ArticlePDF Available

Short Talks Being there versus seeing there: Trust via video

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We studied the emergence of trust in a social dilemma game in four different communication situations: face-to-face, video, audio, and text chat. Three-person groups did 30 rounds of a social dilemma game and we measured trust by the extent to which they cooperated vs. competed. The face-to-face groups quickly achieved cooperative behavior, while the text chat groups continued to compete throughout. The video groups achieved the same levels of trust as the face-to-face groups, although perhaps a bit more slowly. The audio group was intermediate. These results show that trust can emerge through mediated communication.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Being there versus seeing there: Trust via video
Nathan Bos, Darren Gergle, Judith S. Olson, Gary M. Olson
Collaboratory for Research on Electronic Work (CREW)
School of Information, University of Michigan
701 Tappan Suite E2420
Ann Arbor, MI USA 48019
+1 734-647-7730
{serp, dgergle, jsolson, gmo}@umich.edu
ABSTRACT
We studied the emergence of trust in a social dilemma
game in four different communication situations: face-to-
face, video, audio, and text chat. Three-person groups did
30 rounds of a social dilemma game and we measured
trust by the extent to which they cooperated vs. competed.
The face-to-face groups quickly achieved cooperative
behavior, while the text chat groups continued to compete
throughout. The video groups achieved the same levels of
trust as the face-to-face groups, although perhaps a bit
more slowly. The audio group was intermediate. These
results show that trust can emerge through mediated
communication.
Keywords
Trust, social dilemmas, communication, media
INTRODUCTION
There is increasing interest in the nature of trust over the
Internet. The organizational consultant Charles Handy [1]
has received widespread attention for his strong claim that
“trust takes touch.” Does it? A study by Rocco [2] showed
how powerful even brief direct contact can be. She found
that groups meeting over email could not develop enough
trust to reach the optimal outcome in a social dilemma,
whereas groups meeting face-to-face did so easily and
quickly. However, in a third condition, groups that
interacted via e-mail developed trust if they had a brief
face-to-face meeting prior to the experiment.
Our research sought to extend Rocco’s results. We asked
whether other forms of computer-mediated-communication
(CMC) might lead to the formation of trust. In particular,
we looked at groups doing a social dilemma task who
interacted over video, over phone conferencing, and via a
text chat. All these media are more interactive than e-mail,
but less “rich” than face-to-face interactions.
We expected that video might be sufficiently rich to allow
trust to emerge in a social dilemma game. Audio only was
less clear, since it clearly offers fewer cues than video.
Chat
seemed as if it would be close to e-mail, though it is a bit
more interactive than the latter. So our range of conditions
offers a way to assess the character of communication
needed for trust to emerge in this setting.
METHOD
Forty-five three-person groups played a game called
Daytrader, where they periodically communicated with
other players via one of four media: face-to-face,
videoconference, phone conference, or in an Internet
chatroom.
The Daytrader game is in the general class of games called
social dilemmas. Well-known social dilemmas include the
Prisoner’s Dilemma and the Problem of the Commons.
Social dilemmas are defined as situations where the best
interest of the group as a whole conflicts with the best
interest of each individual, so that if each looks out only
for oneself, all lose. In a multi-round social dilemma, the
maximum group benefit accrues only when the group
develops a level of trust, and each individual agrees to act
cooperatively.
In the Daytrader game, players are given 30 tokens each
round of the game and must decide whether to invest as an
individual or risk investing with the group. The individual
investment yields a guaranteed doubling of the payoff
every round. In the group investment all three players’
contributions are lumped together, multiplied by 3, and
then distributed back evenly. So an individual can receive
triple from the group when everyone contributes, but also
risks losing when other players hold back. There was also
a bonus of 90 tokens is given every five rounds to
whichever player made the most money in the previous
five rounds. This bonus has the effect of giving a windfall
profit to players who contribute less than other group
members. (When players invest identical amounts for
five rounds, i.e. cooperate, the bonus is split.) Groups play
30 rounds of this game in total, with discussions held
every five rounds via one of four media channels.
Subjects for this experiment were mostly students or others
affiliated with the University. All were unacquainted
before the study, and never interacted with each other
except via their particular experimental condition. Subjects
were paid according to how well they did in the game,
with each ‘token’ worth 1 cent, and each participant
guaranteed to make at least $15.
RESULTS
We used the total payoff earned by a group to measure the
degree of cooperation – the higher the total group payoff
the more the cooperation. By implication this measures
trust.
FTF Video Audio Text Chat
Communication Condition
5500
6000
6500
7000
7500
Mean Group Payoff
_
_
_
_
Figure 1. Average total group payoffs in four conditions
ANOVA showed a significant difference among the four
conditions (F(3,41) = 6.54, P < .002). The means and
deviations for the four conditions were: FTF n=9, mean=
7599, s.d.=211; Video n=14, mean=7531, s.d.=305; Audio
n=13, mean=7262, s.d.=520; Chat n=9, mean=6859,
s.d.=645. The four conditions produced results ordered as
we predicted, with face-to-face the most cooperative and
quick to build trust, followed by video, audio, and text chat
conditions. A post-hoc comparison using Bonferroni’s test
(regarded as a conservative test) found significant
differences in f-t-f vs. chat and video vs. chat.
Figure 2. Round by round group contribution averages
across four conditions
Figure 2 shows the trial-by-trail performance of the
groups. The top line (FTF) indicates that trust in the face-
to-face condition forms quickly, and continues throughout.
Video also converges at the maximum, but takes somewhat
longer, and the audio (phone) condition takes longer still,
with more variation throughout. The text chat average
does not converge on the maximum at any point, (although
some individual chat groups did cooperate fully by the
end).
Another noteworthy feature of figure 2 are the vertical
dropoffs occurring on a 5 round cycle, with a final dropoff
at the end. The dropoffs are the results of defections
within the game, typically where one player violates an
agreement and the other players rapidly retaliate. The tops
of the spikes are the rounds immediately after discussions,
when cooperation has been re-established. The dropoff at
the end, which is visible in all four conditions, is the result
of last-round defections at the end of the game. A t-test
comparing rounds 29 and 30 showed a large significant
difference (P<.000) between the group investments in
these rounds.
Post-questionnaires on group trust confirm the differences
between the conditions. A 10-item scale measuring the
trustworthiness of the group members (alpha = .96)
including items such as “the other players in the game
could be trusted” was significantly different between
conditions (F(3, 142) = 8.16, P<.000).
DISCUSSION
Videoconferencing may be as good as face-to-face for
building trust. In this experiment video was
indistinguishable from face-to-face, and both were
significantly better than text chat. Although we cannot yet
statistically separate the phone condition, it appears that
phone is somewhere in between text chat and video for
trust building.
It does appear that trust forms more slowly in mediated
conditions. Figure 2 shows that it took video and audio
some time to ‘catch up’ with the face-to-face groups.
Groups using mediated communications seem more likely
to form partial agreements (e.g. “lets all contribute 15,
then 20…”) rather than fully cooperate from the
beginning.
The defections on trial 30 suggest that the kind of trust
that was formed in all of these conditions may be
vulnerable to opportunistic behavior, even when face-to-
face. Late-round defections are a well-known phenomena
in social dilemmas. Our data may allow more insight into
when these are likely to occur, and how communication
affects it. Additional analysis will examine both slow trust
and defection events.
In sum, we have demonstrated that trust can emerge in
mediated conditions. These results are a major extension
of the work reported by Rocco [2], with application for
many forms of distributed collaboration. But our data
suggest that this kind of trust may be fragile. Additional
experimental studies can help clarify this. Field studies
will also be useful to examine whether these mediated
interactions can lead to enduring trust in real
organizations.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by National Science Foundation
grant IIS 9977923 to the Olsons.
References
[1] Handy, C. (1995) Trust and the virtual organization.
Harvard Business Review. 73(3), 40-50.
Round Number
302826242220181614121086
Mean Group Payoff
280
260
240
220
200
180
160
140
120
FTF
Video
Audio
Text Chat
[2] Rocco, E. Trust breaks down in electronic contexts but
can be repaired by some initial face-to-face contact, Conference proceedings on human factors in computing
systems, 1998, pp496-502.
... For example, an experiment examining the trust of participants in a social dilemma game through four communication methods-face-to-face, video, audio, and text chat-found that video and face-to-face communication achieved the highest level of trust. Audio communication achieved an intermediate level of trust and text-based chat achieved the lowest level of trust [27,28]. Supporting these findings are further studies comparing trust among humans according to different presentation modes, where it was shown that audio was more trustworthy than text [13][14][15][16]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, augmented reality (AR) technology has grown, and its use has become widespread among smartphone users. People are consuming more and more digital information from various sources and in different presentation modes. Therefore, in this study, we investigate the extent to which different presentation modes relate to the level of trust in information, while considering demographic variables, as well as personality traits and thinking styles. The participants in our experiments were asked to indicate whether certain statements that were presented in various presentation methods (image + text, image + audio, AR + text, AR + audio) were true or false. The results indicate that users are more likely to trust statements that are accompanied by AR than statements that are accompanied by a static image. In addition, younger participants have greater trust in audio-presented information than text-presented information. As AR is expected to grow considerably in popularity in the next few years, users should be cautious of the potential impact on their trust in digital information while using AR.
... Similarly, Verberne et al. [73] found that, in the context of a trust game, a mimicking virtual agent was not liked and trusted more than a non-mimicking one [73]. Although our results do not appear to corroborate previous observations, and there was no effect of condition order (electronic supplementary material, tables S3 and S4), they are supported by recent research that found no difference between face-to-face and video call conditions in terms of trust [93]. Taken together, these results seem to suggest that video calls might ensure enough back-and-forth of interaction cues for mimicry to occur, at least in our student population during an interaction between strangers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many social species, humans included, mimic emotional expressions, with important consequences for social bonding. Although humans increasingly interact via video calls, little is known about the effect of these online interactions on the mimicry of scratching and yawning, and their linkage with trust. The current study investigated whether mimicry and trust are affected by these new communication media. Using participant-confederate dyads ( n = 27), we tested the mimicry of four behaviours across three different conditions: watching a pre-recorded video, online video call, and face-to-face. We measured mimicry of target behaviours frequently observed in emotional situations, yawn and scratch and control behaviours, lip-bite and face-touch. In addition, trust in the confederate was assessed via a trust game. Our study revealed that (i) mimicry and trust did not differ between face-to-face and video calls, but were significantly lower in the pre-recorded condition; and (ii) target behaviours were significantly more mimicked than the control behaviours. This negative relationship can possibly be explained by the negative connotation usually associated with the behaviours included in this study. Overall, this study showed that video calls might provide enough interaction cues for mimicry to occur in our student population and during interactions between strangers. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue ‘Face2face: advancing the science of social interaction’.
... Compared to a traditional approach, synchronous media sharing promoted equal levels of perceived self-disclosure and trust, greater levels of warranting and relatedness, and formation of trust not susceptible to individual differences in agreeableness. Rather than considering video chat as a potentially inferior place to meet [12,13], we show that this synchronous digital context can be leveraged to build new interactive experiences that effectively support people in getting to know each other. ...
Article
Full-text available
Video chat continues to play an increasing role in the personal and professional lives of many people. When meeting someone for the first time via video chat, traditional icebreakers such as discussion questions may be used to ease conversation and facilitate disclosure; however, icebreaker questions have been shown to be less effective at facilitating relationships for some people than others. In this paper, we propose synchronously sharing online media as a more flexible, robust, and effective method of facilitating initial interactions in video chat. Our comparative study of icebreaker questions and media sharing reveals that shared media supports a different style of conversation than icebreakers, but results in equal levels of self-disclosure and trust along with higher levels of warranting and relatedness. Further, while the effectiveness of icebreakers in building trust is lower for individuals low in agreeableness, media sharing results in similar trust formation at all levels of agreeableness. Synchronous media sharing is a promising way of leveraging the digital context of video chat to better support early stages of relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Most environmental problems face social dilemmas. Focusing on the effects of communication, this study examined whether or not online text-based communication affects the cooperation rate in social dilemmas. For this purpose, we developed an online game that simulates environmental problems using a social dilemma structure. In the game, a participant decides how much recyclable waste they dispose of (cooperative behavior) out of the waste generated in a group of three members. The results of the experiment showed that the rate of cooperation was higher after having chatted with other members, indicating the effectiveness of communication. The groups in which all members agreed to cooperate with the entire amount had a higher cooperation rate than the other groups. These results show that online chat communication can promote cooperation in social dilemmas.
Article
Covered faces has been linked with impaired emotion recognition, yet it is entirely unexplored how an occlusion due to face masks may affect individuals’ behavior in economic decisions. Across two studies, we explored whether partially covered faces (due to mask wearing or a horizontal black bar), and emotion displayed by the Responder influence peoples’ sharing behavior in the Ultimatum Game and perceived fairness of one’s proposal. Study 1 showed participants were more willing to equally share their resources with a happy face (compared to a neutral one). In addition, they were more willing to make a fair proposal when the person displayed was not wearing a face mask. Our results also provide evidence that, when people had to judge how fair was their proposal, participants rated a fair proposal as fairer when when Responders showed happy faces without masks, while unfair proposals were rated as fairer with happy masked faces; similarly, angry faces led to fairer ratings for fair offers without masks and unfair offers with masks. Study 2 partially confirmed previous results, highlighting how a simple occlusion on the face does not have a direct effect on the proposal, but moderates the effect of the displayed emotions. These findings indicate that social interactions might be affected by face occlusion, especially when it is represented by a face mask. Indeed, people might judge the same behavior in different ways based on the fact that their counterpart has a partially covered face.
Chapter
Team creativity can be strongly affected by communication. With the trend of online collaboration, the design of communication tools increasingly affects different creative processes of teams working online. To clarify this effect, this study took the perspective of affordances and reviewed 54 papers to map the needs of teams, the design of communication tools, and the effects of tools on creativity in different creative stages. First, we summarized teams’ requirements for communication tools in different stages. Second, we identified key affordances with relevant features. Third, we discussed how these affordances could affect communication and team creativity in different stages and built a mapping of creative teams’ needs and communication tools. The mapping can provide insights for both designing communication tools for creative teams and expanding the current team creativity theories to fit the online context and new communication technologies.KeywordsCommunicationCommunication ToolsTeam CreativityCreative ProcessOnline Collaboration
Article
Full-text available
The early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic were a time of great job insecurity. Knowledge of how such feelings of insecurity were mitigated during a time of crisis has important implications for the development of future interventions. Drawing from uncertainty management theory (UMT), we explored the impact of informational justice perceptions on the effects of job insecurity in a sample collected during the early stages of the pandemic (March–May 2020). The findings indicated that informational justice buffered the impacts of job insecurity on employee life satisfaction and perceived ability to perform. Furthermore, the source of information regarding COVID-19-related job changes mattered for predicting informational justice perceptions. Our findings bolster UMT and have important practical implications for future times of crisis.
Article
Full-text available
When COVID-19 led to mandatory working from home, significant blind spots in supporting the sociality of working life—in the moment and over time—were revealed in enterprise video meetings, and these were a key factor in reports about videoconferencing fatigue. Drawing on a large study (N = 849) of one global technology company’s employees’ experiences of all-remote video meetings during the COVID-19 pandemic, we use a dialectic method to explore the tensions expressed by employees around effectiveness and sociality, as well as their strategies to cope with these tensions. We argue that videoconferencing fatigue arose partly due to work practices and technologies designed with assumptions of steady states and taken-for-granted balances between task and social dimensions of work relationships. Our analysis offers a social lens on videoconferencing fatigue and suggests the need to reconceptualize ideas around designing technologies and practices to enable both effectiveness and sociality in the context of video meetings.
Article
Full-text available
As communication technology capabilities have improved and the globalization of the workforce has resulted in distributed teams, organizations have been shifting towards virtual teams and virtual meetings over the last decade. This trend has been accelerated with current work-from-home orders due to COVID-19. Even though virtual collaboration has, in the past, been the focus of multiple studies, there are some surprising gaps in our knowledge. For instance, there are few empirical studies examining the impact of virtual devices and tools on creative problem-solving. While there is a substantial body of research on electronic brainstorming and the use of virtual tools for idea generation, less is known about earlier processes such as problem construction or later processes such as idea evaluation and idea selection. Furthermore, as a dynamic process, creativity and innovation is heavily influenced by the people engaged in the process and their collaborative environment, yet there is a gap in the literature regarding the type of virtual tools used in the process (i.e., audio + video vs. audio alone, or the use of file-sharing technologies). In this paper, we will review the current literature on virtual teams, virtual meetings, and creativity. We will then explore theoretical frameworks such as media richness theory that can help us understand how virtuality and virtual tools may influence team creativity across the dynamic range of the creative problem-solving process. Finally, given the limited research in the domain of virtual team creativity we provide questions to help guide future research. Research questions will help identify those areas where virtual teams may be beneficial for creativity and areas where virtual teams may be likely to perform less effectively on creative tasks.
Conference Paper
Trust is the prerequisite for success when a collaborative task involves risk of individualistic or deceitful behaviors of others. Can trust emerge in electronic contexts? This issue is explored in an experiment in which trust emergence is measured in both face-to-face (l-t-F) and electronic contexts. In this experiment trust is revealed by the degree of cooperation the group is able to reach in solving a social dilemma, i.e. a situation in which advantages for individualistic behavior make group cooperation highly vulnerable. The experiment consists of two stages. The first stage analyzes the effects of F-t-F and electronic communication on trust Trust succeeds only with F-t-F communication. The second stage investigates whether a pre-meeting F-t-F can promote trust in electronic contexts- Results are positive. Examination of how people converse in these two contexts sheds some light on the effects of technical characteristics and social circumstances on the emergence of trust.