ArticlePDF Available

Applied Issues in the Construction and Expert Assessment of Photo Lineups

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Issues surrounding lineup fairness have been explored scientifically for over two decades. The present study investigates the applied/external validity of this line of research. First, several factors leading to bias in the construction of photo lineups are examined, and results of a preliminary survey on current law-enforcement practices are presented. Several statistics that have been developed to assess the fairness of lineups are reviewed and the application of these techniques to lineups used in 18 criminal cases is discussed, including the mixed agreement that sometimes occurs between estimates. Finally, we address the usefulness of lineup fairness assessment for expert testimony in the courtroom, and the dilemma that may be faced by the expert witness who is asked to testify by the defence. It is suggested that a useful and empirically justified index of overall lineup fairness can be created by combining a single estimate of bias (Functional Size) and a single estimate of lineup size (Effective Size) into a four-point index. Copyright
Content may be subject to copyright.
Applied Issues in the Construction and Expert Assessment
of Photo Lineups
JOHN C. BRIGHAM,* CHRISTIAN A. MEISSNER
and ADINA W. WASSERMAN
Florida State University, USA
SUMMARY
Issues surrounding lineup fairness have been explored scienti®cally for over two decades. The
present study investigates the applied/external validity of this line of research. First, several
factors leading to bias in the construction of photo lineups are examined, and results of a
preliminary survey on current law-enforcement practices are presented. Several statistics that
have been developed to assess the fairness of lineups are reviewed and the application of these
techniques to lineups used in 18 criminal cases is discussed, including the mixed agreement that
sometimes occurs between estimates. Finally, we address the usefulness of lineup fairness
assessment for expert testimony in the courtroom, and the dilemma that may be faced by the
expert witness who is asked to testify by the defence. It is suggested that a useful and
empirically justi®ed index of overall lineup fairness can be created by combining a single
estimate of bias (Functional Size) and a single estimate of lineup size (Eective Size) into a
four-point index. Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
The `fairness' of a lineup is a crucial piece of information that is taken into account
when evaluating the likely validity of a disputed eyewitness identi®cation. The
concept of fairness may be encountered at the time of construction by law-
enforcement ocers, or subsequently in court when the defence may raise it as an
issue of justice. Before the 1970s, the only way that lineup fairness could be assessed
was by `eyeballing' and making a purely subjective, global judgement. The ®rst
empirical test of lineup fairness was proposed by Doob and Kirschenbaum in 1973.
Since that time, several additional estimates of lineup fairness have been developed.
Much debate has surrounded the usefulness of these statistics, especially in relation to
their use by expert witnesses in actual cases. This paper will begin by reviewing factors
that introduce bias in lineups at the construction phase, and preliminary data on a
survey of current police procedures will be discussed. Following this, the empirical
assessment of fairness indices will be examined, after which we will describe the
application of these techniques to 18 criminal cases that involved disputed eyewitness
identi®cations. Finally, the dilemma faced by the expert witness will be addressed.
CCC 0888±4080/99/SI0S73±20 $17.50
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
*Correspondence to: John C. Brigham, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,
FL 32306-1270, USA. E-mail: brigham@psy.fsu.edu
THE CONCEPTS OF LINEUP SIZE AND LINEUP BIAS
In theory, a lineup is seen as fair to the suspect when it contains a sucient number of
distractors (foils) who are similar in appearance to the general description of the
criminal (Doob and Kirschenbaum, 1973). Two dimensions of lineup fairness have
been proposed (Malpass, 1981; Malpass and Devine, 1983). First, the concept of
lineup size suggests that a lineup should be large enough that the probability of a
chance identi®cation of an innocent suspect is relatively low. In the USA lineups
containing six members (the suspect and ®ve foils) have generally been viewed as the
minimally acceptable lineup size. But not just any six-person lineup is of acceptable
size. It is critical that the ®ve foils be reasonably plausible alternatives for the choice
task (i.e. similar in appearance to the criminal's description). For example, if three of
the foils are so dissimilar from the prior description of the perpetrator that they do
not represent viable alternatives, then it is as if the eyewitness were in reality facing a
three-person lineup containing the suspect and the two remaining foils.
Lineup bias, in contrast, refers to the extent that the suspect is distinctive from the
other lineup members. Anything which causes the suspect to stand out from the other
lineup members compromises the validity of the lineup, as this distinctiveness may be
used by an uncertain eyewitness as a cue on which to base the identi®cation decision.
These can involve personal characteristics of the target as well as characteristics of the
lineup itself. Examples might include aspects of the suspect's physical appearance
(e.g. a blond suspect among ®ve dark-haired foils, a heavyset suspect among ®ve thin
foils, etc.), or of his photo (e.g. its size, texture, background). Any such factor that sets
the defendant apart from other lineup members in a systematic way compromises the
validity of the lineup.
One way to describe this issue is to distinguish between positive and negative bias.
Positive bias represents a situation in which the suspect is likely to be selected from the
lineup by an eyewitness even if he or she has a poor memory of the suspect, because
the suspect or his photo is distinctive in some way. Conversely, negative bias occurs
when the suspect is relatively unlikely to be selected by an eyewitness, even if the
eyewitness has a good memory of the event, because the other lineup members are so
similar to him (i.e. `clones'). Presumably, the lineup task will be most veridical as a
measure of memory when neither type of bias is present. Research to date has focused
most upon ways in which to assess the degree of positive bias in lineup construction,
administration, and assessment.
The concepts of lineup size and bias may also overlap with each other. For example,
if the suspect is the only lineup member who ®ts the description of the criminal, the
lineup is of inadequate size because the suspect is the only viable choice, in terms of
the description of the culprit. In a psychological sense, the size of this lineup is not six
members, it is one. From the eyewitness's perspective, this situation is akin to the
showup procedure wherein a suspect is shown alone to the eyewitness who is asked, `Is
this the guy?' US courts have consistently ruled that showups are impermissibly
suggestive because the chance of misidenti®cation is too great (Brigham, 1989).
CONTROLLING BIAS IN LINEUP CONSTRUCTION
The genesis of bias in any lineup is at the point of construction. Researchers have noted
many factors that appear to in¯uence the subsequent veridicality of a lineup
S74 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
administration. As mentioned previously, optimal fairness would be represented in a
photo array consisting of neither positive nor negative bias toward the suspect. In other
words, what is desired is a `reasonable' test of the witness's memory for the target; a test
in which the suspect is neither too distinctive nor one of several `clones' in the array.
Investigator bias and the eect of race
One source of bias may involve the sentiments of the investigator in charge of the
case, as often it is this individual who constructs the photo array. While the
importance of creating an unbiased array is undoubtedly considered, pressures to
obtain a positive identi®cation from the witness may counter this notion, and thereby
in¯uence the degree of diculty created in the lineup. While there is no empirical
evidence as to the validity of this supposition, variation in the bias of lineups used in
real cases indicates the possibility of such a factor.
Additionally, law ocers' race appears to impact their evaluations of lineups in a
systematic way. In a study in which Brigham and Brandt (1992) asked ocers to rate
the `usefulness' of 23 lineups ranging on a continuum from fair to unfair, White ocers
found the lineups more useful as a whole (68 per cent of the time they were classed as
useful) than did Black ocers (56 per cent useful; zfor proportions 3.86, p50.001).
The eect of ocers' race was most evident on evaluations of Black lineups, which
were rated as useful more often by the White ocers (64 per cent of the time) than by
the Black ocers (49 per cent of the time; zfor proportions 3.22, p50.01).
This ®nding is somewhat consistent with an earlier ®nding by Brigham and Ready
(1985), who found that individuals made ®ner distinctions in evaluating lineups
containing members of their own race compared with lineups containing other-race
individuals. Both Whites and Blacks found it easier to locate `similar' foils for other-
race lineups than for same-race lineups, presumably because the `own-race bias' that
occurs in facial memory (Bothwell et al., 1989) had an eect here. The stereotypic
perception that `they [other-race persons] all look alike' apparently operates in lineup
construction as well as in recognition memory.
Recommendations for guarding against investigator bias in lineup construction
appear fairly straightforward. An investigator who feels a strong sense of immediacy
to obtain a positive identi®cation from an eyewitness should not participate in
construction of the photo array. Likewise, an ocer of the same race as the suspect
should construct the lineup to guard against the selection of dissimilar foils that may
occur in cross-race comparisons.
Further, as pointed out by Wells et al. (1998), the person who administers the
lineup should not know which lineup member is the suspect. Thus, in experimental
terms, a double-blind procedure should be used in which both the administrator of the
lineup and the eyewitness are unaware of whether the suspect is present in the photo
array. Such a procedure also appears to protect against `con®rmatory feedback' from
the person administrating the lineup, which Wells and Brad®eld (1998) have
demonstrated can dramatically aect a witness's reconstruction of the events and later
testimony regarding their identi®cation of the suspect.
Distinctivess of suspect
A second concern revolves around the issue of suspect distinctiveness. As noted by
Brigham et al. (1990), it can be an arduous task to assemble a lineup that contains a
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S75
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
suspect and an adequate number of fair foils. This task is made even more dicult
when the suspect is distinctive or unusual in appearance. Indeed, this situation
occurred in the Brigham et al. (1990) study, where the researchers were unable to
compose a fair lineup for one very distinctive target person.
In Neil v. Biggers (1972), one of a handful of US Supreme Court cases that created
case law on the utilization of eyewitness evidence, law ocers used a one-person
`showup' as the identi®cation task, rather than the usual six- or eight-person lineup.
The police claimed that they had earlier checked the city jail and the city juvenile
home for persons to serve as foils in a live lineup, but could ®nd no one at either place
®tting the suspect's unusual physical description. The US Supreme Court upheld the
conviction, ruling that while the showup procedure may have been suggestive, under
the `totality of circumstances' standard the victim's identi®cation of the suspect was
reliable and was properly allowed to go to the jury. Among the circumstances
enumerated by the Court was the fact that the victim earlier failed to make any
positive identi®cations from 30 to 40 photographs that comprised several photo
lineups and photo showups, and that she had expressed `no doubt' of the correctness
of her identi®cation from the ®nal live showup.
Target/foil similarity and bias
One ®nal issue involves the degree of similarity between lineup foils and the target. It
seems logical to assume that increasing the degree of target±foil similarity would
decrease the chance of positive bias towards the suspect (as de®ned earlier), but that
too much similarity would create negative bias, making the identi®cation task too
dicult. Interestingly enough, though, there is some evidence that increased target ±
foil similarity may not increase negative bias. Laughery et al. (1988) reported results
from a study in which ®ve computerized foils were created by varying only one facial
feature from the original target. Results indicated that positive bias was still present
when using these high-similarity foils, as the target was chosen by mock witnesses as
the most `familiar' of the faces at a rate greater than chance. Similar results were
obtained using actual photographs of faces, in which the lineup was created by
matching foils to the suspect (Wogalter et al., 1992).
These results suggest that a positive bias, and not a negative bias, may occur from
creating a lineup in which the foils are very similar to the target. Alternative methods
to creating lineups in which foils were chosen based on similarities to both the target
and other foils produced signi®cantly less biased lineups (Marwitz and Wogalter,
1988; Wogalter et al., 1991, 1992). Based upon this evidence, Koehnken et al. (1996)
suggested that the similarity±fairness function appears to take on an inverted U-
shape, such that both high and low extremes of similarity can create a positive bias.
Fairness lies somewhere in between, and probably, according to Koehnken et al.,
more toward the high-similarity end of the spectrum.
Luus and Wells (1991) argued for an alternative method in which foils should be
chosen based upon their similarity to the witness's description of the suspect, and not
the appearance of the suspect, per se. Such a method relies upon the knowledge the
witness held at the time of the incident while allowing for variation on features
unmentioned, later termed `propitious heterogeneity' (Wells et al., 1993, 1994). Luus
and Wells argued that a lineup which includes foils that dier on these unmentioned
S76 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
characteristics would increase the likelihood of correct identi®cations while not
increasing misidenti®cations in a target-absent lineup.
However, Koehnken et al. (1996) pointed out several opportunities for error in the
use of this match-to-description procedure. First, descriptions of faces are notoriously
vague or exceedingly general, making the interpretation of the description a subjective
task. Furthermore, if a witness is unable to speci®cally describe characteristics of a
suspect, this may increase the chance that the description may be misinterpreted by
the ocer constructing the lineup. Another potential problem is the inclusion of foils
that ®t the description, but do not resemble the suspect in any way. Finally, com-
plications may also arise if dierent eyewitnesses provide dissimilar descriptions of the
criminal (e.g. see Table 2). This would entail constructing a separate lineup for each
eyewitness's description, an additional step that law ocers might be reluctant to
take.
To compensate for the problems of using either method exclusively (i.e. matching-
to-target or matching-to-description), Koehnken et al. (1996) argued for a two-part
procedure in which foils are ®rst selected on the basis of objective features that are
generally stated in the witness's description (i.e. height, weight, race, build, hair style,
etc.). Next, available foils meeting the ®rst criterion should be selected for lineup
membership based upon more subjective ratings of similarity. While foils that are
nearly identical to the target person should not be selected (thus avoiding what Luus
and Wells, 1991, labelled the `clone paradox'), those with `sucient similarity' should
be sought, such that no combination of features would make the target stand out
conspicuously from the other lineup members.
Current law-enforcement procedures
While a great deal of research has addressed the notion of proper construction tech-
niques, no data are yet available on the actual procedures investigators use in daily
practice. To gather some preliminary data on this issue, we surveyed investigators
(N27) in two urban police departments and one county sheri's department in the
state of Florida who create lineups for the cases on which they work. Ocers
responded to questions regarding their procedures in constructing photo lineups, and
their beliefs concerning an appropriate level of diculty for the eyewitness.
When asked `How many foils [in a six-person lineup] should there be that closely
resemble the suspect, in terms of facial similarity, for it to be a reasonable measure of
the witness's memory?' 67 per cent responded `®ve' and 22 per cent `four'. While
Brigham et al. (1990) contended that a lineup should contain a minimum of three
viable alternatives in a six-person lineup, Malpass (1981) argued for a more conserva-
tive criterion of ®ve acceptable members. Most investigators seemed to agree with this
more conservative approach.
When asked the source on which they usually relied to choose lineup foils, two-
thirds responded `the appearance of the suspect' and one-third used some com-
bination of `the suspect's appearance and the witness's description' (the latter
procedure is consistent with what Koehnken et al., 1996, suggest). None relied solely
on the eyewitness's description, although some researchers (e.g. Luus and Wells,
1991) have argued that this would be the best procedure.
We also asked `When constructing a lineup, how dicult do you attempt to make
the identi®cation of the suspect?' Almost all respondents (96 per cent) responded 4 or
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S77
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
5 on a 7-point scale where 7 represented `very dicult'. Finally, in an attempt to gauge
feelings about a reasonable test of eyewitness memory, we asked `Hypothetically
speaking, if 100 witnesses with a good to very good chance at viewing the suspect were
presented a six-person lineup, what number should fail to identify the suspect for it to
be considered a reasonable assessment of the witness's memory?' Responses ranged
from 5 per cent to 80 per cent, with modal responses of 20 per cent and 10 per cent.
Hence, these law ocers appeared cognizant of the fact that, if a measure is to provide
a reasonable assessment of memory, there will be some associated `cost' (i.e. cases in
which an eyewitness does not identify the perpetrator when he is in the lineup).
This preliminary assessment of construction practices used by investigators in the
®eld suggests that they are somewhat sensitive to issues of lineup fairness. However, as
with any survey assessment, there remains the possibility that demand characteristics
may have in¯uenced individual responses. Additionally, it is yet undetermined
whether these issues are actually considered by investigators during everyday lineup
construction. Further training for ocers on the issues of lineup fairness, and
departmental adoption of the procedures discussed in this section, would greatly
improve both the precision and fairness with which suspects are identi®ed by an
eyewitness.
ASSESSING LINEUP FAIRNESS
Lineup size
The concept of lineup size is based on the premise that lineups should be large enough
to ensure that the probability of a chance identi®cation of an innocent suspect is low
(Malpass and Devine, 1983). A lineup's size is dependent upon the number of
`acceptable' members it contains (i.e. members who are similar in general appearance
to the suspect's description). To date, lineup fairness has most often been assessed on
the basis of responses from `mock witnesses' who have not observed the crime, but
who attempt to identify the target (suspect) from a lineup based solely on a descrip-
tion of the criminal's general appearance. A maximally fair six-person lineup would
be one in which mock witnesses selected the suspect and the ®ve foils equally often.
Assessments of lineup size, then, analyse how many lineup members were acceptable,
in terms of being selected relatively often by the mock witnesses (Table 1). However,
increasing the nominal size (number of members) of a lineup does not necessarily
reduce the risk of a false identi®cation of an innocent suspect, if additional lineup
members are not similar enough to the suspect to represent plausible alternatives
(Malpass, 1981). The relevance of lineup size for investigating eyewitness accuracy is
re¯ected in the two assessment techniques below.
Eective size technique
The Eective Size (ES) technique is based on the supposition that lineup foils who are
selected by mock witnesses at a level below that expected by chance are unacceptable
for inclusion in the lineup (Malpass, 1981; Malpass and Devine, 1983). In order to
obtain the ES of a lineup, the nominal size of the lineup is ®rst adjusted to re¯ect the
removal of any zero cells (members not chosen by any of the mock witnesses). The
chance expectation is then adjusted to re¯ect the new resulting nominal size. The
choice frequencies of those lineup members chosen by mock witnesses less often than
S78 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
expected by chance are subtracted from the adjusted chance expectation. The dier-
ences are then summed, and divided by the adjusted chance frequency. The resulting
®gure is subtracted from the lineup's nominal size. A lineup is then considered fair if
the ES is equal to or greater than half the original nominal size of the lineup (Brigham
et al., 1990; Brigham and Pfeifer, 1994). Examples of Eective Size values of lineups
used in criminal cases are presented later in Table 2.
Number of acceptable lineup members technique
A potential disadvantage of the ES technique is that it may not be easily understood
by laypersons or judicial ocials because of its numerical transformations and
distance from the `raw data'. Hence, Malpass and Devine (1983) suggested that the
Acceptable Lineup Members (ALM) technique (also termed the Acceptable Foils
Technique ± Brigham and Pfeifer, 1994) better meets these criteria than does ES. The
ALM estimate is derived by counting the number of lineup members that were
selected by mock witnesses with a frequency that exceeds the chance expectation, or
exceeds some percentage of it. The minimum percentage of the chance expectation
that is considered acceptable may be based on value judgements or empirical
standards. Malpass and Devine (1983, p. 93) utilized three dierent levels (50 per cent,
75 per cent, 90 per cent of chance expectation) in their analyses. Brigham et al. (1990)
adopted a criterion of 75 per cent of chance expectation, in which case lineup
members chosen from a 6-person lineup by at least 13 per cent of mock witnesses
(0.75 0.17) would be acceptable.
Lineup bias
Even when a lineup has an adequate size, it still may be biased against the suspect in
that the suspect or his photo is distinctive in comparison to the other lineup members.
Lineup bias has been examined via several dierent estimates.
Table 1. Measures of lineup fairness
Measures of lineup size
(i.e. number of `good' lineup members)
Measures of lineup bias
(i.e. distinctiveness of suspect)
Eective Size (ES):
.Assumes that foils selected by mock
witnesses at below-chance levels are
unacceptable
Proportions technique
.Can use zfor proportions to detect
deviations from expected chance rate of
selection
.Signi®cance aected by # of mock witnesses
.Also insensitive to addition to `bad' foils
Number of Acceptable Lineup Members
(ALM):
.Those lineup members selected at 75%
(or 50%, 25%, etc.) or more of chance
expectation by mock witnesses are seen as
acceptable
Functional Size (FS):
.Parameter estimation procedure:
#mock witnesses
#mock witnesses choosing suspect
Suspect bias:
.Similar to proportions technique, but uses
ES instead of nominal size as a basis for
calculation
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S79
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
Proportions technique
Doob and Kirschenbaum (1973) originally described an assessment of lineup bias
which compares the proportion of time the suspect is selected by mock witnesses with
the proportion of choices expected by chance alone (0.17 in a six-person lineup). If the
suspect were selected at a rate signi®cantly greater than chance (z-test for propor-
tions), the lineup would be seen as biased against the suspect. Wells et al. (1979) noted
several limitations of this hypothesis-testing procedure. First, the likelihood of
obtaining a signi®cant dierence is aected by the number of mock witnesses as well
as by the magnitude of the dierence between the proportions. Thus, the larger the
sample of mock witnesses, the smaller the dierence needed to achieve statistical
signi®cance.
Second, the hypothesis-testing procedure is not sensitive to the addition of
irrelevant lineup foils. For example, if another four foils were added to a six-person
lineup, the expected proportion of mock witnesses who pick the suspect out of a ten-
person lineup would be 0.10 (1/10). However, if the additional four foils did not draw
any mock-witness choices because they were not similar to the description of the
suspect, then the obtained proportion would be compared with an expected propor-
tion of 0.10, rather than with the original proportion of 0.17. This would increase the
chances that a signi®cant dierence between proportions would occur and that the
lineup would be labelled unfair.
Functional Size
As a result of these potential problems, Wells and colleagues (1979) advocated the use
of a parameter estimation procedure that they labelled Functional Size (FS). FS is
derived by dividing the total number of mock witnesses by the number who chose the
suspect from the lineup. Both the FS estimate and the Proportions technique can be
seen as indicators of lineup bias, rather than lineup size, because they do not take into
account the distribution of foil misidenti®cations (Malpass, 1981).
Suspect Bias technique
In contrast, the Suspect Bias technique
1
takes into consideration the overall distribu-
tion of foil choices, but uses the calculated Eective Size of the lineup, rather than the
lineup's nominal size, as the basis for calculating chance expectancies (Malpass and
Devine, 1983). From this perspective, a photo lineup is seen as biased if the observed
choice frequency for the suspect is signi®cantly dierent (z-test for proportions) from
the adjusted expected choice frequency (1/ES).
Sensitivity and discriminability of lineup fairness estimates
While the comprehensibility of a lineup fairness calculation technique is important,
the accuracy of the technique in assessing lineup fairness is also a crucial factor.
Brigham et al. (1990) asserted that fairness assessments should be evaluated in terms
of both their sensitivity and discriminability. Sensitivity refers to an absolute standard
of acceptability that is inherent in, or attributed to, a statistic. As such, the concept
involves the cuto point, in terms of the calculated size or degree of bias, at which a
1
Malpass and Devine (1983) called this the defendant bias technique, but we prefer suspect bias (cf.
Brigham and Pfeifer, 1994) which more accurately re¯ects the fact that when a lineup identi®cation is
attempted, the suspect has usually not yet been indicted, and hence, is not yet a defendant.
S80 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
given lineup should be classi®ed as unfair. Wells et al. (1979, p. 289) proposed that
researchers should not attempt to specify appropriate levels of sensitivity for fairness
indices, arguing that these value-related distinctions should be left to the courts.
However, other researchers (cf., Brigham et al., 1990; Brigham and Pfeifer, 1994;
Malpass and Devine, 1983) have suggested that appropriate levels of sensitivity can be
estimated. In contrast, discriminability has been de®ned as the ability of a lineup
fairness estimate to distinguish between lineups that are fair and those that are unfair,
as established by some independent criterion.
Empirical assessment of lineup size estimates
In order to assess the sensitivity and discriminability of lineup size estimates, Brigham
et al. (1990) created ®ve `selected' photo lineups (ostensibly fair lineups, as based on
ratings of high foil similarity to the target and on police judgements of foil
appropriateness) and ®ve less-fair `random' lineups (created by randomly matching
with each target photo ®ve foils that had been rated in the middle one-third of
similarity ratings). Results indicated that the Eective Size (ES) technique was able to
statistically discriminate between the selected ( fair) lineups and the random (less fair)
lineups, with the selected lineups showing higher ES values. The Acceptable Lineup
Members (ALM) technique, however, did not signi®cantly discriminate between the
selected and random lineups. Based on these results, Brigham et al. suggested that the
ES technique appeared to be the more useful size statistic.
In terms of sensitivity, Brigham et al. argued for a criterion that an acceptable ES
(denoting a fair lineup) should be more than half of the nominal size (i.e. greater than
3.0 for a six-person lineup). Four of their ®ve selected lineups and three of the ®ve
random lineups were classi®ed as fair by this criterion. Malpass (1981), however,
argued that a six-person lineup should have an ES of at least 5.0 in order to be
considered fair. According to this more stringent criterion, none of the ten lineups
would have been classi®ed as fair according to the ES estimate. The application of
these criteria to actual lineups will be discussed later.
Empirical assessment of lineup bias estimates
As with lineup size, Brigham et al. (1990) also investigated the appropriateness of
assessment techniques for evaluating lineup bias, utilizing the experimental paradigm
described previously. Both bias estimates, Proportions and Suspect Bias, were able to
discriminate between the selected and random lineups. In terms of sensitivity, the
Proportions statistic classi®ed three of the ®ve selected lineups as fair, while all ®ve of
the random lineups diered signi®cantly from chance expectancy. Overall, the
Suspect Bias was able to discriminate between the selected and random lineups.
However, subsequent analyses (see Table 2) have indicated that the Suspect Bias
measure does not have adequate sensitivity, an issue that will be addressed further in
the next section.
Based on this pattern of results, Brigham et al. (1990) concluded that the Propor-
tions technique appeared to be the most useful estimate of lineup bias, showing
considerable discriminability and sensitivity. As discussed previously, however, with
this method the statistical signi®cance of a given dierence between proportions is
directly related to sample size (McNemar, 1969; Wells et al., 1979). Brigham et al.
(1990) used subsamples of 18 mock witnesses each in calculating proportions, ®nding
it to be both practical and somewhat reliable. Additionally, a signi®cant degree of bias
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S81
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
(p50.05 by the zfor proportions test) will be shown whenever the target person in a
six-person lineup is selected by at least 34 per cent of the mock witnesses, thereby
producing a Functional Size of 2.94 or less. In practical terms, this statistical criterion
could be approximated by using a Functional Size of 3.00 or less (N18) as the
criterion for meaningful bias in a six-person lineup.
Relationship of fairness estimates to evaluations made by law-enforcement personnel
The use of mock witnesses's responses as a source of lineup fairness assessments is
often criticized by attorneys and law-enforcement personnel who fail to understand,
or decline to accept, the rationale underlying the various indices. These observers are
critical of a technique that utilizes respondents who have no expertise in lineups and
are not evaluating the entire lineup's acceptability in any direct way. In an attempt to
validate the use of these statistics, Brigham and Brandt (1992) constructed a sample of
lineups that could be evaluated by three sets of subjects performing dierent tasks:
college students acting as mock witnesses, and college students and law ocers
providing personal evaluations of the lineups.
A set of 23 photo lineups were created and categorized into one of three groups: fair
(eight lineups), moderately fair (eight lineups), and least fair (seven lineups). The law
ocers and students ®rst subjectively assessed the global fairness of each lineup and
then evaluated the acceptability of each of the ®ve foils on a 6-point scale. Finally, a
question designed to elicit `estimated proportions' scores was asked. Four general
types of lineup fairness indices were derived from the three samples: global fairness,
lineup bias (Proportions) and two estimates of lineup size (ES and ALM).
Across the 23 lineups, law-enforcement evaluators were signi®cantly more willing
to use the lineups than were the college-student evaluators. With respect to the
most central question, college-student mock-witness responses were predictive
(r(21) 0.42) of how law ocers subjectively evaluated the same lineups based on
the overall fairness estimate (a composite of the three mock witness indices). Two of
the mock witness estimates, Proportions and ES, were consistently related to fairness
indices derived from the other two samples. However, the other mock witness size
estimate, ALM, was not consistently related to the evaluators' responses. This is
consistent with results of Brigham et al. (1990) discussed earlier.
The overall pattern of results indicated that estimates of lineup fairness derived
when college students play the role of mock witnesses are related to direct evaluations
of lineup fairness made by people (law ocers or college students) who evaluate entire
lineups in a more straightforward manner. If one assumes that, on average, law
ocers' direct evaluations of lineup fairness are somewhat valid but subject to various
biasing in¯uences, then the moderate degree of relationship found by Brigham and
Brandt (1992) seems appropriate. That is, we would predict only a moderate
relationship if the samples are responding in terms of the same concept but are
aected by dierent types of external biasing factors.
THE LINEUP EXPERT'S DILEMMA: ESTIMATING FAIRNESS IN
REAL CASES
The validity and utility of lineup fairness estimates is more than an issue of purely
academic interest. For over two decades, some researchers have been utilizing these
S82 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
estimates to assess the fairness of lineups used in actual criminal cases, and some have
tried to present their ®ndings to judges and jurors via expert testimony. Below we will
discuss several aspects of this contentious issue. We begin by analysing the applic-
ability of these estimates to a set of 18 real cases, after which we present estimates of
the frequency with which such analyses have been admitted in court in the past, and
discuss the likely admissibility of such analyses in the future.
Estimating fairness of real lineups: 18 cases
How well do lineup fairness statistics apply to lineups used in actual criminal cases?
This is a dicult issue to assess, but we will present some data that are directly
relevant, based on the analyses of photo lineups (or, in two cases, photos of a live
lineup) that were used in 18 criminal felony prosecutions in the 1980s and 1990s
(Table 2). These were cases in which a defence attorney contacted one of us (JCB)
because the attorney felt that the identi®cation procedures in the case were
unsatisfactory. In 16 of the 18 cases there was at least one eyewitness identi®cation
of the perpetrator, totalling 23 eyewitness descriptions that could be used for match-
to-description analyses. (One of those analyses involved two diering descriptions
given by the same eyewitness at dierent times.) In three cases two dierent lineups
were used by the police, so a total of 26 lineup fairness analyses, using the match-to-
description paradigm, could be conducted across the 18 cases to assess lineup bias
(Proportions, FS, and Suspect Bias) and lineup size (ES and ALM).
In addition, lineup fairness was also assessed for all 20 lineups using a no-
description procedure in which mock witnesses were simply asked which person or
photo looked most distinctive or most dierent from the others. The employment of
`blind' witnesses provided us with an indication of whether a witness, in the absence of
a any memory for the suspect, would respond to the distinctiveness of the individual
( feature-based) or of the photo (lighting, odd markings, pose, etc.).
It should be stressed that these lineups are not presented as a representative sample
of lineups used in criminal cases. Presumably, they are located towards the unfair end
of the continuum, since the attorneys felt it desirable to seek out a researcher to assess
the lineup's fairness. That said, what do the lineup fairness indices tell us about these
lineups? One relevant question is whether the estimates show an appropriate degree of
sensitivity. If they are indiscriminant, such that all lineups are classi®ed as fair or as
unfair, then the statistics would not be useful in the criminal justice setting because
they would not distinguish between more-fair and less-fair lineups. Recall that
Brigham et al. (1990) argued that a lineup fairness estimate denotes a fair lineup when
the value of FS or ES is greater than half of the lineup's nominal size (greater than 3.0
for a six-person lineup), or when the ALM is at least half of the lineup's nominal size.
Statistical signi®cance of the z-test for proportions can be used as the criterion for
signi®cant bias according to the Suspect Bias index.
Under these criteria, the match-to-description analyses found that almost 60 per
cent were classi®ed as biased according to FS, but only 15 per cent were classi®ed as
biased according to the Suspect Bias statistic. Looking at size, 50±60 per cent of the
lineups were classi®ed as unfair due to insucient size according to the ES and ALM
indices. The no-description procedure yielded a somewhat dierent pattern. Here the
lineup bias estimates yielded a designation of unfair in only 5 per cent of the lineups
according to FS, and 35 per cent of the lineups according to the Suspect Bias
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S83
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
Table 2. Assessing the fairness of lineups used in actual cases
Case # Type of case
Assessment
task
Nominal
size
# of mock
witnesses
Estimates of bias Estimates of size
Overall
fairness
index
Proportions
technique
Functional
Size
Suspect
Bias # of ALM
Eective
Size
1 Murder EW 1 6 LIVE/C 18 0.50** 2.00 ns 3 2.89 1
EW 2 15 0.33 3.00 ns 4 3.33 2
EW 3 16 0.50** 2.00 ns 3 3.50 2
ND 29 0.38** 2.64 ns 4 3.86 2
2 Murder EW 6 B/W 66 0.26 3.94 ns 4 3.87 4
ND 28 0.08 14.00 ns 1#2.07 3
3 Murder EW 1 6 B/W 16 0.66*** 1.52 bias*2 2.75 1
EW 2 17 0.44** 2.27 ns 3 3.71 2
ND 64 0.30** 3.37 ns 3 4.03 4
4 Murder CD 6 B/W 58 0.66*** 1.53 bias*2 2.69 1
ND 89 0.37*** 2.70 ns 23.62 2
5 Bank robbery EW 7 COLOUR 43 0.53*** 1.72 ns 2 1.81 1
ND 28 0.43*** 2.33 ns 24.36 2
6 Bank robbery ND 7 B/W 46 0.17 5.75 ns 4 3.52 4
7 Armed robbery EW 6 B/W 31 0.23 4.43 ns 2 2.97 3
ND 34 0.15 6.80 ns 3 2.91 3
8 Armed robbery CD 6 B/W 22 0.41** 2.44 ns 2 2.36 1
ND 34 0.03 34.00 ns 2#2.74 3
9 Armed robbery CD 6 COLOUR 47 0.23 4.27 ns 4 4.66 4
CD 4 LIVE/C 46 0.50** 2.00 ns 2 2.61 2
ND 33 0.15 6.60 ns 3#2.58 4
10 Rape ND 8 B/W 88 0.53*** 1.89 bias*** 3 3.76 1
11 Sexual assault EW 6 COLOUR 12 0.75*** 1.33 ns 2 1.75 1
ND 30 0.41*** 2.45 ns 23.04 2
EW 6 LIVE/C 12 0.83*** 1.20 ns 2 1.33 1
ND 30 0.33** 3.00 ns 4 3.44 2
S84 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
12 Sexual battery EW 8 B/W 56 0.68*** 1.47 bias*** 2 3.75 1
ND 53 0.04 26.50 ns 3#4.59 4
13 Sexual battery EW 6 COLOUR 37 0.73*** 1.37 bias*** 2 2.35 1
ND 34 0.29 3.40 ns 3 4.06 4
14 Sexual battery EW 1a{6 COLOUR 49 0.18 5.44 ns 4 4.35 4
EW 1b{28 0.21 4.67 ns 3 2.61 3
ND 28 0.82** 1.22 ns 2 1.54 1
15 Burglary and
assault
EW 6 B/W 25 0.88*** 1.14 ns 1 1.37 1
ND 34 0.26 3.78 ns 4 4.06 4
EW 6 B/W 34 0.35** 2.83 ns 4 3.53 2
ND 33 0.06 16.50 ns 4#4.45 4
16 Burglary and
assault
EW 6 B/W 22 0.32 3.14 ns 23.59 4
ND 33 0.09 11.00 ns 1#2.82 3
17 Burglary EW 1 8 B/W 26 0.19 5.20 ns 3 3.69 3
EW 2 27 0.37 2.70 ns 4 4.85 2
EW 3 22 0.18 5.50 ns 4 5.87 4
ND 34 0.03 34.00 ns 2#3.23 3
18 Lewd and
lascivious
behaviour
EW 1 8 B/W 15 0.07 15.00 ns 5#6.14 4
EW 2 19 0.16 6.33 ns 4 5.21 4
ND 33 0.24 4.13 ns 2 3.09 3
Notes: Measures in bold denote an unfair lineup according to the criteria suggested by Brigham et al. (1990).
EW: eyewitness description; CD: composite description; ND: no description.
LIVE/C: coulour photograph of live lineup; COLOUR: colour photo array; B/W; black and white photo array.
ns: non-signi®cant degree of bias against suspect; ALM: Acceptable Lineup Members.
***p50.001; **p50.01; *p50.05; #target was not an ALM; {two descriptions given by the same eyewitness.
Overall fairness index values (1 most unfair; 2 unfair; 3 somewhat unfair; 4 fair).
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S85
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
technique. The no-description analyses found lineups to be of insucient size 40 per
cent of the time according to the ES estimate, and 55 per cent of the time according to
the ALM estimate (Table 3).
If we look at the degree of agreement for the fair/unfair dichotomous classi®ca-
tions, a somewhat dierent picture emerges. The two bias estimates yielded dierent
categorizations almost half the time for the match-to-description analyses and about
one-third of the time for the no-description analyses. On the other hand, the degree of
agreement in fairness categorizations between the two size estimates (ALM and ES)
was considerably greater, with 80± 90 per cent agreement across the cases (Table 4).
The lack of agreement between the two estimates of bias was mostly a re¯ection of the
fact that the Suspect Bias statistic was much less sensitive to lineup unfairness than
was Functional Size, as illustrated in Table 3. It appears that when a given lineup's
size is relatively low (i.e. less that half its nominal size), chance frequency increases
such that the proportion of mock witnesses selecting the suspect must be extremely
high (i.e. 50±60 per cent) to yield a signi®cant zfor proportions dierence.
What about agreement across the fairness estimates: did two statistics relevant to
lineup size (ES and ALM) and an estimate of lineup bias (Proportions, on which FS is
based) yield roughly equivalent scores across the set of lineups? (The Suspect Bias
Table 3. Percentage of lineups used in actual cases classi®ed as
`unfair'
Match-to-description
paradigm (N26)
No-description
paradigm (N20)
Lineup bias
Functional Sizea58.8% 35.0%
Suspect Biasb15.4% 5.0%
Lineup size
Eective Sizec50.0% 40.0%
# of ALMd1 60.0% 55.0%
aCriterion FS is half or less of the lineup's nominal size
bCriterion Statistical signi®cance of zfor proportions test
cCriterion ES is half or less of the lineup's nominal size
dCriterion # if ALM is less than half of the lineup's nominal size
1ALM: Acceptable Lineup Members
Table 4. Within-linup agreement for estimates of lineup size and lineup bias
Lineup bias statistics
(FS and SB)
Lineup size statistics
(ES and ALM)
Lineup fair
(both) Mixed
Lineup unfair
(both)
Lineup fair
(both) Mixed
Lineup unfair
(both)
Match-to-description
paradigma
38.5% 46.2% 15.4% 46.2% 11.5% 42.3%
No-description
paradigmb
65.0% 30.0% 5.0% 40.0% 20.0% 40.0%
Notes:a26 analyses; b20 analyses.
FS: Functional Size; SB: Suspect Bias.
ES: Eective Size; ALM: Number of Acceptable Lineup Members.
S86 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
technique does not yield an equivalent score that can be correlated with the others.)
As illustrated in Table 5, correlational analyses indicated that the answer is `yes' when
the match-to-description procedure was used, but `no' when the no-description
format was utilized. The three estimates correlated strongly with each other (r's above
0.75) across the actual cases and also across the 23 made-for-research lineups used by
Brigham and Brandt (1992), when descriptions were used. Under the no-description
procedure, however, the bias estimate (Proportions scores) was not signi®cantly
related to the two size estimates, ES, r(20) ÿ0.07, and the ALM score,
r(20) 0.04. Because the Proportions score focuses solely on mock witness responses
to the target person (bias), while the other two indices evaluate responses across all
the lineup members (size), it is entirely possible for a lineup to appear fair in terms of
Proportions because few mock witnesses chose the target person, but unfair in terms
of ES and ALM because responses focused mostly on one or two other lineup
members.
Given that size and bias statistics assess dierent features of a lineup, how should
these estimates be used together in coming to an overall forensic evaluation of a
lineup's worth? Ideally, a lineup should be both of sucient size and without bias.
But, from the perspective of the accused, positive bias is the most critical issue. If the
lineup is positively biased (i.e. the suspect is distinctive in some way) then it will
probably be de®cient in size as well ± this was the case for 52 per cent of the lineups
that were assessed as positively biased in Table 2. For the remaining 48 per cent of the
biased lineups, one would still argue that the lineup is fundamentally unfair to the
suspect, even though insucient size itself was not a problem. But what about lineups
that have inadequate size but are not positively biased, because mock witness choices
were focused not on the suspect but on one or two lineup foils (e.g. Case 18 based on
the eyewitness's description, and Cases 2, 8, 16, and 17 based on no-description in
Table 5. Relationship (correlations) between measures
of lineup size and bias
Eyewitness's description used
Actual cases: 26 descriptions
Eective Size # of ALM
Proportions ÿ0.77*** ÿ0.76***
Eective Size ± 0.80***
Research-created fair and less-fair lineups:
23 lineups and descriptions (Brigham and Brandt, 1992)
Eective Size # of ALM
Proportions ÿ0.83*** ÿ0.80***
Eective Size ± 0.85***
No description used
Actual cases: 20 lineups
Eective Size # of ALM
Proportions ÿ0.07 ÿ0.04
Eective Size ± 0.59***
Note: *** p50.001; # of ALM: number of Acceptable Lineup
Members.
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S87
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
Table 2)? It is less clear what one might argue in this situation. The lineup is not ideal
in that there are less than three viable choice options, but the suspect is not one of the
viable choice options, according to the pattern of mock witness responses.
It seems to us that overall lineup fairness may best be estimated by combining size
and bias estimates. Given that bias appears to be the most important factor, the order
from most unfair type of lineup to fair would be: most unfair ± both positive bias and
inadequate size; unfair ± positive bias only; somewhat unfair ± inadequate size only;
and fair ± no bias and adequate size. In order to utilize such a scale, one would need to
specify a single bias statistic and a single size estimate. We suggest the use of
Functional Size as the estimator of bias; it does not rely on sample size as the
Proportions statistic does, and it has greater sensitivity than the Suspect Bias statistic.
As a size estimator, we suggest the use of Eective Size, which previous research
(Brigham and Brandt, 1992; Brigham et al., 1990) found to be a more discriminant
tool for detecting unfairness due to size than was the ALM. A potential disadvantage
of ES is that it is not easily understandable to laypersons and could prove confusing if
an expert attempted to explain it in detail to a judge or jury (cf. Malpass and Devine,
1983). Nevertheless, Eective Size appears to be the most appropriate estimator of
lineup size based upon its statistical properties.
2
Utilizing these two estimates, FS and
ES, produces the overall fairness categorizations in the far-right column of Table 2.
In multiple-eyewitness cases, where there is more than one description on which
mock-witness judgements can be based, there is the possibility that overall fairness
evaluations may dier depending on which descriptions the mock witnesses are given.
Four of the cases (1, 3, 17, and 18) in Table 2 involved more than one eyewitness's
description. In two of these cases the overall fairness estimate was the same regardless
of which description was analysed, while Case 3 yielded somewhat dierent estimates
in the degree of unfairness (very unfair; unfair). The only major between-witness
dierence involved Case 17, which yielded estimates of unfair, somewhat unfair, and
fair depending upon which of the three descriptions was used.
Comparing the match-to-description estimates with estimates from the no-
description format, it is evident that a lineup may be judged unfair by both sets of
analyses (e.g. Cases 1, 4, 5, and 11), or fair by one set of analyses but not the other.
Interestingly, in no cases was the lineup categorized as fair by all the analyses.
Focusing on a single estimate of bias (FS) and a single estimate of lineup size (ES)
allows one to derive a meaningful overall estimate of lineup fairness. In contrast, if
one attempts to assess all ®ve proposed estimates of lineup fairness that are listed in
Table 2, the situation becomes much more murky. For example, for Case 1 which
involved three eyewitnesses who gave diering descriptions, all four analyses
indicated signi®cant unfairness (bias) in terms of FS, but none in terms of Suspect
Bias. Turning to lineup size, only one of the four analyses indicated unfairness in
terms of ES, and none of the four analyses indicated unfairness insofar as the ALM
was concerned. What could a scientist or expert witness legitimately conclude about
the fairness of such a lineup? Or consider Case 17: the lineup is classed as fair by all
2
It is not the duty of the scienti®c community to choose statistical measures that are easiest for laypersons
to comprehend. Rather, they should be chosen on a basis of fundamental reliability and validity. If ease of
comprehension were the standard, DNA evidence, which has been considered quite confusing to some
jurors, might not be admitted into evidence. We contend that as long as the ultimate goal of the statistic is
made clear to the jurors, that of determining fairness due to size, the method of calculation is less relevant.
When ES is used as a step towards obtaining a single, global fairness estimate, the details of its calculation
may not seem as great an issue.
S88 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
four statistics when the third eyewitness's description is used, fair in three quarters of
the analyses using eyewitness #2's description, but fair in only two of the four analyses
when eyewitness #1's description was used. Case 14 provides an instance in which
using two descriptions given by the same eyewitness at dierent times produced
somewhat inconsistent outcomes (see also Corey et al., this issue).
Lineup fairness analyses of the 18 actual cases listed in Table 2 produced consistent
outcomes for most of the cases. Based upon this non-random sample of 18 cases, it
appears that in some cases, lineups are so unquestionably fair or unfair that they will
be classi®ed clearly, regardless of which of the fairness indices are used. If the
researcher wishes to present these analyses to the court via expert testimony or
adavit, the expert's task seems relatively straightforward. But vexing issues remain
when analyses of a case yields mixed results, as in Case 17. Should the expert report
the inconsistent data and let the attorney, judge, or jury decide how to interpret it? Or
should the expert decline to testify on the grounds that the data are too inconsistent
from a scienti®c perspective, or because the resulting testimony would be confusing
and unhelpful to jurors (thereby violating the `helpfulness' prong of the Federal Rules
of Evidence and many state evidence standards)? It seems likely that if such expert
testimony is proered before a judge, the judge will be less likely to admit it at trial if
the results are inconsistent.
Current practice in applying lineup fairness indices
It has been widely documented that courts in most jurisdictions in the USA are
usually unwilling to admit expert testimony from researchers concerning the accuracy
of eyewitness evidence (e.g. Brigham, 1989; Cutler and Penrod, 1995; Lipton, 1996).
While there is documentation that such expert testimony has been admitted in
hundreds of cases over the past 25 years (Fulero, 1993, unpublished manuscript;
Kassin et al., 1989), the general consensus is that such admission is unlikely, except in
a few jurisdictions (e.g. the states of Washington, Alaska, Arizona, California, and
Ohio) in which appellate courts have ruled that exclusion of such testimony is an
abuse of judicial discretion under some circumstances.
In order to get a crude estimate of the frequency of acceptance of such expert
testimony, we conducted an informal survey of 11 of the leading eyewitness memory
researchers in the USA and Canada, asking them to estimate the number of cases in
which an attorney had attempted to have their testimony admitted before a jury at
trial, and the number of times it had been admitted. The pattern of responses was
bimodal, seeming to depend largely on the state or judicial district in which the expert
worked. Three experts said that they had testi®ed in one hundred or more cases, and
estimated that their testimony had been accepted 75 per cent or more of the time. In
contrast, seven other experts, also leading researchers, reported that their proposed
expert testimony had been permitted at trial only occasionally ± individual estimates
ranged from 1 per cent to 35 per cent of the time.
Turning speci®cally to lineup fairness estimates, we also asked the experts to
consider the cases in which they had consulted with an attorney about disputed
eyewitness evidence, and to estimate the percentage of these cases in which they
gathered mock witness data to assess the fairness of the lineup(s) used. The estimates
ranged from 2 per cent to 60 per cent of the cases, with a median of 15 per cent across
experts. We also asked them to consider all the cases in which they had attempted to
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S89
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
deliver expert testimony about eyewitness memory at a pretrial evidentiary hearing or
at trial in front of a jury, and to estimate the proportion of times that this testimony
included, or would have included, data on mock witness-based lineup fairness
estimates. Responses varied widely here as well. Most experts reported that this data
was almost never a part of their expert testimony. The median percentage of cases in
which experts testi®ed about eyewitness memory and also presented mock-witness-
based lineup fairness data was 8.5 per cent for pretrial hearings, and 3 per cent at trial.
Hence, although many psychologists have spent a great deal of time and eort in
developing these statistics and speculating about their relevance and utility for actual
cases, it appears that they are almost never presented in the courtroom.
CONCLUSION
In terms of lineup construction procedures, some of the investigators we surveyed
reported behaviours consistent with Koehnken and colleagues' (1996) suggestions
in which some combination of a match-to-description and match-to-appearance
strategy was used. They also appeared somewhat cognizant of biasing factors in the
construction of photospreads. However, the majority of construction methods
utilized diered from the manner in which researchers later attempt to assess the
validity of the lineup. This may account for some of the dierences across lineup
fairness estimates demonstrated both in the current data and in previous studies. For
example, we have seen that dierent eyewitness's descriptions sometimes produced
dissimilar patterns of mock witness responses (and subsequent fairness categoriza-
tions) across the same lineup. Were a match-to-description strategy used when
constructing the lineup, as suggested by Luus and Wells (1991), such diculties
should be eliminated when later assessing the fairness of the lineup with the match-to-
description technique. On the other hand, if one were to employ a match-to-
appearance strategy (as do the majority of those investigators we surveyed), then
alternative procedures for assessing the fairness of the lineup may need to be explored,
so as to avoid inconsistent ®ndings for a single lineup.
Jurors and judges are faced with a dicult task when they are asked to assess the
fairness of a lineup at trial. In the vast majority of occasions, this judgement is made
on a purely subjective basis. Most often, the triers of fact may listen to a defence
attorney's arguments regarding the lineup's purported unfairness (in the attorney's
opening or closing statements), and may be given a brief look at the set of photos used
by law-enforcement ocials. However, they are given no further guidance in
estimating the fairness of the lineup. As we have noted, expert testimony that might
provide the court with a scienti®cally based frame of reference for making this
dicult judgement is seldom admitted by trial judges in most jurisdictions. Further,
even in cases where such expert testimony is admitted, experts seldom present the
results of an empirical analysis of the lineup's fairness. Does this puzzling state of
events mean that mock witness-based lineup assessments are seen (by researchers or
by judges) as irrelevant to actual criminal cases? Are the defence attorney's protesta-
tions and a quick `eyeballing' of the photospread the only information jurors need to
accurately assess the fairness of a lineup? We think not.
For example, many of the cases from the current data set (e.g. Cases 1, 3, 4, 5, 8, 10,
11, and 13 in Table 2) provide clear empirical evidence of overall unfairness: positive
S90 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
bias and inadequate size. Such information could be presented to jurors in a way that
gives them additional (helpful) information with which to evaluate that lineup. We
suggest that an overall fairness evaluation, based on the concepts of Functional Size
and Eective Size (but not Suspect Bias or number of Acceptable Lineup Members),
can be presented meaningfully to judges and jurors, provided the expert is given the
latitude to explain the fairness estimates carefully.
In contrast, some cases (e.g. Cases 2, 6, 16, and 18 in Table 2) yielded data that the
lineup was basically structurally fair. This information would be valuable to jurors as
well, although it is unlikely to reach them since the research was probably commis-
sioned by the defence, and in such a case, the defence attorney is unlikely to call the
researcher as an expert witness. If the researcher is called, the defence attorney is
unlikely to ask the expert about lineup fairness analyses.
Most courts have taken a negative stance towards admitting expert testimony about
eyewitness memory in general. Several experts told us that they seldom or never tried
to present empirical evidence about lineup fairness because they believed that judges
would be even more sceptical towards this type of testimony than towards more
general testimony. However, it is not clear that this would necessarily be the case,
particularly if an understandable empirically based and conceptually justi®ed overall
estimate of fairness is employed. Courts have often rejected general expert testimony
on the basis that it would cover material already thought to be within the `common
knowledge' of jurors. However, it may be that empirically based lineup fairness
information would be less susceptible to this criticism if it seeks to provide assistance
to the court for those cases in which fairness analyses had shown the lineup to be
unambiguously unfair or fair.
In any event, it seems to us that it remains an important quest to extend and clarify
the conceptualizations of lineup fairness, to further develop and re®ne methods of
lineup construction, and to seek assessment techniques that provide conceptually
justi®ed and empirically sound estimates of lineup fairness. If assessment techniques
can be developed that provide consistent estimates of fairness, coupled with
appropriate degrees of sensitivity, discriminability, and ease of understanding to
laypersons, psychology may be able to furnish the courts with worthwhile testimony
that would ensure a greater measure of protection against unfair practices in the use
of eyewitness evidence.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to thank Rebecca Williams for her assistance on this project. Portions
of these data were presented at the Biennial Meeting of the American Psychology-
Law Society in Redondo Beach, CA, in March, 1998.
REFERENCES
Bothwell, R. K., Brigham, J. C. and Malpass, R. S. (1989). Cross-racial identi®cations.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,15, 19±25.
Brigham, J. C. (1989). Disputed eyewitness identi®cations: Can experts help? The Champion,8,
691±695.
Applied Issues in Lineup Fairness S91
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
Brigham, J. C. and Brandt, C. C. (1992). Measuring lineup fairness: Mock witness responses
vs. direct evaluations of lineups. Law and Human Behavior,16, 475±489.
Brigham, J. C. and Ready, D. J. (1985). Own-race bias in lineup construction. Law and Human
Behavior,9, 415±424.
Brigham, J. C., Ready, D. J. and Spier, S. A. (1990). Standards for evaluating the fairness of
photograph lineups. Basic and Applied Psychology,11, 149±163.
Brigham, J. C. and Pfeifer, J. E. (1994). Evaluating the fairness of lineups. In D. F. Ross, J. D.
Read and M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments
(pp. 201±222). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cutler, B. L. and Penrod, S. D. (1995). Mistaken identi®cation: The eyewitness, psychology, and
the law. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Doob, A. N. and Kirshenbaum, H. M. (1973). Bias in police lineups ± partial remembering.
Journal of Police Science and Administration,1, 518±544.
Kassin, S. M., Ellsworth, P. C. and Smith, V. L. (1989). The `general acceptance' of psycho-
logical research on eyewitness testimony: A survey of the experts. American Psychologist,44,
1089±1098.
Koehnken, G., Malpass, R. S. and Wogalter, M. S. (1996). Forensic applications of line-up
research. In S. L. Sporer, R. S. Malpass and G. Koehnken (Eds.), Psychological issues in
eyewitness identi®cation (pp. 205±231). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Laughery, K. R., Jensen, D. G. and Wogalter, M. S. (1988). Response bias with prototypic
faces. In M. M. Gruneberg, R. Sykes and P. Morris (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory:
Current research and issues (pp. 157±162). Chichester: Wiley.
Lipton, J. P. (1996). Legal aspects of eyewitness testimony. In S. L. Sporer, R. S. Malpass and
G. Koehnken (Eds.), Psychological issues in eyewitness identi®cation ( pp. 7±22). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Luus, C. A. E. and Wells, G. L. (1991). Eyewitness identi®cation and the selection of
distracters for lineups. Law and Human Behavior,14, 43±57.
Malpass, R. S. (1981). Eective size and defendant bias in eyewitness identi®cation lineups.
Law and Human Behavior,5, 299±309.
Malpass, R. S. and Devine, P. G. (1983). Measuring the fairness of eyewitness identi®cation
lineups. In S. M. A. Lloyd-Bostock and B. R. Cliord (Eds.), Evaluating witness evidence
(pp. 81±102). Chichester: Wiley.
McNemar, Q. (1969). Psychological statistics (4th edn). New York: Wiley.
Marwitz, D. B. and Wogalter, M. S. (1988). Bias in photo-spreads of faces: A comparison of
two lineup construction methods. In Proceeding of the Human Factors Society 32nd Annual
Meeting (pp. 541±543). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Wells, G. L. and Brad®eld, A. L. (1998). `Good, you identi®ed the suspect': Feedback to
eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied
Psychology,83, 360±376.
Wells, G. L., Lieppe, M. R. and Ostrom, T. M. (1979). Guidelines for empirically assessing the
fairness of a lineup. Law and Human Behavior,3, 285±293.
Wells, G. L., Rydell, S. M. and Seelau, E. P. (1993). On the selection of distractors for
eyewitness lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology,78, 835± 844.
Wells, G. L., Seelau, E. P., Rydell, S. M. and Luus, C. A. E. (1994). Recommendations for
properly conducted lineup identi®cation tasks. In D. F. Ross, J. D. Read and M. P. Toglia
(Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments (pp. 223±244).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wells, G. L., Small, M. A., Penrod, S. D., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M. and Brimacomb,
C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identi®cation procedures: Recommendations for lineups and
photospreads. Law and Human Behavior,23, 603±648.
Wogalter, M. S., Marwitz, D. B. and Leonard, D. C. (1992). Suggestiveness in photospread
lineups: Similarity induces distinctiveness. Applied Cognitive Psychology,6, 443±453.
Wogalter, M. S., Van't Slot, L. J. and Kalsher, M. J. (1991). Bias in police lineups and its
reduction an alternative construction procedure. In Proceedings of the Human Factors
Society 35th Annual Meeting (pp. 1561±1565). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
S92 J. C. Brigham et al.
Copyright #1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 13: S73±S92 (1999)
... To test predictive validity, we examined whether estimates of lineup fairness measured by the mock-witness paradigm can reliably predict eyewitness behaviors (e.g., suspect and filler choices) associated with lineups. Unlike Mansour et al. (2017) and Brigham et al. (1999) ...
... Since Malpass (1981) first distinguished between these subdimensions, they have been in use, yet, limited data has supported the theoretical distinction. Brigham et al. (1999) reported a high correlation between lineup size and bias (though they used a different bias measure from us) but their analysis was based on suspect identifications and could not differentiate between TA and TP lineups because they were using archival data. Our results are more in line with Mansour et al. (2017) who found moderate to strong intradimensional correlations and weak to moderate interdimensional correlations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although eyewitness researchers have used mock-witness measures to assess aspects of lineup fairness, they have paid little attention to their validity. The current study tested predictive validity, convergent validity, and discriminant validity of mock-witness measures from a meta-analytic perspective. Overall, mock-witness measures had predictive validity, particularly in target-absent (TA) lineups—the lineup fairness estimated by the measures reliably predicted eyewitnesses’ choosing behaviors and discriminability of a suspect from fillers in TA lineups. However, correlations between lineup fairness estimated by mock-witnesses and eyewitness performance were significant in target-present (TP) lineups only when eyewitnesses had a moderate memory for the perpetrator. Multitrait-multimethod correlations demonstrated significant intradomain correlations between mock-witness measures and other lineup fairness indices and nonsignificant interdomain correlations between the mock-witness measures and indices reflecting memory strength for the perpetrator, which supported convergent validity and discriminant validity, respectively. The implications for research and practice are discussed.
... Those processes can be influenced by the lineup itself. In order to prevent witnesses from making incorrect identifications, the lineup assembling task is among the top research topics of the psychology of eyewitness identification [1,4,6,9,10]. ...
... One of the principal recommendations for inhibiting errors in identification is to assemble lineups according to the lineup fairness principle [1,5]. Lineup fairness is usually assessed on the basis of data obtained from "mock witnesses" -people who have not seen the offender, but received a short description of him/her. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
In this paper, we aim to present a novel application domain for recommender systems: police photo lineups. Photo lineups play a significant role in the eyewitness identification prosecution and subsequent conviction of suspects. Unfortunately, there are many cases where lineups have led to the conviction of an innocent persons. One of the key factors contributing to the incorrect identification is unfairly assembled (biased) lineups, i.e. that the suspect differs significantly from all other candidates. Although the process of assembling fair lineup is both highly important and time-consuming, only a handful of tools are available to simplify the task. We describe our work towards using recommender systems for the photo lineup assembling task. Initially, two non-personalized recommending methods were evaluated: one based on the visual descriptors of persons and the other their content-based attributes. Next, some personalized hybrid techniques combining both methods based on the feedback from forensic technicians were evaluated. Some of the personalized techniques significantly improved the results of both non-personalized techniques w.r.t. nDCG and recall@top-k.
... Those processes can be influenced by the lineup itself. In order to prevent witnesses from making incorrect identifications, the lineup assembling task is for several decades among the top research topics of the psychology of eyewitness identification [2,3,4,5,14,15,19,20,21]. The sources of error in eyewitness identifications are numerous. ...
... One of the principal recommendations for inhibiting errors in identification is to assemble lineups according to the lineup fairness principle [2,14]. Roughly speaking, fair lineups should ensure that the suspect is not substantially different from the fillers [23]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Photo lineups play a significant role in the eyewitness identification process. This method is used to provide evidence in the prosecution and subsequent conviction of suspects. Unfortunately, there are many cases where lineups have led to the conviction of an innocent suspect. One of the key factors affecting the incorrect identification of a suspect is the lack of lineup fairness, i.e. that the suspect differs significantly from all other candidates. Although the process of assembling fair lineup is both highly important and time-consuming, only a handful of tools are available to simplify the task. In this paper, we describe our work towards using recommender systems for the photo lineup assembling task. We propose and evaluate two complementary methods for item-based recommendation: one based on the visual descriptors of the deep neural network, the other based on the content-based attributes of persons. The initial evaluation made by forensic technicians shows that although results favored visual descriptors over attribute-based similarity, both approaches are functional and highly diverse in terms of recommended objects. Thus, future work should involve incorporating both approaches in a single prediction method, preference learning based on the feedback from forensic technicians and recommendation of assembled lineups instead of single candidates.
... Those processes can be influenced by the lineup itself. In order to prevent witnesses from making incorrect identifications, the lineup assembling task is for several decades among the top research topics of the psychology of eyewitness identification [2,3,4,5,14,15,19,20,21]. The sources of error in eyewitness identifications are numerous. ...
... One of the principal recommendations for inhibiting errors in identification is to assemble lineups according to the lineup fairness principle [2,14]. Roughly speaking, fair lineups should ensure that the suspect is not substantially different from the fillers [23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Photo lineups play a significant role in the eyewitness identification process. This method is used to provide evidence in the prosecution and subsequent conviction of suspects. Unfortunately, there are many cases where lineups have led to the conviction of an innocent suspect. One of the key factors affecting the incorrect identification of a suspect is the lack of lineup fairness, i.e. that the suspect differs significantly from all other candidates. Although the process of assembling fair lineup is both highly important and time-consuming, only a handful of tools are available to simplify the task. In this paper, we describe our work towards using recommender systems for the photo lineup assembling task. We propose and evaluate two complementary methods for item-based recommendation: one based on the visual descriptors of the deep neural network, the other based on the content-based attributes of persons. The initial evaluation made by forensic technicians shows that although results favored visual descriptors over attribute-based similarity, both approaches are functional and highly diverse in terms of recommended objects. Thus, future work should involve incorporating both approaches in a single prediction method, preference learning based on the feedback from forensic technicians and recommendation of assembled lineups instead of single candidates.
... At first blush, this practice of estimating mistaken identification rates by averaging the identifications of all lineup members may seem reasonable-but it assumes that lineups are unbiased, that all lineup members have an equal chance of being chosen by the witness. However, that assumption is likely untrue, with mock witnesses choosing suspects from actual police lineups twice the rate of chance (Brigham et al., 1999;Py et al., 2003). What do we know about how innocent suspects become suspects? ...
... A group of simulated witnesses, who have not witnessed the crime video and who did not know the identity of the perpetrator, received a brief description of the perpetrator, and were asked to select the suspect from the list based on this description. For the lineup to be considered fair, the mock witnesses should not be able to identify the suspect at a rate greater than chance (lineup bias), and the distribution of their choices should be spread equally over the lineup members (lineup size, Brigham et al., 1999). In order to measure the lineup size, the Acceptable Lineup Members technique (ALM) was used (Malpass and Devine, 1983). ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has caused major disruptions in people’s lives around the globe. Sleep habits and emotional balance have been disturbed in a way that could be comparable to the havoc caused by a deep personal crisis or a traumatic experience. This unfortunate situation provides a unique context in which to study the impact of these imbalances on cognitive processes. In particular, the field of eyewitness science could benefit from these conditions, since they are also often present in crime victims, but can only be generated in the laboratory up to a certain ethical and practical limit. For several decades, eyewitness studies have tried to discover what variables affect people’s ability to properly recognize faces. However, the disparity of experimental designs and the limitations of laboratory work could be contributing to the lack of consensus around several factors, such as sleep, anxiety, and depression. Therefore, the possibility of observing the influence of these agents in natural contexts could shed light on this discussion. Here, we perform simple and repeated lineups with witnesses of mock-crime, considering the conditions related to the COVID-19 pandemic, which to some extent allow emulating the deterioration in general well-being that often afflicts crime victims. For this, 72 participants completed symptomatology scales, and watched a video portraying a staged violent episode. Subsequently, they gave testimony and participated in two lineups, in which we manipulated the presence/absence of the perpetrator, to recreate critical scenarios for the appearance of false recognitions. We found an increase in recognition errors in those individuals who did not have access to the perpetrator during the Initial lineup. Additionally, the conditions of the pandemic appear to have adversely affected the ability to witness and accurately perform lineups. These results reaffirm the need to move toward the standardization of research practices and methods for assessing testimonial evidence, especially in relation to the results of the lineups. Considering the degree of fallibility of these processes can lead to a reduction of wrongful convictions.
... Σε κάθε δίκαια κατασκευασμένη σειρά, η πιθανότητα να επιλεγεί καθένα από τα μέλη της είναι στα πλαίσια του τυχαίου όπως αυτό προκύπτει από τον αριθμό των περισπασμών (1/Ν). Αρχειακές έρευνες που εφάρμοσαν τη μέθοδο των εικονικών μαρτύρων, καταδεικνύουν ότι οι σειρές συχνά είναι μεροληπτικές έναντι του υπόπτου (Brigham, Meissner, & Wasserman, 1999;Valentine & Heaton, 1999;Wells & Bradfield, 1999). Επομένως, οι εικονικοί μάρτυρες μπορούν να λειτουργήσουν προστατευτικά και είναι οι μόνοι που μπορούν ασφαλέστερα να διαφυλάξουν την αντικειμενικότητα της σειράς. ...
Article
Full-text available
Eyewitness identification stands as one of the core aspects of the judicial system. However, when it comes to identifying faces, people often make mistakes. Thus, it should not come as a surprise that eyewitness identification has been shown to be the number one factor of wrongful convictions (www.innoccenceproject.org). Therefore it is importantto understand the reasons that make eyewitnesses so error prone and investigate how we could enhance their performance. In the present article we examine the factors that have an impact on eyewitnessidentification performance. More specifically, we will refer to those variables over which the research community has reached consensus. These include estimator and system variables as well as postdictors,which are variables capable to diagnose the identification accuracy once it has taken place. In doing so we aim to reveal those parameters that are based on a sturdy research base, but have notwithstanding beenneglected by the Greek judicial system. We suggest a number of alterations and improvements, based on this research basis that can improve identification performance.
... Dies würde bedeuten, dass sie eher geneigt sind, einen Schwarzen zu identifizieren, während sie bei der eigenen Gruppe vorsichtiger vorgehen. Auch scheint es für weiße Polizisten schwieriger, faire Gegenüberstellungen mit Gesichtern von Schwarzen zusammenzustellen (Brigham, Meissner & Wasserman, 1999). ...
... One of the principal recommendations for inhibiting errors in identification is to assemble lineups according to the lineup fairness principle [1,9]. In general, fair lineups should ensure that the suspect is not substantially different from the fillers. ...
Conference Paper
Photo lineups play a significant role in the eyewitness identification process. Lineups are used to provide evidence in the prosecution and subsequent conviction of suspects. Unfortunately, there are many cases where lineups have led to the incorrect identification and conviction of innocent suspects. One of the key factors affecting the incorrect identification is the lack of lineup fairness, i.e. that the suspect differs significantly from other candidates. Although the process of assembling a fair lineup is both highly important and time-consuming, only a handful of tools are available to simplify the task. In this paper, we follow our previous work in this area and focus on defining and tuning the inter-person similarity metric that will serve as a base for a lineup candidate recommender system. This paper proposes an inter-person similarity metric based on DCNN descriptors of candidates’ photos and their content-based features, which is further tuned by the feedback of domain experts. The recommending algorithm further considers the need for uniformity in lineups. The proposed method was evaluated in a realistic user study focused on lineup fairness over solutions proposed by domain experts. Results shown indicate that the precision of the proposed method is similar to the solutions proposed by domain experts and therefore the approach may significantly reduce the amount of manual work needed for assembling photo lineups.
... Despite guidelines that state suspects should not stand out in lineups (National Institute of Justice, 1999Justice, , 2017, assessments of lineups used in real cases suggest they are often biased toward the suspect (Brigham, Meissner, & Wasserman, 1999). If knowing who the suspect is does not prevent creating lineups in which the suspect stands out, might it help to have administrators who are blind to which lineup member is the suspect evaluate whether anyone in the lineup stands out from the other lineup members? ...
Article
Full-text available
Many have recommended that lineups be conducted by administrators who do not know which lineup member is the suspect (i.e., a double-blind administration). Single-blind lineup administration, in which the administrator knows which lineup member is the suspect, increases the rate at which witnesses identify suspects, increasing the likelihood that both innocent and guilty suspects are identified. Although the increase in correct identifications of the guilty may appear desirable, in fact, this increase in correct identifications is the result of impermissible suggestion on the part of the administrator. In addition to these effects on witness choices, single-blind administration influences witness confidence through an administrator’s feedback to witnesses about their choices, reducing the correlation between witness confidence and accuracy. Finally, single-blind administration influences police reports of the witness’s identification behavior, with the same witness behavior resulting in different outcomes for suspects depending upon whether the administrator knew which lineup member was the suspect. Administrators who know which lineup member is the suspect in an identification procedure emit behaviors that increase the likelihood that witnesses will choose the suspect, primarily by causing witnesses who would have chosen a filler (known innocent member of the lineup who is not the suspect) to choose the suspect. To avoid impermissible suggestion, photo arrays and lineups should be administered using double-blind procedures.
Article
Full-text available
The present research examined whether eyewitness identification lineups produced by law enforcement personnel are biased or suggestive. Experienced police officers were asked to construct two six-face photographic lineups, first using their usual (traditional) method, and second using an alternative method. The primary basis of the traditional method is that foils are selected based on their similarity to the target. The alternative method includes foils that are not only similar to the target but also similar to other foil faces in the lineup. Both types of lineups were shown to subjects who had not seen the faces before (mock witnesses) and were asked to guess the respective targets. The results showed that mock witnesses selected the targets significantly more often than expected by chance (1/6 probability) when embedded in the traditional lineups, thus demonstrating that these lineups were suggestive. Mock witnesses did not select alternative-method targets more often than expected by chance. These results indicate that foil selection procedures incorporating foil-to-foil similarity produce fairer lineups than those exclusively based on target similarity. Implications for forensic lineup construction procedures and for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This article reviews the research on differential recognition for own-versus other-race faces. A meta-analysis of 14 samples revealed that the magnitude of the own-race bias is similar for both Black and White subjects, accounting for about 10o of the variance in recognition accuracy. There is a considerable consistency across studies, indicating that memory for own-race faces is superior to memory for other-race faces. Both Black and White subjects exhibited own-race bias in 79%0 of the samples reviewed.
Article
Full-text available
Sixty-three experts on eyewitness testimony were surveyed about their courtroom experiences and opinions on various issues. There was a strong consensus indicated by an agreement rate of at least 80% that the data on the following topics are reliable enough to present in court: the wording of questions, lineup instructions, misleading postevent information, the accuracy-confidence correlation, attitudes and expectations, exposure time, unconscious transference, showups, and the forgetting curve. Over 70% of the experts also endorsed lineup fairness, the cross-race identification bias among White witnesses, and the tendency to overestimate the duration of events. Although most eyewitness experts who have testified have done so on behalf of criminal defendants, they were just as likely to consent for the prosecution as for the defense; moreover, they were more likely to agree to testify in civil cases than in criminal. Concerning their role in court, most respondents indicated that their main objective is to educate the jury, and that juries are more competent with the aid of experts than without. The results are discussed in relation to the "general acceptance" provision of the Frye test and the limitations of this test for determining the admissibility of expert testimony.
Chapter
An eyewitness takes the stand and describes salient aspects of an event that he or she witnessed several months earlier. Then, in the hush of the courtroom, points to the defendant and says “That's him. That's the man I saw.” Simple, clean, and convincing. And therein rests the problem; what appears to be a simple identification is in fact the result of a series of complex and potentially unreliable social and cognitive events that began unfolding several months earlier when the event was originally witnessed. This chapter, and much of the empirical research on which it is based, operates on an assumption that there are two sources of unreliability in eyewitness accounts. First, there are some inherent limitations in human information processing. These limitations exist at sensory levels (for example, Sperling, 1960), attentional levels (for example, Broadbent, 1958; Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963; Triesman, 1964), and memory levels (for example, Miller, 1956; Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968). But inaccuracies in eyewitness accounts are not entirely attributable to human imperfections in sensation, perception, and memory. The second source of inaccuracy in eyewitness accounts can be attributed to the methods the justice system uses to obtain information from eyewitnesses. The work of Elizabeth Loftus on the effects of misleading questions serves to make this point (see Loftus, 1979; and this volume). The account one gets from an eyewitness depends very much on the methods used to solicit the information. The study of how to improve eyewitness accuracy by manipulating the methods used to obtain information from eyewitnesses is known as a systemvariable approach to eyewitness research (Wells, 1978).
Chapter
Adult Eyewitness Testimony: Current Trends and Developments provides an overview of empirical research on eyewitness testimony and identification accuracy, covering both theory and application. The volume is organized to address three important issues. First, what are the cognitive, social and physical factors that influence the accuracy of eyewitness reports? Second, how should lineups be constructed and verbal testimony be taken to improve the chances of obtaining accurate information? And third, whose testimony should be believed? Are there differences between accurate and inaccurate witnesses, and can jurors make such a distinction? Adult Eyewitness Testimony: Current Trends and Developments is crucial reading for memory researchers, as well as police officers, judges, lawyers and other members of the judicial system.
Article
ABSTRACf Recent research suggests that the current method of lineup construction produces biased or suggestive lineups. Earlier studies used face composite stimuli to assemple the lineups. The present study uses more realistic materials, actual face photographs. Ten pairs of subjects constructed photospread lineups using the traditional method of selecting lineup members who are similar in appearance to the suspect. Another ten pairs of subjects constructed lineups using an alternative construction method. The lineups were then given to a separate group of subjects who had never seen the photographs before and were asked to try to select the face that was the basis for each lineup. The results showed that traditional lineup con' ,uction method produced bias towards the target/suspect. The alternative construction method produ(.ed "..:ssbias, but not significantly less than the traditional method. These results have implications hr Aaw enforcement personnel concerned with the construction and presentation of lineups.