
Applied
So-
cial Psychology,
l, 278-283.
Zimbardo,
P.
(Chair). (1971).
Freaks, hippies and voters: The effect
of
deviant
dress and appearance on political persuasion process.
Symposium conducted
at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, New York City.
Journal
of
Social Psychology,
85,87-92.
Psychology,
4,47-6 1.
... For example, a phisher may send "reminders" to encourage individuals to perform a fake scheduled update of login information. Authority influence uses power such as job titles [36], appearance [37], and demeanor [38] to influence others. Scarcity influence takes advantage of individuals' tendency to feel that an object, event, or experience is more valuable when they perceive it as rare, inimitable, or available for a limited time. ...
Article
Full-text available
Influence-based deceptive messages constantly play a critical role in email phishing attacks. However, the literature lacks adequate understanding about how phishing messages with attractive and coercive influence result in the receivers’ adverse consequences. We therefore take the perspective of mindless response and mindful interpretation to address this issue by examining comprehensive relationships among message influence, cognitive processing, and phishing susceptibility. To accomplish this, a survey approach was adopted after a simulated phishing attack was conducted in campuses. Our empirical evidence shows that both message influence and cognitive processing can lead to people being phished, and a combination of different influences can also trigger cognitive processing. This research makes contributions to the literature of information security, persuading influence, and cognitive psychology.
... However, a larger sample size may demonstrate differences between a randomly chosen participant and a person that holds out-of-experiment authority. However, other studies have repeatedly shown, that individuals tend to be more compliant with the requests of individuals presumed to carry real-world authority (Bushman, 1984;Milgram, 1963). ...
... In particular, compared with women, men are more likely to adopt suppression as an emotion regulation strategy (Gross and John, 2003) so we controlled for leader gender. Moreover, as age can be a source of authority (Bushman, 1984), along the lines that elder supervisors may be more likely to adopt the authoritarian leadership style, we also included leader age as a control variable. In addition, we controlled for leader's tenure within the team because leadership styles may change when the leader becomes more familiar with members. ...
Article
Full-text available
How do authoritarian leaders in modern organizations influence work team emotional climate and performance? Defining authoritarian leadership as an ambient, demanding, and controlling leadership style, we conducted a survey study of 252 leaders and 765 subordinates matched in 227 work teams in three large public Japanese organizations. The results indicate that authoritarian leaders are more likely to create a team climate of emotion suppression, which induces a higher level of team emotional exhaustion that negatively impacts team performance. Furthermore, we found that authoritarian leaders’ own emotion suppression enhances the above sequential mediation effects, i.e. the more emotion suppression the authoritarian leader him/herself exercises, the stronger the team climate of emotion suppression, the higher the level of team emotional exhaustion, and the lower the team performance. These findings suggest that leadership effectiveness may be improved if leaders can reduce their authoritarian behaviors and identify appropriate channels for employees to release emotions in the workplace.
... Research in domains other than prejudice confrontation has shown that people view authority figures as responsible for taking action in difficult or uncomfortable situations. In most of these experiments, the authority is status-based, and signaled to others through a formal title or uniform (Bushman, 1984). Perhaps the most famous example is Milgram's (1963) research on obedience to authority. ...
Article
Full-text available
Perceived responsibility for responding predicts whether people confront others’ discriminatory behavior, but who is seen as and actually feels responsible for confronting prejudice? Study 1 examined whether people view status-based authority figures, stigmatized targets, or other bystanders as responsible for confronting a witnessed prejudicial remark. Results revealed that participants viewed the authority figure as most responsible for responding, and they reported feeling less personally responsible in the presence of both authorities and targets. Study 2 examined whether being in a position of authority enhances perceptions of responsibility for responding to discrimination and, in turn, facilitates confrontation. Participants who were randomly assigned to a leadership (vs. non-leader control) condition witnessed a racially insensitive remark. Leadership increased perceived responsibility, but did not significantly increase confrontation. Study 3 builds on the previous two studies by showing that leaders in actual organizations feel more responsible for confronting prejudice compared to those who are not conferred authority status. These findings extend previous studies by uncovering an important antecedent (i.e., conferred authority) of feeling responsible for addressing prejudice, which is shown to be a key factor in predicting whether bystanders confront discrimination. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
... To strengthen Self-Identity and Status related to active travel modes, we based Action 2 on the Principle of Authority, which states that when making a choice, it is usual to search for expert guidance from a recognized source [64]. Participants were asked to attend a speech given by a cardiologist and a sports trainer, who talked about how general health can be improved by walking and cycling more. ...
Article
Full-text available
The main objective of the present study is to analyse the effect of a Travel Behaviour Change Program (TBCP) based on health improvement actions, in relation to a potential for an increase in walking and cycling, with reference to particular groups of people defined according to sociodemographics. A TBCP consisting of three persuasion actions based on health improvements was planned and executed in Valencia (Spain). A two-wave panel survey was used to study the effects of taking part in the TBCP. The panel survey collected data related to activity-travel scheduling process before and after the execution of the TBCP. To study the influence of participating in the TBCP, respondents were separated into two groups: those directly implicated in the TBCP and those who were not involved in any action (CG), which were formed on the basis of sociodemographic similarity. We developed descriptive and explanatory analyses and predictive models, to study the effect of the TBCP with reference to particular groups of people defined according to sociodemographics. Results indicate that being involved in the TBCP affects more males than females, those who share their household and do not live with their parents during weekdays, those with lower car availability, those willing to reduce car use and students. Several implications for research and transport policy are derived from these results.
... The principle of authority states that when a person makes a decision, it is common to seek expert advice from an acknowledged source, for example, medical, legal, financial, or any other professional expertise (40) or to comply with the rules of a properly constituted authority (25). Interestingly, the external appearance of authority represented by specific symbols such as a uniform or a professional title is often sufficient to establish expert appearance (41). ...
Article
Social psychology offers a series of persuasion techniques that are able to strengthen the impact of travel demand management programs. This is particularly the case for community-based programs of voluntary travel behavior change such as the TravelSmart programs currently being conducted in Melbourne, Australia. This paper presents the results of an experiment that has applied six particular persuasion techniques as part of a community-based TravelSmart campaign. In a pilot test of 160 households, combinations of persuasion elements were tested with eight different treatment groups while a number of sociodemographic variables were controlled. Although the results were not statistically significant at the 95% confidence level because of a limited sample size, the results indicated an increase in the rate of participation when persuasion strategies were integrated into the TravelSmart recruitment process. Modeling of the intervention uptake as a function of sociodemographic variables indicated the problem of linguistic barriers in association with a multicultural urban population. In contrast, bicycle availability and the current use of public transit both had positive impacts on TravelSmart participation. Initial analysis of the results from a larger field test of some 800 test and control households reinforced the positive impact of the persuasion strategies. Overall, the results indicate the need to explore an extension of the persuasion principles from their use in the recruitment process to all other implementation stages of voluntary travel behavior change programs.
Chapter
Die äußere Erscheinung von Polizist*innen im Einsatz ist durch Uniform und Ausrüstung für andere deutlich wahrnehmbar. Mit der Wahrnehmbarkeit beginnt auch die Eindrucksbildung. Im Sinne des Brunswik’schen Linsenmodells formt der/die Betrachtende aus proximalen („wahrnehmbaren“) Hinweisreizen (cues) einer Person ein Urteil über distale („entfernte“) Merkmale der Person (zum Beispiel „Kompetenz“, „Vertrauenswürdigkeit“ oder „Bedrohlichkeit“). Bei Begegnungen mit der Polizei fließen sämtliche Merkmale der eingesetzten Polizeikräfte zu einem Gesamteindruck beim polizeilichen Gegenüber zusammen – und umgekehrt. Die Polizeiuniform als äußeres Merkmal führt dabei üblicherweise zu einer positiveren Einschätzung des/des Träger*in durch Beobachtende (Uniform-Effekt). Auch von Führungs- und Einsatzmitteln, insbesondere Waffen, ist der Einfluss auf Wahrnehmung und Verhalten bekannt (Waffen-Effekt; Waffen-Fokus-Effekt). Wenn das äußere Erscheinungsbild eine einsatzrelevante und empirisch nachweisbare Einflussgröße darstellt und hierdurch insbesondere Einsatzerfolg und Einsatzrisiko beeinflusst werden, dann ist dessen Wirkung im Einsatzgeschehen von den beteiligten Einsatzkräften zu beachten und muss konsequenterweise auch Gegenstand des Einsatztrainings sein. Theoretische Konzepte, empirische Befunde und Implikationen für das polizeiliche Einsatztraining werden nachfolgend diskutiert.
Article
Both, the number of people wearing visible body modifications, e.g., tattoos, and their acceptance in society have been significantly increased. Thus, rules and regulations of German police officers’ appearance prohibiting to show tattoos during police services are challenged. Basically, such standards aim to minimize risk factors in police service and to maximize safety at work, thus fostering police operations and finally promoting a favorable image of the police organization. Within the scope of the “Police Officer Appearance”-project (2016–2018) of the Rhineland-Palatinate Police University, we aimed to answer the question how police officers in uniform with visible tattoos are evaluated by others. Within a sub-project, we focused on an important target group in police services: criminal offenders. While previous studies found evidence that wearing a uniform was associated with more favorable ratings of police officers as compared to people in civilian clothing (“uniform-effect”), showing a tattoo had a rather detrimental effect of attitudes toward police officers (“tattoo-effect”). In the present study, we investigate whether these effects transfer to the sub-population of criminal offenders. A total of N = 98 inmates of a correctional institution in Rhineland-Palatinate participated in the study. Inmates were presented pictures of police officers in police uniform or civilian clothing with or without visible tattoos. Results revealed the “uniform-effect” among inmates, i.e., a more positive evaluation of police officers wearing police uniform. However, the “tattoo-effect” was not present in this subgroup. Implications for theory, limitations and future research directions, as well as practical implications are discussed.
Book
Full-text available
Primer texto de Psicología Social editado en Chile
Article
Full-text available
The degree and basis of social power of uniformed figures was investigated in two field experiments. In the first experiment, subjects were stopped in the street by an experimenter dressed in one of three ways: a civilian, a milkman, or a guard. They were asked to pick up a paper bag, or to give a dime to a stranger, or to move away from a bus stop. The results indicated that the subjects complied more with the guard than with the civilian or milkman. In the second field experiment, designed to examine the basis of the guard's power, subjects were asked to give a dime t o a stranger under conditions of either surveillance or nonsurveillance. The guard's power was not affected by the surveillance manipulation. A logical analysis of social power indicated that the guard's power was most likely based on legitimacy. Two questionnaire studies indicated, however, that college students did not perceive the guard as having either more. power or more legitjmacy than the milkman or civilian. The nature and importance of understanding legitimacy was discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Full-text available
The degree and basis of social power of uniformed figures was investigated in two field experiments. In the first experiment, subjects were stopped in the street by an experimenter dressed in one of three ways: a civilian, a milkman, or a guard. They were asked to pick up a paper bag, or to give a dime to a stranger, or to move away from a bus stop. The results indicated that the subjects complied more with the guard than with the civilian or milkman. In the second field experiment, designed to examine the basis of the guard's power, subjects were asked to give a dime to a stranger under conditions of either surveillance or nonsurveillance. The guard's power was not affected by the surveillance manipulation. A logical analysis of social power indicated that the guard's power was most likely based on legitimacy. Two questionnaire studies indicated, however, that college students did not perceive the guard as having either more. power or more legitimacy than the milkman or civilian. The nature and importance of understanding legitimacy was discussed.
Article
The uniform is viewed as a device to resolve certain dilemmas of complex organizations-namely, to define their boundaries, to assure that members will conform to their goals, and to eliminate conflicts in the status sets of their members. The uniform serves several functions: it acts as a totem, reveals and conceals statuses, certifies legitimacy, and suppresses individuality. The interaction of these components and the acceptance or rejection of the uniform and its associated status by the wearer are described.
Article
Two female experimenters, one dressed as a “hippie” and the other wearing “straight” dress, solicited signatures for an anti-war petition at the April 1971 Washington peace demonstration. Although the petition was identical in both conditions, and a large degree of attitudinal homogeneity in the subject population could be assumed, the hippie condition produced more signatures, more subjects who signed without looking at the petition, and attracted a larger number of unsolicited signatures. These differences were attributed to the effect of implied attitude (reference group) similarity between experimenters and subjects in the hippie and dissimilarity in the straight condition; in-group members were more effective than out-group members, despite experimenter-subject similarity in explicit attitudes under both conditions.
Article
To determine whether correspondence in appearance between helper and helped will increase the rate of helping behavior, two types (Hippie and Straight) and two sexes of experimenters approached each of the four corresponding types of subjects and asked to borrow a dime for a telephone call. As predicted, a significantly greater number of persons were willing to lend money to someone who resembled them in appearance. Furthermore, as predicted, his tendency to match on the basis of appearance was stronger for male subjects than for female subjects. It was hypothesized that dress styles, like race, provide a basis for assumptions about other areas of similarity and create a greater willingness to help a similar other.
Article
Two experiments investigated the effects of the presence of an observer on aggressive responding. In one experiment, male subjects observed by a male aggressed more than those observed by a female. When the male observer was removed from the situation, subjects' level of aggressiveness more closely matched the level manifested by the opponent. The removal of the female observer had little effect on the subjects' behavior. In the second experiment, the male or female observer of the subject's behavior was disguised as a member of an organization with explicit values (aggressive or pacifistic) regarding the use of aggression. In this case, significant differences in aggression were associated with the observer's values but not the observer's sex. Following the departure of the observer, the shock settings of subjects in the two aggressive-value observer groups showed a signifcant decrease. The average shock setting of subjects in the two pacifistic-value observer groups remained at about the same level. In sum, the results indicated that the subjects' aggressive behavior was apparently a function of their expectations of approval for such behavior, based on the inferred or explicit values of the observer. The results were further discussed in terms of social learning theory.
Freaks, hippies and voters: The effect of deviant dress and appearance on political persuasion process. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association
  • P Zimbardo
Zimbardo, P. (Chair). (1971). Freaks, hippies and voters: The effect of deviant dress and appearance on political persuasion process. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, New York City. Journal of Social Psychology, 85,87-92.
The effects of social status on the honesty of others The social power of a uniform Witnessed aggression: Influence of an observer's sex and values on aggressive responding
  • L Bickman
  • L Bickman
Bickman, L. (1971). The effects of social status on the honesty of others. Bickman, L. (1974). The social power of a uniform. Journal of Applied Social Borden, R.J. (1975). Witnessed aggression: Influence of an observer's sex and values on aggressive responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 31,567-573.