ArticlePublisher preview available

The Philosophical Roots of Western and Eastern Conceptions of Creativity

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

This essay reviews the philosophical roots and the devel-opment of the concept of creativity in the West and East. In particular, two conceptions of creativity that originated in the West – divinely inspired creativity and individual creativity—are discussed and compared to the two East-ern conceptions of creativity that are rooted in ancient Chinese philosophical thought — natural and individual creativity. Both Western and Eastern conceptions of indi-vidual creativity come from a theistic or cosmic tradition of either divinely inspired or natural creativity. However, a defining feature of the Western concept of creativity— novelty—is not necessarily embraced by ancient Chinese concepts of creativity, but does exist in both modern Eastern conceptions. Reasons for cultural differences are explored and discussed. The influence of culture on people's conceptions of creativity has been one of the most exciting topics in the recent literature on creativ-ity. It has attracted the interest of many psychologists (e.g., Lubart, 1999; Niu & Sternberg, 2002; Lau, Hui & Ng, 2004; Rudowicz & Hui, 1997; Rudowicz & Yue, 2000). A review of contemporary research has shown that people from the East and West hold similar, yet not identi-cal conceptions of creativity. In general, Easterners are more likely to view creativity as having social and moral values, and as making a con-nection between the new and the old. Their Western counterparts focus more on some special individual characteristics in understanding the concept of creativity (Niu & Sternberg, 2002).
\\server05\productn\T\THE\26-1-2\THE1210.txt unknown Seq: 1 26-FEB-07 9:25
ARTICLES
The Philosophical Roots of Western and Eastern
Conceptions of Creativity
Weihua Niu
Pace University
Robert J. Sternberg
Tufts University
Abstract
This essay reviews the philosophical roots and the devel-
opment of the concept of creativity in the West and East.
In particular, two conceptions of creativity that originated
in the West – divinely inspired creativity and individual
creativity—are discussed and compared to the two East-
ern conceptions of creativity that are rooted in ancient
Chinese philosophical thought — natural and individual
creativity. Both Western and Eastern conceptions of indi-
vidual creativity come from a theistic or cosmic tradition
of either divinely inspired or natural creativity. However,
a defining feature of the Western concept of creativity—
novelty—is not necessarily embraced by ancient Chinese
concepts of creativity, but does exist in both modern
Eastern conceptions. Reasons for cultural differences are
explored and discussed.
The influence of culture on people’s conceptions of creativity has
been one of the most exciting topics in the recent literature on creativ-
ity. It has attracted the interest of many psychologists (e.g., Lubart,
1999; Niu & Sternberg, 2002; Lau, Hui & Ng, 2004; Rudowicz & Hui,
1997; Rudowicz & Yue, 2000). A review of contemporary research has
shown that people from the East and West hold similar, yet not identi-
cal conceptions of creativity. In general, Easterners are more likely to
view creativity as having social and moral values, and as making a con-
nection between the new and the old. Their Western counterparts
focus more on some special individual characteristics in understanding
the concept of creativity (Niu & Sternberg, 2002).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... For Confucians, the sages are unordinary people who are benevolent, wise, and have the knowledge of Heaven's mandates, and thus they are entitled by Heaven to create all the PANG AND PLUCKER 4 important things for the people. This conception partially parallels Big-C Creativity which emphasizes great creative contributions that are achieved by the few unusual people (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2007;Niu & Sternberg, 2006). However, compared with the Western creative genius, Chinese sages are defined firstly in terms of benevolence (Niu & Sternberg, 2006). ...
... This conception partially parallels Big-C Creativity which emphasizes great creative contributions that are achieved by the few unusual people (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2007;Niu & Sternberg, 2006). However, compared with the Western creative genius, Chinese sages are defined firstly in terms of benevolence (Niu & Sternberg, 2006). ...
... The primary goal of Confucian creative actions is to maintain or restore a stable and harmonious society to improve people's well-being (Chan, 2011). Therefore, Confucian creators or innovators prefer to choose evolutionary or incremental approaches to create, so as not to bring about social chaos which may harm the people (Chan, 2011;Niu & Sternberg, 2006). In contrast, any radical approach to create/innovate, such as Wang Anshi's Political Reform, is usually rejected because it increases the risk of social unrest (J. ...
Article
Full-text available
Confucianism is often criticized as a disincentive for creativity and innovation. This article investigated Confucian conceptions of creativity and innovation from a sociocultural perspective using Glăveanu’s Five A framework. Confucian aims of creation and innovation were lofty, but the range of people who could create was exclusionary, pathways to creative achievements were relatively narrow, and thus fewer creative artifacts were produced by Confucians. At the same time, Confucian thought contains a more nuanced conceptualization of both appropriateness/usefulness and aim/purpose than is found in contemporary, largely Western approaches to creativity and innovation. Based on the conceptual analysis, the authors recommend adding a sixth A, aim, to the 5A sociocultural framework.
... Although such division is a broad dichotomization and can lead to overgeneralization, it is reasonable to propose that distinct cultural perspectives of creativity are embedded in the different philosophical traditions from which they derive (Niu & Sternberg, 2002). Conceptions of creativity in Western thought originated in the Biblical foundations of God's creation as well as Hellenistic philosophy, and more recently from the spirit of humanism and experimentation arising from the European Renaissance, while Eastern conceptions of creativity emanated from the Chinese philosophy of yin-yang (i.e., the renewing changes of nature) (Niu & Sternberg, 2002;Niu & Sternberg, 2006). Although these different cultural roots of creativity are (Kaufman & Sternberg, 2006). ...
Article
This study explores Chinese EFL teachers’ perceptions of what characteristics define a creative pupil, beliefs about the malleability and creative competency of students, viewpoints on creativity’s relationship to academic subject matter, and assumptions of how creativity is facilitated in the EFL classroom. With the primary aim of contributing to the understanding of creativity as a cultural construct, rather than just a cognitive or social concept, this mixed method research design employed an online questionnaire to include teachers working in different regions and diverse educational contexts in China, as well as interviews to better understand and articulate teachers’ viewpoints about creativity in the language classroom. The results of the current study are remarkably inconsistent with the findings of the existing literature that largely support traditional Chinese perceptions and philosophies in relation to creativity, indicating that the concept may be changing in China and/or it may be defined and understood differently in the context of language education. Nonetheless, some traditional perspectives of creativity remain, such as its relationship to academic performance and intellectual aptitude. The study holds important implications for highlighting teachers’ conceptualizations of creativity and thus helping to illuminate and advance a culturally situated understanding of creativity in the Chinese EFL classroom.
... At a cultural level, cross-cultural studies reveal that creativity is universally perceived in terms of novelty and value, although the definition of novelty and value may vary among cultural groups. Research on cultural dimensions, such as individualism/collectivism, highlights differences in the level of creative activity, the sources of creative work (individuals or groups), and the domains in which creativity is valued across societies (Amabile, 1983;Niu & Sternberg, 2006). Although there is a lack of meta-analyses on the effects of a creative school climate on students' creative achievements in the literature, this may be due to the challenges associated with defining and measuring creative accomplishments. ...
Article
Full-text available
Creativity plays a crucial role in fostering innovative thinking and driving socioeconomic development, particularly in developing countries like Zambia. However, it remains unclear to what extent schools, especially teachers, encourage and harness their students' creativity. This study had to achieve two objectives: first, to determine the behaviours of teachers that promote creativity among their students, and second, to evaluate the impact of teacher characteristics such as age, academic qualifications, level of teaching, and gender on their ability to nurture creativity. The study involved forty-eight in-service teachers who were enrolled in a degree programme at the University of Zambia. These teachers were administered with the Creativity Nurturing Behaviour Scale for Teachers, which assessed their overall creativity and specific components such as abstraction, creative thinking, motivation for creativity, and inquisitiveness during classroom interactions. The results revealed that teachers demonstrated slightly above-average abilities in fostering creativity, with motivation for creativity being the most effectively nurtured trait, while creative thinking appeared to be the least effectively nurtured. Teacher characteristics had only modest effects on enhancing creativity among learners. Interestingly, age and level of teaching showed an inverse relationship with creativity, as younger teachers were found to be more effective than their older counterparts, and primary school teachers were better at fostering creativity compared to secondary school teachers. Additionally, female teachers were found to be more adept at nurturing creativity than male teachers. These findings contradict the results observed in individualistic Western societies but align with GSJ: Volume 12, Issue 9, September 2024 ISSN 2320-9186 1051 GSJ© 2024 www.globalscientificjournal.com those from more collectivistic societies. This study provides valuable insights for policymakers and educators regarding the abilities of teachers to nurture creativity in Zambia.
... A school culture that encourages innovation, tolerates failure, and values the process rather than focusing solely on outcomes can provide students with a safe space to experiment, thereby fostering the development of creativity [28]. Additionally, societal culture profoundly impacts individual creativity, as cultural values, societal expectations, and attitudes towards innovation determine the extent to which creativity is accepted by society and the motivation of individuals to engage in creative activities [29]. Societal support and rewards for innovation can motivate individuals to pursue innovation, while tolerance for failure and understanding of the innovation process are important conditions for cultivating creativity. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study utilizes a literature review methodology to explore creativity in educational management. We searched for relevant articles using three key words: "creativity, " "educational management, " and "innovation in education. " Gathering data from a wide range of academic databases, the research delves deeply into the definitions of creativity and its theoretical models from perspectives of psychology, education, and management, such as the Four P Model, Systems Model, Input-Process-Output Model, and Dual Pathway Model. The review identifies key areas where creativity is applied within educational management, including the development of policies that promote creative environments, leadership styles, and teaching methods. It also discusses the influence of personal and psychological factors, as well as environmental and cultural factors, on creativity. Current research trends highlight an emphasis on interdisciplinary approaches, integration of digital technology, innovation in assessment methods, and the impact of educational policies, underlining the necessity for personalized learning paths. Lastly, the article calls for future research to expand the literature base, employ mixed methods, engage in cross-cultural comparative studies, explore new technologies, and develop new tools for assessing creativity, to more comprehensively understand and foster creativity development within the context of educational management.
... These differences focus on how people perform creatively and how they assess creativity. From the literature it can be concluded that Western countries focus more on novelty, innovation and individual success, whereas Eastern countries are more likely to view creativity collectively, having social and moral values, with a connection made between the new and the old (Niu and Sternberg 2006). The differences between East and West are related to the collectivist orientation of the East and the individualistic origin of the West (Lubart 2010;Ng 2001;Niu and Sternberg 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined generative artificial intelligences (GenAIs), as popularized by ChatGPT, in standardized creativity tests. Benchmarking GenAI against human performance, the results showed that ChatGPT demonstrated remarkable fluency in content generation, though the creative output was average. The random nature of AI creativity and the dependency on the richness of the training database require a reassessment of traditional creativity metrics, especially for AI. Our findings highlight the integral role humans play in guiding AI to foster genuine originality, suggesting the need for future research in human–AI co-creation and the development of robust AI creativity measurement mechanisms.
Chapter
The chapter considers how language sparks discovery and innovation by examining creativity and problem-solving through the unique vantage point of multilingualism. The chapter begins with an overview of how creativity and problem-solving are operationalized and measured, followed by a review of how multilingualism impacts the ability to innovate and solve problems. The relationship between multilingualism and creativity is modulated by proficiency and age of second language acquisition. Similarly, performance on problem-solving tasks depends on which language multilinguals use and on their proficiency level in each language. The final section discusses multilingualism, creativity, and problem-solving in real-world settings, as well as potential future directions, concluding with the suggestion that knowing multiple languages can lead to more creative outcomes and better problem-solving skills.
Article
Cross‐cultural studies on creativity, mainly focusing on the creative potential aspect (e.g., divergent thinking), are emerging in recent years. However, the creative self‐belief (e.g., creative self‐efficacy) aspect of creativity is under‐researched cross‐culturally. Moreover, studies that address the measurement invariance of creativity assessments to ensure the measures' sound psychometric properties cross‐culturally are rare. Thus, we aimed to address (a) the measurement invariance and (b) the mean comparisons of divergent thinking and creative self‐efficacy between American and Chinese adults in two studies. Study 1 investigated four divergent thinking (DT) tests (Line Meanings, Uses, Instances, and Consequences) between American ( n = 341) and Chinese ( n = 345) college students. Multi‐group confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) for fluency‐supported scalar invariance based on a three‐factor model (removing one Instances item) and those on originality‐supported partial scalar invariance (freeing Uses intercepts) based on a three‐factor model. American respondents exhibited higher latent means on fluency and originality compared to their Chinese counterparts. Study 2 investigated a creative self‐efficacy (CSE) scale between American ( n = 302) and Chinese ( n = 316) college students. Multi‐group CFA supported scalar invariance based on a one‐factor model (removing one item). Latent mean comparisons showed no difference in CSE across the two groups.
Article
Where does creativity come from and what is its purpose ? The paper revisits these ever‐turning questions to correct the prevalent but, arguably, inaccurate historical interpretation of creativity as a concept that emerged in modernity. First, I substantiate that a close study of the ancient Greek texts suggests that although creativity seems to occupy a peripheral position in the ancient philosophical thinking, it is a central, yet, hidden, subject of speculation. Second, I overturn the worn‐out view that the ancient Greek philosophy conceives creativity as a God‐given talent breathed into a fortunate few. Last, I demonstrate that a more encompassing understanding of creativity is implied by several Greek philosophers. To achieve this, (a) I discuss the contemporary interpretations of the ancient notion; (b) I examine the historical origin and etymology of the Greek term demiourgein (“to create”) to discuss its relevant uses; and (c) I develop a nuanced framework for conceptualizing ancient creativity and its interrelation to human agency, politics, and social life.