ArticlePDF Available

Motivating environmentally sustainable behavior changes with a virtual polar bear

Authors:

Abstract

Personal choices and activities directly account for much of the energy consumption in the U.S. with secondary impacts of those activities influencing an even larger proportion of energy consumption. Although there is a long history of investigation into motivators for energy-conservation, it is still unclear how to encourage persistent behavior change, and technology has only recently been applied to the problem. In this study we sought to learn if virtual pets could have a positive impact on real-world, environmentally responsible behavior. The polar bear is a powerful iconic symbol for many individuals, and its fate can be simply and directly connected to environmentally responsible behavior. Just as Tamagotchis evoked a powerful response from their owners, we hoped to use attachment to a virtual polar bear as a motivator for energy conservation. We ran a study exploring the impact of attachment on real-world actions. The results of our study suggest that an interactive virtual polar bear may increase environmentally responsible behaviors, especially when emotional attachment takes place.
Motivating Environmentally Sustainable Behavior Changes
with a Virtual Polar Bear
Tawanna Dillahunt, Geof Becker, Jennifer Mankoff and Robert Kraut
HCII, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA
{tdillahu, gbecker, jmankoff, robert.kraut}@andrew.cmu.edu
Abstract
Personal choices and activities directly account for much of the energy consumption in the U.S.
with secondary impacts of those activities influencing an even larger proportion of energy
consumption. Although there is a long history of investigation into motivators for energy-
conservation, it is still unclear how to encourage persistent behavior change, and technology has
only recently been applied to the problem. In this study we sought to learn if virtual pets could
have a positive impact on real-world, environmentally responsible behavior. The polar bear is a
powerful iconic symbol for many individuals, and its fate can be simply and directly connected to
environmentally responsible behavior. Just as Tamagotchis evoked a powerful response from
their owners, we hoped to use attachment to a virtual polar bear as a motivator for energy
conservation. We ran a study exploring the impact of attachment on real-world actions. The
results of our study suggest that an interactive virtual polar bear may increase environmentally
responsible behaviors, especially when emotional attachment takes place.
1. Introduction
Global warming continues to be one of the world’s major issues. In the U.S. alone, Americans
consumed 100 quadrillion BTUs of energy in 2005 [11], with personal, individual activities
accounting for much of this consumption. For example, 40% of the energy consumed is used for
residential or commercial lighting, heating and cooling. All of this is driven directly by
individual choices or indirectly because of consumer needs. Thus, personal choices can lead to a
significant reduction in energy consumption, with a corresponding reduction in the release of
CO2, one of the primary gases responsible for climate change. However, this requires persuading
individuals to change their behavior, and maintain those changes over time, both difficult
propositions. Our approach is to leverage the power of the polar bear as a symbol of climate
change by creating a virtual pet, a technology that has promise for supporting behavior change
(e.g. [7]). We describe a pilot study showing that increased attachment to a virtual polar bear had
a significant positive impact on the number of actions that individuals had taken as reported a
week after they used our software. This work is currently being incorporated into a mobile tool
intended to influence transportation choices and into a social networking website.
There is extensive literature in the areas of environmental sociology, public policy, and more
recently, conservation psychology that discusses the promotion of environmentally responsible
behavior. In past work, we have explored the impact of motivators such as public commitment,
Figure 1: (top) a polar
bear with lots of ice
(bottom) a polar bear
with little ice
frequent feedback, and personalization on environmentally responsible behavior [10]. Research
in conservation psychology implies that animals help humans connect with nature [9]. Vining’s
literature review demonstrates the extensive evidence for strong emotional bonds between
humans and animals but she states that the answer to whether caring about animals or the
environment leads to environmentally responsible behavior remains open [12]. Technology may
provide a mechanism for leveraging these bonds to encourage behavior change. For example,
Tamagotchis are virtual pets requiring nurturing interaction in order to be sustained. This in turn
led to an emotional attachment to these virtual pets, for example owners mourned when their pets
“died” [2]. Lin and Strub’s “Fish’n’Steps” study is an example of how an interactive computer
game using a virtual pet, in this case fish, encourage physical activity [7].
Our approach integrates conservation psychology, a study which looks into relationships
between nature and humans, with persuasive technology, the study of how computers can
leverage psychological cues to motivate and influence behavior [3]. For example, motivators of
environmentally responsible behavior are more effective when they have a direct impact on
people's needs or concerns [1]. Also, caring for real animals can inspire conservation behavior
[12]. In this paper we show that an emotional connection to a virtual pet that responds to
environmental behavior can help motivate an individual by making that
behavior seem to directly impact an individual’s concerns.
2. Experiment
We conducted a one week, between subjects study to explore the effect
of higher attachment to a virtual pet (the attachment group) to lower
attachment to the same pet (the control group) on environmentally
responsible behavior. To create attachment, we used a story describing
environmental change, specifically the impact of climate change on the
habitat of polar bears, pre-tested to elicit sadness. We asked participants
in the attachment group to read it, reflect on their emotions and write
about environmental responsibility, and name the polar bear on their
display.
Participants were shown a Flash-based virtual polar bear on an ice floe
that would grow as they committed to environmentally responsible
actions and decrease as they chose not to commit to actions. As shown in Figure 1, the size of
the ice floe would change depending on the number of actions a subject committed to taking.
Our study tested the following hypotheses:
H1: Commitments - Users who form emotional attachment to the virtual polar bear will commit
to more environmentally responsible actions than users who do not form bonds
H2: Fulfilled Commitments - Users who form emotional attachment to the virtual polar bear
will fulfill (act on) their commitments
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
H1: Num Commitments H2: Num Sustained
Commitments
H3: Donations ($)
Mean Number of Units
Control
Attachment
Figure 2: (above) The attachment graph was
higher for all hypotheses, and H2 (Fulfilled
Commitments) is significant (F[1,11]=6.527,
p=.0309).
H3: Donations - Users who form emotional attachment to the virtual polar bear will donate
more to a zoo than those from the control group
We recruited 20 subjects (10 in each
condition) associated with local universities.
Participants were given $15 and a shower
timer for their time. All participants
completed an initial survey at the start of the
experiment to test whether either group was
more likely to have higher motives for
environmentally responsible behavior and
whether this changed during the experiment.
We used a subset of questions from De
Young’s scales measuring competence and
participation [1]. Participants also completed
two additional scales both before and after
the main intervention (interacting with the
polar bear): the first measured levels of care
on agreeableness and empathy [5] and the
second measured overall environmental
concerns [6]. All scales had a reliability of .65 or higher, measured using Cronbach’s α (values
were α=.8615 for competence, α=.8920 for participation, α=.7579 for care and α=.655 for
environmental concern).
The actions are included in Table 1. Fourteen were taken from sixty actions previously designed
and tested as part of a field study of the StepGreen website [10]. We created an additional
action, “Take an environmental sustainability flyer and give it to a friend,” and made flyers
available to participants.
3. Results and Discussion
Out of the 20 participants, 60% were male and
40% female; 95% of the participants were
students. Out of the 20 participants, 19
completed the first task; we reached 11 to ask
about donations (5 control, 6 attachment), and
we were only able to reach 11 participants to ask
which actions they fulfilled (5 control, 6
attachment). The initial survey showed balance
across the groups in terms of motivation for
environmentally responsible behavior (p=.08833
and .08199).
1 Turn off the water while brushing your teeth
2 Turn off lights if you are leaving a room for more than 10 minutes
3 Wash only full loads of clothes
4 Wash only full loads of dishes
5 Dry only full loads of clothes
6 Carpool 1 day a week when you would otherwise drive
7 Pledge to ride the bus 1 day a week when you would otherwise drive
8 Combine trips in vehicle (i.e., visit multiple destinations on one trip)
9 Take the stairs instead of the elevator a minimum of 5 times per week
10 Take a shower instead of a bath
11 Restrict length of shower to 5 minut es
12 Use a low wattage night light
13 Lower heating thermostat to 68 degrees and wear warmer clothes
14 Unplug any electronic devices when not in use
15 Take an environmental sustainability flyer and give it to a friend
Table
1
-
List of Actions
As shown in Figure 2, participants in the attachment group had a higher mean number of
committed actions (H1), fulfilled commitments (H2) and a higher mean donation (H3). Of these,
the difference in fulfilled commitments was statistically significant: F[1,11]=6.527, p=.0309*.
The attachment group also demonstrated significantly greater environmental concern and greater
care after reading about and interacting with the polar bear (F[1,19]=5.1273, p=0.0369* and
F[1,19]=3.8124, p=.0675, respectively).
Our findings demonstrate that participants in the attachment group were more concerned about
the environment, and this translated directly into significantly higher reported actions. Of
particular interest is the fact that while there was not a significant difference in the number of
commitments between the groups, reported follow through in terms of fulfilling those
commitments was significantly higher. However, it is possible that participants in the attachment
group lied about their follow through out of guilt or some other emotion. Also, we do not know
how long these differences will be sustained.
4. Future Work
Our next step is to deploy the polar bear over a longer period of time, in a setting where we can
objectively measure its impact on action. This addresses the two biggest concerns mentioned
above. We plan to deploy the polar bear on a mobile platform that can track use of different
transportation options. Additionally, we are currently creating a virtual polar bear plugin for
MySpace and Facebook. The large numbers of users on sites such as MySpace (used by over
61.2 million unique visitors) and Facebook (over 19.5 million unique users)[8] presents exciting
opportunities to encourage personally- and socially-desirable change in behaviors. Users will be
able to publicly make commitments and have the status of their polar bear on display. Based on
consistency theory, users would be more likely to behave consistently with their commitment
[4]. This may also lead to further research on the impact peer pressure may have on
environmentally sustainable actions.
5. Reason for Attending
The work we described is an initial contribution to the issue of persuading individuals to take
more sustainable actions. As such, it addresses one of the three main topics of the workshop,
how to motivate sustainable action. Additionally, though there was no room to discuss it here,
our research is expanding to address the appropriate deployment platforms and messaging for
varied socio-economic and ethnic groups. We are excited by the opportunity to attend this
workshop and engage researchers on these critical fronts.
6. Author Bios
Tawanna Dillahunt is a first year Ph.D. student at Carnegie Mellon University. Her research
interests include environmental sustainability and pervasive computing.
Geof Becker works at the Tepper School of Business at Carnegie Mellon in Marketing and
Public Relations and is interested in encouraging development of online communities that
commit to positive social action.
Jennifer Mankoff is an assistant professor at CMU in the HCII. Her research interests include
environmental sustainability, pervasive computing, and Assistive Technologies.
Robert Kraut is a Herbert A. Simon Professor of HCI at CMU. He conducts research in four
major areas: online communities, everyday use of the Internet, Technology and conversation,
collaboration in small work groups, and computers in organizations.
7. References
1. De Young, R. (2003). Expanding and Evaluating Motives for Environmentally Responsible
Behavior. Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 56, No.3, pp. 509-526.
2. Donath, J. (2004). Artificial Pets: Simple Behaviors Elicit Complex Attachments. In M.
Bekoff (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, Greenwood Press.
3. Fogg, B.J. (2002). Persuasive Computers Using Technology to Change What We Think and
Do. Morgan Kaufman Publishers, San Francisco, CA.
4. Harmon-Jones, E. (2002). Cognitive Dissonance Theory. Perspective on Persuasion. The
Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice. Sage Publications. p. 99-116.
5. International Personality Item Pool (as of February 2008): A Scientific Collaboratory for the
Development of Advanced Measures of Personality and Other Individual Differences, Big-
Five Factors Markers: http://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm.
6. Kaufman, T. (1999). A Study of the Motivations Behind Heritage Site Selection in the
United States, PhD Dissertation, Virginia Tech.
7. Lin, J., Lindtner, M.S., Strub, H. (2006). Fish'n'Steps: Encouraging Physical Activity with an
Interactive Computer Game. in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on
Ubiquitous Computing, pp. 261-278.
8. McCarthy, C (September 13, 2007). Nielsen/NetRatings’ August social media numbers: Not
much change, CNet News.com. http://www.news.com/8301-13577_3-9777942-36.html.
9. Myers, O.E. Jr., Saunders C.D. (2002). Animals as links toward developing caring
relationships with the natural world. In Kahn and Kelert (eds). Children and Nature, 153-
178. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
10. Mankoff, J, Matthews, D., Fussell, S.R., Johnson, M. (2007). Leveraging social networks to
motivate individuals to reduce their ecological footprints. In Proceedings of HICSS.
11. U.S. Department of Energy (2006). Annual Energy Review 2005. Energy Information
Administration, Washington, DC DOE/EIA-0384.
12. Vining, Joanne (2003). The Connection to Other Animals and Caring for Nature. Human
Ecology Review, Vol. 10, No. 2
... At the heart of the system lies a digital avatar called Infi, whose vitality depends on energy-saving behavior and interactions with its owner. The novelty of this system, in comparison to other similar approaches (e.g., [26,84]), lies in its augmentation of a virtual avatar approach with state-of-the-art conversational agents. Indeed, with current generative artificial intelligence, exemplified by systems like ChatGPT or Bard, computer conversation facilitates lifelike human-computer conversations [68,76,95]. ...
... Emotional attachment refers to the emotional bond between individuals and a specific target [55]. An early and promising study explored how an eco-feedback system could leverage emotional attachment with a virtual polar bear to promote environmentally responsible behavior [26]. Their results, based on a single case experiment with 20 participants, suggest that higher emotional attachment to the virtual pet significantly increased environmentally responsible actions. ...
... In the context of eco-feedback systems, these approaches encourage the design of digital artifacts that not only respond to user actions but also embody a relationship of care and responsibility. Such artifacts, like the polar bear system presented above [26], are not just tools for feedback but become entities with which users form an emotional bond, influencing their behavior towards more sustainable and caring interactions with their environment. As such, digital avatars, particularly in the form of virtual pets, have been utilized to evoke emotional connections and responsibilities [12,21,62,67,89]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Lights out! With the escalating climate crisis, eco-feedback has gained prominence over the last decade. However, traditional approaches could be underperforming as they often use data-driven strategies and assume that people only need additional information about their consumption to change behavior. A proposed path to overcome this issue is to design eco-feedback to foster emotional connections with users. However, not much is known about the effectiveness of such designs. In this paper, we propose a novel care-based eco-feedback system. Central to the system is a Tamagotchi-inspired digital character named infi who gets its life force from the user's energy savings. Additionally, we harness the latest advancements in generative artificial intelligence to enhance emotional attachment through conversational interactions that users can have with infi. The results of a randomized controlled experiment (N=420) convey the fact that this design increases emotional attachment, which in turn increases energy-saving behavior.
... These simulated interactions, that placed an onus of care on children, have been shown to increase engagement and empathy (Tsai and Kaufman, 2009). Analogous virtual care models (ie., of polar bears) are similarly promoted, and have been shown to stimulate environmentally responsible behaviour (Dillahunt et al., 2008). However, authors have argued that the disposable nature of such artificial substitutes can confuse children (Bloch and Lemish, 1999). ...
Article
Full-text available
Soil health underpins ecosystem services like food security and therefore underpins human health. Poor soil health is a global problem which is hindering attempts to deliver the UN’s Sustainable Development Goals. We focus on goals 3 (human health), 13 (climate change) which are intimately linked to goal 15 (soil health). Soil health is arguably most fragile in regions such as sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) where aged soils are characterised by poor nutrient and water holding capacity, and are largely deficient in micronutrients such as Zinc. Poor soil health coupled with the largely cereal-based diets can mean that micronutrient malnutrition is high in the region. In sub-Saharan Africa, where much of the population is too poor to purchase mineral supplements, poor soil health (SDG15) can therefore negatively impact on human health (SDG3). We surveyed 3661 school children aged 13–15 in three African countries, Ghana, South Africa and Zimbabwe, for their ‘Attitudes, Behaviours and Competencies’ of soil, which we termed ‘ABC’. The ‘ABC’ survey results showed significant soil illiteracy. The survey showed that although students were generally equipped with a good attitude to (overall 52% positive) and behaviour towards soil (overall 60% engagement), they had little competency as to how to improve soil health (overall 23% knowledge). For example, less than 35% of respondents across all countries know that soil is living. Less than 13% of students are aware of the important role of soil in climate change mitigation. We believe that these two knowledge gaps must be addressed for Generation Z to understand the important linkages between climate change, soil and human health. We propose a hands-on ‘ethics of care’ approach to engage society with soil, piggybacking on existing climate change educational resources by building terrariums with living soil can empower children to learn about soil, plant, human and planetary health. The future of food security depends on Generation Z having soil literacy. Our survey clearly shows that students who think farming is a good way to make money have significantly higher levels of overall soil literacy. We propose that the future of human health depends on soil literacy.
... The design attempts to encourage change through an emotional attachment to a virtual pet dog. The benefits of using emotional attachment to pets to motivate behavior change have been documented in previous research (e.g., [16,40]). In the design, the pet dog is displayed below the text-area, and it is described as "our digital friend. ...
... The most striking historical example of an interactive device that included with a virtual pet is the Tamagotchi, which allowed users to grow different virtual pets by delivering various gamified forms of care [44,58]. Outside of gaming and toys, these virtual pets or virtual companions have also been widely explored across HCI as useful for learning [33], companionship [45,69], medical interventions [52], and more [21]. ...
... The benefits of using emotional attachment to pets to motivate behaviour change have been documented in previous research (e.g. Dillahunt et al. 2008;Lin et al. 2006). The pet dog is displayed below the text area, and it is described as 'our digital friend'. ...
Article
Full-text available
While HCI literature offers general frameworks for understanding user-centred quality, specific application areas may call for more detailed contextualisation of it. This paper focuses on socio-technical context of online news commenting by investigating speculative UI interventions intended to influence users’ emotions and social behaviour. To understand the aspects of quality that matter to users in such UI interventions, we conducted an international online survey (N = 439) and qualitatively analysed respondents’ first impressions of eight different design proposals. The findings describe contextually relevant socio-technical viewpoints and offer actionable considerations for design. For example, the findings imply that designers should be mindful of possible unintentional misuse that may result from the UI reinforcing specific emotional states or affording stigmatisation of individual users. The study advances understanding of which aspects of quality should be considered when designing and deploying UI interventions for digital media services and evaluating them with potential end-users.
Thesis
Full-text available
Food systems activities produce around 30% of global anthropogenic greenhouse gas (GHG) emissions and are responsible for numerous environmental issues which could ultimately harm our ability to grow food reliably. One way to reduce the food system’s impacts is to transition to a more sustainable diet composed of low impact foods. The objective of this study is to identify consumers demonstrating an intention to consume sustainably produced foods, and to identify their characteristics to inform the design of targeted gamified interventions that would promote sustainable food purchasing. A survey incorporating variables from the theory of planned behaviour (e.g. attitude), socio-demographic information (e.g. age), gamification profiling variables (e.g. player typology), as well as preferred mobile applications, was developed in this study and distributed via a market survey company. Statistical analysis in the form of hierarchical clustering was used to segment and identify target markets, while contingency analysis assessed the most effective means of promoting sustainable diets. A total of four hundred and ninety surveys were distributed and three hundred and seventy-six of them were validated because they agreed to participate, were not detected as potential AI powered responses, and their responses from the theory of planned behaviour were completed. Linear regression was used to assess the significance of all variables on the intent to consume a sustainable diet. Cluster analysis identified 3 potential target segments, and contingency analysis was used to detect their unique features. Two consumer segments were identified as having high potential as a target market. Individuals in this market intended to consume a sustainable diet but lacked follow through. Strong evidence towards the effectiveness of gamification of interventions was not observed due to low and medium frequency in gaming behaviours for the two target segments. Interventions distributed through mobile applications would be most effective if they were delivered through social media and included game design elements associated with Philanthropist and Free Spirit user types. The survey was confined to Ontario, therefore it may not be generalizable to other regions. Nevertheless, this study is unique in its assessment of the profiles of consumers with high intention to purchase sustainably sourced foods through a combination of the theory of planned behaviour, socio-demographic factors, gamification player types and game behaviours, as well as preferred mobile application usage.
Article
EcoSanté is a mobile lifestyle intervention that encourages individual behavior change while also helping participants understand the deep connections between daily lifestyle choices and our collective impact on the planet. Informed by research on “small” intervention approaches, we sent participants daily behavioral challenges that demonstrated connections between personal health and environmental impact at large. Through a 20-day mobile intervention study, 139 participants uploaded 1,920 submissions documenting their attempts to engage in these challenges. We found that participants’ self-reported healthy eating behavior and general self-efficacy improved significantly immediately after the intervention. Moreover, 30 days after the intervention, participants’ self-reported eating, exercise, and general self-efficacy all significantly improved compared to the beginning of the study. Participants had a more negative reaction when being asked to come up with their own challenges. Based on quantitative and qualitative findings, we provide implications for future researcher on mobile behavior intervention research.
Chapter
We are surrounded by self-tracking technologies that provide us with vast amounts of personal data. Despite an abundance of information, the lack of personal impact and poor design often leads to ineffective eco-feedback technology. This paper explores how the communication of personal impact and different levels of specificity in information presentation can influence pro-environmental behavioral change. For the duration of one week, 7 participants inputted self-measured food waste data into a website that showed four data visualizations with different levels of specificity and personal impact. We found that communicating food waste data in terms of personal impact (i.e. money wasted, daily progress) better motivated participants as opposed to visualizations emphasizing altruistic values or uninterpreted data (i.e. interactive plant, aggregated waste). We identify what effects eco-feedback technologies with different levels of specificity have in user attention and satisfaction. The reported findings provide insight into elements to consider when designing eco-feedback technology.KeywordsEco-Feedback TechnologyData VisualizationFood WasteSustainabilitySelf-Tracking Technology
Chapter
The anthropogenic pressures humans place on the Earth create social issues situated around science that are so complex talking about them can divide countries, communities, and even families. Known as socioscientific issues (SSI), these controversial issues are useful for garnering interest in and promoting science literacy by facilitating dialogue and encouraging evidence-based reasoning as a platform for learning about science. Prominent in formal classrooms, the SSI framework has been successful in engaging students in science learning while increasing their science understanding through the development of skills like questioning, argumentation, empathy, and moral reasoning. Drawing from the fields of developmental psychology, sociology, and philosophy, the SSI framework examines the processes in which an instructional resource facilitates learning about science content embedded in socially relevant situations, while recognizing the epistemological growth of the learner and the potential development of character as they engage with the content. Despite its potential in informal science education, research on the SSI framework in out-of-school contexts is limited. This lack of study is problematic given that most of our lives are spent outside of a formal classroom. While no less important, engaging in dialogue about SSI cannot exist solely in the formal classroom. In this chapter, we adapt the SSI framework for exhibit design and use the framework as lens for examining current aquarium exhibits that focus on the particular SSIs of climate change and water sustainability. Based on our work, we believe the SSI framework could be beneficial across multiple types of ISIs through its use as a tool for the development of exhibits and programming that stimulates higher order thinking in visitors as they engage in discourse about science content, as well as a guide for exploring the effectiveness of existing and future exhibits and programming.
Article
Full-text available
This article contends that while striving to promote environmentally responsible behavior, we have focused attention too narrowly on just two classes of motives. There is a need to expand the range of motives available to practitioners and to provide a framework within which motives can be evaluated for both their immediate and long-term effectiveness. The article then examines a strategy for promoting environmentally responsible behavior that has significant potential. This strategy is based on a particular form of motivation called intrinsic satisfaction. Nine studies are reviewed that have outlined the structure of intrinsic satisfaction. A key theme discussed is the human inclination for competence. This fundamental human concern is shown to have both a general form and a resource-specific version.
Article
Full-text available
In an era in which natural resources are under unprece-dented pressure, it is vital to ask how the human relationships with the natural world might be improved. One avenue of inquiry is to explore whether there is a relationship between caring for a non-human animal, for a species, and for an ecosystem and whether this relationship may be a key to encouraging resource conservation and environmental pro-tection. In most occidental industrialized societies, interest in domestic and wild animals is intense. Despite a wealth of research on human-wildlife interactions and human-pet interactions there is a dearth of work on the connection between these relationships and interest in environmental protection. In this article, I will address three topics that I believe are crucial to the understanding of the relationship between humans and other animals: the split between humans and the natural world, the development of caring, empathy, and emotional relationships to non-human animals, and finally the magic, or deep and abiding attraction to other species. These topics are especially relevant to current efforts by zoos and aquariums as they try to document the impact they have on the lives of their visitors.
Article
Full-text available
This article contends that while striving to promote environmentallyresponsible behavior, we have focused attention too narrowly on just two classes of motives. There is a need to expand the range of motives available to practitioners and to provide a framework within which motives can be evaluated for both their immediate and long-term effectiveness. The article then examines a strategy for promoting environmentally responsible behavior that has significant potential. This strategy is based on a particular form of motivation called intrinsic satisfaction. Nine studies are reviewed that have outlined the structure of intrinsic satisfaction. A key theme discussed is the human inclination for competence. This fundamental human concern is shown to have both a general form and a resource-specific version.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A sedentary lifestyle is a contributing factor to chronic diseases, and it is often correlated with obesity. To promote an increase in physical activity, we created a social computer game, Fish'n'Steps, which links a player's daily foot step count to the growth and activity of an animated virtual character, a fish in a fish tank. As further encouragement, some of the players' fish tanks in- cluded other players' fish, thereby creating an environment of both cooperation and competition. In a fourteen-week study with nineteen participants, the game served as a catalyst for promoting exercise and for improving game players' at- titudes towards physical activity. Furthermore, although most player's enthusi- asm in the game decreased after the game's first two weeks, analyzing the re- sults using Prochaska's Transtheoretical Model of Behavioral Change suggests that individuals had, by that time, established new routines that led to healthier patterns of physical activity in their daily lives. Lessons learned from this study underscore the value of such games to encourage rather than provide negative reinforcement, especially when individuals are not meeting their own expecta- tions, to foster long-term behavioral change.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
What role can social networking websites play in supporting large-scale group action and change? We are proposing to explore their use in supporting individual reduction in personal energy consumption. Here we summarize some existing uses of social networking on the web and propose an approach that integrates feedback about ecological footprint data into existing social networking sites and Internet portal sites. Integrating such feedback into popular, commonly used sites allows frequent feedback about performance, while enabling the exploration motivational schemes that leverage group membership. We propose to compare different motivational schemes in three ways: Reduction in CO2 emission; lifestyle changes; and ongoing use by users who join the site (retention).
Article
Can computers change what you think and do? Can they motivate you to stop smoking, persuade you to buy insurance, or convince you to join the Army? "Yes, they can," says Dr. B.J. Fogg, director of the Persuasive Technology Lab at Stanford University. Fogg has coined the phrase "Captology"(an acronym for computers as persuasive technologies) to capture the domain of research, design, and applications of persuasive computers.In this thought-provoking book, based on nine years of research in captology, Dr. Fogg reveals how Web sites, software applications, and mobile devices can be used to change peoples attitudes and behavior. Technology designers, marketers, researchers, consumers-anyone who wants to leverage or simply understand the persuasive power of interactive technology-will appreciate the compelling insights and illuminating examples found inside. Persuasive technology can be controversial-and it should be. Who will wield this power of digital influence? And to what end? Now is the time to survey the issues and explore the principles of persuasive technology, and B.J. Fogg has written this book to be your guide.
Article
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1999. Includes bibliographical references. Oct. 2000, UMI, da, IT. Photocopy.
Article
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1999. Includes bibliographical references. Photocopy.
Cognitive Dissonance Theory. Perspective on Persuasion. The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice
  • E Harmon-Jones
Harmon-Jones, E. (2002). Cognitive Dissonance Theory. Perspective on Persuasion. The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice. Sage Publications. p. 99-116.