ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The function of anti-predator signalling is a complex, and often-overlooked, area of animal communication. The goal of this study was to examine the behavioural function of an anti-predator acoustic signal in the ocean. We observed the acoustic and defensive behaviours of California spiny lobsters (Palinuridae: Panulirus interruptus) to a model predator, model conspecific and blank pole, both in the tank and in the field. We found that P. interruptus make a 'rasp' sound once physically contacted by an aggressor, rather than during the approach. The model predator and conspecific elicited no discernable changes in defensive behaviour, but the responses by the lobsters to aggressors in the tank versus field were distinct. Our results indicate that the spiny lobster's rasp is used as a startle or aposematic signal, which may be coupled with visual aposematism of their spines. Alternatively, the rasp may function as a vibratory escape mechanism or as an acoustic analogue to eye-spots. This study offers insights into the role of acoustic signalling in the marine environment and demonstrates a central role for sound production in spiny lobster ecology.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Disentangling defense: the function of spiny
lobster sounds
E.R. Staaterman,T. Claverie & S.N. Patek1)
(Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA)
(Accepted: 20 August 2009)
Summary
The function of anti-predator signalling is a complex, and often-overlooked, area of animal
communication. The goal of this study was to examine the behavioural function of an anti-
predator acoustic signal in the ocean. We observed the acoustic and defensive behaviours
of California spiny lobsters (Palinuridae: Panulirus interruptus) to a model predator, model
conspecific and blank pole, both in the tank and in the field. We found that P. interruptus make
a ‘rasp’ sound once physically contacted by an aggressor, rather than during the approach.
The model predator and conspecific elicited no discernable changes in defensive behaviour,
but the responses by the lobsters to aggressors in the tank versus field were distinct. Our
results indicate that the spiny lobster’s rasp is used as a startle or aposematic signal, which
may be coupled with visual aposematism of their spines. Alternatively, the rasp may function
as a vibratory escape mechanism or as an acoustic analogue to eye-spots. This study offers
insights into the role of acoustic signalling in the marine environment and demonstrates a
central role for sound production in spiny lobster ecology.
Keywords: anti-predator signals, aposematism, warning, startle, Palinuridae.
Introduction
As a general rule it is better to mate tomorrow than be a meal today
(Bailey, 1991).
Approximately 125 million years ago, spiny lobsters evolved a sound-
producing apparatus at the base of their spiny antennae (Palero et al., 2009).
1) Corresponding author’s current address: Department of Biology, 221 Morrill South,
University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA, e-mail: patek@bio.umass.edu
©Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2010 Behaviour 147, 235-258
DOI:10.1163/000579509X12523919243428 Also available online - www.brill.nl/beh
236 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Lacking claws, palinurid lobsters rely on their long and powerful antennae
to defend themselves against intruders (Kanciruk, 1980; Spanier & Zimmer-
Faust, 1988; Kelly et al., 1999; Herrnkind et al., 2001; Barshaw et al., 2003;
Briones-Fourzán et al., 2006). With the origin of the sound-producing ap-
paratus in one group of spiny lobsters (the ‘Stridentes’) (George & Main,
1967), the antennae became both mechanical and acoustic weaponry (Patek
& Oakley, 2003). Indeed, since its origin, the sound-producing apparatus
has diversified into a fantastic array of sizes, shapes and colors (George &
Main, 1967; Patek & Oakley, 2003). Given that the acoustic structures are
indistinguishable between males and females (Patek, 2002; Patek & Oakley,
2003; Patek & Baio, 2007; Patek et al., 2009), and that the spiny lobsters
produce the sounds when interacting with potential predators, the function
of the sound is assumed to deter predators (Lindberg, 1955; Moulton, 1957;
Moulton, 1958; Smale, 1974; Meyer-Rochow & Penrose, 1976; Mulligan &
Fischer, 1977; Bouwma & Herrnkind, 2009). Remarkably, over the millennia
of documentation of these sounds in the literature (Athenaeus, 3rd century;
Parker, 1878, 1883), not until recently has the anti-predator function of these
signals been experimentally tested (Bouwma & Herrnkind, 2009). Even with
this foundational study of function, how the spiny lobsters’ sounds deter
predators (Edmunds, 1974; Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 1998; Caro, 2005) re-
mains unknown in this system.
Most spiny lobster taxa exhibit forms of gregarious behaviour that offer
defense against predators (Butler IV et al., 1999; Kelly et al., 1999; Her-
rnkind et al., 2001; Barshaw et al., 2003; Childress, 2007; Briones-Fourzán
& Lozano-Álvarez, 2008). For example, many species share dens (Childress
& Hernkind, 1997; Childress & Herrnkind, 2001), aggregate when presented
with predators (Kelly et al., 1999; Herrnkind et al., 2001), migrate in for-
mation (Bill & Herrnkind, 1976) and sense conspecific olfactory alarm sig-
nals (Shabani et al., 2008). The spiny lobster’s acoustic signal, the ‘rasp’, is
used in both solitary and gregarious settings when interacting with potential
predators (S.N.P., pers. observ.). Three behavioural studies have suggested
the possibility of intraspecific communication with sound as acoustic warn-
ing signals to conspecifics (Lindberg, 1955; Berrill, 1976; Meyer-Rochow et
al., 1982), but strong experimental evidence is lacking and it is not presently
known whether spiny lobsters can hear beyond the near-field region (approx.
1 wavelength from the source =4 m: Patek et al., 2009) (reviewed in Budel-
mann, 1992; Popper et al., 2001).
Spiny lobster sounds 237
The sound-producing mechanism itself may relate to its anti-predator
function. Spiny lobsters produce sound by rubbing a soft-tissue extension
(the ‘plectrum’) at the base of each antenna over an oblong, macroscopically
smooth ‘file’ under each eye; rasp sounds are generated using stick-slip fric-
tion between the two surfaces (Patek, 2001, 2002; Patek & Baio, 2007). Patek
(2001) proposed that the use of non-rigid surfaces to produce these stick-slip
sounds allowed the animals to generate sound throughout their moult cy-
cles when the exoskeleton is softened, thereby providing an acoustic defence
when their other physical defences are compromised. Latha et al. (2005)
confirmed that recently moulted lobsters can effectively generate loud rasps.
The first published performance tests of the spiny lobster’s anti-predator
rasp examined how silencing Caribbean spiny lobsters (Panulirus argus) af-
fected their nocturnal interactions with predatory octopus (Octopus briareus)
in experimental tanks (Bouwma & Herrnkind, 2009). The authors found that
stridulating lobsters were better able to escape octopus attacks and resist at-
tacks for a longer duration than silenced lobsters. While the first approach
of the octopus did not yield an acoustic response until physical contact with
the lobster, in subsequent approaches, the spiny lobsters initiated tail flip es-
cape responses (not necessarily with sound) before contact. Once caught,
the lobsters stridulated for an extended time (average 90 s). Even though
the stridulating lobsters fared better than the silenced lobsters, the octopuses
showed no obvious response to the rasp signal, leading the authors to sug-
gest that perhaps the rasp does not function to startle the octopus and instead
the vibrations make it more difficult to grasp the rasping lobster. While this
study offers a number of keen insights into the acoustic function of the rasp,
it is important to note that there was no control for the surgery that removed
the sound-producing apparatus in silenced lobsters. Given that the plectrum
is an integral part of the antennal joint (Patek, 2002), it is possible that the
differences in performance could be due to the experimental removal of this
joint articulation, rather than the absence of sound production. In an unpub-
lished dissertation (Bouwma, 2006), similar performance results were found
in daytime face-offs between triggerfish and tethered silenced and stridulat-
ing spiny lobsters.
Ideally, in order to determine the behavioural function of the rasp, one
would observe naturally occurring predator–prey interactions in the field
with freely-moving individuals (e.g., Cocroft, 1999). However, spiny lob-
sters pose particular challenges to this experimental approach. First, spiny
lobsters are nocturnal foragers, often going on excursions far from their
238 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
daytime rocky and coral crevices, thus making visualization of behaviours
and tracking difficult. Nonetheless, illumination at night with red lights is
minimally disruptive to spiny lobsters, although the lights can attract small
fish (Weiss et al., 2006). Second, and more critically, spiny lobsters gener-
ate these rasp sounds in water. Sound travels approximately five times more
quickly in water than in air, making localization of sounds nearly impossible
without an array of hydrophones or only while in very close range of the
sound source. Furthermore, the ambient background noise in their habitats
can be high enough that rasps may not be reliably recorded from distances
beyond approx. 1 m from the source (Patek et al., 2009). Tethering lobsters is
one solution, but it affects the ability of the lobsters to escape, and, therefore,
the dynamics of the predator–prey interactions and the type of predators that
approach (Zimmer-Faust et al., 1994) as well as possibly the acoustic behav-
iour of the lobsters (Bouwma & Herrnkind, 2009).
Given these limitations, one approach is to conduct the experiments in
the field with freely-moving spiny lobsters, record the rasps in close range
and use model predators rather than real predators. Numerous studies have
documented the importance of olfactory cues during predatory interactions
(e.g., Sih et al., 1998; Dicke & Grostal, 2001; Lima, 2002). Clawed lobsters
(Nephropidae) (Wahle, 1992) and spiny lobsters (Berger & Butler IV, 2001)
are able to detect predators using chemical cues. In addition to chemore-
ception, crustaceans use their antennae as mechanosensory structures and
can detect the low-frequency signatures of locomoting animals (Tautz &
Sandeman, 1980; Tautz, 1990; Derby & Steullet, 2001). Thus, models would
lack both the olfactory cues of predators and the finer-tuned vibratory cues
of swimming predators. However, the models would generate the low-
frequency waves produced by any approaching object in a fluid environment,
thus giving them advanced warning of an approaching model predator. Vi-
sual cues, however, can be sufficient to alert arthropods to predators; in a
study of spider responses to a range of predator signals, spiders were able to
appropriately respond solely on the basis of visual cues (Lohrey et al., 2009).
In addition, the study by Bouwma and Herrnkind (2009) on octopus preda-
tors suggest that the visual and physical contact experience may supersede
ambient olfactory cues in the Caribbean spiny lobster.
Thus, in this study, we present the first published field and laboratory ex-
periments to examine the behavioural function of rasps produced by freely-
moving California spiny lobsters (Panulirus interruptus) interacting with a
model predator, a model conspecific and a blank pole. Panulirus interruptus
Spiny lobster sounds 239
Figure 1. The California spiny lobster (Panulirus interruptus) hides in rocky crevices dur-
ing the day (A) and often the only visible parts of the body are the extended antennae and
eye-spots (B). The sound-producing apparatus is located at the base of each antenna, beneath
the eyes. The characteristic eye-spots are located adjacent to the sound-producing apparatus,
immediately below the eyes. This figure is published in colour in the online version of this
journal, which can be accessed via http://www.brill.nl/beh
are gregarious (although coordinated anti-predator behaviours have yet been
described in this species), nocturnal spiny lobsters with brightly coloured
‘eye-spots’ adjacent to their sound-producing apparatus (Figure 1). Like
240 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
other sound-producing spiny lobsters, this species also generates stick-slip
‘rasp’ sounds when interacting with potential predators (Patek & Baio, 2007;
Patek et al., 2009). We addressed three central questions. First, based on the
timing and context of sound production, which type of anti-predator signal is
being used? Second, do spiny lobsters respond differently to a model preda-
tor than to a control model (conspecific) or a blank pole? Lastly, given that
most previous studies of anti-predator signaling have taken place in the lab-
oratory or confined conditions, how does defensive behaviour vary between
field and tank environments?
Materials and methods
Lindberg (1955) found that sheepshead (Pimelometopon pulchra) preyed
most heavily on P. interruptus; thus, one of our models approximated the
size and morphology of a sheepshead fish. A second model represented a
spiny lobster as a generally non-predatory aggressor. We cast a frozen fish
(33 cm body length) and a frozen spiny lobster (8 cm carapace length) with
a commercial mold material, and then made the models with a commercial
silicone material and spray paint (alginate mold and Silicone RTV SR-1610,
Douglas and Sturgess, Richmond, CA, USA). We also used a blank pole
(without an attached model) as a control.
During all of the experiments, we simultaneously recorded the lobsters’
behavioural and acoustic responses using two separate audio–video systems,
one attached to the aggressor pole and one attached to the observer pole (Fig-
ures 2 and 3). By using two systems, we could verify the timing of acoustic
response from two vantage points during the experiments. The first system,
which we call the ‘aggressor pole’, was used to approach the lobsters with the
model aggressor. This pole was equipped with a hydrophone (20–25 000 Hz;
HTI-96-Min hydrophone, High Tech, Gulfport, MS, USA), a low-light cam-
era (Submersible Under-Water CCD 480TVL Bullet Color Camera, Sony,
New York, NY, USA) and a small, dimmed, white dive light. Depending
on the particular experiment, the aggressor pole also had the fish or lob-
ster model attached to it. The second system, termed the ‘observer pole’,
recorded the sound of the focal lobster before and during the approach of
the aggressor pole. The observer pole was equipped with the same type of
hydrophone as the aggressor pole. The video and audio data were recorded
Spiny lobster sounds 241
Figure 2. Field experiments were conducted with an aggressor pole and observer pole
focused on a freely-moving live spiny lobster. A hydrophone, camera and small flashlight
were attached to the aggressor pole. Only a hydrophone was attached to the observer pole.
Both poles had cables leading to recording devices on the surface. For each experiment, one
person held the observer pole above the lobster, while the other person approached the lobster
with the aggressor pole equipped with either a model fish, model lobster, or blank pole.
Figure 3. Tank experiments utilized an aggressor and observer pole setup similar to the
field experiments. A camera, hydrophone and small dimmed dive-light were attached to the
aggressor pole. The observer hydrophone was positioned above the lobster, and the observer
camera was positioned at a right-angle to the aggressor pole to capture the lobster’s behaviour
during the approach. We approached lobsters with the aggressor pole, equipped with either
the model fish, model lobster, or a blank pole.
242 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
for both poles using the same equipment (Sony GV-A500 Hi8 Video Walk-
man, Sony; digital audio recorder, 48 kHz sample rate, maximum 20 kHz
frequency response (0.5 dB), PMD670, Marantz, Mahwah, NJ, USA).
Experiment 1: response to nocturnal approaches in the field
The goal of this experiment was to measure the defensive acoustic and be-
havioural responses of spiny lobsters during nocturnal foraging. Spiny lob-
sters hide deep in rocky crevices during the day and emerge to forage at
night. As a result, they were inaccessible to our equipment during the day
and we only conducted experiments at night. During these nocturnal exper-
iments, one person held the observer pole above the lobster while a second
person approached the lobster with the aggressor pole (both scientists were
equipped with snorkelling equipment). The observer pole was used to record
the acoustic response during the approach of the aggressor pole, and after the
pole made contact with the lobster (Figure 2).
Data were collected in three distinct regions of the subtidal zone; each
region was sampled twice, over a two-month interval in the spring of 2008
(Big Fisherman’s Cove, University of Southern California, Wrigley Institute
for Environmental Studies, Santa Catalina Island, CA, USA). The data from
one region were eliminated from the first sampling session due to technical
problems. Water temperature ranged from 16–20C. We approached differ-
ent individuals for each trial; however, there are thousands of lobsters living
in this marine sanctuary, so there is a small chance that the same lobsters
were measured twice across the two-month interval.
Experiment 2: response to nocturnal and diurnal approaches in tanks
In the spring of 2008, spiny lobsters were collected at Santa Catalina Island
in baited lobster traps or by hand (CA Department of Fish and Game permit
No. SC-5751). The lobsters were maintained in a large rectangular tank with
a continuous supply of seawater (14–16C) and were fed frozen squid every
two days. Twenty-eight females (carapace length range: 68–104 mm) and 15
males (carapace length range 64–111 mm) were used for this study. Lobsters
were transferred individually to cylindrical experimental tanks (152 cm dia-
meter, 81 cm deep); each tank had a burrow made of rocks. Lobsters were
given 15–30 min to acclimate before the trial began. After each trial, we
determined the animal’s sex and measured its carapace length to the nearest
Spiny lobster sounds 243
0.01 mm (Absolute Coolant Proof Digital Calipers IP67, Mitutoyo, Hacienda
Heights, CA, USA).
We used the same aggressor pole setup as in the field experiments. The
‘observer pole’ from the field experiments was separated into an ‘observer
hydrophone’ and an ‘observer camera’ to capture lobster behaviour from a
distance and record the acoustic response before and during the entire trial
(Figure 3). We performed tank trials both at night (2200–0400 h) and during
the day (0900–1800 h). All tank experiments were performed during the first
field session.
Experimental design
As we approached lobsters in both the tank and the field, we presented the
fish, lobster, or blank pole at random. In the tank trials, we randomized the
aggression level of the aggressor pole, and made physical contact with the
lobsters in about half of the trials. In the field, randomization of physical con-
tact was attempted but not always possible due to logistical constraints. For
the field experiments, we approached 98 lobsters. For the tank experiments,
14 lobsters were first sampled during the day, then at night, and the other 29
lobsters were sampled first at night, then during the day.
Analysis of lobster behaviour and sound production
In order to independently analyze the acoustic and behavioural responses of
the lobsters, we digitally separated the video and audio recordings from each
trial (iMovie 4.0.1, Apple, Cupertino, CA, USA; Raven 1.3, Cornell Lab of
Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA). Audio recordings were scanned visually and
acoustically for the stereotypical ‘rasp’ spectrogram (settings: Hanning win-
dow, 512 sample window size; 3 dB filter bandwidth at 135 Hz resolution)
(Figure 4) and waveform. Each video trial was watched several times and
the movements of the lobsters were described and quantified (Table 1). Any
trials in which we could not see at least the anterior end of the lobster were
omitted from analysis.
χ2tests were used to determine whether aggressor type, exposure, or
direction of approach relative to antennal position affected lobster behaviour
or rasping during the approach (cf. Table 1 for details). We noted whether the
lobsters made sound during the approach (before physical contact) or during
the attack (once physical contact was made), because this information can be
244 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Figure 4. A typical rasp spectrogram from a field recording. One rasp is indicated between
the dotted vertical lines. The rasp is composed of a series of broadband pulses.
Table 1. The definitions and states of the variables used in the behavioural
analyses.
Variable Definition
Exposure Not exposed =lobster hidden in burrow
Partially exposed =lobster near shelter (<approx. 1 m)
Exposed =lobster distant from any shelter (>approx. 1 m)
Direction of ap-
proach relative
to antennal
position
Match =anterior approach with forward antennae; lateral approach
with antennae pointing out; posterior approach with posteriorly-
pointing antennae
Non-match =anything that is not a match (dorsal approach is always a
non-match)
Behavioural re-
sponse of
lobster
Behaviour 1 =no movement of antennae or other body parts
Behaviour 2 =movement of antennae/antennules towards camera but
minimal leg movement
Behaviour 3 =movement of antennae/antennules and use of legs to
move away from camera
Behaviour 4 =tail flip
Physical contact Contact =model or pole touches the lobster
No contact =nothing touches the lobster
Rasp Present =lobster makes at least one identifiable rasp
Absent =no rasps detected
Spiny lobster sounds 245
used to determine which signal type the lobsters are using (see Discussion).
We also compared the lobsters’ response between the tank and field using
the first field session only. To compare the lobsters’ rasping and behaviour
during the day and at night in the tanks, we performed paired McNemar
tests.
Results
Experiment 1: response to nocturnal approaches in the field
The aggressor type, exposure and direction of approach relative to anten-
nal position did not affect lobster behaviour or rasping during the approach
(Table 2). We did, however, find a difference in lobster behaviour once the
aggressor made physical contact with the lobster (χ2=47.1;df =3;
p < 0.001). After contact was made, lobsters exhibited a tail-flip escape
response 95% of the time (Table 2), whereas when no contact was made,
Table 2. Statistical results of the aggressor experiments across field and tank
trials.
Trial Variable Behaviour Rasp
df χ2df χ2
Field Aggressor type 6 11.79 2 0.35
Exposure 6 7.41 2 0.20
Direction of approach relative to antennal position 3 0.33 1 0.85
Contact 3 47.05∗∗ 1 10.81∗∗
Daytime Aggressor type 6 9.39 2 3.56
tank Exposure 6 2.30 2 1.08
Direction of approach relative to antennal position 3 6.99 1 0.23
Contact 3 4.17 1 1.97
Nighttime Aggressor type 6 2.89 2 0.05
tank Exposure 6 16.352 5.06
Direction of approach relative to antennal position 3 6.61 1 0
Contact 3 5.62 1 1.05
Chi-square analyses were used to test the correlation between the behavioural response and
aggressor variables, as well as the presence or absence of a rasp in response to the aggressor
variables (Table 1). Results are shown as degrees of freedom andchi-square value. p < 0.05,
∗∗p < 0.01.
246 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Figure 5. Defensive behaviour of lobsters depending the presence or absence of physical
contact with an aggressor. The total number of trials is indicated for each bar; data from the
first field session are represented by the hatched portion of the bar and data from the second
field session are represented by the solid portion of the bar. While antennal and leg movement
(dark grey) and tailflips (black) were observed in both treatments, antennal movement only
(light gray) and no movement (white) were only observed in trials with no contact. In both
field sessions, more tail-flips occurred after physical contact with an aggressor.
lobsters tail-flipped only 17% of the time (Figure 5). We also observed a
difference in lobster rasping depending on contact (χ2=10.8;df =2;
p=0.001);lobsters rasped in 56% of the trials when touched, but only
rasped in 18% of the trials when not touched (Figure 6).
Given that the two field sessions were conducted over an interval of two
months, we independently analyzed the data from each session. In the first
session alone, both lobster behaviour (χ2=4.414;df =1; p=0.035)and
lobster rasping (χ2=3.556;df =1; p=0.059)were affected by physical
contact, although the effect on rasping was not significant (Figures 5 and 6).
The second session, however, yielded fewer trials in which we made physi-
cal contact with the lobster, which resulted in insufficient data for analysis.
Regardless of the aggressor type, exposure, and direction of approach rela-
tive to antennal position, we saw a difference in defensive behaviour prior
to physical contact between the two field sessions (χ2=9.18;df =3;
p=0.027), but there was no difference in the acoustic response prior to
physical contact (χ2=0.21;df =1; p=0.645). Lobsters were more
physically active in the second field session (when water temperatures were
warmer).
Spiny lobster sounds 247
Figure 6. Number of trials with a rasp (black) and without a rasp (white), depending on
physical contact by the aggressor (predator, conspecific and blank pole combined). Each bar
indicates the total number of trials; within each bar the non-hatched region includes trials
from the second field session and the hatched region indicates data from the first field session.
There was a significantly higher number of rasps after contact in both experimental datasets.
Experiment 2: response to nocturnal and diurnal approaches in tanks
The aggressor type, exposure (during the day), and direction of approach
relative to antennal position had no significant effect on lobster rasping or
behaviour during the approach (Table 2). The only significant factor influenc-
ing lobster behaviour was the exposure level at night (χ2=16.35;df =6;
p=0.01). Exposed lobsters at night were more physically and acoustically
active than sheltered lobsters at night or during the day. There was no effect
of physical contact on lobster behaviour or rasping in tanks, during the day
or at night (Table 2).
Lobsters in tanks responded more actively to intrusion during the day
compared to the night (McNemar’s test: χ2=20.40, df =6, p < 0.005),
but their rasping behaviour did not change depending on time of day (Mc-
Nemar’s test: χ2=1.5, df =1, p=0.221). During the day, in 51% of
the trials, lobsters pointed their antennae toward the aggressor and retreated
more deeply into their shelters. At night, however, lobsters were less active;
in 59% of the trials, lobsters did not move their legs or antenna.
Comparison of nocturnal behaviour in the tank versus field
Lobsters exhibited a more physically active behavioural response in the field
compared to the tank, both during the approach (χ2=31.3;df =3; p <
248 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
0.001)and after physical contact (χ2=22.1;df =3; p < 0.001). We
did not, however, find differences between the acoustic response in the tank
versus the field, during the approach (χ2=0.14;df =1; p=0.70)or after
contact (χ2=2.2;df =1; p=0.14).
Discussion
Interpreting the function of anti-predator signals is rarely straightforward,
given that many are multi-functional, multi-modal and not mutually exclu-
sive. Nonetheless, we can draw conclusions about the function of the spiny
lobster’s rasp from our experiments, both by using existing frameworks for
understanding anti-predator signal function and by examining the use of
these signals in the aquatic environment.
Useful for examining the rasp’s function, Bradbury & Vehrencamp (1998)
offers an organizational framework for the function of what they term ‘en-
vironmental signals’ (Table 3). Environmental signals contain information
about external factors, such as available resources or potential predators, and
can be directed at either conspecifics or heterospecifics. A starting point for
understanding the function of the rasp is to determine whether it is directed
at the potential predator or toward conspecifics.
Environmental signals directed toward conspecifics include: (1) recruit-
ment of conspecifics to food resources, (2) low-risk warnings to conspecifics
that danger is nearby, possibly including information about the type and loca-
tion of predator, (3) high risk warnings to conspecifics during attack that put
the sender at risk, but the risk is offset by a direct benefit to the sender (May-
nard Smith, 1965; Charnov & Krebs, 1975) and (4) distress calls in which
conspecifics are recruited to help during attack (Maynard Smith, 1965; Ro-
hwer et al., 1976).
Although not directly related to anti-predator signalling, spiny lobsters
regularly recruit conspecifics to feeding sites (a feature used by fishermen
who put lobsters in traps to attract more lobsters; Hunt et al., 1986), indicat-
ing a gregarious approach to feeding, and, by association, a potential use for
gregarious anti-predator behaviours. However, our field observations did not
yield any obvious coordinated response to predators one rasping lobster did
not apparently influence nearby lobsters (which typically continued to forage
during our simulated attacks), thus suggesting that low-risk warnings are un-
likely, although it is possible that the threshold for response was decreased
Spiny lobster sounds 249
Table 3. A number of inter- and intra-specific environmental signals are used
during interactions with predators (Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 1998).
Signal type Modality Sender Receiver Timing Purpose
Inter-specific acoustic kangaroo snake before attack inform predator that
warning rat1(predator) it has been detected
Intra-specific acoustic squirrel squirrel pups before or warn offspring of
warning adults3during attack danger
Inter-specific acoustic starling raptors before or attract second
distress birds4(predators) during attack predator to interfere
with first predator’s
attack
Intra-specific acoustic marmot marmot before or elicit help from
distress pups2parents during attack parents
Inter-specific acoustic insects5mice, spiders during attack startle predator,
startle (predators) induce hesitation
Inter-specific acoustic tiger bats during attack warn of toxicity
aposematic moths6(predators)
visual bivalves, variety of before or highlight weaponry
insects, predators during attack (i.e., spines)
crustaceans7
Examples of inter- and intra-specific warning, distress, startle and aposematic signals, partic-
ularly in the acoustic realm, and their timing relative to predator detection and attack are pro-
vided. References: 1, Randall & Stevens (1987); 2, Blumstein et al. (2008); 3, Davis (1984);
4, Hogstedt (1983); 5, Masters (1979); 6, Hristov & Conner (2005); 7, Inbar & Lev-Yadun
(2005).
(which we did not assess in this study). Furthermore, there was no indication
that the lobsters responded differentially to the predator model and controls
and, thus, were unlikely to be providing information about predator type and
location to conspecifics. Alternatively, when lobsters congregate in burrows
during the day, it is possible that they make use of low-risk warning effects,
such as diluting the probability of attack, influencing group movements to
decrease success of attack or generating chaos to confuse the predator (Caro,
2005), that we did not observe in these experiments. However, we observed
that lobsters in crevices during the day typically retreat further back into the
burrow and brace their bodies against the crevice walls rather than chaoti-
cally or synchronously swimming out and away.
250 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
High risk warnings to conspecifics also seem unlikely given that there
are no obvious benefits to helping conspecifics in this system (Maynard
Smith, 1965; Rohwer et al., 1976); spiny lobster larvae cycle through the
ocean for 6–9 months before settling on the substrate and it is unlikely
that co-denning lobsters are kin (Lindberg, 1955; Childress & Hernkind,
1997; Phillips et al., 2006). Lastly, there have been no reports of, and we
have never observed, conspecifics offering assistance during attack, making
distress signals unlikely. Thus, beyond gregarious feeding, using the rasp as
an environmental signal to conspecifics is improbable in this system.
Signals directed toward predators typically (Table 3): (1) warn a predator
that the prey is aware of the predator’s presence and, thus, is more likely
to evade attack (Hasson, 1991; Caro, 1995; Blount et al., 2009), (2) star-
tle the predator, causing the predator to hesitate and providing an opportu-
nity for the prey to escape (Edmunds, 1974; Sargent, 1990; Ruxton et al.,
2004), (3) enhance predator learning and avoidance through aposematic sig-
nals (e.g., noxious chemicals, abrasive sounds, etc.), such that conspicuous
prey are more likely to be avoided (Guilford, 1990; Rowe & Guilford, 1999;
Ruxton et al., 2004), (4) inform the predator that a group of animals is cog-
nizant of its presence, typically through mobbing behaviours and (5) attract
more predators to generate confusion and perhaps interfere with the initial
predator’s attack (reviewed in Chivers et al., 1996).
For the interspecific warning function, the spiny lobsters would have to
warn the predator in advance of the attack, and we found that lobsters rarely
rasped prior to the attack of our model. Physical contact with the model ag-
gressors in our experiment and with the octopus in Bouwma & Herrnkind
(2009) was the critical stimulus for generating the rasp. Thus, we can rule
out interspecific warning signals as a function for the rasp. The mobbing
function is also unlikely, given that most lobster predators are fish that op-
erate in a 3-D environment, whereas lobsters are benthic and lack fine-tuned
locomotor control while swimming. Thus, coordinated mobbing behaviour is
unlikely and, to our knowledge, has not been observed. More commonly, but
something we did not observe with P. interruptus, some spiny lobster species
aggregate into a circular ‘spiny pincushion’ to jointly repel predatory attacks
(Kelly et al., 1999) rather than aggressively attacking the predator.
The attraction of secondary predators is possible, but also improbable.
The rasp is quickly obscured by background noise within 1 m from the
source (Patek et al., 2009). Thus, if the rasp were to function in this way,
Spiny lobster sounds 251
only nearby predators would be attracted to the scene. That being said, the
propagation of olfactory cues is effective and fast in the aquatic environment,
and it would seem more parsimonious to expect that the olfactory cues re-
leased by damaged tissue would attract secondary predators more quickly
and from a greater range. The demonstration of conspecific alarm cues in P.
argus (Shabani et al., 2008) further suggests that the olfactory channel is an
important one during predator interactions.
Thus, using the standard paradigm for anti-predator signal function, the
remaining two functions that may be operational in spiny lobster rasps are
startle and aposematism. Discerning between a startle and aposematic sig-
nal requires several lines of information; it is also important to recognize
that these functions are not necessarily mutually exclusive. To demonstrate
a startle function, it would be necessary to show that predators pause dur-
ing attack and that prey subsequently have a higher probability of escape.
This would require experiments similar to those conducted by Bouwma &
Herrnkind (2009), with the use of a surgical control for silencing the lob-
sters or an alternative mechanism for silencing that did not require surgery.
An aposematic signal requires learning by the predator, such that noxious
stimuli (e.g., coloration, noise or odor) increase the learned association with
unpalatable prey and, therefore, increase avoidance of the prey (e.g., Rowe
& Guilford, 1999; Rowe, 2002; Gamberale-Stille et al., 2009). To test this
function in spiny lobsters, it would be necessary to measure the response of
initially naïve predators over time as they approach silenced vs. stridulating
and palatable vs. unpalatable lobsters.
It is clear from the present study that tank experiments should be ap-
proached with caution, whether in the experiments testing the startle or
aposematic hypotheses. In the field, lobsters actively foraged away from
their dens whereas in the nighttime tank experiments with a short acclima-
tion time, the lobsters hid in their burrows and were minimally responsive to
the aggressor. Previous studies of tank-held lobsters suggest that nocturnal
foraging resumes after an acclimation period of several hours (S.N.P., pers.
observ.), so, at the minimum, future studies should allot more time for the
lobsters to re-establish their nocturnal behaviour patterns in a tank.
Anti-predator signaling in air versus water
It is possible that the existing framework for understanding environmental
acoustic signals does not fully encompass the realm of signalling in aquatic
252 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
environments (e.g., Greenstreet & Tasker, 1996). To our knowledge, there
have yet to be any studies of the function and performance of arthropod
acoustic anti-predator signals in the aquatic environment. The fundamental
difference between terrestrial acoustic signals and aquatic signals is due to
the physics of sound and vibration: wavelengths and speed of sound in water
are approximately five times greater than in air. As we mentioned above,
this leads to difficulties localizing sound sources in water, especially for
small animals (Denny, 1993). This also means that animals can sense the
vibrational component of sound (the region called the ‘near-field’; Kalmijn,
1988) over five-fold greater distances in water than in air and that even
animals that lack pressure-sensitive ears can sense most biological acoustic
signals within a meter or more of the sound source.
So how might these physical aspects of the aquatic environment affect
the range of possible functions for anti-predator signals? The paradigm for
acoustic anti-predator signals in the terrestrial environment is that they are
broad-band and, therefore, more difficult to localize and not tuned to parti-
cular receivers (Morton, 1977). These features are relevant in the ocean as
well as in air; however, in water, acoustic signals are inherently difficult to
localize, but, more importantly, virtually any receiver could sense a pulsatile
signal within 1 wavelength of the source, i.e., in the near-field. Furthermore,
beyond one wavelength, the near-field component would be undetectable ex-
cept to the much narrower range of receivers with pressure-sensitive ears.
Given that aquatic crustaceans are not known to have true ears (Budelmann,
1992; Popper et al., 2001) and many lobster predators rely on near-field sig-
nals, this close range region offers a highly effective channel for environ-
mental signalling that is absent beyond a few centimetres in the terrestrial
environment.
What are the implications of the aquatic environment for the function of
the rasp? First, there may not be any need for lobsters to distinguish among
predators or other intruders if all can be assumed to sense the rasp. Sec-
ond, the idea of Bouwma & Herrnkind (2009) that the vibration itself might
loosen the predator’s grip may be especially relevant in a liquid environ-
ment. Indeed, this function might be termed a ‘vibratory escape mechanism’.
Third, the rasp may function simultaneously as a seismic (through the sub-
strate or body), near-field and far-field pressure-wave signal, whereas in air,
the rasp would only be transmitted through the body vibrations and air-borne
pressure waves.
Spiny lobster sounds 253
Lastly, with sufficient agitation and physical grasping of their antennae,
spiny lobsters will automatise one or both antennae (which grow back over
subsequent molts). Visually orienting the predator toward the eye-spots that
are adjacent to the sound-producing apparatus (Figure 1) and vibrationally
orienting a predator toward the extended vibrating antennae may cause the
predator to attack the antennae which can be left behind if necessary, thereby
allowing the spiny lobsters a costly, but effective escape. In air, a rapidly
vibrating antenna would transmit a faint vibrational signal over a millimetre
to centimetre length scale which most organisms would be unable to detect.
A vibrating antenna in the aquatic environment could generate a vibrational
field from centimetres to metres, effective for nearly any aquatic animal’s
sensory capabilities. Perhaps, in the aquatic environment, such ‘acoustic eye
spots’ may be both more common and effective than presently realized.
Conclusions
Our understanding of anti-predator signals is primarily based upon exper-
imental and theoretical research in terrestrial environments, with research
on acoustic anti-predator signals largely conducted on birds and primates
(reviewed in Edmunds, 1974; Bailey, 1991; Greenfield, 2002; Ruxton et
al., 2004; Caro, 2005). While this research has produced a strong organi-
zational framework for understanding anti-predator signal functions (Ed-
munds, 1974; Caro, 2005; Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 1998), we are only be-
ginning to understand whether the same principles apply to acoustic signals
in the aquatic environment.
An additional consequence of the physics of underwater sound production
may be the disproportionate affects of anthropogenic noise on the proper
functioning of these systems. For example, anthropogenic noise has de-
creased the population density (Bayne et al., 2008) and altered acoustic be-
haviour (Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser, 2006) of songbirds. In the marine
environment, anthropogenic noise increased the auditory threshold and de-
creased the ability to detect conspecific signals in fish (Vasconcelos et al.,
2007). Marine mammals generated higher amplitude sounds at greater ener-
getic costs to offset high noise levels (Parks et al., 2007; Holt et al., 2009).
Studies of noise pollution in the ocean tend to focus on large vertebrates, but
the effects of anthropogenic noise on invertebrate communities should also
be taken into account when considering the impacts of noise pollution on
animal communication systems.
254 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Our study offers some answers to the function of spiny lobster rasps by
narrowing the range of possibilities to interspecific startle or aposematic sig-
nals while also suggesting future experimental approaches to disentangling
the possible signal functions, including what might be termed ‘acoustic eye-
spots’ and ‘vibratory escape mechanisms’. In the millions of years since its
origin, the spiny lobsters’ acoustic mechanism still poses challenges and of-
fers insights into this untapped frontier of signalling in the sea.
Acknowledgements
We especially thank T. Zack, P. Tompkins and P. Pehl for their extensive field assistance.
Thank you to J. Bell, M. deVries, S. Nunn, M. Patek, V. Patek, D. Elias and L. Shipp for
assistance in the field and for comments on the manuscript. J. E. Baio photographed the
lobsters depicted in Figure 1. We greatly appreciate the constructive comments from two
anonymous reviewers. We also thank the staff at the Wrigley Institute for Environmental
Studies. Funding to SNP was provided by the UC Berkeley Committee on Research Junior
Faculty Research Grant and the Hellman Family Faculty Fund.
References
Athenaeus (3rd century). Le Banquet des Savants. Lamy, Paris.
Bailey, W.J. (1991). Acoustic behaviour of insects: an evolutionary perspective. Chapman
& Hall, New York, NY.
Barshaw, D.E., Lavalli, K.L. & Spanier, E. (2003). Offense versus defense: responses of three
morphological types of lobsters to predation. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 256: 171-182.
Bayne, E.M., Habib, L. & Boutin, S. (2008). Impacts of chronic anthropogenic noise from
energy-sector activity on abundance of songbirds in the boreal forest. Conserv. Biol.
22: 1186-1193.
Berger, D.K. & Butler IV, M.J. (2001). Octopuses influence den selection by juvenile
Caribbean spiny lobster. Mar. Freshw. Res. 52: 1049-1053.
Berrill, M. (1976). Aggressive behaviour of post-puerulus larvae of the western rock lobster
Panulirus longipes (Milne-Edwards). Aust. J. Mar. Freshw. Res. 27: 83-88.
Bill, R.G. & Herrnkind, W.F. (1976). Drag reduction by formation movement in spiny lob-
sters. Science 193: 1146-1148.
Blount, J.D., Speed, M.P., Ruxton, G.D. & Stephens, P.A. (2009). Warning displays may
function as honest signals of toxicity. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. 276: 871-877.
Blumstein, D.T., Richardston, D.T., Cooley, L., Winternitz, J. & Daniel, J.C. (2008). The
structure, meaning, and function of yellow-bellied marmot pup screams. Anim.
Behav. 76: 1055-1064.
Bouwma, P.E. (2006). Aspects of antipredation in Panulirus argus and Panulirus guttatus:
behaviour, morphology and ontogeny. Ph.D. dissertation at Florida State University.
Bouwma, P.E. & Herrnkind, W.F. (2009). Sound production in Caribbean spiny lobster Pan-
ulirus argus and its role in escape during predatory attack by Octopus briareus. N.Z.
J. Mar. Freshw. Res. 43: 3-13.
Spiny lobster sounds 255
Bradbury, J.W. & Vehrencamp, S.L. (1998). Principles of Animal Communication. Sinauer
Associates, Sunderland, MA.
Briones-Fourzán, P. & Lozano-Álvarez, E. (2008). Coexistence of congeneric spiny lobsters
on coral reefs: differences in conspecific aggregation patterns and their potential an-
tipredator benefits. Cor. Reefs 27: 275-287.
Briones-Fourzán, P., Párez-Ortiz, M. & Lozano-Álvarez, E. (2006). Defense mechanisms and
antipredator behaviour in two sympatric species of spiny lobsters, Panulirus argus and
P. guttatus. Mar. Biol. 149: 227.
Budelmann, B.U. (1992). Hearing in Crustacea. In: The evolutionary biology of hearing
(Webster, D.B., Fay, R.R. & Popper, A.N., eds). Springer, New York, NY, p. 131-139.
Butler IV, M.J., MacDiarmid, A.B. & Booth, J.D. (1999). The cause and consequence of
ontogenetic changes in social aggregation in New Zealand spiny lobsters. Mar. Ecol.
Prog. Ser. 188: 179-191.
Caro, T. (2005). Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. The University of Chicago
Press, Chicago, IL.
Caro, T.M. (1995). Pursuit-deterrence revisited. TREE 10: 500-503.
Charnov, E.L. & Krebs, J.R. (1975). The evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation?
Am. Nat. 109: 107-112.
Childress, M.J. (2007). Comparative sociobiology of spiny lobsters. In: Evolutionary
ecology of social and sexual systems: crustaceans as model organisms (Duffy, J.E. &
Thiel, M., eds). Oxford University Press, New York, NY, p. 272-293.
Childress, M.J. & Hernkind, W.F. (1997). Den sharing by juvenile Caribbean spiny lobsters
(Panulirus argus) in nursery habitat: cooperation or coincidence? Mar. Freshw. Res.
48: 751-758.
Childress, M.J. & Herrnkind, W.F. (2001). The guide effect influence on the gregariousness
of juvenile Caribbean spiny lobsters. Anim. Behav. 62: 465-472.
Chivers, D.P., Brown, G.E. & Smith, R.J.F. (1996). The evolution of chemical alarm signals:
attracting predators benefits alarm signal senders. Am. Nat. 148: 649-659.
Cocroft, R.B. (1999). Parent-offspring communication in response to predators in a subso-
cial treehopper (Hemiptera: Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). Ethology 105:
553-568.
Davis, L.S. (1984). Alarm calling in Richardson’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus richard-
sonii). Z. Tierpsychol. 66: 152-164.
Denny, M. (1993). Sound in air and water: listening to the environment. In: Air and water:
the biology and physics of life’s media. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Derby, C.D. & Steullet, P. (2001). Why do animals have so many receptors? The role of
multiple chemosensors in animal perception. Biol. Bull. 200: 211-215.
Dicke, M. & Grostal, P. (2001). Chemical detection of natural enemies by arthropods: an
ecological perspective. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 32: 1-23.
Edmunds, M. (1974). Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defenses. Longman
Group, Harlow.
Gamberale-Stille, G., Bragée, C. & Tullberg, B.S. (2009). Higher survival of aposematic
prey in close encounters with predators: an experimental study of detection distance.
Anim. Behav. 78: 111-116.
George, R.W. & Main, A.R. (1967). The evolution of spiny lobsters (Palinuridae): a study of
evolution in the marine environment. Evolution 21: 803-820.
256 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Greenfield, M.D. (2002). Signalers and receivers: mechanisms and evolution of arthropod
communication. Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Greenstreet, S. & Tasker, M. (1996). Aquatic predators and their prey. Fishing News
Books, London.
Guilford, T. (1990). The evolution of aposematism. In: Insect defenses: adaptive mecha-
nisms and strategies of prey and predators (D. Evans & J. Schmidt, eds). State University
of New York Press, Albany, NY, p. 23-61.
Hasson, O. (1991). Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator.
TREE 6: 325-329.
Herrnkind, W.F., Childress, M.J. & Lavalli, K.L. (2001). Cooperative defence and other
benefits among exposed spiny lobsters: inferences from group size and behaviour.
Mar. Freshw. Res. 52: 1113-1124.
Hogstedt, G. (1983). Adaptation unto death: function of fear screams. Am. Nat. 121:
562-570.
Holt, M.M., Noren, D.P., Veirs, V., Emmons, C. & Veirs, S. (2009). Speaking up: killer whales
(Orcinus orca) increase their call amplitude in response to vessel noise. J. Acoust.
Soc. Am. 125: EL27-EL29.
Hristov, N.I. & Conner, W.E. (2005). Sound strategy: acoustic aposematism in the bat-tiger
moth arms race. Naturwissenschaften 92: 164-169.
Hunt, J.H., Lyons, W.G. & Kennedy, J.F.S. (1986). Effects of exposure and confinement on
spiny lobsters, Panulirus argus, used as attractants in the Florida trap fishery. Fish.
Bull. 84: 69-76.
Inbar, M. & Lev-Yadun, S. (2005). Conspicuousness and aposematic spines in the animal
kingdom. Naturwissenschaften 92: 170-172.
Kalmijn, A.J. (1988). Hydrodynamic and acoustic field detection. In: Sensory biology of
aquatic animals (Atema, J., Fay, R.R., Popper, A.N. & Tavolga, W.N., eds). Springer,
New York, NY, p. 83-130.
Kanciruk, P. (1980). Ecology of juvenile and adult Palinuridae (spiny lobsters). In: The
biology and management of lobsters (Cobb, J.S. & Phillips, B.F., eds). Academic Press,
New York, NY, p. 59-96.
Kelly, S., MacDiarmid, A.B. & Babcock, R.C. (1999). Characteristics of spiny lobster, Ja-
sus edwarsii, aggregations in exposed reef and sandy areas. Mar. Freshw. Res. 50:
409-416.
Latha, G., Senthilvadivu, S., Venkatesan, R. & Rajendran, V. (2005). Sound of shallow and
deep water lobsters: measurements, analysis and characterization (L). J. Acoust. Soc.
Am. 117: 2720-2723.
Lima, S.L. (2002). Putting predators back into behavioural predator–prey interactions.
TREE 17: 70-75.
Lindberg, R.G. (1955). Growth, population dynamics, and field behaviour in the spiny lobster,
Panulirus interruptus (Randall). Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 59: 157-248.
Lohrey, A.K., Clark, D.L., Gordon, S.D. & Uetz, G.W. (2009). Antipredator responses of
wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae) to sensory cues representing an avian predator.
Anim. Behav. 77: 813-821.
Masters, W.M. (1979). Insect disturbance stridulation its defensive role. Behav. Ecol.
Soc. 5: 187-200.
Maynard Smith, J. (1965). The evolution of alarm calls. Am. Nat. 99: 59-63.
Spiny lobster sounds 257
Meyer-Rochow, V.B. & Penrose, J.D. (1976). Sound production by the western rock lobster
Panulirus longipes (Milne Edwards). J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 23: 191-209.
Meyer-Rochow, V.B., Penrose, J.D., Oldfield, B.P. & Bailey, W.J. (1982). Phonoresponses
in the rock lobster Panulirus longipes (Milne Edwards). Behav. Neur. Biol. 34:
331-336.
Morton, E.S. (1977). On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in
some bird and mammal sounds. Am. Nat. 111: 855-869.
Moulton, J. (1957). Sound production in the spiny lobster Panulirus argus (Latreille). Biol.
Bull. 113: 286-295.
Moulton, J. (1958). Age changes in stridulation and the stridulatory apparatus of the spiny
lobsters, Panulirus argus (Latreille). Anat. Rec. 132: 480.
Mulligan, B.E. & Fischer, R.B. (1977). Sounds and behaviour of the spiny lobster Panulirus
argus. Crustaceana 32: 185-199.
Palero, F., Crandall, K.A., Abelló, P., Macpherson, E. & Pascual, M. (2009). Phylogenetic
relationships between spiny, slipper and coral lobsters (Crustacea, Decapoda, Achelata).
Mol. Phyl. Evol. 50: 152-162.
Parker, T.J. (1878). Note on the stridulation organ of Panulirus vulgaris. Proc. Zool. Lond.:
442-444.
Parker, T.J. (1883). On the structure of the head in Palinurus with special reference to the
classification of the genus. Nature 29: 189-190.
Parks, S.E., Clark, C.W. & Tyack, P.L. (2007). Short- and long-term changes in right whale
calling behaviour: the potential effects of noise on acoustic communication. J.
Acoust. Soc. Am. 122: 3725-3731.
Patek, S. (2002). Squeaking with a sliding joint: mechanics and motor control of sound
production in palinurid lobsters. J. Exp. Biol. 205: 2375-2385.
Patek, S.N. (2001). Spiny lobsters stick and slip to make sound. Nature 411: 153-154.
Patek, S.N. & Baio, J.E. (2007). The acoustic mechanics of stick-slip friction in the California
spiny lobster (Panulirus interruptus). J. Exp. Biol. 210: 3538-3546.
Patek, S.N. & Oakley, T.H. (2003). Comparative tests of evolutionary tradeoffs in a palinurid
lobster acoustic system. Evolution 57: 2082-2100.
Patek, S.N., Shipp, L.E. & Staaterman, E. (2009). The acoustics and acoustic behaviour
of the California spiny lobster (Panulirus interruptus). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125:
3434-3443.
Phillips, B.F., Booth, J.D., Cobb, J.S., Jeffs, A.G. & McWilliam, P. (2006). Larval and postlar-
val ecology. In: Lobsters: biology, management, aquaculture and fisheries (Phillips,
B.F., ed.). Blackwell, Oxford, p. 231-262.
Popper, A.N., Salmon, M. & Horch, K.W. (2001). Acoustic detection and communication by
decapod crustaceans. J. Comp. Phys. A 187: 83-89.
Randall, J.A. & Stevens, C.M. (1987). Footdrumming and other anti-predator responses in
the bannertail kangaroo rat (Dipodomys spectabilis). Behav. Ecol. Soc. 20: 187-194.
Rohwer, S., Fretwell, S.D. & Tuckfield, R.C. (1976). Distress screams as a measure of kinship
in birds. Am. Midl. Nat. 96: 418-430.
Rowe, C. (2002). Sound improves visual discrimination learning in avian predators. Proc.
Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. 269: 1353-1357.
Rowe, C. & Guilford, T. (1999). Novelty effects in a multimodal warning signal. Anim.
Behav. 57: 341-346.
258 Staaterman, Claverie, & Patek
Ruxton, G.D., Sherratt, T.N. & Speed, M.P. (2004). Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology
of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Sargent, T.D. (1990). Startle as an anti-predator mechanism, with special reference to the un-
derwing moths, (Catocala). In: Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies
of prey and predators (Evans, D.L. & Schmidt, J.O., eds). State University of New York
Press, Albany, NY, p. 229-249.
Shabani, S., Kamio, M. & Derby, C.D. (2008). Spiny lobsters detect conspecific blood-borne
alarm cues exclusively through olfactory sensilla. J. Exp. Biol. 211: 2600-2608.
Sih, A., Englund, G. & Wooster, D. (1998). Emergent impacts of multiple predators on prey.
TREE 13: 350-355.
Slabbekoorn, H. & den Boer-Visser, A. (2006). Cities change the songs of birds. Curr.
Biol. 16: 2326-2331.
Smale, M. (1974). The warning squeak of the Natal rock lobster. S. Afr. Ass. Mar. Biol.
Res. Bull. 11: 17-19.
Spanier, E. & Zimmer-Faust, R.K. (1988). Some physical properties of shelter that influence
den preference in spiny lobsters. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 121: 137-149.
Tautz, J. (1990). Coding of mechanical stiumuli in crustaceans what and why? In:
Frontiers in crustacean neurobiology (Wiege, K., Krenz, W.D., Tautz, J., Reichest, H. &
Mulloneg, B., eds). Birkhäuser, Basel, p. 200-206.
Tautz, J. & Sandeman, D.D. (1980). The detection of waterborne vibration by sensory hairs
on the chelae of the crayfish. J. Exp. Biol. 88: 351-356.
Vasconcelos, R.O., Amorim, M.C.P. & Ladich, F. (2007). Effects of ship noise on the de-
tectability of communication signals in the Lusitanian toadfish. J. Exp. Biol. 210:
2104-2112.
Wahle, R.A. (1992). Body-size dependent anti-predator mechanisms of the American lobster.
Oikos 65: 52-60.
Weiss, H.M., Lozano-Álvarez, E., Briones-Fourzán, P. & Negrete-Soto, F. (2006). Using red
light with fixed-site video cameras to study the behaviour of the spiny lobster, Panulirus
argus, and associated animals at night and inside their shelters. Mar. Tech. Soc. J.
40: 86-95.
Zimmer-Faust, R.K., Fielder, D.R., Heck Jr., K.L., Coen, L.D. & Morgan, S.G. (1994). Effects
of tethering on predatory escape by juvenile blue crabs. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 111:
299-303.
... Lobster behaviour is represented by some kind of acoustic signaling. Some marine biota produce sound, including fish (Amron et al., 2017;Amron et al., 2018), and crustacean (Patek, 2002;Patek and Oakley, 2003;Henninger and Watson, 2005;Patek and Baio, 2007;Patek et al., 2009;Staaterman et al ., 2010 ;Buscaino et al., 2011a;De Vincenzi et al ., 2015;Jézéquel et al., 2018;Bouwma & Herrnkind, 2009;Jézéquel et al., 2020). The sound produced by these species provides important information about their existence, distribution, and behaviors ( Hamilton et al., 2019). ...
... The sound produced by these species provides important information about their existence, distribution, and behaviors ( Hamilton et al., 2019). Most of the lobsters emits sound as means of protection from predators (Staaterman et al., 2010;Buscaino et al., 2011b;Ward et al, 2011) and response to the environment (Bouwma and Herrnkind, 2009). The main organs in lobsters producing this sound are the plectrum and file (Meyer-Rochow and Penrose, 1976;Cobb and Phillips, 1980;Henninger and Watson, 2005;Buscaino et al., 2011a;Fornshell and Tesei, 2017) through the stick and slip mechanism (Patek, 2001;Patek, 2002;Patek and Oakley, 2003;Patek and Baio, 2007). ...
... This type of sound was a form of lobster defense strategy, during predator attacks (Buscaino et al., 2011b). Human activities also caused changes in behavioral responses and sound production of lobsters (Bouwma and Herrnkind, 2009;Staaterman et al., 2010). The response to stress due to acoustic stimulation, was another factor that affected the sound emitted by lobsters (De Vincenzi et al., 2015;Jézéquel et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Highlight Research The sound of d spiny lobster (Panulirus homarus) was identified, named rasp; slow rattle; and popping sound The sound characteristics of spiny lobster (Panulirus homarus) represent its behavior Passive acoustic and visual record method combination was implemented to analyze the relation between sounds and behavior of spiny lobster (Panulirus homarus) Abstract Spiny lobster Panulirus homarus is one of many important economic fishery resources in Indonesia. Continuous exploitation activities in the natural habitat can potentially disturb the sustainability of lobster resources. This species is known to produce sound with certain characteristics and highly dependent on environmental changes. This study aimed to investigate the characteristics of sound as a representation of behaviour. The sound produced by P. homarus was recorded for 24 hours using a hydrophone (a passive acoustic technique). The hydrophone converts the sound wave produced by the lobsters into electric voltage in millivolts, then were converted to a digital number, in order to be saved in a virtual format with the *.wav extension. The video system captured data on behaviors of resting, territoriality, aggression, and social attitudes. The results showed that P. homarus emitted rasp, slow rattle, and popping type of sound, with certain characteristics. All sounds types were characterized by a broadband frequency (4 - 20 kHz), and a source level ranging from 125 - 145 dB re 1 µPa. Rasp sound consisting of 12 - 40 pulses with a sound duration of 100 - 400 ms, represents a defensive behavior. Slow rattle sound with 2 - 7 pulses did not represent a specific behaviour and it is dominant during the night. However, the popping sound with only one pulse represented feeding behavior.
... Further studies will be required to characterize antennal rasps without handling animals in situ. An interesting perspective is the use of fake predators, as described by Staaterman et al. (2010). ...
... Most studies in the bioacoustics literature have proposed an anti-predator role for the antennal rasps (Moulton 1957, Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1976, Staaterman et al. 2010. In this study, we elicited the production of antennal rasps in P. elephas by gently handling individuals, which is intended to imitate capture by a natural predator. ...
... Our results clearly suggest that antennal rasps produced by P. elephas could be detected in situ above the ambient noise. Surprisingly, earlier studies carried out with tropical spiny lobsters Panulirus interruptus, 176 using the same protocol of sound recordings as in this study, stated that antennal rasps recorded in situ are as loud as the ambient noise, so that they could not be detected (Patek et al. 2009, Staaterman et al. 2010. As discussed in Section 4.2, because the use of metrics to characterize sounds is not normalized (and sometimes missed), it is not possible to do a direct comparison of our results with those of Patek et al. (2009). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Le rôle écologique des sons chez les crustacés est mal défini comparé aux mammifères marins et aux poissons. Or, comprendre l’importance des sons pour la biologie d’une espèce est crucial lorsque les impacts des bruits anthropiques sont recherchés. Par ailleurs, l’écologie cherche encore à développer de nouveaux outils de suivi par acoustique passive (PAM). L’enjeu principal de cette thèse était d’étudier la bioacoustique de deux espèces de crustacés à fort intérêt commercial et patrimonial en Europe : le homard Européen Homarus gammarus, et la langouste rouge Palinurus elephas. En tenant compte de l’effet physique des cuves sur les sons, nous avons mis en évidence la forte production de buzz entre homards mâles lors de rencontres agonistiques pour établir des statuts de dominance. Une approche neurophysiologique nous a ensuite permis de caractériser les capacités sensorielles des homards qui détectent les sons dans la même bande de fréquence que les buzz qu’ils émettent, renforçant l’hypothèse d’une communication acoustique. La deuxième partie de cette thèse présente comment, chez la langouste, leurs rasps d’antennes pourraient être utilisés avec du PAM par les biologistes et les halieutes. En effet, après avoir quantifié leur propagation in situ, nous avons mis en évidence que ces sons peuvent être détectés à l’échelle du kilomètre, et que leurs caractéristiques dépendent de la taille des individus. Nous avons également montré que ces rasps d’antennes sont largement énergétiques dans les basses fréquences (< 1 kHz), ce qui permet aussi de poser l’hypothèse de leur utilisation pour une communication acoustique. Les résultats de ce travail de thèse ouvrent des perspectives importantes sur l’impact potentiel des bruits anthropiques et le développement du PAM pour la gestion et conservation des crustacés.
... Infraorder Achelata including palinurid spiny lobsters had lost large chelae about 125 million years ago [70]. They show agonistic behavior by using the long, highly movable antennae bearing numerous spines [71,72]. The antennae have also evolved as an acoustic apparatus [8]. ...
... Stridulation signals may also have a role in eliciting a "conditioned response" by which the predators would suppress predatory motivation due to association with prey counter attacks using spiny antennae [72]. The lobster's threatening sound was emitted at the predators and conspecific intruders when tested experimentally with pseudomorphs regardless of timing (day or night) [71]. The agonistic behaviors were much more aggressive at night than during daytime and emitted sound was more frequent when having contact with model attackers, indicating that the acoustic signal can draw the attention of the intruder to the antennae. ...
... The agonistic behaviors were much more aggressive at night than during daytime and emitted sound was more frequent when having contact with model attackers, indicating that the acoustic signal can draw the attention of the intruder to the antennae. This increases successful escaping opportunities from deadly predatory attacks in conjunction with autotomy of the antennae, a costly strategy in return for their survival (Acoustical eye-spot, Staaterman et al. 2010) [71]. ...
Chapter
Crustaceans often have highly diversified and specialized acoustic apparatus. Over the centuries, the sound production and detection capabilities of this group have been brought to the attention of investigators and intensively described in the literature. However, the role of acoustic signals had remained largely unknown until early 21st century and only over the last couple of decades, it is being understood. Mechanisms of neuroendocrinal control that underlie acoustic behaviors have remained unknown until the present day. In this chapter, we aim to provide information on the ecological and behavioral backgrounds of endocrinological regulation of acoustic behavior in crustaceans, useful for future studies related to this field. First, we introduce the mechanisms of production/detection of acoustic and vibration signals and their function in crustaceans inhabiting different habitats from land to sea. We argue what makes differences or variables in the acoustic methods and transfer of information among different species, habitats, or contexts. Finally, we introduce recent studies highlighting anthropogenic noises as environmental pollutants capable of causing detrimental effects on crustacean habitats and communication.
... These antennal rasps are described as broadband sounds with peak frequencies below 1 kHz (J ez equel et al., 2022a). The ecological meaning of antennal rasps has mostly been attributed to an anti-predator context, such as helping clawless spiny lobsters to deter or escape from predators (Bouwma and Herrnkind, 2009;Staaterman et al., 2010). ...
Article
Marine crustaceans produce broadband sounds that are useful for passive acoustic monitoring to support conservation and management efforts. However, the propagation characteristics and detection ranges of their signals are poorly known, limiting our leveraging of these sounds. Here, we used a four-hydrophone linear array to measure source levels (SLs) and sound propagation from Caribbean spiny lobsters (Panulirus argus) of a wide range of sizes within a natural, shallow water habitat (3.3 m depth). Source level in peak-peak (SLpp) varied with body size; larger individuals produced SLpp up to 166 dB re 1 lPa. However, transmission losses (TL) were similar across all sizes, with a global fitted TL of 12.1 dB. Correspondingly, calculated detection ranges varied with body size, ranging between 14 and 364 m for small and large individuals (respectively). This increased up to 1612 m for large spiny lobsters when considering lower ambient noise levels. Despite the potential ease of tank studies, our results highlight the importance of empirical in situ sound propagation studies for marine crustaceans. Given the important ecological and economic role of spiny lobsters, these data are a key step to supporting remote monitoring of this species for fisheries management and efforts to acoustically quantify coral reefs' health.
... A louder call travels farther in the marine environment, which tends to promote low-frequency propagation. While the ecological function of AR is not clear yet (but see Staaterman et al., 2010), our results suggest larger lobsters would be more suitable for acoustic communication, as in insects. ...
Article
Sound is an important cue for arthropods. In insects, sound features and sound-producing apparatus are tightly correlated to enhance signal emission in larger individuals. In contrast, acoustic scaling in marine arthropods is poorly described even if they possess similar sound-producing apparatus. Here, the acoustic scaling of the European spiny lobster is analyzed by recording sounds in situ at 1 m from a wide range of body sizes. The dimensions of associated sound-producing apparatus increased with body size, indicating sound features would also be influenced by spiny lobster size. Indeed, temporal sound features changed with body size, suggesting differences in calling songs could be used for spiny lobster acoustic communication. Source levels (peak–peak) ranged from 131 to 164 dB re 1 μPa for smaller and larger lobsters, respectively, which could be explained by more efficient resonating structures in larger animals. In addition, dominant frequencies were highly constrained by ambient noise levels, masking the low-frequency content of low intensity sounds from smaller spiny lobsters. Although the ecological function of spiny lobster sounds is not clear yet, these results suggest larger body sizes benefit because louder calls increase the broadcast area and potential interactions with conspecifics, as shown in the insect bioacoustic literature.
... Other well-known sound-producing invertebrates are lobsters and sea urchins. Lobsters produce broadband pulse trains when facing predators or competing conspecifics(Staaterman et al. 2010; Jézéquel et al. 2019). Jézéquel et al. (2019) characterized pulse trains of the European spiny lobster (Palinurus elephas) as signals with a mean bandwidth of 5-23 kHz. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Soundscapes have been likened to acoustic landscapes, encompassing all the acoustic features of an area. The sounds that make up a soundscape can be grouped according to their source into biophony (sounds from animals), geophony (sounds from atmospheric and geophysical events), and anthropophony (sounds from human activities). Natural soundscapes have changed over time because of human activities that generate sound, alter land-use patterns, remove animals from natural settings, and result in climate change. These human activities have direct and indirect effects on animal distribution patterns and (acoustic) behavior. Consequently, current soundscapes may be very different from those a few hundred years ago. This is of concern as natural soundscapes have ecological value. Losing natural soundscapes may, therefore, result in a loss of biodiversity and ecosystem functioning. The study of soundscapes can identify ecosystems undergoing change and potentially document causes (such as noise from human activities). Methods for studying soundscapes range from listening and creating visual (spectrographic) displays to the computation of acoustic indices and advanced statistical modeling. Passive acoustic recording has become an ecological tool for research, monitoring, and ultimately conservation management. This chapter introduces terrestrial and aquatic soundscapes, soundscape analysis tools, and soundscape management.
... Countless marine animals rely on sound to survive, e.g. to detect predators, echolocation, navigation and communication with conspecifics (Leis et al., 2011;Montgomery et al., 2006;Slabbekoorn, 2010;Staaterman et al., 2010;Tolimieri et al., 2000). In recent decades, however, anthropogenic low-frequency noise has invaded natural soundscapes, pervading oceanic ambient sounds and potentially disrupting the biotic interactions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sounds from human activities such as shipping and seismic surveys have been progressively invading natural soundscapes and pervading oceanic ambient sounds for decades. Benthic invertebrates are important ecosystem engineers that continually rework the sediment they live in. Here, we tested how low-frequency noise (LFN), a significant component of noise pollution, affects the sediment reworking activities of selected macrobenthic invertebrates. In a controlled laboratory setup, the effects of acute LFN exposure on the behavior of three abundant bioturbators on the North Atlantic coasts were explored for the first time by tracking their sediment reworking and bioirrigation activities in noisy and control environments via luminophore and sodium bromide (NaBr) tracers, respectively. The amphipod crustacean Corophium volutator was negatively affected by LFN, exhibiting lower bioturbation rates and shallower luminophore burial depths compared to controls. The effect of LFN on the polychaete Arenicola marina and the bivalve Limecola balthica remained inconclusive, although A. marina displayed greater variability in bioirrigation rates when exposed to LFN. Furthermore, a potential stress response was observed in L. balthica that could reduce bioturbation potential. Benthic macroinvertebrates may be in jeopardy along with the crucial ecosystem-maintaining services they provide. More research is urgently needed to understand, predict, and manage the impacts of anthropogenic noise pollution on marine fauna and their associated ecosystems.
... Sound production in crustacean zooplankton has rarely been described (Giguère and Dill 1979) even though larger crustaceans are known to produce a variety of sounds during feeding (Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1976;Tolstoganova 2002), during anti-predator behaviour (Henninger and Watson 2005;Bouwma and Herrnkind 2009;Staaterman et al. 2010;Hamilton et al. 2019) and for intraspecific communication (Buscaino et al. 2015;Jézéquel et al. 2019). Krill and copepods play a significant role in the ocean's carbon cycle, both as a key linkage between trophic levels, and as a carbon sink (Steinberg and Landry 2017;Cavan et al. 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Sound is an effective channel for the transfer of information underwater. While it is known that fish and whales can use sound for communication, and as a cue to localise predators and prey, much less is known about sound production in invertebrates. Here we describe sounds produced by two of the most common marine crustacean zooplankton in the Northern hemisphere: Northern krill (Meganyctiphanes norvegica) and copepods (Calanus spp.). The recorded sounds were taxon-specific and within the hearing range of common planktivorous fish. We recorded similar sounds in the laboratory and in the field. In krill, the sound co-occurred with a tail flip, and the amplitude of the sound was correlated to the displacement distance of the animal, indicating a potential sound producing mechanism. Our findings highlight the possibility that zooplankton sounds could be used as a cue for their predators and for intraspecific communication.
... As sound is essential for the survival of most marine species, comprehensive understanding of the marine soundscape is important. Marine animals from invertebrates to mammals use sound to forage (e.g., Au, 1993;Chapuis and Bshary, 2010), communicate (e.g., Fine et al., 1977;Staaterman et al., 2010;Janik and Sayigh, 2013;Ladich, 2015), guide settlement (e.g., Mann et al., 2007;Radford et al., 2011;Rice et al., 2017), navigate (e.g., Payne and Webb, 1971;George et al., 1989;Jaquet et al., 2001), and facilitate reproduction (e.g., Popper et al., 2001;Parsons et al., 2008). Regardless of the exact purpose of the sounds that an animal produces or receives, animals experience the soundscape as a holistic environment, and their ability to detect a signal depends on both their sounddetection capabilities as well as the presence of other sounds (e.g., Ketten, 1994;Szymanski et al., 1999;Popper et al., 2001Popper et al., , 2019Mooney et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Many animals use sound for communication, navigation, and foraging, particularly in deep water or at night when light is limited, so describing the soundscape is essential for understanding, protecting, and managing these species and their environments. The nearshore deep-water acoustic environment off the coast of Kona, Hawai’i, is not well documented but is expected to be strongly influenced by anthropogenic activities such as fishing, tourism, and other vessel activity. To characterize the deep-water soundscape in this area we used High-frequency Acoustic Recording Packages (HARPs) to record acoustic data year-round at a 200 or 320 kHz sampling rate. We analyzed data spanning more than 10 years (2007-2018) by producing measurements of frequency-specific energy and using a suite of detectors and classifiers for general and specific sound sources. This provided a time series for sounds coming from biological, anthropogenic and physical sources. The soundscape in this location is dominated by signals generated by humans and odontocete cetaceans (mostly delphinids), generally alternating on a diel cycle. During daylight hours the dominant sound sources are vessels and echosounders, with strong signals ranging from 10 Hz to 80 kHz and above, while during the night the clicks from odontocetes dominate the soundscape in mid-to-high frequencies, generally between 10 and 90 kHz. Winter-resident humpback whales are present seasonally and produce calls in lower frequencies (200-2,000 Hz). Overall, seasonal variability is relatively subtle, which is unsurprising given the tropical latitude and deep-water environment. These results, and particularly the inclusion of sounds from frequencies above 2 kHz, represent the first long-term analysis of a marine soundscape in the North Pacific, and the first assessment of the intense, daily presence of manmade noise at this site. The decadal time series allows us to characterize the dynamic nature of this location, and to begin to identify changes in the soundscape over time. This type of analysis facilitates protection of natural resources and effective management of human activities in an ecologically important area.
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACT: Sound is the most effective means of communication in marine and freshwater ecosystems. However, no data about acoustic emissions from non-malacostracan crustaceans are currently available, so their ability to produce sounds is unknown. For the first time, this study investigates the sound produced by two tadpole shrimp species, Triops cancriformis and Lepidurus lubbocki. L. lubbocki individuals were collected from a natural temporary pond in Sicily (Italy), whereas T. cancriformis individuals were obtained from eggs contained in sediment from a rock pool in Sardinia (Italy). In the laboratory, experimental tanks with the animals (one species per time) were acoustically monitored. Both species produced high-frequency wideband pulses distinguishable by their sound pressure level, which was higher in L. lubbocki (146 dB) than in T. cancriformis (130 dB), and by their first and second peak frequencies, which were higher in L. lubbocki (65 and 86 kHz) than in T. cancriformis (63 and 71 kHz). The energy distributions in the power density spectra showed different shapes, as revealed by the 3 dB bandwidth and centre frequency. The pulse durations were 88 and 97 µs in L. lubbocki and T. cancriformis, respectively. L. lubbocki presented a higher emission rate than T. cancriformis and a marked circadian pattern, with a higher abundance of sounds during the night. This study reports the first evidence of sound emissions from non-malacostracan crustaceans and reveals the high potential of passive acoustic monitoring to detect the presence, abundance and life cycle of these elusive keystone species of temporary water bodies.
Book
Full-text available
In most terrestrial and aquatic habitats, the vast majority of animals transmitting and receiving communicative signals are arthropods. This book presents the story of how this important group of animals use pheromones, sound, vibration, and light for sexual and social communication. Because of their small to minute body size most arthropods have problems sending and receiving acoustic and optical information, each of which have their own severe constraints. Because of these restraints they have developed chemical signaling which is not similarly limited by scale. Presenting the latest theoretical and experimental findings from studies of signaling, it suggests that close parallels between arthropods and vertebrates reflect a very limited number of solutions to problems in behavior that are available within the confines of physical laws.
Article
Full-text available
Effects of tethering on predatory escape by juvenile blue crabs Callinectes sapidus Rathbun were examined. Trials were conducted in a large flume to simulate the natural sedimentary and hydrodynamic properties of open sand flats in low-energy estuarine environments. Behavioral interactions between juvenile (prey) and adult (predator) blue crabs were video recorded, then quantified by cornputer-video motion analysis. Sig~uficantly more tethered juveniles (70%) were captured by adults than non-tethered juveniles (10%). Kinematics of the prey swimming escape response changed little as a result of the tether. Mean velocity, maximum velocity, and maximum acceleration did not differ significantly between tethered and non-tethered juveniles. But tethered juveniles traveled shorter distances during bursts of sw~rnrning to escape predatory adults, and they could not evade persistent attackers. Whereas tethering is used to assess relative rates of predation, it increases vulnerability of prey organisms to attack. Predation rates will be most inflated for highly mobile prey that effectively evade capture when they are not restrained. Investigators are strongly encouraged to quantify the impacts of tethering on prey escape and survivorship.
Book
Avoiding Attack discusses the diversity of mechanisms by which prey avoid predator attacks and explores how such defensive mechanisms have evolved through natural selection. It considers how potential prey avoid detection, how they make themselves unprofitable to attack, how they communicate this status, and how other species have exploited these signals. Using carefully selected examples of camouflage, mimicry, and warning signals drawn from a wide range of species and ecosystems, the authors summarise the latest research into these fascinating adaptations, developing mathematical models where appropriate and making recommendations for future study. This second edition has been extensively rewritten, particularly in the application of modern genetic research techniques which have transformed our recent understanding of adaptations in evolutionary genomics and phylogenetics. The book also employs a more integrated and systematic approach, ensuring that each chapter has a broader focus on the evolutionary and ecological consequences of anti-predator adaptation. The field has grown and developed considerably over the last decade with an explosion of new research literature, making this new edition timely.
Chapter
Sensory systems are not perfect instruments for the measurement of physical quantities but are shaped by natural selection to serve the needs of the animal. Thus in order to understand fully the perceptive capabilities of animals one has to ask the biological relevance in addition to the questions, “what is perceived” and “how is it done”. Vibratory communication in fiddler crabs and the directionality of mechanosensory interneurons in crayfish are used to investigate two theses on the match between stimulus coding capabilities in animals and their physical environment.
Book
The book is based on a conference held in Aberdeen in August 1994, presenting 21 case studies and reviews representing current knowledge on the subject area and indicating where necessary information is not available or required. The contributions are generally from the UK and its coastal waters, presenting case studies and reviews of freshwater and marine trophic interactions involving birds, fish, mammals and their prey. The volume begins by describing the behavioural tactics used by predators to capture prey and the evasion strategies employed by prey in avoiding capture, the factors which affect these behaviour patterns and their evolution. It goes on to examine how predators affect prey populations directly, and vice versa, before exploring how predation forces influence other species indirectly throughout the food web, thereby structuring entire communities. Finally the impact of man on aquatic systems, as an active harvester, through the alteration of food resource supply, and as a potential system manager is addressed. All the papers are abstracted separately in Ecological Abstracts.
Book
Understanding of animal social and sexual evolution has seen a renaissance in recent years with discoveries of frequent infidelity in apparently monogamous species, the importance of sperm competition, active female mate choice, and eusocial behavior in animals outside the traditional social insect groups. Each of these findings has raised new questions, and suggested new answers about the evolution of behavioral interactions among animals. This volume synthesizes recent research on the sexual and social biology of the Crustacea, one of the dominant invertebrate groups on earth. Its staggering diversity includes ecologically important inhabitants of nearly every environment from deep-sea trenches, through headwater streams, to desert soils. The wide range of crustacean phenotypes and environments is accompanied by a comparable diversity of behavioral and social systems, including the elaborate courtship and wildly exaggerated morphologies of fiddler crabs, the mysterious queuing behavior of migrating spiny lobsters, and even eusociality in coral-reef shrimps. This diversity makes crustaceans particularly valuable for exploring the comparative evolution of sexual and social systems. Despite exciting recent advances, however, general recognition of the value of Crustacea as models has lagged behind that of the better studied insects and vertebrates. This book synthesizes the state of the field in crustacean behavior and sociobiology, and places it in a conceptually based, comparative framework that will be valuable to active researchers and students in animal behavior, ecology, and evolutionary biology. It brings together a group of experts in fields related to crustacean behavioral ecology, ranging from physiology and functional morphology, through mating and social behavior, to ecology and phylogeny. Each chapter makes connections to other non-crustacean taxa, and the volume closes with a summary section that synthesizes the contributions, discusses anthropogenic impacts, highlights unanswered questions, and provides a vision for profitable future research.