ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

There has long been interest in identifying and studying ''successful psychopaths." This study sampled psychologists with an interest in law, attorneys, and clinical psychology professors to obtain descriptions of individuals considered to be psychopaths who were also successful in their endeavors. The results showed a consistent description across professions and convergence with descriptions of traditional psy-chopathy, though the successful psychopathy profile had higher scores on conscientiousness, as mea-sured within the five-factor model (FFM). These results are useful in documenting the existence of successful psychopathy, demonstrating the potential benefit of informant methodology, and providing an FFM description that distinguishes successful psychopaths from unsuccessful psychopaths studied more routinely within prison settings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Author's personal copy
Brief Report
The search for the successful psychopath
Stephanie N. Mullins-Sweatt
a,*
, Natalie G. Glover
b
, Karen J. Derefinko
b
, Joshua D. Miller
c
,
Thomas A. Widiger
b
a
Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74074, United States
b
Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, United States
c
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, United States
article info
Article history:
Available online 23 May 2010
Keywords:
Psychopathy
Five-factor model
General personality
Successful psychopath
abstract
There has long been interest in identifying and studying ‘‘successful psychopaths.” This study sampled
psychologists with an interest in law, attorneys, and clinical psychology professors to obtain descriptions
of individuals considered to be psychopaths who were also successful in their endeavors. The results
showed a consistent description across professions and convergence with descriptions of traditional psy-
chopathy, though the successful psychopathy profile had higher scores on conscientiousness, as mea-
sured within the five-factor model (FFM). These results are useful in documenting the existence of
successful psychopathy, demonstrating the potential benefit of informant methodology, and providing
an FFM description that distinguishes successful psychopaths from unsuccessful psychopaths studied
more routinely within prison settings.
!2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
There has long been interest in studying ‘‘successful psycho-
paths” (e.g., Hall & Benning, 2006). Successful psychopaths are, in
theory, individuals who fit the criteria of a psychopath, having cer-
tain fundamental traits (e.g., callousness), but largely succeed in
their exploitation. Several psychopathy theorists have made anec-
dotal references to psychopathic lawyers, professors, businessmen,
and politicians who have not committed crimes that warranted ar-
rest or have successfully avoided investigation (e.g., Cleckley,
1988; Hare, 2003). It also has been argued that certain psycho-
pathic traits (e.g., fearlessness) might be assets within some pro-
fessions (e.g., Lykken, 1995).
There has been little empirical research characterizing such per-
sons. Widom (1977) recruited participants using advertisements
requesting ‘‘charming, aggressive, carefree people who are impul-
sively irresponsible but are good at handling people and at looking
after number one” (p. 675). Characteristics associated with psy-
chopathy (low empathy, psychopathic deviance, and hypomania)
were found, though participants’ scores on impulsivity and Machi-
avellianism did not differ from scores of community members.
Widom and Newman (1985) replicated this work using the same
strategy. Hall and Benning (2006), however, argued that it might
not be accurate to characterize these participants as successful as
a substantial portion of participants in both studies had significant
arrest records, and most were of low socio-economic status.
Ishikawa, Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, and Lacasse (2001) examined the
correlates of psychopathy among ‘‘successful and unsuccessful”
psychopaths within a community sample. They defined successful
psychopaths as ‘‘community-based psychopaths who escape con-
viction for the crimes they perpetrate” (Ishikawa et al., 2001, p.
423). Psychopathy status was determined with the Revised Psy-
chopathy Checklist (PCL-R; Hare, 2003) and collateral measures.
Unsuccessful psychopaths had higher PCL-R total scores than suc-
cessful psychopaths (possibly due to criminal acts) but the two
groups did not differ on traits considered to be central to the disor-
der. Ishikawa et al. though acknowledged that they might not have
identified truly successful psychopaths, as participants were re-
cruited from temporary employment agencies. Therefore findings
‘‘cannot be extrapolated to socioeconomically successful psycho-
paths functioning in industry, public office, the criminal justice
system, or academia” (Ishikawa et al., 2001, p. 431).
A difficulty in studying successful psychopaths is recruitment.
‘‘Research with the more socially successful psychopaths is badly
needed, although it is recognized that there are real difficulties in-
volved in obtaining suitable subjects” (Hare, 1975, cited by Widom
(1977), p. 675). It would be difficult to sample enough individuals
within a respective profession to find the rare psychopath. Once
found, it is possible that this psychopathic person would not be
forthcoming or would refuse to participate.
Although successful psychopaths may not be willing to partici-
pate in studies, individuals who are closely familiar with him/her
may be able to provide useful information regarding his/her
0092-6566/$ - see front matter !2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.010
*Corresponding author. Address: 116 North Murray Hall, Department of
Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74078, United States. Fax:
+1 405 744 8067.
E-mail address: stephanie.sweatt@gmail.com (S.N. Mullins-Sweatt).
Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2010) 554–558
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Research in Personality
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / j r p
Author's personal copy
personality. Kirkman (2005) sampled women who had been vic-
timized by a partner who evidenced psychopathic characteristics.
Participants described their partners using the Hare P-SCAN (Hare
& Herve, 1999). These descriptions were compared to ratings of
partners provided by women in a comparison group. The ratings
of the former group indicated significantly greater levels of psy-
chopathy than the comparison group.
The current study sampled persons within professions likely to
come in contact with psychopathic individuals. We asked if they
had ever known anyone whom they would characterize as a ‘‘suc-
cessful psychopath” and, if so, to describe him/her in terms of traits
associated with psychopathy and the personality traits of the five-
factor model (FFM; Costa & McCrae, 1992). Lynam and Widiger
(2007) integrated findings from three approaches describing psy-
chopathy in terms of FFM traits (i.e., expert ratings, empirical cor-
relations and translations of psychopathy measures) in order to
generate a consensus psychopathy profile. Prototypic psychopathy
was described across the approaches as low in five facets of agree-
ableness (all except trust), three facets of conscientiousness (duti-
fulness, self-discipline, deliberation), and one facet of neuroticism
(self-consciousness) and extraversion (warmth) as well as high in
impulsiveness from neuroticism and excitement-seeking from
extraversion. Several traits appeared across two of the three ap-
proaches. This expanded profile included high angry hostility,
assertiveness, and openness to actions, and low anxiousness,
depressiveness, vulnerability, trust, and openness to feelings.
In the current study, we hypothesized that successful psycho-
paths would be described with the Lynam and Widiger (2007) con-
sensus profile, except that successful psychopathy would be
associated with high, rather than low, scores on conscientiousness
(i.e., competence, achievement-striving, discipline and delibera-
tion). Similarly, the successful psychopath would be characterized
as high in such psychopathic traits as callousness, dishonesty,
exploitative, and remorseless, but low in irresponsibility and
impulsivity.
2. Method
Participants were provided with a definition of a psychopath;
‘‘social predators who charm, manipulate, and ruthlessly plow
their way through life.... Completely lacking in conscience and
feeling for others, they selfishly take what they want and do as
they please, violating social norms and expectations without the
slightest sense of guilt or regret” (Hare, 2003, p. xi). They were then
asked if they had known any such person – it could not be someone
they knew of (e.g., a person within the media or literature); it had
to be someone they knew personally. ‘‘Equally important, this per-
son must be someone whom you felt was actually successful in his/
her psychopathic endeavors. It can not be someone who has largely
failed (at least to this point) in his/her psychopathic pursuits.” Tar-
gets consisted of 120 males and 26 females (M= 43.48 years old,
SD = 11.63; 86% Caucasian). Respondents indicated they had
known the targets on average for 10.65 years (SD = 11.04 years).
2.1. Psychologists
Psychologists were drawn from the directory of the American
Psychological Association’s (APA) Division 41 (‘‘psych-law”), the
organization that promotes applying psychology within the legal
system. Seven hundred and fifty-two persons were solicited by
postal mail, 83 envelopes were returned by the postal service, sug-
gesting that 669 probably reached their intended recipients. From
that number, 118 returned the survey (18%) and 81 indicated they
knew someone they would describe as a successful psychopath.
Respondents (53 males, 29 females) were doctoral level psycholo-
gists (78% Ph.D., 16% Psy.D., 2% Ed.D.). Experience of participants
ranged from 2 to 55 (M= 23) years since earning their degree. Par-
ticipants indicated they knew the target ‘‘strongly” (M= 3.61;
SD = 0.90).
2.2. Attorneys
Attorneys who practice criminal law (N= 642) were surveyed.
One hundred and forty-three envelopes were returned, suggesting
that 499 probably reached their intended recipients. From that
number, 31 returned the survey (6%) and 25 indicated they knew
someone they would describe as a successful psychopath. Respon-
dents (18 males, 7 females) had been in practice from 2 to 43
(M= 22) years since earning their degree. Participants indicated
they knew the target moderately well (M= 3.36; SD = 1.11).
2.3. Professors
Clinical psychology faculty members (n= 1000) were surveyed.
Two hundred and thirty-two envelopes were returned, suggesting
that 768 probably reached their intended recipients. From that
number, 58 returned the survey (8%) and 41 indicated they knew
someone they would describe as a successful psychopath. Respon-
dents (24 males, 17 females) were doctoral level psychologists
[95% Ph.D. (4% Ph.D. and J.D.), 2% Psy.D.]. Experience of participants
ranged from 1 to 58 (M= 20) years since earning their degree. Par-
ticipants indicated they knew the target ‘‘strongly” (M= 3.57,
SD = 0.98).
2.4. Materials
Beyond demographics of oneself and the target, and how well
they knew the person (1 = slightly, 5 = extremely well), partici-
pants were also asked to indicate the extent to which they consid-
ered the person to be psychopathic (1 = only slightly to
5 = complete match/prototypic case). Participants then described
in their own words attributes that made the person psychopathic
and why the person was successful.
2.5. Five factor form (FFF)
Participants completed the FFF, an updated version of the five-
factor model rating form (Mullins-Sweatt, Jamerson, Samuel, Ol-
son, & Widiger, 2006), a one-page form consisting of 30 items rep-
resenting the facets of the FFM. Participants described the
individual where 1 = maladaptively low, 2 = normal low, 3 = neu-
tral, 4 = normal high, and 5 = maladaptively high. For example,
for competence, maladaptively low competence was ‘‘disinclined,
lax,” low was ‘‘casual,” high was ‘‘efficient, resourceful” and mal-
adaptively high was ‘‘perfectionistic” (a copy may be obtained from
the authors).
2.6. Psychopathy rating form (PRF)
Participants described the target in terms of 15 traits commonly
cited in psychopathy literature (e.g., callous, exploitative, irrespon-
sible). For example, for ‘‘carefree lifestyle” the description was
‘‘lacking in long term plans or commitments; lives day-to-day;
happy-go-lucky”. Participants described the individual where 1
represented ‘‘extremely low” and 5 represented ‘‘extremely high”
(a copy may be obtained from the authors).
S.N. Mullins-Sweatt et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2010) 554–558 555
Author's personal copy
3. Results
The modal choice of target for the professors and psych-law
psychologists was a current/former colleague (75% and 34.5%,
respectively). Of the psych-law psychologists 18.5% described a
current/former client, 7.5% of the professors described a current/
former student, and just as many described a friend (16% and
7.5%, respectively). The modal choice for the attorneys was a client
(48%), which may reflect that they considered these clients to be
successful in avoiding convictions.
There were no differences between the three samples with re-
spect to length of relationship or how well the participant knew
the target. Attorneys were significantly less familiar (M= 3.87,
SD = 0.94) with the concept of psychopathy than the psych-law
sample (M= 4.55, SD = 0.90) and clinical psychology professors
(M= 4.48, SD = 0.75), F(2, 134) = 4.83, p< .01. Participants indi-
cated that the extent to which the target met the description of a
prototypic psychopath fell close to a strong match (M= 3.92,
SD = 0.67). Attorneys described their targets as slightly more psy-
chopathic (M= 4.16, SD = 0.67) than the psych-law sample
(M= 3.80, SD = 0.72) and clinical psychology professors (M= 3.80,
SD = 0.72), F(2, 134) = 2.34, p< .05.
Narrative descriptions of the targets were consistent with the
presence of what we would consider successful psychopathy (a ta-
ble of descriptions is available online, see Supplementary Table A,
or from the first author). For example, indicators of success in-
cluded ‘‘a top notch police detective, a hero,” ‘‘dean from a major
university,” ‘‘successful retail business,” ‘‘made large sum of money
and was mayor for three years,” ‘‘managerial position in govern-
ment organization,” ‘‘full professor at two major universities,”
and ‘‘endowed professor with numerous federal grants.” Qualita-
tive descriptions of psychopathy included ‘‘utter absence of empa-
thy;” ‘‘manipulated women and children despite pain/damage
caused, dishonest in business, superficial/forced emotionality;”
‘‘absence of remorse;” ‘‘chronic deceitfulness.”
Narrative descriptions also were consistent with ratings of
these persons with respect to psychopathic personality traits
(Table 1). Members from all three professions described the suc-
cessful psychopath as being dishonest, exploitative, low in re-
morse, minimizing of self-blame, arrogant, and shallow. The
mean profiles generated by the three samples using the PRF were
strongly related (r= .86, p< .001), using the average of the three
possible correlations. The Wilks-lambda multivariate test of overall
differences among the samples was not significant [F(30, 152) =
1.38, p= .106]. We completed univariate between-subjects ANOVA
comparisons for each of the PRF variables to verify minimal group
differences. Attorneys described targets as engaging in significantly
more criminal behavior than the clinical psychology professors and
the psych-law samples [F(2, 139) = 6.03, p< .01].
A table of the FFM profiles generated by the samples may be ob-
tained online (see Supplementary Table B) or from the first author.
These profiles were highly correlated (r= 0.96, p< .001), using the
average of the three possible correlations. The Wilks-lamda multi-
variate test of overall differences among the samples was not
significant at p< .01 [F(60, 166) = 1.58, p= .012]. Univariate
between-subjects ANOVA comparisons indicated no significant dif-
ferences in how the professions described successful psychopaths
on any FFM facets.
Table 2 provides the mean scores reported for the successful
psychopaths averaged across samples in terms of facets of the
FFM. In order to provide a quantitative indication of the extent that
present results match the predictions of Lynam and Widiger
(2007), we correlated the mean FFM profile with the consensus
profile using dummy coding (i.e., L=!1, H= 1, 0 if not included).
The correlation between the mean profile of successful psycho-
paths with the consensus profile of the prototypic psychopath
was .49, p< .01. The correlation between the mean profile with
the expanded profile was .66, p< .01 (i.e., !2=L,!1=l, 2 = H,
1=h). Consistent with expectations, the successful psychopaths
were rated high in assertiveness, excitement-seeking, and activity,
and especially low in agreeableness traits like straightforwardness,
altruism, compliance, and modesty. Most importantly with respect
to the hypotheses of the study, successful psychopaths were high
in competence, order, achievement-striving, and self-discipline.
We also correlated the mean successful psychopathy profile
with FFM descriptions of prototypic personality disorders (PDs; Ly-
nam & Widiger, 2001). Pearson correlations ranged from !.15
(obsessive compulsive PD) to .86 (narcissistic PD). It is worth not-
ing that the correlations with antisocial PD and narcissistic PD (.80
and .86) were higher than with the consensus profiles, due to the
use of dummy coding for the latter analysis. Finally, the mean suc-
cessful psychopathy profile was not significantly related to an
average personality profile based on NEO PI-R norms (r=!.07).
4. Discussion
The current results suggest that the successful psychopath is
distinguished from the unsuccessful (or prototypic) psychopath
Table 1
Psychopathy rating form.
Psych-law Attorneys Clinical psychology professors F
a
Mean Standard deviation Mean Standard deviation Mean Standard deviation
Callous 3.22 1.46 3.91 1.31 3.85 1.11 4.220
Dishonest 4.28 1.00 4.43 0.90 4.36 0.71 0.325
Exploitative 4.46 0.93 4.78 0.42 4.64 0.63 1.701
Criminal behavior 2.89 1.41 3.78 1.24 2.61 1.13 6.029
*
Low remorse 4.03 1.29 4.26 0.92 4.42 0.60 1.771
Low anxiousness 3.54 1.17 4.26 0.96 3.63 0.91 4.132
Minimizes self-blame 3.99 1.21 4.30 0.63 4.54 0.72 3.971
Arrogance 4.25 0.85 4.39 1.03 4.46 0.76 0.731
Shallow 4.23 0.89 3.91 1.21 4.03 1.04 1.153
Impulsive 3.16 0.99 3.26 1.25 3.03 1.13 0.373
Excitement-seeking 3.68 1.17 3.68 0.99 3.28 0.88 1.805
Carefree lifestyle 2.81 1.01 2.91 1.41 2.46 1.19 1.540
Irresponsible 3.09 1.16 2.91 1.44 3.03 1.22 0.184
Aggressive 2.53 1.35 3.26 1.29 2.57 1.30 2.775
Childhood delinquency 2.72 1.27 3.64 1.22 2.50 1.32 6.343
a
df ranged from (2, 129) to (2, 139) for all variables except childhood delinquency (df = 2, 95).
*
p< .01.
556 S.N. Mullins-Sweatt et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2010) 554–558
Author's personal copy
via differences in conscientiousness. Unlike the current successful
psychopaths, prototypic psychopaths are said to be high in irre-
sponsibility, impulsivity, and negligence, and perhaps these traits
contribute to their arrests and convictions for crimes. In other
words, the profile switches from being low in conscientiousness
to being high in conscientiousness. This finding is consistent with
a considerable literature that documents the importance of consci-
entiousness to a variety of positive life outcomes (e.g., Ozer & Ben-
et-Martinez, 2006). Conversely, studies have demonstrated a
significant negative relationship between conscientiousness and
number of arrests (e.g., Clower & Bothwell, 2002).
The current study also demonstrates the benefit of a compre-
hensive model of PD that includes adaptive traits within its classi-
fication system (Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009). The DSM-IV PDs
are confined to maladaptive traits, not recognizing that persons
with PDs can also have personality strengths. Some more recent
models of psychopathy include traits that may represent adaptive
strengths. The psychopathic personality inventory (PPI), for in-
stance, includes such scales as Stress Immunity (remaining calm
in spite of stress), Social Potency (interpersonal impact and skill
at influencing people), and Fearlessness (lacking anticipatory anx-
iety) that could have considerable benefits toward achieving suc-
cessful life outcomes (Lilienfeld & Fowler, 2006). These scales
largely define the PPI psychopathy factor ‘‘Fearless Dominance”,
which has been associated with educational attainment, sociabil-
ity, and executive functioning (Patrick, 2006). Similar to the cur-
rent findings, Fearless Dominance has been associated with low
neuroticism and high extraversion (Ross, Benning, Patrick, Thomp-
son, & Thurston, 2009). However, Fearless Dominance has not cor-
related with conscientiousness and it is this particular domain of
the FFM that distinguishes in the current study the successful from
the unsuccessful psychopath.
One might question whether the persons described in the current
study should be described as psychopaths, given they did not fit tra-
ditional descriptions (e.g., they were not irresponsible or impulsive).
Though a diagnostic measure was not administered, the psycholo-
gists, attorneys, and professors, who were moderately to highly
familiar with the psychopathy construct, considered these persons
to be psychopathic. Further, these individuals were identified as
having traits of dishonesty, exploitation, low remorse, minimizing
self-blame, arrogance, callousness, and shallow affect (often said
to be among the core features of psychopathy; Hare, 2003).
Cleckley (1988) suggested that the psychopathic businessman,
physician, psychiatrist, and scientist described anecdotally repre-
sented ‘‘incomplete manifestations or suggestions of the disorder”
(p. 188). However, as Patrick (2006) indicated, he did not mean it
would be inaccurate to describe them as psychopaths. Cleckley felt
that these individuals probably have the core traits and underlying
pathology of psychopathy seen within prison settings. Though
there are a number of studies that have examined ‘‘noninstitution-
alized psychopathy,” these persons may not be described as suc-
cessful. The current study suggests that psychopathy also may
exist in a manner quite different from that of the ‘‘unsuccessful”
psychopaths routinely studied within prison settings.
The current study used informant descriptions to provide infor-
mation about successful psychopaths. An advantage of this method
was the ability to obtain descriptions on persons who would have
been difficult to research (e.g., college dean, university president,
police detective, mayor, and director of a medical center). Such
persons have been described in papers and texts on psychopathy
but only anecdotally. This was the first study to conduct a system-
atic, quantitative analysis of such persons.
Nevertheless, there are potential limitations to the informant
approach. One concern would be whether the informants knew
the person sufficiently well. Informants will not have access to
all relevant data. To combat this, raters were encouraged to leave
a specific question blank if they had insufficient knowledge of a
trait. This option was rarely chosen except for the ‘‘childhood
delinquency” item, indicating the informant methodology is per-
haps more difficult for assessing a target’s history. An additional
limitation was the relatively low response rates. Low response
rates have perhaps occurred in other successful psychopathy stud-
ies [Ishikawa et al. (2001) did not report how many declined and
Widom (1977) cannot report who considered advertisements but
declined participation]. Individuals in the current study may have
chosen not to respond because they were not interested or because
they did not know anyone who would be characterized as a suc-
cessful psychopath. Mitigating response rate concerns was the con-
sistency of findings across the different professions that were
surveyed. Nevertheless, the current results should be interpreted
with caution given possible response bias. Future informant sur-
veys might benefit from efforts to increase response rate (e.g.,
payment).
Appendix A. Supplementary material
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.010.
Table 2
Average scores across three samples and Lynam and Widiger (2007) consensus
profile.
Average across
samples
Consensus profile—
psychopathy
Neuroticism
Anxiousness 2.51 l
Angry hostility 3.55 h
Depressiveness 2.07 l
Self-consciousness 1.69 L
Impulsivity 3.77 H
Vulnerability 1.65 l
Extraversion
Warmth 3.06 L
Gregariousness 4.04
Assertiveness 4.45 h
Activity 3.95
Excitement-seeking 3.99 H
Positive emotions 3.62
Openness
Fantasy 3.43
Aesthetics 3.24
Feelings 3.43 l
Actions 3.49 h
Ideas 3.23
Values 3.07
Agreeableness
Trust 1.54 l
Straightforwardness 1.46 L
Altruism 1.77 L
Compliance 1.93 L
Modesty 1.59 L
Tender-mindedness 1.66 L
Conscientiousness
Competence 3.42
Order 3.44
Dutifulness 2.63 L
Achievement-striving 3.72
Self-discipline 3.38 L
Deliberation 2.76 L
Note. Based on integrated findings from three approaches describing psychopathy
in terms of the FFM (i.e., expert ratings, empirical correlations, translations of
psychopathy measures): L= traits consistently identified as low in the consensus
psychopathy profile according to the three approaches. l= traits included in the
expanded profile, consistent across two approaches. H= traits consistently identi-
fied as high in the consensus psychopathy profile according to the three approaches.
h= traits included in the expanded profile, consistent across two approaches.
S.N. Mullins-Sweatt et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2010) 554–558 557
Author's personal copy
References
Cleckley, H. M. (1988). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Clower, C. E., & Bothwell, R. K. (2002). An exploratory study of the relationship
between the Big Five and inmate recidivism. Journal of Research in Personality,
35, 231–237.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The NEO PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Hall, J. R., & Benning, S. D. (2006). The ‘‘Successful” psychopath: Adaptive and
subclinical manifestations of psychopathy in the general population. In C. J.
Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 459–478). New York: Guilford.
Hare, R. D. (1975). Psychopathy. In P. Venables & M. Christie (Eds.), Research in
psychophysiology. New York: Wiley.
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare psychopathy checklist—Revised (2nd ed.). Toronto, ON,
Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Hare, R. D., & Herve, H. F. (1999). Hare P-SCAN. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health
Systems.
Ishikawa, S. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., & Lacasse, L. (2001). Autonomic stress
reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal
psychopaths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110,
423–432.
Kirkman, C. A. (2005). From soap opera to science. Towards gaining access to the
psychopaths who live amongst us. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research, and Practice, 78, 379–396.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Fowler, K. A. (2006). The self-report assessment of psychopathy:
Problems, pitfalls, and promises. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy
(pp. 107–132). New York: Guilford.
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale: New Jersey.
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five-factor model to represent the
DSM-IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 110, 401–412.
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2007). Using a general model of personality to
identify the basic elements of psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21,
160–178.
Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Jamerson, J. E., Samuel, D. S., Olson, D. R., & Widiger, T. A.
(2006). Psychometric properties of an abbreviated instrument of the five-factor
model. Assessment, 13(2), 119–137.
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of
consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401–421.
Patrick, C. J. (2006). Back to the future: Cleckley as a guide to the next generation of
psychopathy research. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy
(pp. 605–617). New York: Guilford.
Ross, S. R., Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Thompson, A., & Thurston, A. (2009). Factors
of the psychopathic personality inventory: Criterion-related validity and
relationship to the BIS/BAS and the five-factor models of personality.
Assessment, 16, 71–87.
Widiger, T. A., & Mullins-Sweatt, S. N. (2009). Five factor model of personality
disorder: A proposal for DSM-V. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5,
197–220.
Widom, C. S. (1977). A methodology for studying noninstitutionalized psychopaths.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 674–683.
Widom, C. S., & Newman, J. P. (1985). Characteristics of noninstitutionalized
psychopaths. In J. Gunn & D. Farrington (Eds.). Current research in forensic
psychiatry and psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 57–80). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
558 S.N. Mullins-Sweatt et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2010) 554–558
... Current researchers have also suggested that psychopathy and its components as a continuum also exist within non-criminal and non-institutionalised populations (Edens, Marcus, Lilienfeld, & Poythress Jr, 2006;Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995) and that psychopathy within the general community may be quite distinct from that found within an incarcerated environment (Mullins-Sweatt, Glover, Derefinko, Miller, & Widiger, 2010). ...
... proposed that psychopaths who have some of the traits characteristic of psychopathy but without exhibiting the antisocial behaviour may be highly successful in fields including law and law enforcement . A similar argument was put forward by other researchers (Lykken, 1995;Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010). In line with the concept that psychopathy may be a dimensional trait rather than a discrete category, suggested that the capacity that successful psychopaths have to continue to function and succeed in society was due to an incomplete or weaker expression of the disorder compared to those identified within a criminal population. ...
... This suggests that while the lack of emotion and remorse are essential characteristics, criminal behaviour may not be . This distinction is important, as psychopathy within the general community, i.e. successful psychopaths, may be distinct from those within a criminal population (Mullins-Sweatt, Glover, Derefinko, Miller, & Widiger, 2010). "Successful" psychopaths are individuals who exhibit psychopathic traits, egocentricity, superficial charm and irresponsibility, but not the extreme "criminal" antisocial behaviours . ...
Thesis
Full-text available
To make an omelette, you must break a few eggs! Some individuals would argue that, to make the world a better place, you must break a few social and legal constraints. The belief that social norms and the law are unjust and hinder social improvement is not uncommon. Most individuals at times imagine how they could make the world a better place if only they were free from the shackles of society and the law. The majority do not follow up these views with behaviour due to the costs involved. A small subset of altruists, however, pursue their prosocial desires and goals via behaviours that would be classified as antisocial. Due to the high costs involved in such behaviour, these individuals are Extreme altruists. Comparatively little research has been done on the phenomenon of Extreme Altruism. This research seeks to address that. A literature review of the Dark Triad suggested that individuals high in antisocial personality traits are not incapable of acting prosocially as originally considered. Instead, their reliance on cognitive empathy may direct them towards utilitarian approaches of helping others. Furthermore, a systematic review of the positive relationship between prosocial behaviour and antisocial traits suggests that narcissists and psychopaths are capable of altruism, if given the means to express this behaviour. The lack of evidence of such expression may be due to an inherent bias in the reporting of such individuals’ actions. An extreme altruist subculture, the Real Life Superhero movement (RLSH), was used to test the proposed relationship between antisocial traits and extreme altruism. Analysis suggests that components within the narcissism spectrum may be the most likely candidates to be related to extreme altruism. The findings of this thesis not only support the proposal that traits from within the antisocial personality continuum can fuel extreme altruism but also suggest an exciting new direction for research into altruism as a whole.
... Factor 1 (primary psychopathy) predominantly comprises the interpersonal and affective traits such as lack of empathy, grandiose sense of self, guiltlessness, and cunning behaviour where factor 2 (secondary psychopathy) primarily comprises impulsive and anti-social behaviours (Hare, 1991(Hare, , 2003Smith & Lilienfeld, 2013). In addition, primary psychopathy has more recently been considered as "successful psychopathy" as possessing low levels of empathy and guilt are considered prerequisites of success in the corporate world (Benning et al., 2018;Hare, 2003;Hill & Scott, 2019;Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010;Palmen et al., 2020). However, more recent discussions on the use of the PCL-R for identifying psychopaths has come into question (Skeem & Cooke, 2010a, 2010b. ...
... Many researchers have intimated that those leaders with psychopathic tendencies are better prepared to manage their tendencies than their imprisoned counterparts (Babiak & Hare, 2019). Additionally, they theorise that non-criminal and criminal psychopaths differ on some of the fundamental psychopathic tendencies (Benning et al., 2018;Gao & Raine, 2010;Hall & Benning, 2006;Ishikawa et al., 2001;Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010;Palmen et al., 2018Palmen et al., , 2020Smith & Lilienfeld, 2013). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A quest to find the light 2 A quest to find the light: A review of corporate and successful psychopathy literature with proposal for future development to a full research paper. Summary This is a development paper, and primarily a review of existing literature on successful psychopathy with the aim to develop it into a full research paper. An aim of the proposed research is to encourage discussion on the emerging subject area of the positive aspects of successful psychopathy in scholastic circles in addition to those of the disciplines of criminology and psychology. It would be of particular interest to stimulate debate amongst business and management academics, where there is currently a dearth of literature on the adaptive tendencies of successful psychopaths. Are they the destructive leaders so many researchers believe, or can they have positive impacts on the business they run? Word Count: 4886 A quest to find the light 3 Background and Rationale
... Although one should consider that common method variance probably slightly overestimates this effect (all variables were assessed through self-report), this result might be understood in terms of matching specificity of both variables, psychopathy and antisocial behavior in the sense that psychopathy as a latent superordinate trait (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955;Lilienfeld, 1994) is linked to an infinite variety of possible antisocial behavioral (Lynam & Miller, 2012) are strongly linked to antisocial behavior, even though no officially recorded criminal behavior was detected (Benning et al., 2018). Although the myth of the successful psychopath remains vivid (Dutton, 2012;Mullins-Sweatt, Glover, Derefinko, Miller, & Widiger, 2010), especially with an eye on white collar crimes (Babiak & Hare, 2006), the current study supports the notion that psychopathic traits are most likely associated with unfavorable outcomes, such as antisocial behavior. Other studies in the field also find rather unfavorable correlates of psychopathic traits, such as lower economic outcomes in adulthood (such as household income, Boccio & Beaver, 2015), victimization (Boccio & Beaver, 2021) or lower intelligence itself (DeLisi, Vaughn, Beaver, & Wright, 2010), especially for the selfcentered impulsivity aspects (deficits in executive functioning) of psychopathy (Eisenbarth, Hart, & Sedikides, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
The current study examined the association between psychopathy, criminal behavior, and the role of verbal intelligence. One promising approach is to examine alternative links between psychopathic traits and criminality like moderation and mediation effects by considering the potential relevance of verbal intelligence as a possible moderating variable. We hypothesized that psychopathic traits linearly predict antisocial behavior (ASB) but that a conviction because of ASB is moderated by verbal intelligence. To test a path model of this hypothesis, N = 305 participants (42% women; n = 172 inmates of German correctional facilities) filled in questionnaires to assess psychopathic traits, ASB, criminal behavior, and verbal intelligence. The moderated mediation analysis revealed that high psychopathic traits go along with a higher number of ASB, whereas individuals with higher verbal intelligence were more likely to evade detection, thus being more successful in their antisocial acts. These results sheds further light on the construct of adaptive psychopathy, supporting the notion that also non-incarcerated psychopathic individuals act highly antisocial. Only separate factors like verbal intelligence might mitigate negative consequences. Further implications for the concept of successful psychopathy are discussed.
... People scoring high on primary psychopathy can be quite successful and thrive in organizations (Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010) even though they are ruthless toward others, manipulative, callous, and egocentric (Babiak et al., 2010). The influence of so-called 'corporate psychopaths' can be damaging, particularly when they are able to reach leadership positions, because, as leaders, they usually have considerable control over one's own and others' resources (Galinsky et al., 2015) and have the means to secure and sustain their position (Anderson & Brion, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Primary psychopathy in leaders, also referred to as successful psychopathy or corporate psychopathy, has been put forward as a key determinant of corporate misconduct. In contrast to the general notion that primary psychopaths’ destructiveness cannot be controlled, we posit that psychopathic leaders’ display of self-serving and abusive behavior can be restrained by organizational contextual factors. Specifically, we hypothesize that the positive relationship between leader primary psychopathy on the one hand and self-serving behavior and abusive supervision on the other will be weaker to the extent that the organizational context (clear rules and policies, sanctionability of misconduct, and transparency of behavior) is stronger. Three studies (one experiment, one survey of leader–subordinate dyads, and one survey of teams) showed that clear rules in particular weakened the positive association between leader primary psychopathic traits and their self-serving and abusive behavior. Explanations for why clear rules rein in primary psychopathic leaders’ destructive behavior more than sanctionability of misconduct and transparency of behavior will be discussed.
... Although one should consider that common method variance probably slightly overestimates this effect (all variables were assessed through self-report), this result might be understood in terms of matching specificity of both variables, psychopathy and antisocial behavior in the sense that psychopathy as a latent superordinate trait (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955;Lilienfeld, 1994) is linked to an infinite variety of possible antisocial behavioral (Lynam & Miller, 2012) are strongly linked to antisocial behavior, even though no officially recorded criminal behavior was detected (Benning et al., 2018). Although the myth of the successful psychopath remains vivid (Dutton, 2012;Mullins-Sweatt, Glover, Derefinko, Miller, & Widiger, 2010), especially with an eye on white collar crimes (Babiak & Hare, 2006), the current study supports the notion that psychopathic traits are most likely associated with unfavorable outcomes, such as antisocial behavior. Other studies in the field also find rather unfavorable correlates of psychopathic traits, such as lower economic outcomes in adulthood (such as household income, Boccio & Beaver, 2015), victimization (Boccio & Beaver, 2021) or lower intelligence itself (DeLisi, Vaughn, Beaver, & Wright, 2010), especially for the selfcentered impulsivity aspects (deficits in executive functioning) of psychopathy (Eisenbarth, Hart, & Sedikides, 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The current study examined the association between psychopathy, criminal behavior and the role of verbal intelligence. One promising approach is to examine alternative links between psychopathic traits and criminality like moderation and mediation effects by considering the potential relevance of verbal intelligence as a possible moderating variable. We hypothesized that psychopathic traits linearly predict antisocial behavior (ASB) but that a conviction because of ASB is moderated by verbal intelligence. To test a path model of this hypothesis, N = 305 participants (42% women; n = 172 inmates of German correctional facilities) filled in questionnaires to assess psychopathic traits, ASB, criminal behavior, and verbal intelligence. The moderated mediation analysis revealed that high psychopathic traits go along with a higher number of ASB, whereas individuals with higher verbal intelligence were more likely to evade detection, thus being more successful in their antisocial acts. These results sheds further light on the construct of adaptive psychopathy, supporting the notion that also non-incarcerated psychopathic individuals act highly antisocial. Only separate factors, like verbal intelligence might mitigate negative consequences. Further implications for successful psychopathy are discussed.
... A considerable body of evidence, however, suggests that antisociality and delinquent behavior should be regarded as secondary to the axial, personality-based symptoms of the disorder (Skeem & Cook, 2010). b In the discussion around the phenomenon of psychopathy and its link to criminal and socially undesirable lifestyles, there arises the question of the existence of a non-criminal subtype of the disorder-a category of the so-called successful psychopaths (Hall & Benning, 2006;Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2010;Cigna et al., 2018). Thus, the scope of the studies on the subject includes the search for those features of psychopathy that are not directly maladaptive in nature, but enable psychopathic individuals to function normally in the social environment, and under certain conditions even foster better performance. ...
Article
Although psychopathy is commonly regarded as a disorder closely related to criminality, there is also increasing attention paid to its non-criminal form and the adaptive features of psychopathic personality. The aim of this article is to introduce the concept of psychopathic traits of good adaptation, both in light of the theoretical foundations of the construct and the empirical data. The article demonstrates the presence of such traits in classical and contemporary models of psychopathy, in the evolutionary approach, and in relation to the division into primary and secondary psychopathy. Furthermore, it reviews the studies on the prevalence of adaptive features of psychopathy in different professional groups. The attempt to operationalize the construct of psychopathic traits of good adaptation—the Durand Adaptive Psychopathic Traits Questionnaire (Durand, 2019)—is also discussed. Finally, the limitations of using the concept of adaptive traits of psychopathy are analysed.
Article
This study investigates antecedents of entrepreneurial intentions of young adults, the next generation of entrepreneurs. We determine whether aspiring entrepreneurs' positive and dark traits relate to entrepreneurial intentions. Results indicated that entrepreneurial fitness, a second-order construct comprised of multiple positive and bright traits, positively relates to entrepreneurial intention, while the dark traits of Machiavellianism and narcissism differentially relate to entrepreneurial fitness and intention. Narcissism positively relates to entrepreneurial fitness and intention both directly and indirectly. Though Machiavellianism positively and directly relates to entrepreneurial intention, it also negatively and indirectly relates to intention through a negative relationship with entrepreneurial fitness. However, overall, entrepreneurial fitness positively relates with entrepreneurial intention. Findings extend the core model of entrepreneurial intention by concurrently illuminating the adaptive and maladaptive aspects of the dark side of narcissism and Machiavellianism with the bright, positive antecedents of entrepreneurial intention with entrepreneurial fitness. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Background & Purpose: The existence of psychotic characteristics such as deception, lying and insulting others in managers cause negative attitudes and various deviant behaviors in the workplace. This research has studied the effect of psychopath managers on reducing employees' organizational trust through the occurrence of abusive supervisory behaviors and decline of job satisfaction. Methodology: This is an applied and descriptive-survey research. Its statistical population consists of employees of companies active in the metal industry of Kerman, which 350 of them have been randomly selected as a statistical sample. Data were collected by a questionnaire and analyzed by structural equation modeling and PLS software. Findings: Psychopath management had a positive effect on abusive supervision and a negative effect on job satisfaction and organizational trust. Also, although its direct effect on organizational trust has not been confirmed, it has had a negative effect on job satisfaction and organizational trust through abusive supervisory behavior. Conclusion: Identifying and correcting the psychotic characteristics of managers can prevent the occurrence of abusive supervisory behaviors and prevent the consequences of negative attitudes such as decline in organizational trust and job satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
Psychopath management, as a form of ineffective leadership, can have a negative impact on reputation and its organizational antecedents. There fore the purpose of this paper is to examine how Psychopath management indirectly through the manifestation of abusive supervision and the decline of social responsibility undermine corporates reputation. The research is applied in terms of purpose and in terms of data collection is descriptive correlational. To collect the research data, standard questionnaires of Corporate psychopathy of Mathieu etal (2014), abusive supervision of Tepper (2000), social responsibility of Lii & Lee (2012) and organizational reputation of Welch & Beatty (2009) were used. The statistical population of the study consisted of all employees of the companies operating in the metal industries of Kerman, whom 350 of them were selected by simple random sampling. Cronbach's alpha and composite reliability were used to confirm the reliability of the questionnaires, which was higher than 0.7 for all four questionnaires. The values obtained for convergent validity also indicated the validity of the questionnaires. Data were analyzed using structural equation modeling using PLS software. The findings indicate that with the presence of psychopath managers, the rate of abusive supervisory behaviors increases and the level of employees perceived social responsibility decreases. Also, Although there was no direct relationship between Psychopath management and organizational reputation, there is evidence to suggest that Psychopath management indirectly affects corporate reputation through the : ‫دریافت‬ ‫تاریخ‬ 20 / 01 / 1397 ‫پذیرش‬ ‫تاریخ‬ : 28 / 01 / 1398
Article
Full-text available
Psychopathy is a personality disorder that includes interpersonal-affective and antisocial deviance features. The Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI) contains two underlying factors (fearless dominance and impulsive antisociality) that may differentially tap these two sets of features. In a mixed-gender sample of undergraduates and prisoners, we found that PPI fearless dominance was related to low Behavioral Inhibition System activity, high Behavioral Activation System (BAS) activity, expert prototype psychopathy scores, and primary psychopathy. Impulsive antisociality was related to high BAS activity and all psychopathy measures. High Extraversion and Openness and low Neuroticism and Agreeableness predicted fearless dominance, whereas high Neuroticism and low Agreeableness and Conscientiousness predicted impulsive antisociality. Although low levels of Agreeableness predicted both PPI factors, their differential relations with other five-factor model traits highlight differences in the way psychopathy manifests itself. Consistent with movements toward assessing personality disorder using the five-factor model, the authors report regression-based equations for the clinical assessment of these psychopathy dimensions using the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-PI-R).
Article
As highlighted within several of the chapters in this volume, there are a number of important and basic questions that have yet to be resolved--some of which are crucial to continuing progress in the area of psychopathy research. These include questions about the scope of the psychopathy construct--in particular, how central aggression and anxiety are to the disorder; whether antisocial behavior is a defining feature of psychopathy; whether the construct should be viewed as unitary or configural; what mechanism or mechanisms underlie the disorder; whether distinct subgroups of psychopathic individuals exist; what procedures should be used to assess psychopathy in nonoffender (community) samples; and how "successful" psychopathy (if it exists) should be defined. Several of these key issues have been addressed in the integrative summary chapters that close each major section of the current volume. The valuable points expressed in these excellent commentaries will not be reiterated here. Instead, the goal of the present chapter is to look to Cleckley's (1941/1976) classic monograph The Mask of Sanity (see record 1941-02603-000) for insights into these vexing questions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The aim of this chapter is to review the existing literature concerning noncriminal psychopathy in an effort to clarify some of these issues. Specifically, the authors address three basic approaches to the conceptualization of the noncriminal psychopath and review both adaptive and maladaptive aspects of psychopathic personality. The authors then turn to a review of key methodological issues related to the study of psychopathy in noninstitutional settings. Special emphasis is placed on evaluating existing psychopathy assessment instruments with regard to their validity and appropriateness for use with nonincarcerated samples. This chapter also critically reviews the existing research literature on noncriminal psychopathy and concludes with recommendations for future research. First, however, the authors consider some key conceptual issues. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The current study was conducted at the Lafayette Parish Correctional Center to examine the relationship between the Big Five and inmate recidivism. The NEO Five-Factor Inventory was administered to inmates in adult education classes. The number of times each inmate had been arrested was obtained from their criminal records. Criminal records were available for 56 of the 68 male inmates who were given the five-factor inventory. Data from an additional five inmates were not included in the analyses because they did not respond to some of the items on the inventory. Multiple linear regression revealed that conscientiousness and openness to experience were negatively correlated with number of arrests. Neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness were not significantly related to number of arrests. Of particular interest was an interaction between conscientiousness and openness to experience. The combination of low conscientiousness and low openness to experience was associated with a substantial increase in number of arrests.
Article
The predominant dimensional model of general personality structure within psychology is the five-factor model (FFM). Research indicates that the personality disorders of the American Psychiatric Association's diagnostic manual can be understood as maladaptive variants of the domains and facets of the FFM. The current review provides a proposal for the classification of personality disorder from the perspective of the FFM. Discussed as well are implications and issues associated with an FFM of personality disorder, including the integration of a psychiatric nomenclature with general personality structure, the inclusion of a domain of openness to experience, the identification of problems in living associated with maladaptive personality traits, the setting of a diagnostic threshold, prototypal matching, feasibility, and clinical utility.
Article
Studies of psychopaths have been limited primarily to institutionalized populations. This article describes a methodological approach to studying noninstitutionalized psychopaths and presents data on various criteria frequently associated with the diagnosis of psychopathy. The procedure involved incorporating the characteristics of psychopathy, in a nonpejorative way, into a major Boston counterculture newspaper ad recruiting Ss for an experiment. Ss were 23 males and 5 females aged 19–47 yrs. They were assessed in several ways using the MMPI, a biographical interview, the Porteus Maze Test, a questionnaire battery (including some Eysenck Personality Inventory scales and measures of empathy, socialization, and Machiavellianism), and a delay of gratification task. Ss fulfilled the criteria for psychopathy, and the recruitment method used appears to be a fruitful one. With the present methodology, it should be possible to determine the extent to which previous results from research can be generalized to nonincarcerated psychopaths and particularly to the more successful ones. (32 ref)