ArticlePDF Available

A Test of Self-Determination Theory in the Exercise Domain

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In accordance with self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985), this study examined the relationship between autonomy support, psychological need satisfaction, motivational regulations, and exercise behavior. Participants (N=369) were recruited from fitness, community, and retail settings. Fulfillment of the 3 basic psychological needs (i.e., competence, autonomy, and relatedness) related to more self-determined motivational regulations. Identified and introjected regulations emerged as positive predictors of strenuous and total exercise behaviors. Competence need satisfaction also predicted directly-and indirectly via identified regulation-strenuous exercise. For participants engaged in organized fitness classes, perceptions of autonomy support provided by exercise class leaders predicted psychological need satisfaction. Furthermore, competence need satisfaction partially mediated the relationship between autonomy support and intrinsic motivation. These findings support SDT in the exercise domain.
Content may be subject to copyright.
2240
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2006, 36, 9, pp. 2240–2265.
r2006 Copyright the Authors
Journal compilation r2006 Blackwell Publishing, Inc.
A Test of Self-Determination Theory in the Exercise Domain
JEMMA EDMUNDS
1
Coventry University
United Kingdom
NIKOS NTOUMANIS AND
JOAN L. DUDA
University of Birmingham
United Kingdom
In accordance with self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985), this study
examined the relationship between autonomy support, psychological need satis-
faction, motivational regulations, and exercise behavior. Participants (N5369)
were recruited from fitness, community, and retail settings. Fulfillment of the 3
basic psychological needs (i.e., competence, autonomy, and relatedness) related to
more self-determined motivational regulations. Identified and introjected regula-
tions emerged as positive predictors of strenuous and total exercise behaviors.
Competence need satisfaction also predicted directlyFand indirectly via identified
regulationFstrenuous exercise. For participants engaged in organized fitness
classes, perceptions of autonomy support provided by exercise class leaders pre-
dicted psychological need satisfaction. Furthermore, competence need satisfaction
partially mediated the relationship between autonomy support and intrinsic mo-
tivation. These findings support SDT in the exercise domain.
There is now worldwide acceptance among medical authorities that
physical activity constitutes a fundamental element of healthy living (World
Health Organization, 1995). Yet, despite well documented evidence advo-
cating the benefits of exercise for physical and mental health, and numerous
public health campaigns promoting its importance, data from developed
countries show that the majority of the adult population is not sufficiently
active to derive these benefits. Indeed, evidence suggests that more than 70%
of adults fail to meet current physical activity recommendations (Depart-
ment of Health, 2004; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
[USDHHS], 2000). Furthermore, physical inactivity now constitutes one of
the major behavioral risk factors to health in modern society (United States
Department of Health and Human Services, 1996). In view of this evidence,
promoting physical activity is clearly an increasing public health priority
(Pate et al., 1995).
1
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jemma Edmunds, Health
Services Research Centre, Room CWG04, Coventry University, Priory Street, Coventry, Unit-
ed Kingdom CV1 5FB. E-mail: j.edmunds@coventry.ac.uk
Physical activity engagement involves a complex interaction between
biological, environmental, social, and psychological influences (Biddle &
Mutrie, 2001). Examining the motivational determinants of exercise behav-
ior has become a prominent topic in exercise psychology (Biddle & Mutrie,
2001). One theoretical approach to human motivation that is receiving in-
creasing attention in the exercise domain is self-determination theory (SDT;
Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Essentially, SDT proposes that human motivation varies in the extent to
which it is autonomous (self-determined) or controlling. Behaviors and
actions that are autonomous are initiated freely and emanate from within
oneself (Reeve, 2002). In contrast, when behavior is controlled, it is reg-
ulated by an external force. The individual in this instance feels pressured to
engage in the behavior. Based on these distinctions, SDT proposes that three
forms of motivation exist; namely, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motiva-
tion, and amotivation,
2
which, based on the level of autonomy associated
with them, lie on a continuum ranging from high to low self-determination,
respectively.
Intrinsic motivation constitutes the most autonomous form of motiva-
tion, and refers to an inherent tendency possessed by all humans to seek out
novelty and challenges, to extend and exercise their capabilities, to explore,
and to learn (Ryan & Deci, 2000). An individual who pursues a goal or
activity because it is enjoyable or intrinsically captivating would display
intrinsic motivation (Koestner & Losier, 2002).
Not all human behaviors can be considered enjoyable, however. To
understand how such behaviors are regulated, SDT proposes extrinsic
motivation as an additional motivational force, and a process called inter-
nalization. Extrinsic motivation refers to behaviors that are carried out to
attain outcomes unrelated to the activity itself (e.g., social comparisons;
Deci, 1971). Internalization refers to an inherent tendency possessed by all
humans to integrate the regulation of extrinsically motivated activities that
are useful for effective functioning in the social world, but that are not
inherently interesting (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994).
SDT further proposes that the extent to which extrinsic motives are
internalized can vary. A multidimensional conceptualization of extrinsic
motivation is hypothesized to exist, consisting of external, introjected, iden-
tified, and integrated regulations.
3
These regulations lie on a continuum
2
Amotivation has been defined by Markland and Tobin (2004, p. 191) as representing
‘‘a state lacking of any intention to engage in behavior’’ and constitutes a completely non-
self-determined form of motivation. Given that all participants in the current study engaged in
at least some form of exercise, amotivation is not discussed in this study.
3
Integrated regulation constitutes the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation,
occurring ‘‘when identified regulations have been fully assimilated to the self’’ (Ryan & Deci,
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2241
from lower to higher self-determination and reflect the extent of the inter-
nalization process (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
External regulation can be defined as exercising to either appease an
external demand or to attain a reward (Ryan & Deci, 2000). An example of
an external regulation in the exercise domain is ‘‘I exercise because my
friends and family say I should.’’ Introjection, which is a slightly more self-
determined form of extrinsic motivation, involves internalizing the behav-
ior’s regulation, but not fully accepting it as one’s own (Ryan & Deci, 2000).
It is a relatively controlling form of regulation in which behaviors (e.g.,
exercise engagement) are performed to avoid negative emotions (e.g., anx-
iety, guilt), to support conditional self-worth, or to attain ego enhancement
(Ryan & Deci, 2000). Identified regulation reflects a more autonomous form
of extrinsic motivation and reflects participation in an activity because one
holds outcomes of the behavior to be personally significant, although one
may not enjoy the activity itself. For example, an individual who exercises
because he or she values the benefits of exercise would display identified
regulation in this domain.
In addition to specifying the different types of motivational regulations
that may guide behavior, SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985) also details specific
conditions that are responsible for more or less self-determined motivation.
Specifically, SDT assumes that all humans possess three basic psychological
needs; that is, the need for competence, autonomy, and relatedness. The
need for competence implies that individuals have a desire to interact effec-
tively with the environment, to experience a sense of competence in pro-
ducing desired outcomes, and to prevent undesired events (Deci, 1975; Deci
& Ryan, 1985). The need for autonomy reflects a desire to engage in activities
of one’s choosing and to be the origin of one’s own behavior (deCharms,
1968; Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 1985). Finally, the need for relatedness
involves feeling connected, or feeling that one belongs in a given social
milieu (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Deci & Ryan, 1985). Essentially, SDT
suggests that the most self-determined forms of regulation will guide be-
havior when the needs are satisfied. In contrast, low self-determination is a
consequence of a thwarting of the three basic needs.
According to Deci and Ryan (1985), SDT also specifies that differential
levels of psychological need satisfaction in a given domain will result in
diverse cognitive, affective, and behavioral consequences (e.g., interest, per-
formance, creativity, general well-being; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Furthermore,
need satisfaction has been postulated to influence outcomes indirectly via
2000, p. 73). However, integrated regulation was not examined in the current investigation, as
the measurement instrument utilized in this study to tap the different forms of motivation
proposed by SDT does not include a scale assessing this regulation.
2242 EDMUNDS ET AL.
the promotion of different types of motivational regulation (Vallerand,
1997). It is assumed that intrinsic motivation will engender the most positive
consequences, followed by identification (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Vallerand,
1997).
However, some research findings in physical activity settings (e.g.,
Wilson, Rodgers, Blanchard, & Gessell, 2003)Fas well as in other domains,
such as politics and education (e.g., Koestner & Losier, 2002)Fhave been
less conclusive regarding the positive implications of intrinsic motivation
compared to other self-determined forms of regulation. Wilson et al. (2003)
provided evidence suggesting that among participants recruited to engage in
a 12-week structured exercise program, identified regulation was a stronger
predictor of self-reported exercise behavior than was intrinsic motivation,
although both regulations predicted exercise behaviors, exercise attitudes,
and physical fitness. In addition, introjected regulation has been shown to be
correlated positively with strenuous exercise behavior in some studies (e.g.,
Wilson, Rodgers, & Fraser, 2002), but not in others (e.g., Wilson et al.,
2003).
Ryan (1995) proposed that the characteristics of the situation in
question will determine the extent to which intrinsic and internalized
extrinsic regulations will produce positive behavioral outcomes. With
respect to the latter, in contexts in which the activities undertaken are
important, but may lack in intrinsic appeal, it is assumed that the innate
tendency to internalize the role of such activities will be witnessed (Ryan,
1995). In view of the considerable value that society bestows upon exercise
for health and aesthetic gains, research demonstrating that introjected
and identified regulations positively predict exercise behavior may indicate
that, for some individuals, exercise engagement is maintained via the
process described by Ryan. That is, exercise behavior constitutes an
externally motivated activity that requires internalization to initiate and
sustain action.
An additional tenet of SDT relevant to the current investigation concerns
the social context in which individuals operate. According to SDT, auton-
omy-supportive contexts are conducive to need satisfaction and ensuing self-
determined motivational regulations. Such contexts are characterized by the
minimization of controls by significant others, the understanding of other
people’s perspectives, and the provision of choices that guide and facilitate
the decision-making process (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000).
Supporting these propositions, Wilson and Rodgers (2004) found that
among female students and staff enrolled in a team-based intramural phys-
ical activity event, perceived autonomy support from friends was associated
positively with intrinsic motivation and identified regulation. Furthermore,
Standage, Duda, and Ntoumanis (2003) recently demonstrated that, among
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2243
secondary school physical education students, an autonomy-supportive
climate was related positively to satisfaction of the need for competence,
autonomy, and relatedness, which, in turn, predicted greater self-determined
motivation. However, as far as the present authors are aware, no study has
considered the implications of an autonomy-supportive environment
provided by an exercise class leader.
Study Aims and Hypotheses
The first aim of the current study is to explore how satisfaction of the
three psychological needs relates to the type of motivational regulations
guiding exercise behavior. Furthermore, we examine the extent to which
psychological need satisfaction and motivational regulations can predict
exercise behavior.
To date, published research in the exercise domain has determined only
the direct effects of psychological need satisfaction on motivational regu-
lations and motivational regulations on exercise behaviors (Wilson et al.,
2002, 2003). Thus, extending previous research, the current study also ex-
plores the indirect effects of need satisfaction on behavioral outcomes, with
motivational regulations being tested as potential mediators. The present
research also examines whether, as assumed in SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985), an
autonomy-supportive context provided by an exercise class leader corre-
sponds to greater intrinsic motivation and identified regulation via the sup-
port provided for the three basic psychological needs.
Based on the propositions of SDT and previous research in the physical,
educational, and political domains (Koestner & Losier, 2002; Wilson &
Rodgers, 2004; Wilson et al., 2002, 2003), we hypothesize first that positive
relationships will be observed between psychological need satisfaction and
identified and intrinsic motives; and a negative link will emerge between
psychological need satisfaction and introjected and external regulations.
Second, identified and introjected regulation and intrinsic motivation are
expected to predict exercise behaviors positively and to mediate the rela-
tionship between psychological need satisfaction and exercise behaviors. In
turn, external regulation is hypothesized to predict exercise behaviors neg-
atively and to mediate the relationship between inadequate psychological
need satisfaction and more negative behavioral outcomes. Third, it is pre-
dicted that perceived autonomy support (PAS) provided by the exercise class
leader will be related positively to satisfaction of the three basic needs and
self-determined motivation. Finally, PAS is also hypothesized to predict
intrinsic motivation and identified regulation via satisfaction of the basic
psychological needs.
2244 EDMUNDS ET AL.
Method
Participants
Participants (N5369; 173 male, 192 female, 4 unspecified) ranged in age
from 16 to 64 years (M531.86, SD 511.28). The majority (88.7%) of the
participants were White. One hundred six (37 male, 68 female, 1 unspecified)
of the participants reported taking part in regular exercise classes, and thus
constituted the subsample with which we examined relationships between
PAS, need satisfaction, and motivational regulations. The subsample ranged
in age from 16 to 62 years (M530.24, SD 510.32).
An a priori power analysis, conducted using GPower (Version 2; Faul &
Erdfelder, 1992), ensured that these sample sizes were sufficient to yield
adequate statistical power for all statistical procedures planned and sub-
sequently conducted in the current study. More specifically, to detect a
significant finding (at the .05 level) at a desired power level of .95, a min-
imum of 143 participants was required for analyses conducted on the total
sample, and 41 for the substudy analyses.
Measures
Psychological need satisfaction. Psychological need satisfaction was
measured via the 21-item Basic Need Satisfaction at Work Scale (Deci
et al., 2001), adapted by the authors to make it relevant to the exercise
domain. This 21-item scale is based on a 15-item measure developed by
Kasser, Davey, and Ryan (1992) to tap reported autonomy, relatedness, and
competence in the work domain. In the development of the original 15-item
measure, some items were taken from the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory
(IMI; Ryan, 1982), support for which has been garnered in the physical
domain (McAuley, Duncan, & Tammen, 1989). The 21-item Basic Need
Satisfaction at Work scale exhibited alphas of .73 for competence, .84 for
relatedness, and .79 for autonomy in a sample of U.S. workers (Deci et al.,
2001).
The 21-item scale utilized by Deci et al. (2001) includes six items that
measure competence (e.g., ‘‘Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from
exercising’’), eight items that measure relatedness (e.g., ‘‘People I exercise
with take my feelings into consideration’’), and seven items that measure
autonomy (e.g., ‘‘I feel like I am free to decide for myself how to exercise’’)
need satisfaction. Following the stem ‘‘Please indicate how true each of the
following statements is for you, given your experiences of exercise,’’ par-
ticipants responded to each item on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not true
for me)to7(very true for me).
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2245
Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire (BREQ; Mullan, Mark-
land, & Ingledew, 1997).Participants completed the BREQ, which is a 15-
item self-report measure assessing the reasons why people exercise. The
BREQ includes scales assessing external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic
regulations. Following the stem ‘‘Why do you exercise?’’ participants re-
sponded to each item on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not true for me)to5
(very true for me). Previous research supports the BREQ’s multidimensional
four-factor structure, the invariance of this factor structure across gender,
and the internal consistency of each subscale (i.e., alphas ranged from .76 to
.90; Mullan & Markland, 1997; Mullan et al., 1997).
Godin Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire (GLTEQ; Godin & Shepard,
1985).The GLTEQ was used to assess self-reported exercise behavior. The
GLTEQ contains three questions assessing the frequency of mild, moderate, and
strenuous exercise engaged in (for a minimum of 15 min) during a typical week.
Exercise behavior scores can be calculated by multiplying weekly fre-
quencies of strenuous (e.g., running, vigorous gym workout), moderate (e.g.,
easy cycling), and mild activities (e.g., easy walking) by 9, 5, and 3 METS
(units of metabolic equivalence), respectively. An overall exercise behavior
score is calculated by summing the weighted product of each question as
follows: (mild 3) 1(moderate 5) 1(strenuous 9). Based on its
correlations with objective indicators of exercise and physical fitness (e.g.,
exercise monitor and maximal aerobic capacity test scores), a previous study
has concluded that the GLTEQ is a reliable and valid measure of leisure-
time exercise behavior (Jacobs, Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993).
Perceived autonomy support. PAS from the exercise class leader was mea-
sured using a short (6 items) version of the original 15-item Health Care Cli-
mate Questionnaire (HCCQ; Williams, Grow, Freedman, Ryan, & Deci, 1996).
The original scale assesses participants’ perceptions of the degree of autonomy
support provided by a relevant health care provider and includes items such as
‘‘I feel that my health care provider provides me with choices and options.’’ In
the current study, the term ‘‘my health care provider’’ was replaced with ‘‘my
exercise class leader,’’ and participants were asked to respond to items in ref-
erence to the exercise class in which they participated most commonly.
Participants responded to each item on a 7-point scale ranging from
1(strongly disagree)to7(strongly agree). Previous studies using the original
HCCQ (Williams et al., 1996) revealed a one-factor solution measuring PAS
and an alpha value of .95.
Procedure
The current research was approved by the ethics subcommittee of a
university in the United Kingdom. Participants were recruited in a number
2246 EDMUNDS ET AL.
of different settings, including sports clubs, public leisure centers, private
fitness clubs, shops, and supermarkets in the West Midlands, UK.
Participants were approached by the first author, who explained the
purpose of the study, and were asked if they were willing to complete a
multisection questionnaire packet. Those who agreed to take part provided
their informed consent.
The first section of the questionnaire assessed psychological need sat-
isfaction via exercise, motivational regulations, and exercise behaviors.
Those participants who reported taking part in regular exercise classes
completed an additional section of the questionnaire tapping PAS provided
by the exercise class leader in the class in which they participated most
commonly.
Results
Preliminary Data Analysis
Data were screened according to the recommendations of Tabachnick
and Fidell (2001). Four multivariate outliers were removed from the sample
based on the Mahalonobis distance criterion (see Tabachnick & Fidell,
2001), leaving a final sample of 369 participants. Examination of the as-
sumptions associated with regression analyses (i.e., normality, linearity, and
homoscedasticity) suggests that there were no particular problems in the
data. More specifically, inspection of a scatterplot of the residuals indicates
that both linearity and homoscedasticity assumptions were tenable.
To explore whether the data were marked by multicollinearity, both
variance inflation (1.06–2.28) and tolerance (0.44–0.95) values were exam-
ined. No particular problems were found, since the obtained values are
within acceptable limits. In addition, based on Belsley (1991) and Belsley,
Kuh, and Welsch’s (1980) suggestions, the condition indexes (CI) and var-
iance proportions factors (VPF) for all multiple regression analyses were
explored. Using the criterion proposed in Pedhazur (1997), in no instances
when the CI was greater than 10 did the VPF values observed exceed 0.50
for two or more predictors, suggesting that there was no collinearity in the
data.
Reliability Analysis and Descriptive Statistics
Internal consistency estimates (Cronbach’s coefficient alpha) and de-
scriptive statistics were computed for all variables (see Table 1). Reliability
analyses indicate that, in general, internal consistency coefficients were
greater than .70. However, the alpha values observed for two of the need
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2247
Table 1
Reliability Analyses, Descriptive Statistics, and Pearson Correlations for Age, Gender, Psychological Need Satisfaction
Via Exercise, Motivational Regulations for Exercise, and Exercise Behaviors
aMSD 123456789101112
1. Age F31.86 11.28 F
2. Gender FF F .05 F
3. Autonomy via exercise .65 5.49 0.82 .09 - .10 F
4. Relatedness via exercise .85 5.10 1.15 - .15

- .08 .37

F
5. Competence via exercise .65 5.02 0.95 - .17

- .14

.45

.52

F
6. External regulation .70 1.30 0.48 - .08 -.05 - .33

- .12

- .22

F
7. Introjected regulation .74 1.96 0.89 - .18

.09 - .17

- .00 .01 .35

F
8. Identified regulation .78 3.47 0.90 - .15

.05 .15

.14

.40

- .02 .41

F
9. Intrinsic motivation .92 3.55 1.02 - .13
.00 .26

.34

.47

- .14

.14

.64

F
10. Mild exercise F7.59 9.31 .06 .11
.02 .05 - .09 .09 - .00 - .07 - .01 F
11. Moderate exercise F14.51 19.82 .01 .10 .11
- .01 - .02 - .06 - .05 .00 - .00 .20

F
12. Strenuous exercise F35.17 30.93 - .34

- .15

.11
.17

.38

- .09 .28

.41

.33

- .10 - .08 F
13. Total exercise F57.28 36.83 - .27

- .04 .16

.15

.29

- .08 .20

.32

.27

.28

.52

.77

Note.N5369. Reliability estimates are Cronbach’s alphas.
po.05.

po.01.
2248 EDMUNDS ET AL.
scales were marginal: autonomy, a5.65; and competence, a5.65. Thus,
results based on these variables should be interpreted with caution.
All participants engaged in at least some form of mild exercise (range
53–223 METS; M57.59, SD 59.31). The mean level of total self-reported
exercise (M557.28, SD 536.83) was higher than that reported in previous
studies examining the propositions of SDT in the exercise domain (e.g.,
Wilson et al., 2002, 2003). Autonomy was the most highly satisfied need,
followed by relatedness and competence. Intrinsic motivation was the most
strongly endorsed exercise regulation, closely followed by identified regu-
lation.
Relationships Between Psychological Need Satisfaction, Exercise Regulations,
and Exercise Behaviors
Pearson correlations were computed between age, gender, autonomy,
relatedness, and competence need satisfaction, each of the BREQ (Mullan
et al., 1997) subscales, and reported exercise behaviors (Table 1). Small to
moderate negative correlations were observed between all three psycholog-
ical needs and external regulation. Autonomy was correlated negatively with
introjected regulation. Small to moderate positive correlations were ob-
served between all three psychological needs and identified regulation and
intrinsic motivation. Small to moderate positive relationships also emerged
between the three needs and strenuous and total exercise behavior. Auto-
nomy correlated positively with moderate exercise. No significant cor-
relations emerged between the needs and mild exercise. Small to moderate
positive relationships were observed between introjected and identified reg-
ulation and intrinsic motivation and strenuous and total exercise behavior.
None of the motivational regulations were correlated with mild and mod-
erate exercise.
Factors Predicting Total and Strenuous Exercise Behaviors
Separate regression analyses were carried out to predict total and stren-
uous self-reported exercise from psychological need satisfaction and moti-
vational regulations. Mild and moderate exercise were not examined because
they did not correlate with the needs or the regulations.
Preliminary MANOVA revealed significant age and gender differences in
exercise behaviors: age, F(6, 686) 58.51, p5.00, Pillai’s trace 5.14; and
gender, F(3, 361) 54.38, p5.01, Pillai’s trace 5.04. In view of these results,
as well as the fact that the existing literature has linked these characteristics
to exercise behavior (e.g., Department of Health, 2004; USDHHS, 1996), we
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2249
controlled for their influence in the first step of the regression. By doing so,
we could determine whether the theoretical constructs embedded in SDT
accounted for additional variance in exercise behavior above and beyond
important demographic variables. Needs were entered in the second step of
the regression, as they are postulated to affect behavioral outcomes indi-
rectly via motivational regulations (Vallerand, 1997), which were entered in
the final step.
As seen in Table 2, 18% of the variance in total exercise behavior was
explained by this model. Two of the variables contributed independently to
the prediction of total exercise behavior; namely, age and introjected reg-
ulation. Six of the variables contributed independently to the prediction of
strenuous exercise behavior: gender, age, competence, external regulation,
introjected regulation, and identified regulation (Table 3). This set of pre-
dictors predicted 32% of the variability in strenuous exercise behavior.
4
Test of Mediation
The regression procedures of Baron and Kenny (1986) were employed to
examine potential mediation effects. Three basic steps are proposed in es-
tablishing mediation: (a) the predictor variable (i.e., psychological need)
must have an effect on the criterion variable (i.e., exercise behavior); (b) the
predictor variable (i.e., psychological need) must have an effect on the me-
diator variable (i.e., motivational regulation); and (c) the mediator (i.e.,
regulation) must affect the outcome (i.e., exercise behavior), after control-
ling for the predictor (i.e., psychological need). To establish complete me-
diation, the effect of the predictor on the outcome should be zero in the third
step of the analysis. Partial mediation occurs when this effect is reduced, but
remains statistically significant.
Given that SDT assumes that the three psychological needs coexist (Deci
& Ryan, 1985), it was decided that it made theoretical sense to include all
needs in the same step and not to examine them independently. We followed
4
It could be argued that participants in the current study were recruited from two distinct
settings: those that were associated with immediate/current physical activity engagement (e.g.,
fitness clubs), and those that were not (e.g., community and retail settings). Therefore, analyses
were conducted to determine whether individuals recruited from potentially ‘‘active’’ (n5126)
versus ‘‘non-active’’ (n5243) settings differed with regard to their motivational profiles (avail-
able from the first author upon request). Despite some subtle differences between groups in the
size of the predictions, no new predictor variables emerged (results can be obtained from the
first author upon request). Thus, the findings suggest that competence need satisfaction as well
as introjected and identified regulations are associated with increased exercise behavior and that
external regulation is linked negatively to physical activity. However, as we have no way of
determining whether those individuals comprising the non-active setting group actually
belonged to fitness clubs, this supplementary analysis must be interpreted with caution.
2250 EDMUNDS ET AL.
the same logic for the motivational regulations. Examining Step 2 of the
regression analyses results for total and strenuous exercise (see Tables 2 and
3), it is apparent that competence was the only need to predict behavioral
outcomes and thus meet Baron and Kenny’s (1986) first criterion for es-
tablishing mediation.
Testing Baron and Kenny’s (1986) second criterion for establishing
mediation, competence was found to be a significant predictor of identified
regulation (b5.46, p5.00), but none of the other regulations (these results
are not included in Tables 2 and 3). Identified regulation was a positive
Table 2
Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Total Exercise Be-
havior From Gender, Age, Psychological Needs, and Motivational Regulations
Independent variable Adj. R
2
bt
Step 1: F(2, 344) 513.10, po.00 .07
Gender - .03 - 0.56
Age - .26 - 5.06

Step 2: F(5, 241) 510.84, po.00 .13
Gender .01 0.13
Age - .24 - 4.60

Autonomy .09 1.59
Relatedness - .04 - 0.58
Competence .22 3.51

Step 3: F(9, 337) 59.22, po.00 .18
Gender - .03 - 0.68
Age - .21 - 4.02

Autonomy .09 1.55
Relatedness - .03 - 0.41
Competence .12 1.72
External regulation .09 - 1.58
Introjected regulation .15 2.46
Identified regulation .14 1.89
Intrinsic motivation .06 0.82
Note.N5347.
po.05.

po.01.
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2251
predictor of strenuous, but not total exercise; thus, these findings rule out
the possibility of mediation effects with regard to total exercise.
With respect to Baron and Kenny’s (1986) third criterion for establishing
mediation, the standardized beta coefficient for competence dropped from
.36 to .23 (both ps5.00) when strenuous exercise was being predicted and
the motivational regulations were entered into the regression equation
(Table 3), suggesting partial mediation. Using the Goodman I version of the
Sobel test, as recommended by Baron and Kenny, partial mediation was
Table 3
Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Strenuous Exercise
Behavior From Gender, Age, Psychological Needs, and Motivational
Regulations
Independent variable Adj. R
2
bt
Step 1: F(2, 344) 526.36, po.00 .13
Gender - .13 - 2.59

Age - .34 - 6.66

Step 2: F(5, 341) 521.04, po.00 .23
Gender - .09 - 1.85
Age - .28 - 5.76

Autonomy - .01 - 0.22
Relatedness - .06 - 1.11
Competence .36 6.03

Step 3: F(9, 337) 519.06, po.00 .32
Gender - .14 - 3.14

Age - .24 - 5.15

Autonomy - .02 - 0.30
Relatedness - .05 - 0.87
Competence .23 3.72

External regulation - .14 - 2.68

Introjected regulation .21 3.84

Identified regulation .17 2.56
Intrinsic motivation .05 0.87
Note.N5347.
po.05.

po.01.
2252 EDMUNDS ET AL.
confirmed. The reduction in the effect of competence on strenuous exercise
behavior as a result of identified regulation was significant (z52.56,
p5.01).
Preliminary Substudy Data Analysis
One hundred six participants reported being members of an exercise
group. Relevant data were screened according to the recommendations of
Tabachnick and Fidell (2001). No problems were found.
The assumptions associated with multiple regression analysis (i.e., nor-
mality, linearity, and homoscedasticity) were examined, and again no prob-
lems were observed. Inspection of residual scatterplots indicates that both
the linearity and homoscedasticity assumptions were tenable for all regres-
sion analyses. Furthermore, an examination of the variance inflation (1.01–
1.65), tolerance (0.61–0.99), CI, and VPF values revealed that the data were
not marked by collinearity.
Reliability Analysis and Descriptive Statistics
Reliability analyses indicate that internal consistency coefficients were
above .70 for all variables, except for autonomy (a5.64) and competence
(a5.65). Thus, the present results based on these variables should be
interpreted with caution. PAS scores ranged from 1 to 7 (M54.82,
SD 51.26).
Autonomy was the most highly satisfied need (M55.25, SD 50.82),
followed by relatedness (M55.16, SD 51.03), and then competence
(M55.07, SD 50.90). Intrinsic motivation was the most highly endorsed
form of motivation (M53.65, SD 51.00), followed by identified (M53.61,
SD 50.82), introjected (M52.20, SD 50.95), and external (M51.38,
SD 50.51) regulation.
Pearson Correlations
Pearson correlations were calculated to examine relationships between
age, gender, PAS, psychological need satisfaction, and motivational regu-
lations. Low positive correlations were observed between PAS and auto-
nomy (r5.26), and between PAS and competence (r5.27). A moderate
positive association was observed between PAS and relatedness (r5.45). In
addition, low and moderate positive correlations were observed between
PAS and identified regulation (r5.22), and intrinsic motivation (r5.36).
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2253
Hierarchical Regression Analyses
As positive correlations were observed between PAS and identified
regulation and intrinsic motivation, hierarchical multiple regression analyses
were conducted with each of these regulations as the criterion variables.
Age and gender were entered in the first step of the analysis. PAS was
entered in the second step, and each of the psychological needs in the
third step.
As seen in Table 4, PAS was found to be a significant predictor of
intrinsic motivation, after controlling for demographic and psychological
need satisfaction variables. Competence need satisfaction via exercise also
significantly predicted intrinsic motivation. PAS was not associated with
identified regulation after controlling for age, gender, and the three needs
(b5.17, p5.11). Competence need satisfaction significantly predicted iden-
tified regulation (b5.45, p5.00).
Table 4
Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting
Intrinsic Motivation
Independent variable Adj. R
2
bt
Step 1: F(2, 97) 50.81, p5.45 .00
Gender .01 0.09
Age - .13 - 1.28
Step 2: F(3, 96) 55.18, po.01 .11
Gender - .03 - 0.31
Age - .11 - 1.13
PAS .35 3.70

Step 3: F(6, 93) 56.02, po.00 .23
Sex .04 0.41
Age - .05 - 0.57
PAS .23 2.30
Autonomy .02 0.23
Relatedness .02 0.16
Competence .38 3.43

Note.N5100. PAS 5perceived autonomy support.
po.05.

po.01.
2254 EDMUNDS ET AL.
Test of Mediation
Next, we examined the hypothesized mediating role played by psycho-
logical need satisfaction in the relationship between PAS and motivational
regulations. PAS predicted intrinsic motivation (see Step 2, Table 4), and
thus met Criterion 1 of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures. PAS also
significantly predicted autonomy (b5.28, p501), relatedness (b5.46,
p5.00), and competence (b5.28, p5.01) need satisfaction via exercise, and
thus met Baron and Kenny’s second criterion for establishing mediation
(these findings are not reported in Table 4).
Competence was the only need (i.e., mediator) to predict intrinsic mo-
tivation (i.e., criterion variable) after controlling for the effect of PAS (i.e.,
predictor variable; see Step 3, Table 4). After controlling for the effect of
competence on the relationship between PAS and intrinsic motivation, the
beta coefficient for autonomy support dropped from .35 (p5.00) to .23
(p5.02), suggesting partial mediation. The Goodman I version of the Sobel
test revealed that this effect was significant (z52.59, p5.01).
Discussion
The results of the current research demonstrate the importance of
motivation-related variables to understanding some of the variability in
self-reported exercise behaviors. Overall, the findings indicate that the key
constructs of SDT add to the prediction of exercise behaviors above what is
accounted for by demographic characteristics, such as age and gender. In
accordance with SDT, psychological need satisfaction derived from the
exercise setting was correlated positively with more self-determined moti-
vational regulations. Furthermore, satisfaction of the three psychological
needsFintrojected regulation, identified regulation, and intrinsic motiva-
tionFwere associated positively with strenuous and total exercise behav-
iors. Moreover, regression analysis shows that, as hypothesized, external
regulation was a negative predictor of strenuous exercise behavior, in-
trojected regulation positively predicted total exercise, and introjected and
identified regulation were positive predictors of strenuous exercise behavior.
Identified regulation also partially mediated the relationship between com-
petence need satisfaction and strenuous exercise. Contrary to expectations,
however, intrinsic motivation did not predict either dimension of exercise
behavior significantly.
A further examination of study participants engaged in regular organized
exercise classes revealed that perceived autonomy support (PAS) provided
by the exercise class leader was associated positively with psychological need
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2255
satisfaction and self-determined motivation. Subsequent regression analyses
also support the role of PAS in predicting need satisfaction and intrinsic
motivation. Competence need satisfaction partially mediated the relation-
ship between PAS and intrinsic motivation. PAS did not predict identified
regulation after controlling for age, gender, and the three psychological
needs.
Despite being the most highly endorsed form of motivation, as well as
being positively correlated with self-reported exercise, intrinsic motivation
did not make an independent significant prediction to exercise engagement
when controlling for the other regulations in the regression analyses. In
interpreting this finding, it is important to consider similar findings that
have emerged in the political and educational domains. For example,
Koestner and colleagues (Koestner, Losier, Vallerand, & Carducci, 1996;
Losier & Koestner, 1999) presented evidence indicating that considering
politics as important (i.e., reflecting identified regulation) was a more sig-
nificant predictor of voting behavior than was perceiving politics to be in-
teresting (i.e., an indicator of intrinsic motivation). Generally speaking, such
results suggest that intrinsic motivation may not be the most important
predictor of engagement in the exercise domain, and support claims that
people are unlikely to maintain regular exercise behavior, with all the
organization and commitment that it entails, purely for the intrinsic reasons
of fun and enjoyment (Mullan et al., 1997).
In view of these arguments, the finding that identified regulation signif-
icantly predicted strenuous exercise behavior in the current study is not
surprising. This finding suggests that in order to partake in strenuous ex-
ercise behaviors, which necessitate considerable physical and mental exer-
tion and stamina, individuals must place some value on the exercise and
recognize its importance in terms of health and well-being. Thus, similar to
other activities that may lack in intrinsic appeal, recognizing the significance
of physical activity and valuing its benefits (e.g., improved fitness and phy-
sique) appear to be relevant to active engagement in the exercise setting.
Given that (unlike identified regulation) intrinsic motivation was not a
significant predictor of exercise behavior, one might wonder whether it is
worth trying to cultivate intrinsic motivation for exercise. Might interven-
tion efforts be more efficacious by focusing on the facilitation of identified
regulation? Previous research in the exercise and sports domains would
suggest that the former strategy is still a viable one, as intrinsic motivation
has been shown to be critical to behavioral persistence (Pelletier, Fortier,
Vallerand, & Briere, 2001; Perrin, 1979; Ryan, Frederick, Lepes, Rubio, &
Sheldon, 1997). Perrin, for example, found that whereas new participants in
physical activity programs reported health benefits as their reason for ex-
ercise adoption, long-term participants reported enjoyment as their principal
2256 EDMUNDS ET AL.
reason for continuing. Indeed, as evidenced and advocated by Koestner and
Losier (2002), with regard to educational and political behaviors, it is likely
that promoting high levels of both intrinsic motivation and identification
would be most beneficial to optimal and continued behavioral engagement
in exercise. Further longitudinal research is needed to examine this hypo-
thesis.
In addition, it is important to consider other potential outcomes asso-
ciated with the different motivational regulations. Psychological need sat-
isfaction and self-determined motives (especially intrinsic motivation) have
been associated with indexes of positive well-being in numerous contexts
(Ryan, & Deci, 2000, 2001), including the physical domain (e.g., Gagne
´,
Ryan, & Bargmann, 2003; Wilson & Rodgers, 2002). These findings suggest
that intrinsic motivation contributes significantly to the quality of the ex-
ercise experience.
The finding that introjected regulation significantly predicted both stren-
uous and total exercise also warrants further discussion. Introjected regu-
lation is a controlling form of motivation that lies toward the lower end of
the self-determination continuum. Despite its positive role in predicting ex-
ercise behavior in the current cross-sectional study, there is evidence to
suggest that introjected regulation will not bode well for long-term physical
health (Frederick-Recascino, 2002) or sustained exercise involvement. Al-
though the longer term implications of being motivated by introjected reg-
ulation over time cannot be addressed in the current study, evidence from
the sports and exercise domain has shown this type of motivation to be
associated with poor adherence (Frederick & Ryan, 1993; Pelletier et al.,
2001). Research in other domains (e.g., education) also has shown in-
trojected regulation to be related to poor emotional functioning, such as
high levels of distress and low levels of adjustment (Koestner & Losier,
2002). Longitudinal research is warranted to examine whether self-deter-
mined motivation, as opposed to introjected regulation, is linked positively
to exercise adherence and indexes of psychological and emotional health.
Finally, as hypothesized, a negative relationship emerged between the
least self-determined motivational regulation (i.e., external regulation) and
strenuous exercise behaviors. This finding clearly supports the proposition
of SDT that performing an activity to satisfy external demands will not
result in behavioral investment.
No previous studies in the exercise domain have considered whether the
relationships between psychological need satisfaction and behavioral out-
comes are mediated by motivational regulations. Providing some support
for the propositions of Vallerand (1997), the relationship of competence
need satisfaction to strenuous exercise was mediated partially by identified
regulation in the current investigation. In addition to this mediating effect,
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2257
however, competence need satisfaction was found to play a direct role in
predicting strenuous exercise behavior. Considering these direct and indirect
effects, it seems prudent for exercise interventions to focus on increasing
feelings of competence within participants so that there is an increased
probability that self-determined motivation and adaptive behavioral out-
comes will ensue.
According to Ryan and Deci (2000), understanding the conditions that
foster versus undermine psychological need satisfaction holds great practical
significance. Such awareness can contribute to the creation of social envi-
ronments that satisfy the three needs and promote self-determined motiva-
tional regulations, personal development, and well-being (Ryan & Deci,
2000). In a substudy of regular exercise class participants, PAS from the
exercise class leader was related positively to each of the three psychological
needs, as well as identified regulation and intrinsic motivation. In addition,
competence need satisfaction partially mediated the observed relationship
between PAS and intrinsic motivation.
It should be noted that PAS did not predict identified regulation when
we controlled for age, gender, and the three psychological needs. This find-
ing, which is in contrast to our hypotheses, may suggest that the provision of
an autonomy-supportive environment may not suffice to facilitate internal-
ization processes. The distinction between autonomy support and structure
features of environments (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1990) may help to explicate
these findings.
In autonomy-supporting contexts, choice is provided, pressure to engage
in the behavior is minimized, and individuals are encouraged to initiate
actions themselves. In contrast, structure concerns the degree to which the
link between the behavior and salient outcomes is apparent, expectations are
clear, and positive feedback is provided. Deci and Ryan (1985, 2000) and
Koestner and Losier (2002) hypothesized that high levels of autonomy sup-
port, even without the provision of structure, will result in high levels of
intrinsic motivation. However, autonomy support alone will not promote an
understanding of why it is personally important and meaningful to perform
certain activities, even the most uninteresting, which are nevertheless im-
portant to optimal functioning.
It is worth noting that, contrary to previous research (e.g., Wilson et al.,
2002), the motivational regulations considered within SDT were not cor-
related significantly with moderate and mild forms of exercise behavior. One
explanation for this finding is that, in the current study, the majority of mild
and moderate exercise reported by the participants was in the form of easy
or fast walking, or easy cycling. We suggest that these activities are usually
habitual in nature, and thus may require less cognitive processing than more
structured and vigorous forms of exercise. Future research should examine
2258 EDMUNDS ET AL.
whether the motivational processes embedded in SDT are more important
for purposeful, rather than incidental forms of exercise (e.g., walking for
transportation, walking to shop). Indeed, other social cognitive models have
been found to predict habitual or low-intensity behaviors such as walking
poorly (Sallis & Hovell, 1990).
Future work on motivational predictors may benefit from being more
specific regarding the type of exercise behavior under examination. It re-
mains possible that different activities may be guided by different psycho-
logical needs, and thus different regulatory styles. For example, for some
individuals, playing squash, which is typically an interesting activity, may
satisfy different needs or be a far more intrinsically motivated activity than a
vigorous gym workout.
In addition, the current research, like previous studies in this area (e.g.,
Wilson et al., 2002, 2003; Wilson & Rodgers, 2004), incorporated only a
self-reported measure of physical activity. Although shown to be valid and
reliable (Jacobs et al., 1993), such an assessment may still be subject to
reporting bias. Future work should focus on establishing the interrelation-
ships between psychological needs, motivational regulations, and exercise
behaviors using more objective measurements of physical activity (e.g., via
use of triaxial accelerometers) to ascertain whether the present findings can
be replicated.
As the current study has provided preliminary evidence supporting the
major tenets of SDT in the exercise domain, future research may extend this
research to explore the propositions of Deci and Ryan (2000) and Koestner
and Losier (2002). These authors identified specific patterns of psychological
need satisfaction that will be most salient to the emergence and sustenance
of each of the different forms of motivation. For the least self-determined
forms of extrinsic motivation, relatedness and competence need satis-
faction are postulated to be most important. Autonomy is believed to be
central to intrinsic motivation and self-determined forms of extrinsic
motivation. With respect to self-determined forms of extrinsic motivation,
autonomy is assumed to combine with relatedness. For intrinsic motivation,
autonomy and competence are proffered. If these different predictions are
upheld, it would provide practitioners with valuable information regarding
which needs to focus on in attempting to facilitate a specific motivational
orientation.
Inspection of the psychometric properties of the current assessments gave
some cause for concern regarding one of the measurement tools utilized;
that is, assessment of psychological need satisfaction. In the absence of a
more psychometrically sound instrument to measure the three specific psy-
chological needs proposed by SDT in the exercise domain, we chose a
questionnaire that provided a comprehensive assessment of these constructs.
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2259
However, the alpha values obtained for autonomy and competence need
satisfaction in the present study were marginal. This latter finding highlights
a need for new and improved assessments of psychological needs in the
exercise domain.
In terms of the three psychological needs, it was interesting to note that
PAS was most highly correlated with satisfaction of the need for relatedness.
This may lead to questioning the convergent validity of the PAS measure
utilized, as one may expect PAS to be most highly correlated with autonomy
need satisfaction. However, consistent with the current findings, previous
research in the sporting and healthcare domains suggests that autonomy
support is an important nutrient in the satisfaction of all three psychological
needs, not solely autonomy (e.g., Gagne
´et al., 2003; Sheldon, Williams, &
Joiner, 2003).
With regard to relatedness, specifically, autonomy support is believed to
boost the quality of interpersonal relatedness between the patient and prac-
titioner (Sheldon et al., 2003). Patients’ sense of competence is predicted also
by healthcare providers’ perceived autonomy supportiveness (Sheldon et al.,
2003). In addition, evidence from research conducted in sports settings
found autonomy support provided by the parents of young gymnasts to be
correlated significantly with relatedness need satisfaction, but not auto-
nomy. In contrast, autonomy support from the coach was correlated sig-
nificantly with both relatedness and autonomy (Gagne
´et al., 2003). Given
that other studies (e.g., Wilson & Rodgers, 2004) in the exercise domain
have failed to examine the mediating role of psychological need satisfaction
between PAS and each of the motivational regulations, we cannot discern
whether this finding is indeed pertinent to the exercise domain or is a con-
sequence of a poor measurement instrument that consequently requires
further psychometric validation. Even so, the finding that the auto-
nomy support provided by the exercise class leader predicted competence
need satisfaction should be considered a promising finding, given that com-
petence plays such a key and central role in predicting exercise behavior in
the current study. It is also important to reinforce the point that the current
study is cross-sectional in design. Thus, we cannot infer causality when
considering the findings of the current investigation. To rectify this short-
coming, future research would benefit from employing experimental designs.
In addition, subsequent studies might strive to recruit a sufficient number of
participants so that the use of structural equation modeling techniques is
appropriate. Unfortunately, given the small number of participants consti-
tuting the subsample in the current investigation, it was not possible to test a
model describing sequential links between autonomy support, psychological
needs, motivational regulations, and exercise behaviors. However, despite
the limitations presented, the results of the present investigation support the
2260 EDMUNDS ET AL.
tenability of the constructs and propositions embedded in SDT with respect
to the prediction of total and, in particular, vigorous exercise behavior. Such
work should help to provide a theoretical base on which behavioral inter-
ventions aimed at increasing and sustaining levels of physical activity can be
designed, tested, and implemented.
References
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51, 1173-1182.
Baumeister, R., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psy-
chological Bulletin,117, 497-529.
Belsley, D. A. (1991). Conditioning diagnostics: Collinearity and weak data in
regression. New York: Wiley.
Belsley, D. A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R. E. (1980). Regression diagnostics:
Identifying influential data and sources of collinearity. New York:
Wiley.
Biddle, J. H., & Mutrie, N. (2001). Psychology of physical activity: Deter-
minants, well-being, and interventions. New York: Routledge.
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determinants
of behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrin-
sic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,18,
105-115.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. (1994). Facilitating
internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of
Personality,62, 119-142.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determina-
tion in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1990). A motivational approach to the self:
Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium
on Motivation. Perspectives on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 237-288). Lincoln,
NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological
Inquiry,11, 227-268.
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2261
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagne
´, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., &
Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being
in the work organizations of a former Eastern bloc country: A cross-
cultural study of self-determination. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin,27, 930-942.
Department of Health. (2004). Choosing health? Choosing activity: A con-
sultation on how to increase physical activity. London, UK: Department
of Health/Department of Culture Media and Sport.
Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). GPOWER: A priori, post hoc, and com-
promise power analyses for MS DOS (computer program). Bonn,
Germany: Bonn University, Department of Psychology.
Frederick, C. M., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). Differences in motivation for sport
and exercise and their relations with participation and mental health.
Journal of Sport Behavior,16, 124-146.
Frederick-Recascino, C. M. (2002). Self-determination theory and
participation motivation research in the sport and exercise domain.
In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination
research (pp. 277–294). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester
Press.
Gagne
´, M., Ryan, R., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy support and need
satisfaction in the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology,15, 372-390.
Godin, G., & Shepard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise
behavior in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science,
10, 141-146.
Jacobs, D. R., Ainsworth, B. E., Hartman, T. J., & Leon, A. S. (1993).
A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical activity
questionnaires. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,25,
81-91.
Kasser, T., Davey, J., & Ryan, R. M. (1992). Motivation and employee–
supervisor discrepancies in a psychiatric vocational rehabilitation
setting. Rehabilitation Psychology,37, 175-187.
Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways of being
internally motivated: A closer look at introjection, identification, and
intrinsic motivation. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
self-determination research (pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press.
Koestner, R., Losier, G. F., Vallerand, R. J., & Carducci, D. (1996). Iden-
tified and introjected forms of political internalization: Extending self-
determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70,
1025-1036.
2262 EDMUNDS ET AL.
Losier, G. F., & Koestner, R. (1999). Intrinsic versus identified regulation in
distinct political campaigns: The consequences following politics for
pleasure versus personal meaningfulness. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin,25, 287-298.
Markland, D., & Tobin, V. (2004). A modification of the Behavioral
Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to include an assessment of
amotivation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,26, 191-
196.
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. V. (1989). Psychometric pro-
perties of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport
setting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Sport and
Exercise,60, 48-58.
Mullan, E., & Markland, D. (1997). Variations in self-determination across
the stages of change for exercise in adults. Motivation and Emotion,21,
349-362.
Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). A graded con-
ceptualization of self-determination in the regulation of exercise
behavior: Development of a measure using confirmatory factor
analytic procedures. Personality and Individual Differences,23,
745-752.
Pate, R. R., Pratt, M., Blair, S. N., Haskell, W. L., Macera, C. A.,
Bouchard, C., et al. (1995). Physical activity and public health. A rec-
ommendation from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and
the American College of Sports Medicine. Journal of the American
Medical Association,273, 402-407.
Pedhazur, EJ. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation
and prediction (3
rd
ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Briere, N. M. (2001).
Associations among perceived autonomy support, forms of self-regulation,
and persistence: A prospective study. Motivation and Emotion,25,
279-306.
Perrin, B. (1979). Survey of physical activity in the regional municipality of
Waterloo. Recreation Research Review,6, 48-52.
Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educational settings.
In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination
research (pp. 183–203). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester
Press.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the interpersonal sphere:
An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,43, 450-461.
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative
processes. Journal of Personality,63, 397-427.
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2263
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
American Psychologist,55, 68-78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A
review of research in hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review
of Psychology,52, 141-166.
Ryan, R. M., Frederick, C. M., Lepes, D., Rubio, N., & Sheldon, K. (1997).
Intrinsic motivation and exercise adherence. International Journal of
Sport Psychology,28, 335-354.
Sallis, J. F., & Hovell, M. F. (1990). Determinants of exercise behavior.
Exercise and Sports Science Reviews,18, 307-327.
Sheldon, KM., Williams, G., & Joiner, T. (2003). Self-determination theory
in the clinic: Motivating physical and mental health. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model of contextual
motivation in physical education: Using constructs from self-determi-
nation theory and achievement goals to predict physical activity inten-
tions. Journal of Educational Psychology,91, 97-110.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics
(4
th
ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity
and health; A report of the Surgeon General (executive summary). Pitts-
burgh, PA: Superintendent of Documents.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2000). Healthy People
2010: Understanding and improving health (2
nd
ed.). Washington, DC:
U.S. Government Printing Office.
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and ex-
trinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (pp. 271-360). New York: Academic Press.
Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan, R. M., & Deci,
E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and weight-loss
maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70,
115-126.
Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2002). The relationship between exercise
motives and physical self-esteem in female exercise participants: An
application of self-determination theory. Journal of Applied Biobehavi-
oral Research,7, 30-43.
Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2004). The relationship between per-
ceived autonomy support, exercise regulations, and behavioral intentions
in women. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,5, 229-242.
Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., Blanchard, C. M., & Gessell, J. (2003). The
relationship between psychological needs, self-determined motivation,
2264 EDMUNDS ET AL.
exercise attitudes, and physical fitness. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
chology,33, 2373-2392.
Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., & Fraser, S. N. (2002). Examining the
psychometric properties of the Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Ques-
tionnaire. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science,6,
1-21.
World Health Organization. (1995). Exercise for health: WHO/FIMS Com-
mittee on Physical Activity for Health. Bulletin of the World Health
Organization,73, 135-136.
SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY AND EXERCISE 2265
... According to Deci & Ryan (1985), when a person's basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness are satisfied, he/she will feel motivated. Scholars from various fields have taken a keen interest in basic psychological needs such as health care (Ng et al., 2012), education (Vansteenkiste et al., 2006), sports and exercise (Edmunds et al., 2006). ...
Thesis
The aim of this research is to define, explain and study the influence of customer control that is the result of technological development. Technological evolution has transformed the past business practices and changed the role of customer from passive to active. This role change has the direct influence on employees. Customer being part of a business is studied in various forms such as a co-creator, co-producer, participator, enabler, coordinator, innovator and differentiator. This study is extending the role of customer to the controller. Customer control can improve social needs among employees but can also cause powerlessness, and role ambiguity. To keep employees aligned with organizational objectives, Management Control Systems (MCS) are adapted. In past, MCSs were mostly focused on the internal environment, yet the external environment such as market and customer has proved its importance. This study defined customer control as the authority given to the customer to evaluate employees. It will help to recognize the impact of customer control on employees’ behaviours such as affective commitment, job involvement and alienation. Another purpose of this research is to observe moderating effect of organizational and supervisory support. Furthermore, to investigate the impact of self-determination theory by studying its mediating role between customer control and employee behaviours. This study is conducted in two countries, Pakistan and France. The data for both studies is collected through questionnaires and analysed by using SPSS and AMOS.
... The major findings of this study imply that the absence of an indirect effect suggested that high or low values of RAI did not increase or decrease volitional high-intensity exercise. Nevertheless, studies have shown that exercise behavioral regulation was found to be predictive of vigorous exercise, where introjected regulation, identified regulation and intrinsic motivation were positively associated with strenuous exercise behaviors [32]. Still, intrinsic motivation appears to be a more consistent predictor of moderate and vigorous exercise than identified regulation, and autonomous motivation was predictive of long-term moderate-to-vigorous exercise [33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
We aimed to investigate the relationship between physical fitness and motivation in adolescents and analyze if the associations of physical fitness with volitional exercise intensity in adolescents are mediated by motivation. The participants were 108 adolescents (58 girls 16.0 ± 0.92 years). Cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) was assessed using the Yo-YoITL1, and the push-up test was used to evaluate strength. Body composition was measured by bioelectrical impedance analysis. The intervention was applied in the first 10-15 min of each Physical Education class (PEC), twice a week, for 16 weeks and ranged from 14 to 20 all-out bouts intervals, adopting a 2:1 work to rest ratio. A cut-point of ≥90% of the maximal heart rate (HR) was used as a criterion for satisfactory compliance with high-intensity exercise. Volition intensity was assessed through a forearm wearable plethysmography heart rate sensor to ensure compliance with the exercise stimulus at the predetermined target HR zone. Motivation was estimated with a validated questionnaire (BREQ-3). Mediation effects were estimated using bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals and were deemed significant if zero was not included in the intervals, and values below 0.05 were considered to indicate statistical significance. The mediation analysis revealed a non-significant indirect effect of physical fitness through motivation on exercise intensity, specifically on CRF (B = −0.0355, 95% BootCI [−0.5838; 0.4559]), muscular fitness (B = −0.7284, 95% BootCI [−2.0272; 0.2219]) and body fat (B = 0.5092, 95% BootCI [−0.4756; 1.6934]). These results suggest that high or low values of motivation did not increase or decrease volitional high-intensity exercise, and lower levels of fitness (CRF, muscular and body fat) were associated with higher volitional exercise intensity. These findings highlight the need for regular moderate-to-vigorous physical exercise for maintaining or improving physical fitness, regardless of motivation regulations, and emphasize the importance of new strategies in PEC with acute vigorous-intensity activities that retain the health-enhancing effects.
... Motivation can enhance or deter one's desire to engage in PA (Teixeira et al., 2012). Lack of motivation for PA can be explained by many factors such as competing demands for an individual's time (e.g., education, career, and family obligations) (Teixeira et al., 2012), lack of self-efficacy (situation-specific confidence) in one's ability to perform the behavior (Edmunds et al., 2006), and lack of autonomous motivation (Teixeira et al., 2012). Intrinsic motivation is the foundational component of SDT. ...
Article
Physical activity (PA) levels among the general population remain low despite well-known benefits. Previous research has demonstrated PA-based charity fundraising events may act as a catalyst for increased motivation for PA by fulfilling basic psychological needs while providing an emotional connection to a greater good. Therefore, the current study used a behavior-change based theoretical paradigm to develop and assess the feasibility of a 12-week charity-based virtual PA program intended to increase motivation and PA adherence. Forty-three participants were enrolled in a virtual 5 K run/walk charity event that included a structured training protocol, web-based motivation resources, and charity education. Eleven participants completed the program and results indicated no differences pre- and post-participation in motivation (t(10) = 1.16, p = .14) and self-efficacy (t(10) = 0.66, p = .26), but increased scores on charity knowledge (t(9) = -2.50, p = .02). Attrition was attributed to timing, weather, and isolated nature of a solo virtual program. Participants enjoyed the structure of the program and felt the training and educational information was beneficial, but could have been more robust. Thus, in its current format, the program design lacks efficacy. Integral changes to increase program feasibility should include group programming, participant-chosen charity, and more accountability.
... Estas tres necesidades psicológicas básicas son esenciales para un funcionamiento óptimo del progreso natural hacia el crecimiento y la integración, así como para el desarrollo social/constructivo y el bienestar personal . La TAD establece que la satisfacción de las necesidades psicológicas básicas de autonomía, competencia y relaciones sociales da lugar al desarrollo de motivaciones más autodeterminadas (Alvarez et al., 2009;Balaguer et al., 2008;Edmunds et al., 2006;Marcos Pardo et al., 2014) o también conocida como motivación más autónoma (Hagger et al., 2014). El Modelo Jerárquico de Motivación (HMM) (Vallerand, 2001), fue desarrollado para mejorar y asociar estas necesidades psicológicas básicas con los constructos motivacionales de la TAD (Deci & Ryan, 2002). ...
... For consumers of fitness services, the initial cognitive drivers for consumption are self-perception and health awareness. Wilson, Rodgers, Blanchard, & Gessell (2003) and Edmunds, Ntoumanis, & Duda (2006) stated the positive relationship between self-determined motivation, psychological and exercise attitudes with physical fitness through the application of self-determination theory. Between these two drivers, the health consciousness seems to be more dominant, reflected by studies from Lim et al., (2015) and Khisti & Raizada (2020), with the majority of fitness services users claiming their purchasing and using objectives as health improvement, strength gain, weight loss, and stress reduction. ...
Article
Full-text available
The situation of fitness services in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic in general and in Vietnam in particular implied many complicated dilemmas, highlighted by forceful changes in human behaviors and operational difficulties. A mixed approach with multiple linear regression and thematic analysis was adopted from results of 102 quantitative respondents and 12 qualitative interviewees, helping unveil the insights of membership customer behaviors via their involvement into fitness services and corresponding recognition from service providers. Some extracted highlights are the significant positive relationships between physical preparation, perceived level of program effectiveness, price, and level of customer involvement into new operational fitness services, the surprising non-relationship between customers’ health awareness and their involvement. Those findings are the foundations for fitness businesses in Vietnam to recover and develop in a new-normal post-Covid era.
... Estas tres necesidades psicológicas básicas son esenciales para un funcionamiento óptimo del progreso natural hacia el crecimiento y la integración, así como para el desarrollo social/constructivo y el bienestar personal . La TAD establece que la satisfacción de las necesidades psicológicas básicas de autonomía, competencia y relaciones sociales da lugar al desarrollo de motivaciones más autodeterminadas (Alvarez et al., 2009;Balaguer et al., 2008;Edmunds et al., 2006;Marcos Pardo et al., 2014) o también conocida como motivación más autónoma (Hagger et al., 2014). El Modelo Jerárquico de Motivación (HMM) (Vallerand, 2001), fue desarrollado para mejorar y asociar estas necesidades psicológicas básicas con los constructos motivacionales de la TAD (Deci & Ryan, 2002). ...
Chapter
La Organización Mundial de la Salud relaciona las funciones físicas y mentales con el nivel de autogobierno y la participación social en la comunidad. Dicha participación social es una fuente de apoyo que guía hacia un envejecimiento positivo y exitoso debido a que la interacción en un grupo otorga al adulto mayor (AM) una red de apoyo comunitario que protege y/o amortigua situaciones de estrés, promoviendo una mejor salud física y mental. Habiendo sido evidenciado que las intervenciones centradas en la amistad y el desarrollo de habilidades se encuentran entre las más eficaces frente al aislamiento social y la soledad en el AM, resulta necesario difundir entre la sociedad el gran papel que la práctica de ejercicio físico puede desarrollar como problemáticas sociales en dicho colectivo poblacional, así como en la promoción de su autoestima, afectividad, apoyo emocional y psicológico como medida para contrarrestar la alta prevalencia de las mencionadas
... Autonomy Support. Autonomy support was assessed using the Perceived Autonomy Support: Exercise Climate Questionnaire (PASECQ) (Edmunds, Ntoumanis, & Duda, 2006) which has been adapted and validated for the Portuguese sports context by Moutão et al. (2012). This questionnaire consists of 6 items which evaluate perceptions of autonomy support provided by a coach/instructor (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
This cross-sectional study investigated if the coach's autonomy support, age and time of practice are associated with the development of life skills among 461 young Brazilians athletes (325 boys and 136 girls) aged between 10–17 years (Mage=15.12, SD=1.44). Data collection was conducted via the Life Skills Scale for Sport (P-LSSS) and Perceived Autonomy Support: Exercise Climate Questionnaire (PASECQ). Data analysis was conducted through independent t test, Pearson’s correlation and Multiple Regression (p<.05). Main results showed that coach´s autonomy support made the largest positive contribution to all eight dimensions (βrange=.07-.19, p<.05) and total life skill (β=.18; p<.001). Age made the largest positive contribution to the dimensions of teamwork, goal setting, leadership and communication (βrange=.11-.19, p<.05) and total life skills (β=.13; p<.01). However, practice time showed no contribution to life skills. This study revealed that coach´s autonomy support and age are associated positively with life skills development among youth athletes. Este estudio transversal investigó el association del apoyo a la autonomía del entrenador, la edad y el tiempo de práctica en el desarrollo de habilidades para la vida entre 461 jóvenes atletas brasileños (325 niños y 136 niñas) de entre 10 y 17 años (Mage = 15,12, SD = 1,44). La recopilación de datos se realizó mediante la Escala de habilidades para la vida para el deporte (P-LSSS) y el Apoyo a la autonomía percibida: Cuestionario de clima de ejercicio (PASECQ). El análisis de los datos se realizó mediante prueba t independiente, correlación de Pearson y regresión múltiple (p <.05). Los resultados principales mostraron que el apoyo a la autonomía del entrenador hizo la mayor contribución positiva a las ocho dimensiones (β rango = .07-.19, p <.05) y la habilidad para la vida total (β = .18; p <.001). La edad hizo la mayor contribución positiva a las dimensiones de trabajo en equipo, establecimiento de metas, liderazgo y comunicación (βrange = .11-.19, p <.05) y habilidades para la vida total (β = .13; p <.01). Sin embargo, el tiempo de práctica no mostró ninguna contribución a las habilidades para la vida. Este estudio reveló que el apoyo a la autonomía del entrenador y la edad son associados positivos del desarrollo de habilidades para la vida entre los atletas jóvenes. Este estudo transversal investigou se o suporte de autonomia do treinador, idade e tempo de prática estão associados ao desenvolvimento de habilidades de vida entre 461 jovens atletas brasileiros (325 meninos e 136 meninas) com idade entre 10-17 anos (M = 15,12, DP = 1,44). A coleta de dados foi realizada por meio da Escala de Habilidades de Vida para o Esporte (P-LSSS) e Suporte à Autonomia Percebida: Questionário de Clima de Exercício (PASECQ). A análise dos dados foi realizada por meio de teste t independente, correlação de Pearson e regressão múltipla (p <0,05). Os principais resultados mostraram que o suporte à autonomia do treinador deu a maior contribuição positiva para todas as oito dimensões (faixa β = .07-.19, p <.05) e habilidade de vida total (β = .18; p <.001). A idade deu a maior contribuição positiva para as dimensões de trabalho em equipe, estabelecimento de metas, liderança e comunicação (βrange = .11-.19, p <.05) e habilidades de vida totais (β = .13; p <.01). No entanto, o tempo de prática não mostrou nenhuma contribuição para as habilidades de vida. Este estudo revelou que o suporte à autonomia do treinador e a idade estão associados positivamente ao desenvolvimento de habilidades para a vida entre jovens atletas.
Article
One research university has brought formal training to campus to support women and provide the needed leadership training in a cost-effective manner. Based on a coaching method used at Harvard in their Women's Leadership Forum, our initiative has completed its fifth cycle and provides an on-campus leadership development and support program for women. This paper offers insights into the Women's Leadership Initiative provided by focus group participants and also highlights the overall impact and effectiveness using survey data. Keywords: Women, leadership, higher education, skill-development
Article
Objective this study sought to systematically review and synthesize qualitative data to explore older adult exergame experiences and perceptions. Methods a comprehensive search was conducted in seven databases from the earliest available date to May 2022. All qualitative and mixed-method studies available in English and explored exergame experiences in older adults were included. Tools from the Joanna Briggs Institute were used for data extraction and synthesis. Data were extracted using the Capability, Opportunity and Motivation Model of Behaviour (COM-B model) as a guide, and a pragmatic meta-aggregative approach was applied to synthesize the findings. Results this systematic review identified 128 findings and aggregated 9 categories from the 10 qualitative research articles included, and three synthesized findings were: older adult capability, opportunities in the exergaming program and motivation in the exergaming program. Capability consisted of attitude toward exergames, age- or health-related impairments and exergame knowledge and skills. Opportunities included older adult-friendly exergame design and social influence. Motivation included self-efficacy, support, instruction and feedback, health benefits, as well as unpleasant exergaming experiences. Conclusions it is crucial to tailor the exergaming program to suit the older population. We identified barriers and facilitators of implementing exergaming in older adults and found most barriers are surmountable. The results of the current systematic review could provide evidence for the design and implementation of exergaming programs among older adults. The ConQual score of the synthesized findings was assessed as low. Dependability and credibility should be accounted for in future studies to increase confidence.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
I: Background.- 1. An Introduction.- 2. Conceptualizations of Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination.- II: Self-Determination Theory.- 3. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Perceived Causality and Perceived Competence.- 4. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Interpersonal Communication and Intrapersonal Regulation.- 5. Toward an Organismic Integration Theory: Motivation and Development.- 6. Causality Orientations Theory: Personality Influences on Motivation.- III: Alternative Approaches.- 7. Operant and Attributional Theories.- 8. Information-Processing Theories.- IV: Applications and Implications.- 9. Education.- 10. Psychotherapy.- 11. Work.- 12. Sports.- References.- Author Index.
Article
Self-determination theory is grounded in the belief that people work best and are happiest when they feel that they are in control of their own lives. This invaluable book explains the ramifications of the theory and provides clinical examples to show that it can be used to motivate patients undergoing treatment for such physical or psychological issues as diabetes management, smoking cessation, post-traumatic stress, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and depression. The first part of the book provides historical background to self-determination theory, showing that it is humanistically oriented and has three decades of empirical research behind it. In the process, the authors discuss why humanistic psychology fell out of favor in academic psychology; why "self-help" and New Age books have such perennial popularity; and why it is so important for authorities to support patients' sense of self. The remainder of the book presents many specific case examples to describe the theory's application. © 2003 by Kennon M. Sheldon, Geoffrey Williams, and Thomas Joiner. All rights reserved.
Article
Objective: To encourage increased participation in physical activity among Americans of all ages by issuing a public health recommendation on the types and amounts of physical activity needed for health promotion and disease prevention. Participants: A planning committee of five scientists was established by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine to organize a workshop. This committee selected 15 other workshop discussants on the basis of their research expertise in issues related to the health implications of physical activity. Several relevant professional or scientific organizations and federal agencies also were represented. Evidence: The panel of experts reviewed the pertinent physiological, epidemiologic, and clinical evidence, including primary research articles and recent review articles. Consensus process: Major issues related to physical activity and health were outlined, and selected members of the expert panel drafted sections of the paper from this outline. A draft manuscript was prepared by the planning committee and circulated to the full panel in advance of the 2-day workshop. During the workshop, each section of the manuscript was reviewed by the expert panel. Primary attention was given to achieving group consensus concerning the recommended types and amounts of physical activity. A concise "public health message" was developed to express the recommendations of the panel. During the ensuing months, the consensus statement was further reviewed and revised and was formally endorsed by both the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. Conclusion: Every US adult should accumulate 30 minutes or more of moderate-intensity physical activity on most, preferably all, days of the week.
Article
Two prospective studies tested the hypothesis that intrinsic motives for physical activities facilitate long-term adherence. In Study 1, participants in two physical activity classes, Tae Kwon Do and Aerobics (N = 40), were compared in their motives for participating using the Motivation for Physical Activity Measure (MPAM; Frederick & Ryan, 1993). Participation motives were also used to predict adherence. Results showed that Tae Kwon Do participants were higher in enjoyment and competence motives and lower in body-related motives than those in aerobics. They also showed better adherence. Further analyses revealed that group differences in adherence were mediated by enjoyment motives. Body-focused motives were unrelated to adherence. In Study 2, subjects joining a nautilus center (N-155) rated their initial motives on a revised Motivation for Physical Activity Measure (MPAM-R). They also rated workout length, challenge, and enjoyment after each exercise session. Results revealed that adherence was associated with motives focused on enjoyment, competence, and social interaction, but not with motives focused on fitness or appearance. Post-workout ratings of enjoyment also predicted adherence. Discussion focuses on the importance of intrinsic motivation for exercise adherence.