ArticlePDF Available

Greater Exposure to Sexual Content in Popular Movies Predicts Earlier Sexual Debut and Increased Sexual Risk Taking

Authors:
  • Persistence Plus, LLC

Abstract and Figures

Early sexual debut is associated with risky sexual behavior and an increased risk of unplanned pregnancy and sexually transmitted infections later in life. The relations among early movie sexual exposure (MSE), sexual debut, and risky sexual behavior in adulthood (i.e., multiple sexual partners and inconsistent condom use) were examined in a longitudinal study of U.S. adolescents. MSE was measured using the Beach method, a comprehensive procedure for media content coding. Controlling for characteristics of adolescents and their families, analyses showed that MSE predicted age of sexual debut, both directly and indirectly through changes in sensation seeking. MSE also predicted engagement in risky sexual behaviors both directly and indirectly via early sexual debut. These results suggest that MSE may promote sexual risk taking both by modifying sexual behavior and by accelerating the normal rise in sensation seeking during adolescence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Psychological Science
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/07/18/0956797611435529
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611435529
published online 18 July 2012Psychological Science
Ross E. O'Hara, Frederick X. Gibbons, Meg Gerrard, Zhigang Li and James D. Sargent
Sexual Risk Taking
Greater Exposure to Sexual Content in Popular Movies Predicts Earlier Sexual Debut and Increased
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Association for Psychological Science
can be found at:Psychological ScienceAdditional services and information for
http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
What is This?
- Jul 18, 2012OnlineFirst Version of Record >>
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Psychological Science
XX(X) 1 –10
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611435529
http://pss.sagepub.com
The effects of media on adolescents’ risk behaviors, including
tobacco use (National Cancer Institute, 2008), alcohol use
(P. Anderson, de Bruijn, Angus, Gordon, & Hastings, 2009),
and aggression (C. A. Anderson et al., 2003), have been widely
documented. Relatively less is known, however, about how
media influence adolescents’ sexual behavior, including their
age of sexual debut and subsequent sexual risk taking. Early
sexual debut is associated with an increased number of sexual
partners and inconsistent condom use, as well as an increased
risk of sexually transmitted infections (STIs; Kaestle, Halpern,
Miller, & Ford, 2005). Delaying adolescents’ sexual debut,
therefore, could curb U.S. rates of STIs (more than 9 million
new cases occur annually among adolescents; Weinstock,
Berman, & Cates, 2000), and could potentially reduce instances
of unplanned pregnancy (roughly 64 unplanned pregnancies
occur for every 1,000 female adolescents age 19 or younger;
Guttmacher Institute, 2010). Identifying risk factors for early
sexual debut and sexual risk taking, therefore, is an important
public-health concern. One significant influence on engage-
ment in risky sexual behavior may be media (Wright, 2011)—
specifically, movie sexual exposure (MSE). In the study
reported here, we examined the association of MSE with sex-
ual debut and engagement in risky sexual behaviors, both
directly and indirectly through changes in sensation seeking.
Sex in the Movies
Popular movies provide adolescents with a wealth of sexual
exposure, much of which may promote risk behaviors. A survey
of movies released from 1950 to 2006 revealed that more than
84% contained sexual content (68% of G-rated movies, 82% of
PG-rated movies, 85% of PG-13-rated movies, and 88% of
R-rated movies; Nalkur, Jamieson, & Romer, 2010). Also, the
sexual explicitness of PG-13-rated and R-rated movies has
increased over the past decade (Nalkur et al., 2010). Potentially
even more important for adolescents’ sexual health, however, is
that most of these movies do not portray safe sex. A content
analysis revealed that 70% of the sexual acts depicted in movies
released from 1983 to 2003 occurred between newly acquainted
partners, 98% included no reference to contraception, and
89% resulted in no consequences (Gunasekera, Chapman, &
Campbell, 2005). Additionally, Pardun, L’Engle, and Brown
(2005) found that only 9% of sexual content in movies con-
tained messages promoting sexual health. Adolescents who
Corresponding Author:
Ross E. O’Hara, University of Missouri, 147 Psychology Building, 200 South
7th St., Columbia, MO 65211
E-mail: oharar@missouri.edu
Greater Exposure to Sexual Content in
Popular Movies Predicts Earlier Sexual
Debut and Increased Sexual Risk Taking
Ross E. O’Hara1, Frederick X. Gibbons1, Meg Gerrard2,
Zhigang Li3, and James D. Sargent4
1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College ; 2Department of Psychiatry, Geisel School
of Medicine, Dar tmouth College; 3Department of Community and Family Medicine, Geisel School of Medicine,
Dartmouth College ; and 4Department of Pediatrics, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College
Abstract
Early sexual debut is associated with risky sexual behavior and an increased risk of unplanned pregnancy and sexually
transmitted infections later in life. The relations among early movie sexual exposure (MSE), sexual debut, and risky sexual
behavior in adulthood (i.e., multiple sexual partners and inconsistent condom use) were examined in a longitudinal study
of U.S. adolescents. MSE was measured using the Beach method, a comprehensive procedure for media content coding.
Controlling for characteristics of adolescents and their families, analyses showed that MSE predicted age of sexual debut,
both directly and indirectly through changes in sensation seeking. MSE also predicted engagement in risky sexual behaviors
both directly and indirectly via early sexual debut. These results suggest that MSE may promote sexual risk taking both by
modifying sexual behavior and by accelerating the normal rise in sensation seeking during adolescence.
Keywords
mass media, sex
Received 8/24/11; Revision accepted 12/16/11
Research Article
Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on July 18, 2012 as doi:10.1177/0956797611435529
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
2 O’Hara et al.
watch popular movies, therefore, are exposed to a great deal of
sex, most of which is portrayed in an unrealistic and/or risk-
promoting manner.
How Media Influence Sexual Behavior
Wright (2011) posited that the effect of media on sexual
behavior is driven by the acquisition and activation of sexual
scripts. Scripts provide behavioral options in social situations,
including those that may lead to sexual behavior, and the con-
tent of scripts is often influenced by media. As mentioned in
the previous section, movies generally offer permissive and
risky sexual messages to viewers (Gunasekera et al., 2005;
Nalkur et al., 2010), and a higher degree of sexual media expo-
sure has been found to predict more permissive sexual atti-
tudes (Bleakley, Hennessy, Fishbein, Coles, & Jordan, 2009;
Brown, Halpern, & L’Engle, 2005). Furthermore, adolescents
sometimes seek out sexual media, possibly to learn these
scripts (Brown et al., 2005). In fact, 57% of U.S. adolescents
(ages 14–16) reported using media as a primary source of sex-
ual information (Bleakley et al., 2009).
Activated scripts must be applied to guide behavior, and
media can influence which sexual scripts are used (Wright,
2011). Among adolescents with high MSE, the sexual scripts
portrayed in movies may be readily accessible because of the
frequency of prior activations. The more easily a script is acti-
vated, the more likely it is to be applied in a given situation. In
fact, longitudinal studies have shown that a greater degree of
television sexual exposure predicts greater engagement in
noncoital sexual activities among adolescents (Collins et al.,
2004) and earlier sexual debut (Ashby, Arcari, & Edmonson,
2006; Collins et al., 2004; Martino, Collins, Kanouse, Elliott,
& Berry, 2005), controlling for demographic factors, religios-
ity, and parenting. Furthermore, greater exposure to sexual
content in media, including movies, television, music, and
magazines, has been associated with both a higher likelihood
of engaging in noncoital sexual activities among adolescents
(Brown et al., 2006; L’Engle, Brown, & Kenneavy, 2006;
Pardun et al., 2005) and earlier sexual debut (Brown et al.,
2006), controlling for adolescents’ and their parents’ charac-
teristics. Finally, greater exposure to movies and men’s maga-
zines has been associated with earlier sexual debut and a
greater number of casual partners among male college stu-
dents, effects mediated by sexual norms and beliefs (Ward,
Epstein, Caruthers, & Merriwether, 2011). These results lend
support to Wright’s model by showing that sexual media influ-
ence both sexual attitudes and behavior.
Effects of Movies on Adolescents’
Risk Behavior
Despite the plethora of sex depicted in movies and the popu-
larity of movies among youth, much more research has been
devoted to the influence of television on adolescents’ sexual
behavior. We were interested in the effect of MSE on sexual
debut and engagement in risky sexual behaviors because evi-
dence suggests that adolescents’ sexual attitudes and behavior
are influenced more by movies than by other forms of media
(Bleakley et al., 2009; Pardun et al., 2005). For example, in a
study of male college students (Ward et al., 2011), only expo-
sure to movies (as compared with exposure to television,
music videos, and men’s magazines) directly predicted age of
sexual debut, and exposure to movies (along with exposure to
men’s magazines) indirectly predicted number of casual sex-
ual partners. Moreover, longitudinal studies of movies’ influ-
ence on adolescents’ substance use have shown strong and
consistent effects: Exposure to tobacco use in movies predicts
initiation and escalation of smoking (Dalton et al., 2009;
Sargent et al., 2007; Wills et al., 2010), and exposure to drink-
ing in movies predicts initiation and escalation of alcohol use
(Dal Cin et al., 2009; Gibbons et al., 2010; Sargent, Wills,
Stoolmiller, Gibson, & Gibbons, 2006; Wills et al., 2010).
Our goal in the study reported here was to examine the
effects of early MSE (i.e., before age 16) on age of sexual
debut and risky sexual behaviors (i.e., multiple sexual partners
and inconsistent condom use) in adulthood. These relations
were assessed using data from a longitudinal study of
U.S. adolescents (Sargent et al., 2007). We employed the
Beach method to estimate participants’ exposure to risky sex-
ual behaviors portrayed in movies (Sargent, Worth, Beach,
Gerrard, & Heatherton, 2008). This method entails second-by-
second coding of risk behaviors in movies to maximize valid-
ity and reliability, and it allowed for a more comprehensive
sampling of popular movies than has been used in previous
studies. It has been validated in studies among youth of mov-
ies’ effects on smoking (e.g., Sargent et al., 2007) and on alco-
hol use and alcohol-related problems (e.g., Dal Cin et al.,
2009). The current study was the first to use the Beach method
to estimate MSE and examine its relations with sexual debut
and risky sexual behaviors.
Effects of Movies on Sensation Seeking
There is reason to believe that MSE influences sexual behav-
ior indirectly by increasing sensation seeking—the tendency
to seek novel and intense stimulation (Arnett, 1994). Sensa-
tion seeking rises during adolescence, peaking between the
ages of 10 and 15, and then declines through late adolescence
(Steinberg et al., 2008). Greater sensation seeking is associ-
ated with both earlier sexual debut (Donohew et al., 2000) and
more frequent engagement in casual sex in adulthood (Arnett,
1994). It is important to note that sensation seeking arises from
both biological and socialization factors (Arnett, 1994), which
suggests that environmental influences, such as MSE, could
affect the development of this trait. In fact, research with the
sample used in the current study showed that watching R-rated
movies was associated with later increases in sensation seek-
ing during adolescence (but not vice versa), which in turn
increased the risk of tobacco and alcohol use during adoles-
cence (de Leeuw et al., 2011; Stoolmiller, Gerrard, Sargent,
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Movie Sexual Exposure 3
Worth, & Gibbons, 2010). However, the mediating effect of
sensation seeking has not been tested, to our knowledge, with
respect to media’s influence on sexual behavior. In the current
study, therefore, we examined whether changes in sensation
seeking mediated the anticipated relations of MSE with sexual
debut and with risky sexual behaviors.
The Current Study
This study expanded upon past research in several ways. First,
previous studies combined MSE with exposure to sexual con-
tent in other media (e.g., Brown et al., 2006), thereby obscur-
ing the effect of MSE. In our analysis, we focused exclusively
on MSE. Second, the Beach method allowed for the content
coding of more than 600 popular movies released over a 6-year
period, a much larger sampling than has been used in previous
research. Third, few studies of movies’ effects on sexual
behavior have been longitudinal; the duration over which the
data analyzed in our study were collected allowed us to exam-
ine both sexual debut and postdebut sexual outcomes that
could result in STIs or unplanned pregnancies. Finally, this
study was the first to examine whether media effects on
engagement in risky sexual behaviors are mediated by changes
in sensation seeking. Specifically, our hypotheses were as
follows:
Hypothesis 1: Early MSE predicts age of sexual
debut, an effect mediated by increases in sensation
seeking.
Hypothesis 2: Early MSE predicts engagement in
risky sexual behaviors (i.e., increased number of
sexual partners and frequency of casual sex without a
condom) approximately 6 years later, an effect medi-
ated by age of sexual debut.
Method
Participants and procedure
These data were collected in a six-wave longitudinal study
spanning from June 2003 to October 2009. At Time 1, data
were collected in a random-digit-dial telephone survey of
6,522 adolescents, from 10 to 14 years of age, living in
the United States. The subsequent three follow-up surveys
were conducted approximately every 8 months; the final two
follow-ups occurred approximately 5 years and 7 years after
Time 1. At Time 6, 2,718 participants responded (38.2% reten-
tion), but only participants who were 18 years of age or older
(n = 1,300) were asked to report their sexual behavior. To
ensure that MSE had occurred before sexual debut, we omitted
from the analysis participants whose sexual debut occurred
before Time 2 (n = 72), which left a final sample of 1,228 par-
ticipants. Participants in the final sample were between 12 and
14 years old at Time 1 (M = 12.89 years, SD = 0.79) and
between 18 and 21 years old at Time 6 (M = 18.90 years, SD =
0.81). The sample comprised 611 males (49.8%) and 617
females (50.2%); 891 were European American (72.6%), 159
were Hispanic (12.9%), 71 were African American (5.8%),
and 107 were of other racial or ethnic backgrounds (8.7%).
Participants lost at follow-up were at greater risk for early
sexual debut and engagement in risky sexual behavior at Time
1 than were those who were retained in the sample. The par-
ticipants who were not retained reported higher MSE and sen-
sation seeking and lower maternal responsiveness, and were
more likely to have a television in their bedroom (ps < .001).
Also, significantly more minorities than European Americans
were lost at follow-up (p < .02).
Measures
MSE was measured using the Beach method. At Time 1, the
523 top-grossing movies released between 1998 and 2003
were coded for the number of seconds of sexual content, which
was defined as instances of sexual behavior, such as heavy
kissing or intercourse. Each movie was rated by one of two
trained coders, and a random subsample of 10% of the movies
was double-coded (interrater agreement: r = .92). Each partici-
pant received a unique list of 50 movies randomly selected
from the larger pool and reported which of those movies he or
she had seen. These data were used to extrapolate participants’
total exposure to sexual content from all 523 movies. The
same procedure was used at Time 2 with a smaller pool of
movies (161), comprising the top-grossing movies released
since the previous content coding. (The number of seconds of
sexual content in selected representative movies is presented
in Table S1 in the Supplemental Material available online.) We
calculated MSE by converting seconds of sexual content into
hours, summing the hours of sexual content viewed at Time 1
and Time 2, and performing a square-root transformation to
correct for positive skew.
Sensation seeking was measured with a four-item scale
designed for children (Time 1: α = .60; Time 2: α = .58;
Stephenson, Hoyle, Palmgreen, & Slater, 2003). This measure
tapped two of four constructs identified by Zuckerman (1994)
as important components of sensation seeking, thrill/adventure
seeking and boredom susceptibility; in addition, it tapped inten-
sity seeking, a component of the Arnett Inventory of Sensation
Seeking (Arnett, 1994). Participants responded to each item
using a scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating higher
sensation seeking. Each participant’s scores were summed. This
measure has been validated for predicting adolescents’ tobacco
and alcohol use (de Leeuw et al., 2011; Stoolmiller et al., 2010).
Age of sexual debut was reported by participants at Time 6.
Risky sexual behavior was measured at Time 6 and comprised
two components: lifetime number of vaginal- or oral-sex part-
ners (open response) and number of instances of casual sex
(defined as vaginal sex not with a “serious or steady dating
partner”) without a condom (reported using a scale from 0,
never, to 5, five or more times). Scores for these two items
were recoded into ordinal variables and combined, α = .62.1
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
4 O’Hara et al.
Covariates related to both MSE and sexual behavior
(including sensation seeking) were measured at Time 1. Gen-
der, race, and age were reported by participants’ parents. Par-
ticipants reported how often they attended church or engaged
in religious activities, how many hours of television they
viewed each day, whether they had a television in their bed-
room, and with whom they lived (a measure used to code fam-
ily structure as either intact or divided). Participants also
completed a nine-item maternal-responsiveness measure (α =
.71) and a seven-item maternal-demandingness measure (α =
.59; Jackson, Henriksen, & Foshee, 1998). Finally, we con-
trolled for MSE that occurred between Time 2 and partici-
pants’ sexual debut.2 Including this covariate allowed us to
focus specifically on early MSE (i.e., before age 16) as a pre-
dictor of sexual debut and engagement in risky sexual behav-
iors, controlling for subsequent MSE.
Results
Descriptive statistics
The median MSE was 0.93 hr (interquartile range: 0.43 hr–
1.32 hr). Sensation seeking was generally low, M = 7.90 (SD =
2.39) at Time 1 and M = 8.07 (SD = 2.32) at Time 2. By Time
6, 774 participants (63.0%) had sexually debuted: 40 (5.2%)
before age 15, 79 (10.2%) at age 15, 190 (24.5%) at age 16,
223 (28.8%) at age 17, and 242 (31.2%) at age 18 or older.
Among sexually active participants, the median number of
lifetime sexual partners was 2 (interquartile range: 1–4 part-
ners), and 195 of these participants (25.2%) reported that they
had had casual sex without a condom.
Gender differences
Male and female participants were equally likely to have sexu-
ally debuted by Time 6; moreover, males and females sexually
debuted at approximately the same age and reported similar
MSE. Males, however, reported having more sexual partners
(M = 3.43, SD = 5.94) than did females (M = 2.48, SD = 3.91),
t(1221) = 3.48, p = .001, and engaging in casual sex without a
condom more frequently (M = 0.43, SD = 1.14) than did
females (M = 0.29, SD = 0.87), t(1223) = 2.37, p < .02. Males
also reported higher sensation seeking than females at both
Time 1 and Time 2, ts( 1195) 3.70, ps < .001.
Zero-order correlations
Table 1 displays the full correlation matrix, by gender. Higher
MSE was associated with earlier sexual debut, more sexual
partners, more frequent casual sex without a condom, and
higher sensation seeking for both genders, ps < .001. The rela-
tionship between MSE and sexual debut, however, was sig-
nificantly stronger for males, r(595) = .33, than for females,
r(585) = .21; z = 2.19, p < .03. Higher sensation seeking
was also associated with earlier sexual debut, more sexual
partners, and more frequent casual sex without a condom
among both genders, ps < .01. Finally, earlier sexual debut was
associated with more sexual partners and more frequent casual
sex without a condom for both genders, ps < .001.
Survival analysis
To examine predictors of sexual debut, we performed a Cox
proportional-hazards regression with MSE at Times 1 and 2,
sensation seeking at Time 2, and the covariates entered into
the model (Table 2). Omnibus tests of the model’s coefficients
showed that the model was significant, χ2(13, N = 1,133) =
805.01, p < .001. The hazard ratio for MSE was 5.38, p < .001,
indicating that for each 1-hr increase in MSE on a square-root
scale, the hazard of debut at each age increased more than 5
times. Other significant predictors of sexual debut included
change in sensation seeking (hazard ratio = 1.11, p < .001),
gender (females debuted later than males; hazard ratio =
0.81, p = .006), family structure (participants from divided
homes debuted earlier than did those from intact homes; haz-
ard ratio = 1.22, p = .030), and having a television in the bed-
room (hazard ratio = 1.20, p = .024). The survival analysis was
also performed separately for each gender. The model for each
gender was significant, but the influence of MSE on sexual
debut was stronger for males (hazard ratio = 6.71, p < .001)
than for females (hazard ratio = 4.24, p < .001). Including an
MSE × Gender interaction in the model showed this difference
to be significant, p = .01 (see Fig. 1).
Structural equation model
A structural equation model predicting risky sexual behavior
at Time 6 was evaluated using the robust weighted least
squares approach in Mplus 6.12 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-
2007). Sexual debut was recoded as an ordinal variable (1 = 14
years of age or younger, 2 = 15 years of age, 3 = 16 years of
age, 4 = 17 years of age, 5 = 18 years of age or older; partici-
pants who were virgins at Time 6 were coded as 5). MSE
summed from Times 1 and 2 was exogenous in the model;
sensation seeking at Time 2, age of sexual debut, and risky
sexual behavior at Time 6 were endogenous. Sensation seek-
ing was specified as a multi-indicator manifest variable, MSE
and age of sexual debut were specified as single-indicator
manifest variables, and risky sexual behavior at Time 6 was
specified as a latent variable with two indicators: number of
lifetime partners (factor loading = .90) and instances of casual
sex without a condom (factor loading = .81).
The structural model (Fig. 2) provided an excellent fit to
the data, χ2(12, N = 1,133) = 11.11, p > .51; root-mean-square
error of approximation (RMSEA) < .001; confirmatory fit
index = 1.00; Tucker-Lewis index > 1.00. This model explained
72% of the variance in age of sexual debut and 58% of
the variance in risky sexual behavior at Time 6. Results sup-
ported Hypothesis 1: The indirect effect of MSE on age of
sexual debut via changes in sensation seeking was significant,
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
5
Table 1.Correlations Between Study Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. Age of sexual debut .48*** .37*** .21*** .09* .13*** .24*** .30*** .12** .17*** .08* .14*** .07 .14***
2. Total number of life-
time sexual partners
.43*** .56*** .22*** .06 .07 .18*** .22*** .10* .14*** .09* .11** .00 .05
3. Instances of casual sex
without a condom
.29*** .49*** .15*** .03 .10* .12** .16*** .15*** .08* .13*** .07 .01 .07
4. Time 1 + Time 2 MSE .33*** .28*** .22*** .12** .24*** .31*** .29*** .14*** .13** .12** .11** .08 .19***
5. Daily television view-
ing at Time 1
.02 .05 .03 .05 .22*** .15*** .13*** .02 .01 .01 .15*** .13*** .06
6. Television in bedroom
at Time 1
.09* .08* .06 .21*** .16*** .16*** .21*** .01 .05 .11** .11** .12** .01
7. Sensation seeking at
Time 1
.28*** .26*** .24*** .37*** .07 .09* .67*** .16*** .42*** .25*** .07 .08* .11**
8. Sensation seeking at
Time 2
.33*** .25*** .22*** .32*** .06 .07* .65*** .14*** .34*** .23*** .11** .10* .04
9. Engagement in
religious activities at
Time 1
.13*** .17*** .15*** .14*** .13*** .03 .10* .11** .12** .20*** .04 .03 .07*
10. Maternal responsive-
ness at Time 1
.08* .13*** .12** .12** .06 .05 .32*** .24*** .06 .46*** .06 .07 .02
11. Maternal demanding-
ness at Time 1
.02 .08* .11** .14*** .01 .02 .15*** .12** .12** .33*** — .03 .02 .09*
12. Family structure at
Time 1
.13** .04 .07* .17*** .13** .14*** .14*** .14*** .07 .12** .02 .22*** .09*
13. Race .01 .06 .01 .13*** .05 .14*** .06 .06 .06 .08* .05 .09* — .03
14. Age .07 .14*** .07* .22*** .02 .02 .13*** .12** .05 .08* .16*** .05 .01 —
Note: Correlations below the diagonal are for males (pairwise n 591); correlations above the diagonal are for females (pairwise n 582). Movie sexual exposure (MSE) was square-root trans-
formed. Family structure at Time 1 was coded 0 for intact and 1 for divided. Race was coded 0 for minority and 1 for European American.
*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
6 O’Hara et al.
Table 2.Results of the Cox Regression for Age of Sexual Debut
Overall Males (n = 572) Females (n = 561)
Predictor βHazard ratio βHazard ratio βHazard ratio
Media variables
Time 1 + Time 2 MSE 1.68*** 5.38*** 1.90*** 6.71*** 1.45*** 4.24***
Later MSE –2.70*** 0.07*** –2.70*** 0.07*** –2.75*** 0.06***
Daily television viewing at Time 1 0.01 1.01 0.01 1.01 0.02 1.02
Television in bedroom at Time 1 0.18* 1.20* 0.18 1.20 0.22 1.24
Family variables
Maternal responsiveness at Time 1 –0.09 0.99 –0.10 0.99 –0.01 0.99
Maternal demandingness at Time 1 0.01 1.01 0.02 1.02 0.00 1.00
Engagement in religious activities at Time 1 –0.08 0.93 –0.07 0.93 –0.07 0.93
Family structure at Time 1 0.20* 1.22* 0.21 1.23 0.17 1.18
Individual variables
Sensation seeking at Time 1 0.02 1.02 0.03 1.03 0.00 1.00
Sensation seeking at Time 2 0.10*** 1.11*** 0.12*** 1.13*** 0.09** 1.09**
Age –0.51*** 0.60*** –0.46*** 0.63*** –0.56*** 0.57***
Race 0.08 1.08 0.11 1.12 0.04 1.04
Gender –0.21** 0.81**
Note: Movie sexual exposure (MSE) was square-root transformed. Family structure was coded 0 for intact and 1 for divided. Race was coded 0
for minority and 1 for European American. Gender was coded 0 for female and 1 for male.
*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.
10 12 14 16 18 20 22
Age (Years)
Female, High MSE
Male, High MSE
Male, Low MSE
Female, Low MSE
.0
.2
.4
.6
.8
1.0
Proportion Yet to Sexually Debut
Fig. 1.Adjusted survival curves for age of sexual debut for males and
females with high versus low movie sexual exposure (MSE), as determined on
the basis of median splits.
β = 0.01, p < .002 (MSE changes in sensation seeking:
β = 0.09, p < .001; changes in sensation seeking age of
sexual debut: β = 0.14, p < .001). Also, MSE directly pre-
dicted age of sexual debut, β = 0.33, p < .001. Results also
supported Hypothesis 2: MSE indirectly predicted risky
sexual behavior at Time 6. The indirect effect of MSE on risky
sexual behavior at Time 6 via age of sexual debut was signifi-
cant, β = 0.21, p < .001 (age of sexual debut risky sexual
behaviors at Time 6: β = 0.64, p < .001), as was the indirect
effect via changes in sensation seeking and age of sexual
debut, β = 0.01, p < .005. Finally, MSE directly predicted risky
sexual behavior at Time 6, β = 0.10, p < .05.
This model was modified to allow paths to vary by gender.
The multigroup model also provided an excellent fit to the
data, χ2(43, N = 1,133) = 30.38, p > .92; RMSEA < .001; con-
firmatory fit index = 1.00; Tucker-Lewis index > 1.00. Releasing
the equality constraint on the path from MSE to age of sexual
debut significantly improved model fit, χ2(1, N = 1,133) = 8.28,
p < .005. The direct effect of MSE on age of sexual debut was
stronger for males, b = 2.41, p < .001, than for females, b =
1.38, p < .001; however, the total indirect effects of MSE on
risky sexual behavior at Time 6 were similar for males, β =
0.24, p < .001, and females, β = 0.17, p < .001.
Discussion
Higher early MSE (before age 16) predicted more risky sexual
behaviors (i.e., a higher number of lifetime sexual partners and
more frequent casual sex without a condom) in adulthood, and
it did so both directly and indirectly, via earlier sexual debut.
This result supports previous findings that sexual media diet
predicts age of sexual debut (e.g., Brown et al., 2006), and it
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Movie Sexual Exposure 7
extends those findings to suggest that MSE has a lasting influ-
ence on risky sexual behaviors in adulthood (Ward et al.,
2011). MSE also predicted sexual debut indirectly through an
increase in sensation seeking. This finding provides further
evidence that exposure to movies with sexual content may
accelerate the normal rise in sensation seeking during adoles-
cence (Steinberg et al., 2008), thereby promoting risky behav-
ior generally (de Leeuw et al., 2011; Stoolmiller et al., 2010).
Finally, the influence of MSE on sexual debut and risky sexual
behavior at Time 6 was stronger among males than females,
although its influence on sensation seeking was similar
between the genders. It is worth noting that the sizes of
the effects of MSE on sexual behavior ranged from medium
(|.33|) to small (|.01|). However, the largest direct effect was
found for the influence of MSE on sexual debut. These results
suggest that MSE may have a greater impact on other potential
mediating mechanisms, such as changes in attitudes (Brown
et al., 2005) or sexual scripts (Wright, 2011). Given the preva-
lence of MSE among adolescents, we believe that even small
effects of MSE have important implications for adolescents’
sexual health.
Reducing risky sexual behaviors
Our results suggest that restricting adolescents’ MSE would
delay their sexual debut and also reduce their engagement in
risky sexual behaviors later in life. This strategy could attenu-
ate the direct influence of media on adolescents’ sexual behav-
ior by limiting the acquisition of risky sexual scripts and/or
reducing their likelihood of activation (Wright, 2011). In addi-
tion, restricting MSE may retard the increase in sensation
seeking normally experienced during adolescence (Steinberg
et al., 2008), which, in turn, could delay sexual debut and sub-
sequent engagement in risky sexual behaviors (Arnett, 1994;
Donohew et al., 2000). Limiting youths’ MSE may be a diffi-
cult task, however, given the copious amounts of sex portrayed
in movies (Gunasekera et al., 2005; Nalkur et al., 2010).
One promising approach would involve incorporating media-
literacy training into sexual education. A recent intervention
showed that a peer-led sexual-media-literacy curriculum
increased ninth-grade students’ self-efficacy in resisting peer
pressure with regard to sexual behavior, reduced their percep-
tion of the normative prevalence of sexual activity during ado-
lescence, and improved their attitudes toward abstinence
(Pinkleton, Austin, Cohen, Chen, & Fitzgerald, 2008).
Limitations and future directions
Some limitations of our study should be acknowledged. First,
participants lost at follow-up were at greater risk for early
sexual debut and risky sexual behavior than were those
retained in the study, a pattern typical in longitudinal research
Time 6
Instances of
Casual
Sex Without
Condom
R 2 = .72
Time 6
Risky Sex
R 2 = .58
Sexual
Debut
0.10*
Time 1 +
Time 2
MSE
Sensation
Seeking
Time 6
Lifetime
Partners
(–0.33***)
0.09***
–0.14***
–0.64***
0.90***
0.81***
–2.41***
–1.38*** Covariates
Sensation Seeking at Time 1
Maternal Responsiveness at Time 1
Maternal Demandingness at Time 1
Engagement in Religious Activities
at Time 1
Daily TV Viewing at Time 1
TV in Bedroom at Time 1
Gender
Race
Age
Later MSE
Fig. 2.Ef fects of movie sexual exposure (M SE) on risky sexual behavior as mediated by changes in sensation
seeking and age of sexual debut. MSE was measured at Times 1 and 2; the two components of risky sexual
behavior (i.e., number of lifetime sexual partners and instances of sex without a condom) were me asured
at Time 6. Coefficients for the moderated path from MSE to sexual debut are shown separately for males
(above the line) and females (below the line), with the coefficient for the whole sample noted in parentheses.
Asterisks indicate significant paths (*p .05, ***p .0 01).
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
8 O’Hara et al.
(Boys et al., 2005). This biased attrition may have resulted in
an underestimation of the true effect of MSE on sexual out-
comes. Second, our results may not generalize to nations with
sexual-education curricula and sexual norms that differ from
those of the United States, although effects of media exposure
on alcohol and tobacco use have been found to be similar
among U.S. adolescents and samples from other countries
(e.g., Morgenstern et al., 2011). Third, participants did not
report their sexual behavior until they were at least 18 years
old, and their retrospective memory for age of sexual debut,
number of sexual partners, and instances of casual sex without
a condom may therefore have been biased. This would be more
problematic if these biases were associated with MSE (e.g., if
adolescents who watched more movies with sexual content
were more likely to exaggerate their sexual experience).
Our data also did not include measures of other factors that
may confound relations between MSE and sexual behavior,
such as the sexual behavior of siblings and peers, parental atti-
tudes toward sex, and pubertal status (although we controlled
for age). Likewise, we were unable to examine cognitive or
psychosocial mediators of the effect of MSE on age of sexual
debut and engagement in risky sexual behaviors. Previous
studies using the same data for these mediators have shown
that movies’ effects on substance use are mediated by changes
in perceived favorability of typical substance users (i.e., sub-
stance-user prototypes), behavioral willingness to use sub-
stances, expectancies regarding substance use, and substance
use among peers (Dal Cin et al., 2009; Gibbons et al., 2010;
Wills et al., 2010). Future research should examine potential
mediators to explore why seeing sex on the big screen trans-
lates into having sex in the real world.
Future studies should also attempt to differentiate the
effects of MSE from the effects of exposure to portrayals of
other risk behaviors in popular movies, especially with regard
to changes in sensation seeking. It is unclear whether changes
in sensation seeking were related specifically to MSE or to
other co-occurring elements of adult-oriented movies (e.g.,
alcohol use; Stoolmiller et al., 2010). An important avenue for
future work will be to examine whether the effects of MSE on
sexual behavior are partly attributable to exposure to portray-
als of drinking in movies and subsequent alcohol use (e.g., Dal
Cin et al., 2009), given that adolescents’ alcohol use and risky
sexual behaviors are inherently intertwined (Cooper, 2002).
Finally, our results may have been moderated by race. Afri-
can Americans tend to sexually debut at a younger age, engage
in more risky sexual behaviors, and contract more STIs than
do European Americans (Cavazos-Rehg et al., 2009; Halpern
et al., 2004; Kaestle et al., 2005). However, African Americans
also tend to be less responsive than European Americans to
media depictions of sex (Brown et al., 2006) and alcohol use
(Gibbons et al., 2010). Unfortunately, our study’s sample
included too few African Americans for us to test moderation
by race. Future research may allow for a better understanding
of when and how movies influence youth, and how to impede
this influence in order to promote healthier sexual behavior.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
This research was funded by National Institutes of Health Grants
CA077026 and AA015591 to James D. Sargent.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss.sagepub
.com/content/by/supplemental-data
Notes
1.Participants who were virgins at Time 6 were coded as never hav-
ing had casual sex without a condom. However, because number of
lifetime partners included oral-sex partners, 105 participants who
were virgins (23.1% of virgins) had a risky-sexual-behavior score
greater than zero.
2.For example, this measure comprised MSE at Time 3 for partici-
pants whose sexual debut was prior to Time 4, but comprised MSE
at Times 3, 4, and 5 for participants whose sexual debut was prior to
Time 6.
References
Anderson, C. A., Berkowitz, L., Donnerstein, E., Huesmann, L. R.,
Johnson, J. D., Linz, D., . . . Wartella, E. N. M. (2003). The influ-
ence of media violence on youth. Psychological Science in the
Public Interest, 4, 81–110.
Anderson, P., de Bruijn, A., Angus, K., Gordon, R., & Hastings, G.
(2009). Impact of alcohol advertising and media exposure on
adolescent alcohol use: A systematic review of longitudinal stud-
ies. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44, 229–243.
Arnett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a
new scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 289–296.
Ashby, S. L., Arcari, C. M., & Edmonson, M. B. (2006). Televi-
sion viewing and risk of sexual initiation by young adolescents.
Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 375–380.
Bleakley, A., Hennessy, M., Fishbein, M., Coles, H. C., & Jordan,
A. (2009). How sources of sexual information relate to adoles-
cents’ beliefs about sex. American Journal of Health Behavior,
33, 37–48.
Boys, A., Marsden, J., Stillwell, G., Hatchings, K., Griffiths, P., &
Farrell, M. (2005). Minimizing respondent attrition in longitudi-
nal research: Practical implications from a cohort study of adoles-
cent drinking. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 363–373.
Brown, J. D., Halpern, C. T., & L’Engle, K. L. (2005). Mass media
as a sexual super peer for early maturing girls. Journal of Adoles-
cent Health, 36, 420–427.
Brown, J. D., L’Engle, K. L., Pardun, C. J., Guo, G., Kenneavy, K.,
& Jackson, C. (2006). Sexy media matter: Exposure to sexual
content in music, movies, television, and magazines predicts
Black and White adolescents’ sexual behavior. Pediatrics, 11 7 ,
1018–1027.
Cavazos-Rehg, P. A., Krauss, M. J., Spitznagel, E. L., Schootman,
M., Bucholz, K. K., Peipert, J. F., . . . Bierut, L. J. (2009). Age of
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Movie Sexual Exposure 9
sexual debut among U.S. adolescents. Contraception, 80, 158–
162.
Collins, R. L., Elliott, M. N., Berry, S. H., Kanouse, D. E., Kunkel,
D., Hunter, S. B., & Miu, A. (2004). Watching sex on television
predicts adolescent initiation of sexual behavior. Pediatrics, 114,
e280–e289. Retrieved from http://www.pediatricsdigest.mobi/
content/114/3/e280.full
Cooper, M. L. (2002). Alcohol use and risky sexual behavior among
college students and youth: Evaluating the evidence. Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 14, 101–117.
Dal Cin, S., Worth, K. A., Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., Stoolmiller,
M., Wills, T. A., & Sargent, J. D. (2009). Watching and drinking:
Expectancies, prototypes, and friends’ alcohol use mediate the
effect of exposure to alcohol use in movies on adolescent drink-
ing. Health Psychology, 28, 473–483.
Dalton, M. A., Beach, M. L., Adachi-Mejia, A. M., Longacre,
M. R., Matzkin, A. L., Sargent, J. D., . . . Titus-Ernstoff, L. (2009).
Early exposure to movie smoking predicts established smoking
by older teens and young adults. Pediatrics, 123, 551–558.
de Leeuw, R. N. H., Sargent, J. D., Stoolmiller, M., Scholte, R. H. J.,
Engels, R. C. M. E., & Tanski, S. E. (2011). Association of smok-
ing onset with R-rated movie restrictions and adolescent sensa-
tion seeking. Pediatrics, 127, 96–105.
Donohew, L., Zimmerman, R., Cupp, P. S., Novak, S., Colon, S., &
Abell, R. (2000). Sensation seeking, impulsive decision-making,
and risky sex: Implications for risk-taking and design of inter-
ventions. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 1079–1091.
Gibbons, F. X., Pomery, E. A., Gerrard, M., Sargent, J. D., Weng,
C., Wills, T. A., . . . Yeh, H. (2010). Media as social influence:
Racial differences in the effects of peers and media on adolescent
alcohol cognitions and consumption. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 24, 649–659.
Gunasekera, H., Chapman, S., & Campbell, S. (2005). Sex and drugs
in popular movies: An analysis of the top 200 films. Journal of
the Royal Society of Medicine, 98, 464–470.
Guttmacher Institute. (2010). U.S. teenage pregnancies, births, and
abortions: National and state trends and trends by race and eth-
nicity. Retrieved from http://www.guttmacher.org/pubs/USTP-
trends.pdf
Halpern, C. T., Hallfors, D., Bauer, D. J., Iritani, B., Waller, M. W.,
& Cho, H. (2004). Implications of racial and gender differences
in patterns of adolescent risk behavior for HIV and other sexually
transmitted diseases. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive
Health, 36, 239–247.
Jackson, C., Henriksen, L., & Foshee, V. A. (1998). The Authoritative
Parenting Index: Predicting health risk behaviors among children
and adolescents. Health Education & Behavior, 25, 319–337.
Kaestle, C. E., Halpern, C. T., Miller, W. C., & Ford, C. A. (2005).
Young age at first sexual intercourse and sexually transmitted
infections in adolescents and young adults. American Journal of
Epidemiology, 161, 774–780.
L’Engle, K. L., Brown, J. D., & Kenneavy, K. (2006). The mass
media are an important context for adolescents’ sexual behavior.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, 186–192.
Martino, S. C., Collins, R. L., Kanouse, D. E., Elliott, M., & Berry,
S. H. (2005). Social cognitive processes mediating the relation-
ship between exposure to television’s sexual content and ado-
lescents’ sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89, 914–924.
Morgenstern, M., Poelen, E. A. P., Scholte, R., Karlsdottir, S.,
Jonsson, S. H., Mathis, F., . . . Hanewinkel, R. (2011). Smoking
in movies and adolescent smoking: Cross-cultural study in six
European countries. Thorax, 66, 875–883.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2007). Mplus user’s guide
(5th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Nalkur, P. G., Jamieson, P. E., & Romer, D. (2010). The effectiveness
of the Motion Picture Association of America’s rating system in
screening explicit violence and sex in top-ranked movies from
1950 to 2006. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 440–447.
National Cancer Institute. (2008, June). The role of the media in pro-
moting and reducing tobacco use (Tobacco Control Monograph
No. 19; NIH Pub. No. 07–6242). Bethesda, MD: U.S. Depart-
ment of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of
Health, National Cancer Institute.
Pardun, C. J., L’Engle, K. L., & Brown, J. D. (2005). Linking expo-
sure to outcomes: Early adolescents’ consumption of sexual con-
tent in six media. Mass Communication and Society, 8, 75–91.
Pinkleton, B. E., Austin, E. W., Cohen, M., Chen, Y., & Fitzgerald, E.
(2008). Effects of a peer-led media literacy curriculum on adoles-
cents’ knowledge and attitudes toward sexual behavior and media
portrayals of sex. Health Communication, 23, 462–472.
Sargent, J. D., Stoolmiller, M., Worth, K. A., Dal Cin, S., Wills,
T. A., Gibbons, F. X., . . . Tanski, S. (2007). Exposure to smok-
ing depictions in movies: Its association with established ado-
lescent smoking. Archives of Pediatric & Adolescent Medicine,
161, 849–856.
Sargent, J. D., Wills, T. A., Stoolmiller, M., Gibson, J., & Gibbons,
F. X. (2006). Alcohol use in motion pictures and its relation with
early-onset teen drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67,
54–65.
Sargent, J. D., Worth, K. A., Beach, M., Gerrard, M., & Heatherton,
T. F. (2008). Population-based assessment of exposure to risk
behavior in motion pictures. Communication Methods and Mea-
sures, 2, 134–151.
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., &
Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and
impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a
dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764–1778.
Stephenson, M. T., Hoyle, R. H., Palmgreen, P., & Slater, M. D.
(2003). Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and
large-scale surveys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 72, 279–
286.
Stoolmiller, M., Gerrard, M., Sargent, J. D., Worth, K. A., & Gibbons,
F. X. (2010). R-rated movie viewing, growth in sensation seek-
ing, and alcohol initiation: Reciprocal and moderation effects.
Prevention Science, 11, 1–13.
Ward, L. M., Epstein, M., Caruthers, A., & Merriwether, A. (2011).
Men’s media use, sexual cognitions, and sexual risk behavior:
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
10 O’Hara et al.
Testing a mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 47,
592–602.
Weinstock, H., Berman, S., & Cates, W. (2000). Sexually transmitted
diseases among American youth: Incidence and prevalence esti-
mates, 2000. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health,
36, 6–10.
Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., Sargent, J. D., Gerrard, M., Lee, H.-R.,
& Dal Cin, S. (2010). Good self-control moderates the effect of
mass media on adolescent tobacco and alcohol use: Tests with
studies of children and adolescents. Health Psychology, 29, 539–
549.
Wright, P. J. (2011). Mass media effects on youth sexual behavior:
Assessing the claim for causality. Communication Yearbook, 35,
343–386.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavior expressions and biosocial bases of
sensation seeking. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
at DARTMOUTH COLLEGE on July 24, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Research has established associations between early sexual initiation and adverse physical [7,8] as well as mental health [9] consequences. Research indicates an association between early involvement in sexual behavior and exposure to pornography [10]. Additionally, it has been highlighted that adolescents who initiated sexual activity before turning 15 reported experiencing more strained connections with their mothers than their counterparts [11]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Female adolescent sexual behavior, particularly early sexual debut (ESD), poses significant risks and has garnered attention from researchers and society. While various factors influencing early sexual initiation have been studied, including socioeconomic disparities. This study examines the socioeconomic variation in ESD among female adolescents aged 15 to 19 in Bangladesh. Methods This research examines information gathered from three Bangladeshi Demographic and Health Survey data sets from 2011–2017. Researchers employed chi-square tests to identify relationships between categorical variables and logistic regression to pinpoint factors associated with ESD. To assess the socio-economic variation of ESD, the analysis utilized concentration curves, concentration indices, and decomposition techniques. Results Our study revealed that female adolescents’ age, adolescents from Rajshahi, Chittagong, Rangpur, Barisal, and Dhaka regions, who shared the toilet with other households, who were overweight, who preferred to marry earlier or later, who did not know about sexually transmitted infections, and who used contraceptives were more prone to experience ESD, with statistically notable findings (p < 0.05). In addition, adolescent females who were not Muslim and who pursued higher education were less likely to experience ESD. Further analysis of socioeconomic disparities using concentration curves showed poorer female adolescents had a higher incidence of ESD (CCI = -0.1001, p < 0.001). Decomposition of this inequality revealed that a female adolescent's education level was the largest contributor (48.81%) to the observed disparities, followed by knowledge of sexually transmitted infection (20.66%), adolescent's current age (17.78%), toilet shared with other households (7.55%), preferring to marry time (6.85%), and in the administrative region where female adolescents live (5.09%). Conclusions These findings highlight the complex interplay of socio-demographic factors in shaping adolescent sexual behaviors in Bangladesh. Targeted interventions addressing regional disparities, improving access to education and resources, and promoting reproductive health education will help to prevent ESD among female adolescents in Bangladesh.
... The sexual script acquisition, activation, application model ( 3 AM) of sexual media socialization was developed to explain sexual media effects on sexual behavior more broadly (Wright, 2011(Wright, , 2020a. The 3 AM has guided quantitative studies of adolescents, emerging adults, and older adults across a wide range of media types and sexual behaviors (e.g., Braithwaite et al., 2015;Coyne et al., 2019;Donevan & Mattebo, 2017;Hald & Mulya, 2013;Koleti� c et al., 2019;Li et al., 2023;O'Hara et al., 2012;Rousseau & Eggermont, 2018;Sun et al., 2015;Ward et al., 2020;Willoughby et al., 2021;Wright, 2013;. Meta-analytic results are consistent with the 3 AM supposition that, on average, persons more frequently exposed to sexual behaviors depicted as normative and rewarding will be more likely to emulate those behaviors than less frequently exposed persons (Coyne et al., 2019;Tokunaga et al., 2019Tokunaga et al., , 2020Wright et al., 2016a; see also Burnay et al., 2022). ...
Article
Using national probability data from the 2022 National Survey of Sexual Health and Behavior, the present study evaluated whether moderators of the association between frequency of pornography exposure and condomless sex are consistent with the sexual script acquisition, activation, application model’s (3AM) suppositions about the facilitating effects of wishful identification and decreased self-regulation and forethought capacity. Consistent with the 3AM, two-way interaction effect analysis indicated that the strength of the positive association between exposure frequency and condomless sex increased as identification intensified. Inconsistent with the 3AM, two-way interaction effect analysis indicated that the association between exposure frequency and condomless sex was not significantly different among those who had and had not consumed alcohol before their last sexual encounter. However, the three-way interaction between exposure frequency, identification, and alcohol use did suggest a role for each 3AM moderator, as the catalyzing effect of identification was operable among those who had consumed alcohol only.
... This suggestions by the participants agree with another which explained the impact of exposure to explicit sexual material on early sexual debut. 28 The study findings reveal that majority agreed that early sexual debut resulted from peer pressure. Our findings reenforce the fact that when teenagers associate with antisocial peers and peers who perceive norms that favor sex, they will likely initiate early sex. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Early sexual debut among adolescents' results in sexual and reproductive health consequences including unplanned pregnancies, pregnancy complications and sexually transmitted diseases. Objective: This study investigated the factors influencing early sexual debut among 13-19 years old students in secondary schools in Kiambu County, Kenya. Methodology: A descriptive cross-sectional study was carried out in two secondary schools in Kiambu County. A sample of 208 was calculated using Fischer's two stage formula and proportionately distributed per school population size. Students in each school were stratified into classes and respondents were selected by simple random sampling from each stratum. Responses were sought using self-administered questionnaires. Results: Among the respondents 11(66.5%), 48(28.7%) and 8(4.8%) were aged 13-15, below 13 and above 15 years, respectively. Eighty-four (50.2%) respondents were male, 105(62.9%) lived with both parents while 41(24.6%) lived with mothers. Early sexual debut was (60.5%), being higher in males, 57 (67.9 %) compared to females, 44 (39.6 %). More than half of the respondents, 117(69.6%), thought sex before marriage was appropriate, among whom 75(64.1%), have had sex. Most of those who reported sexual debut, 69.9%, said that sexual encounter happened during school breaks. Reasons for refraining included fear of guardian (49.4%) and fear of HIV/AIDS 89(52.7%). However, more than two thirds, 71(70.3%) of those who feared HIV/AIDS and STI as a consequence of early sexual debut have had sex. Gender (P=.032), knowledge on sex (P=.025), use of mobile phones (P=.019), peer pressure (P=.046) and poverty (P =.037) were significantly associated with early sexual debut. Conclusion: A significant proportion of secondary school adolescents were engaged in early sexual debut. Thus, public health interventions should consider the broader determinants of early sexual debut, including the ecological factors in which the behavior occurs.
... According to Chassiakos et al (4), the effects of these media are complex and depend on the nature, consumption, and volume of the media as well as the individual characteristics of its users, including children and teenagers (5). Media typically features sexually explicit materials that pose dangers, such as early sexual activity (6,7). However, media can also be helpful. ...
Article
This study aimed to assess the efficacy of digital education programs in adolescent reproductive health. This study involved a narrative review that used sources from the databases of scientific journals (Elsevier, BMC, BMJ, SagePub, Taylor & Francis, and Scopus) for adolescents’ reproductive health, promotion, social media, health education, health reproduction, sexual health, digital era, cell phone, mHealth, mcare, eHealth, telemedicine, health services, adolescence, youth, young adults, and young adolescents. Of the 75 studies identified during the search, 21 were eligible for inclusion. Interventions regarding reproductive health for adolescents in the digital era are diverse and range from social media and healthcare applications to gaming models. Therefore, the success of a health intervention program depends strongly on government support. Some interventions involved in-depth preparations and included discussions with decision makers and relevant professionals. Others have pinpointed training to make most of the interactions with adolescents during reproductive health education. In a review of 21 studies, 90.4% showed an increase in knowledge, attitudes, or behavior toward providing reproductive health education to adolescents. Interventions in the digital era of adolescent reproductive health are diverse and range from social media and healthcare applications to gaming models. The range of interventions was diverse and wide as most studies in the literature presented very good quality and validated interventions. As information technology plays an integral role in the lives of adolescents, education that makes greater use of social media and gaming can be very effective.
... My central social and behavioral science research interest is the effects of sexual media. Once of primary interest to communication scientists (e.g., Brown, 2002) and psychologists (Linz & Malamuth, 1993), the topic of sexual media effects now captures the interest of scholars in disciplines such as behavioral medicine, child development, criminology, community studies, economics, education, epidemiology, electronic systems, family studies, gender studies, health sciences, health services, information management, internal medicine, international studies, medical biochemistry, nursing, pediatrics, prevention science, public health, psychiatry, social work, sociology, and women's studies (Bhuptani et al., 2023;Coyne et al., 2019;Damian et al., 2022;Dawson et al., 2019;Guggisberg & Holt, 2022;Hagen et al., 2018;Hu et al., 2022;Kinsler et al., 2019;Koletić et al., 2019;Lin et al., 2020;O'Hara et al., 2012;Marshall et al., 2021;Moss et al., 2022;Perry, 2017;Ragsdale et al., 2014;Rostad et al., 2019;Sun et al., 2015;von Andrian-Werburg et al., 2023;Ward et al., 2020). As Sexuality & Culture is one of the central journals publishing sexual media effects research; and as a member of the Editorial Board who published their first study of sex in the media in Sexuality & Culture (Wright, 2009); and having searched the titles and abstracts of sexual media effect studies published in Sexuality & Culture in the last ten years and finding little evidence of replication research; 1 I decided to write this commentary in service of two goals: one, to encourage sexual media effects scholars to conduct replication studies and submit them to Sexuality & Culture; and two, to encourage by example via the reporting of a replication study myself. ...
Article
Full-text available
Article
This research focuses on identifying factors that influence risky sexual behavior among college students. Risky sexual behavior refers to sexual activities that can increase the risk of transmitting sexually transmitted diseases and unwanted pregnancy. In this context, several factors have been identified as major determinants, including parental socioeconomic status, locus of control, social media use, and peer pressure. The aim of this research is to analyze and understand the influence of several factors on risky sexual behavior among students. This research uses a descriptive survey research design with qualitative research methods. Two hundred forty (240) male and female undergraduate students served as study participants. Data collection was carried out using reliable instruments: The main results of the study are conclusive Primary emphasis should be placed on providing sufficiently informative sex education services to students at universities to equip them with valid facts about safe sexual practices. The implications of this research have a broad impact in both academic and practical contexts, the findings of this research can enrich the academic literature regarding risky sexual behavior, in particular by highlighting the role of factors such as parental socioeconomic status, locus of control, social media use, and pressure. friends of the same age.
Thesis
Full-text available
This study set out to investigate the factors associated with age at first sex among female youth in Coast province by using the Kenya Demographic and Health Survey 2008/9.The main objective of the study was to establish factors that were closely associated with sexual debut among female youth aged 15-24 years. The dependent variable was age at first sex and the independent variables included; education, type and place of residence, age, literacy, religion, wealth index, and exposure to mass media i.e. newspapers, magazines, radio. The study sample was 487 female youth aged 15-24 years obtained from the overall survey sample of 8,444 women aged 15-49 years. The study used descriptive statistics and Cox regression analysis. The results showed that 61.8% of the 487 female youth aged 15-24 were already sexually experienced as at the time of the survey. The age at first sex ranges between age 10 and 23 years among the study population. The average age at sexual debut was 16.14 years. Bivariate results showed that education, age and readership of newspapers and magazines were significantly associated with age at first sexual debut. However the results of Cox regression analysis indicated that only education and age had statistically significant effects on early sexual debut. Female youths who reported to have no education were at the greatest risk of initiate sexual activity early and those with at least secondary education had the least risk. Adolescents aged 15-19 were significantly more likely to initiate sexual activity than the older adolescents (20-24). In view of the study findings, there is a need for the government to implement the life skills curriculum targeting female youths in schools and those out of school. Programmes targeting the out of school youth should also be initiated to also facilitate in delaying age at sexual debut among the youth who have no education. More emphasis on youth friendly health services should be encourage to help expose the youth to more reproductive health information that will enable them to make informed decisions. Newspaper pullouts and magazine articles on reproductive health should be made available to the youth and a reading culture be captivating among the youth to increase their awareness on reproductive health matters, captivating puzzles on reproductive health and competitions may be included to capture their interest.
Article
The aim of this study was to evaluate the fit of an explanatory theoretical model of the relationship between sexual double standards (SDS) and violence in intimate partner relationships when empathy and online access to sexual material are taken into account. Cluster sampling was used to select 490 university students for participation in the study. The mean age of the participants was 21.89 years ( SD = 3.52) and 70% were female. A battery of questionnaires was administered comprising the SDS scale, the sexuality on the internet and social networks questionnaire (CSRI-1), interpersonal reactivity index, and the intimate partner violence (IPV) questionnaire. The questionnaires were administered in a group-classroom setting. The results show that empathy acts as a protective and mediating variable because it reduces the impact of online sexual material on violent behavior. In addition, it was seen that the consumption of online sexual material has a direct impact on IPV and SDS. These findings point to the importance of taking empathy and the consumption of online sexual material into account in the development of prevention and intervention programs for tackling violence in young couples.
Article
Full-text available
Research on violent television and films, video games, and music reveals unequivocal evidence that media violence increases the likelihood of aggressive and violent behavior in both immediate and long-term contexts. The effects appear larger for milder than for more severe forms of aggression, but the effects on severe forms of violence are also substantial ( r = .13 to .32) when compared with effects of other violence risk factors or medical effects deemed important by the medical community (e.g., effect of aspirin on heart attacks). The research base is large; diverse in methods, samples, and media genres; and consistent in overall findings. The evidence is clearest within the most extensively researched domain, television and film violence. The growing body of video-game research yields essentially the same conclusions. Short-term exposure increases the likelihood of physically and verbally aggressive behavior, aggressive thoughts, and aggressive emotions. Recent large-scale longitudinal studies provide converging evidence linking frequent exposure to violent media in childhood with aggression later in life, including physical assaults and spouse abuse. Because extremely violent criminal behaviors (e.g., forcible rape, aggravated assault, homicide) are rare, new longitudinal studies with larger samples are needed to estimate accurately how much habitual childhood exposure to media violence increases the risk for extreme violence. Well-supported theory delineates why and when exposure to media violence increases aggression and violence. Media violence produces short-term increases by priming existing aggressive scripts and cognitions, increasing physiological arousal, and triggering an automatic tendency to imitate observed behaviors. Media violence produces long-term effects via several types of learning processes leading to the acquisition of lasting (and automatically accessible) aggressive scripts, interpretational schemas, and aggression-supporting beliefs about social behavior, and by reducing individuals' normal negative emotional responses to violence (i.e., desensitization). Certain characteristics of viewers (e.g., identification with aggressive characters), social environments (e.g., parental influences), and media content (e.g., attractiveness of the perpetrator) can influence the degree to which media violence affects aggression, but there are some inconsistencies in research results. This research also suggests some avenues for preventive intervention (e.g., parental supervision, interpretation, and control of children's media use). However, extant research on moderators suggests that no one is wholly immune to the effects of media violence. Recent surveys reveal an extensive presence of violence in modern media. Furthermore, many children and youth spend an inordinate amount of time consuming violent media. Although it is clear that reducing exposure to media violence will reduce aggression and violence, it is less clear what sorts of interventions will produce a reduction in exposure. The sparse research literature suggests that counterattitudinal and parental-mediation interventions are likely to yield beneficial effects, but that media literacy interventions by themselves are unsuccessful. Though the scientific debate over whether media violence increases aggression and violence is essentially over, several critical tasks remain. Additional laboratory and field studies are needed for a better understanding of underlying psychological processes, which eventually should lead to more effective interventions. Large-scale longitudinal studies would help specify the magnitude of media-violence effects on the most severe types of violence. Meeting the larger societal challenge of providing children and youth with a much healthier media diet may prove to be more difficult and costly, especially if the scientific, news, public policy, and entertainment communities fail to educate the general public about the real risks of media-violence exposure to children and youth.
Article
Full-text available
A media-use questionnaire was completed by 3,261 7th and 8th graders and a subsample of 1,074 respondents was interviewed about their sexual attitudes and be-haviors. Based on results from the media survey, respondents' top television shows, movies, music, Internet sites, and newspapers were content analyzed for portrayals or references to pubertal development, romantic relationships, body exposure or nu-dity, sexual innuendo, touching and kissing, and sexual intercourse. Overall, 11% of the media used by respondents contained sexual content. A measure called the Sexual Media Diet (SMD) was developed to assess each individual's exposure to sexual con-tent in the media, based on the combination of media consumption and content. The SMD measure showed a statistically significant association with adolescents'sexual activity and future intentions to be sexually active, with measures of movie and music exposure showing the strongest associations.
Article
Full-text available
A new conception of sensation seeking is presented, along with a new scale [the Arnett Inventory of Sensation Seeking (AISS)]. The new conception emphasizes novelty and intensity as the two components of sensation seeking. Two studies were conducted to validate the new scale. In the first study, the AISS was found to be more strongly related to risk behavior than Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS) among 116 adolescents aged 16–18 years, although the new scale contains no items related to risk behavior (in contrast to the SSS). In the second study, involving 139 adolescents, similar relations were found between the AISS and risk behavior, and the new scale was also found to be significantly correlated with the Aggression subscale of the California Psychological Inventory (CPI). In addition, adults (N = 38) were found to be lower in sensation seeking than adolescents. In both studies, males were higher in sensation seeking than females.
Article
We analyse the portrayal of sex and drug use in the most popular movies of the last 20 years using the Internet Movie Database list of the top 200 movies of all time. Films released or set prior to the HIV era (pre 1983), animated, not about humans or G/PG rated, were excluded. Films were reviewed by one of two teams of two observers using a data extraction sheet tested for inter-rater reliability. Sexual activity, sexually transmitted disease (STD) prevention, birth control measures, drug use and any consequences discussed or depicted were recorded. There were 53 sex episodes in 28 (32%) of the 87 movies reviewed. There was only one suggestion of condom use, which was the only reference to any form of birth control. There were no depictions of important consequences of unprotected sex such as unwanted pregnancies, HIV or other STDs. Movies with cannabis (8%) and other non-injected illicit drugs (7%) were less common than those with alcohol intoxication (32%) and tobacco use (68%) but tended to portray their use positively and without negative consequences. There were no episodes of injected drug use. Sex depictions in popular movies of the last two decades lacked safe sex messages. Drug use, though infrequent, tended to be depicted positively. The social norm being presented is concerning given the HIV and illicit drug pandemics.
Article
Studies of the impact of the mainstream mass media on young people’s sexual behavior have been slow to accumulate despite longstanding evidence of substantial sexual content in the mass media. The sexual media effects landscape has changed substantially in recent years, however, as researchers from numerous disciplines have answered the call to address this important area of sexual socialization scholarship. The purpose of this chapter is to review the subset of accumulated studies on sexual behavior effects to determine whether this body of work justifies a causal conclusion. The standards for causal inference articulated by Cook and Campbell (1979) are employed to accomplish this objective. It is concluded that the research to date passes the threshold of substantiation for each criterion and that the mass media almost certainly exert a causal influence on United States’ youth sexual behavior.
Article
Several items of the Sensation Seeking Scale which had become dated or irrelevant to current generations were modified, but some investigators are apparently unaware of these modifications. This note describes the item-changes and a new scale, ‘Impulsive Sensation Seeking’ which has no description of specific activities.
Article
In an HIV prevention study, 2949 ninth-grade students in 17 high schools in two Midwestern U.S. cities were administered scales measuring sensation seeking and impulsive decision-making and their separate and combined relationships to a number of indicators of sexual risk-taking. Measures of sexual risk-taking included intentions to have sex, ever had sex, number of lifetime sexual partners, been pregnant or caused a pregnancy, used a condom, used marijuana, had unwanted sex when drunk, had unwanted sex under pressure, said no to sex, used alcohol or partner used alcohol before sex. Strong associations were observed between each of the measures and sexual risk-taking for most of the indicators. Strongest associations were found among sexually active students high on both sensation seeking and impulsive decision-making and weakest associations among students low on both measures. Implications for design of interventions in health campaigns are discussed.