ArticlePDF Available

Evaluation of plasma cortisol levels and behavior in dogs wearing bark control collars

Authors:
  • Auburn University, College of Veterinary Medicine

Abstract

On wk −1, 24 healthy mixed breed kennel dogs were screened by physical examination, complete blood count, serum biochemistry, and plasma cortisol measurement. Dogs were tested to ensure they barked at an unfamiliar dog. Dogs were randomly assigned to control, electronic bark collar or lemon spray bark collar groups (n = 8 per group). On wk 0 (acclimation baseline), dogs wore inactivated collars 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. On wks 1 and 2, dogs wore an activated collar 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. Controls wore an inactivated collar. The bark stimulus was an unfamiliar dog walked in front of the run, three times, 30 s per presentation. Plasma cortisol was measured wk −1, wk 0 d 3, wk 1 d 1, wk 1 d 3 and wk 2 d 3. ACTH was measured wk 0 d 3 and wk 1 d 1. Barking and activity were measured each session.
Evaluation of plasma cortisol levels and behavior in
dogs wearing bark control collars
Janet E. Steiss
a,
*, Caroline Schaffer
b
, Hafiz A. Ahmad
c
,
Victoria L. Voith
d
a
Department of Biomedical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Nursing & Allied Health,
Tuskegee University, Tuskegee, AL 36088, USA
b
Center for Human–Animal Interdependent Relationships, College of Veterinary Medicine,
Nursing & Allied Health, Tuskegee University, Tuskegee, AL 36088, USA
c
Center for Computational Epidemiology, Bioinformatics & Risk Analysis,
College of Veterinary Medicine, Nursing & Allied Health, Tuskegee University,
Tuskegee, AL 36088, USA
d
College of Veterinary Medicine, Western University of Health Sciences, Pomona, CA, USA
Accepted 29 June 2006
Available online 8 August 2006
Abstract
On wk 1, 24 healthy mixed breed kennel dogs were screened by physical examination, complete blood
count, serum biochemistry, and plasma cortisol measurement. Dogs were tested to ensure they barked at an
unfamiliar dog. Dogs were randomly assigned to control, electronic bark collar or lemon spray bark collar
groups (n= 8 per group). On wk 0 (acclimation baseline), dogs wore inactivated collars 30 min/day for 3
consecutive days. On wks 1 and 2, dogs wore an activated collar 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. Controls
wore an inactivated collar. The bark stimulus was an unfamiliar dog walked in front of the run, three times,
30 s per presentation. Plasma cortisol was measured wk 1, wk 0 d 3, wk 1 d 1, wk 1 d 3 and wk 2 d 3. ACTH
was measured wk 0 d 3 and wk 1 d 1. Barking and activity were measured each session.
Results: Dogs wearing electronic or lemon spray collars barked less than controls (P<0.05) by the 2nd day
wearing an activated collar, with no significant difference in barking between collars. Mean numbers of
collar corrections per dog on the 1st day wearing an activated collar were 4.0 (electronic) and 2.0 (lemon
spray); the values decreased to 0 for both collars on the 3rd day. ACTH levels did not differ among groups
(P>0.05). Mean plasma cortisol levels were within the reference range for all groups throughout the study.
Overall, there was a significant time effect (P<0.05) but no significant difference in plasma cortisol
between the control, lemon spray and electronic collar groups (P>0.05). Activity did not change
significantly over time (P>0.05).
www.elsevier.com/locate/applanim
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106
* Corresponding author at: Department of Anatomy, Physiology & Pharmacology, College of Veterinary Medicine,
Auburn University, Alabama 36849, USA. Tel.: +1 334 844 8505; fax: +1 334 844 4542.
E-mail addresses: steisje@auburn.edu,steisje@vetmed.auburn.edu (J.E. Steiss).
0168-1591/$ – see front matter #2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.06.018
Conclusion: Electronic and lemon spray bark collars significantly reduced barking, with no significant
difference between collar types. Dogs with either type of bark collar had an increase in plasma cortisol the
1st day wearing the activated collar, but this was not statistically significant (P>0.05). There was no
statistically significant difference in plasma cortisol levels between dogs wearing control, lemon spray or
electronic collars.
#2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Dog; Bark collar; Cortisol; Scent collar; Barking; Welfare
1. Introduction
Dog owners in the USA and United Kingdom who have been surveyed regarding canine
behavior problems have typically listed excessive barking as one of the most common complaints
(Beaver, 1994; Campbell, 1986; Wells and Hepper, 2000). When Patronek et al. (1996)
conducted a case–control study of households that relinquished dogs for adoption, they found
that if the work of caring for a dog was more than expected, then the dog was at increased risk of
relinquishment. Unwanted barking was one of the factors which significantly contributed to care
of the dog being more than expected. Recently, Villalobos (2005) pointed out that responsible pet
ownership includes controlling excessive barking, and that veterinarians have a consultant role to
guide pet owners who need help with this problem.
Anti-bark collars such as electronic (‘‘shock’’) collars have been considered effective bark
deterrents (Hart and Hart, 1985), but some owners have concerns that these collars may inflict
undue pain and stress (Rudolph and Myers, 2004). Citronella spray collars are an alternative.
Studies comparing electronic and spray collars have indicated that citronella spray collars (Juarbe-
Diaz and Houpt, 1996; Moffat et al., 2003) and unscented spray collars (Beaudet, 2001) also deter
barking. Moffat et al. (2003) compared citronella and unscented spray collars. Both collars reduced
barking, but there was a trend toward higher efficacy with the citronella spray collar.
Some authors have suggested that a citronella spray collar may be gentler and more effective than
an electronic collar (Rudolph and Myers, 2004). The mechanism of action of the citronella spray
collar is not known. Collar mounted training devices marketed to suppress barking are designed to
deliver an aversive stimulus immediately, reliably, and consistently to a specific stimulus, such as
vibrations of the throat and/or barking noise. The correction should startle the dog sufficiently to
interrupt the unwanted behavior and deter repetition of that behavior (Coleman and Murray, 2000).
The element of surprise of a disruptive stimulus may be responsible for deterring barking (Beaudet,
2001). Alternatively,dogs with citronella spray collars may be distractedby the odor and try to locate
the source, or calmed by the odor, startled by the noise, or find the spray’s odor offensive (Juarbe-
Diaz, 1997). But regardless of which collar is used, experts have stressed that no device should be
recommended without concomitant behavior modification (Juarbe-Diaz and Houpt, 1996).
The purposes of this study were to (1) measure plasma cortisol and ACTH levels as indicators
of physiological stress in healthy adult dogs wearing electronic and lemon spray bark control
collars; (2) determine the effectiveness of the two collar types for control of barking.
2. Materials
2.1. Outline of study
On wk 1, 24 mixed breed kennel dogs were screened (physical examination, complete blood count,
serum biochemistry, and plasma cortisol measurement) to ensure that they were healthy. Dogs also were
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106 97
tested to ensure they barked at an unfamiliar dog. Dogs admitted to the study were randomly assigned to
three groups: control, electronic bark collar or lemon spray bark collar (n= 8 per group). On wk 0
(acclimation baseline), dogs wore inactivated collars for 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. On wks 1 and 2,
dogs wore an activated collar for 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. Controls wore an inactivated collar. The
bark stimulus was an unfamiliar dog walked in front of the run at specific time periods.
2.2. Subjects
The project was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee and the Human Participant Review
Committee, Tuskegee University. The original population was comprised of 24 dogs housed in a private no-
kill shelter. All dogs had been relinquished by their owners. The study was approved by the shelter’s
manager and board of directors, who acknowledged that excessive barking was a major cause of
relinquishment of dogs and an impediment to adoption. It was hoped that participating in the study would
enhance the adoptability of the dogs. Three dogs were eliminated on wk 0 for failure to bark when presented
with the bark stimulus (an unfamiliar dog on leash) while wearing an inactivated collar. Because these three
dogs had barked at unfamiliar dogs during screening on wk 1, the investigators conjectured that the three
dogs may have experienced bark collars previously (Moffat et al., 2003). The 21 remaining dogs included 11
males (9 intact, 2 neutered), and 10 intact females. All dogs were considered healthy based on physical
examination, serum biochemistry, and complete blood count. Body weights ranged from 15.5 to 35.6 kg
(mean, 22.5 kg). Mean age was 20 months (S.D. 14 months; range 10–64 months). Most dogs were mixed
breed; 15 dogs appeared to be Labrador Retriever crosses. Mean duration of housing at the kennel prior to
the study was 5 months (S.D. 3 months; range 1–15 months).
The initial screening prior to acceptance into the trial included a cursory behavior evaluation performed
while observing each dog in its run (Schaffer and Schaffer, 1996). Dogs admitted to the trial were classified
as playful (n= 5), playful and attentive (n= 2), attentive (n= 3), submissive (n= 7), submissive and
attentive (n= 1), passive submissive (n= 1) and submissive and slightly fearful (n= 2).
2.3. Housing
Dogs were housed individually in an animal shelter, in an indoor kennel with concrete floors, side walls
with the lower half concrete and the upper half wire mesh, and wire mesh doors. Individual runs consisted of
two sections (each approximately 1.2 m 2.4 m) separated by a guillotine door. One section of each run
faced a concrete wall toward the interior of the building and was air conditioned. The section on the other
side of the guillotine door faced a concrete walkway and windows overlooking a yard, with fans but no air
conditioning. Dogs were moved to this part of the run for recording sessions. Two dogs were studied at the
same time, with approximately 10 empty runs and a hallway between each pair of dogs (total distance
approximately 14 m). With this arrangement, the dogs were not in direct view of each other. Only personnel
involved in the study were admitted to the area during testing in order to minimize distractions. All dogs had
been housed in their run for a minimum of 7 days to avoid stress associated with exposure to a new
environment.
Dogs were kept on the same commercial dog food and feeding schedule over the entire study. Recordings
were performed between 11:00 and 16:00 h. Dogs were fed once per day at 16:45 h. Ambient temperature
and humidity were measured at the start and end of each session. Ambient temperature ranged from 23 to
33 8C. Humidity ranged from 57 to 97%.
2.4. Study schedule and data acquisition
Wk 1 (screening). Dogs were screened by physical examination, blood collection for complete blood
count, serum biochemistry, and plasma cortisol measurement, and presentation of the bark stimulus (an
unfamiliar dog on leash) to ensure that they barked. Then, dogs were randomly assigned to one of three
groups (control, electronic bark collar or lemon spray bark collar, n= 8 per group), by picking a card out of a
hat.
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–10698
Wk 0 (acclimation baseline). Dogs wore inactivated collars 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. Data were
recorded on wk 0 d 3.
Wks 1 and 2. Dogs wore an activated collar 30 min/day for 3 consecutive days. Controls wore an
inactivated collar. Data were recorded on 3 consecutive days for both weeks.
Plasma cortisol was measured five times (wk 1, wk 0 d 3, wk 1 d 1, wk 1 d 3 and wk 2 d 3). ACTH was
measured wk 0 d 3 and wk 1 d 1. Barking duration and activity were measured each session.
Two observers evaluated two dogs simultaneously in individual runs spaced 10 empty runs apart (14 m).
Dogs were paired based on the location of their kennel. Approximately equal distances were maintained
between all pairs of test dogs. A third person presented the bark stimulus. The observer sat quietly,
approximately 1.2 m in front of the run, in full view of the test dog. A digital camcorder mounted on a tripod
was positioned a fixed distance behind the observer to videotape each session. The observer timed barking
with a stopwatch, and recorded the number of collar corrections. Each observer held a pocket voice recorder
and clipboard to record observations. At the end of each 30 min session, personnel entered the run, turned off
activated collars, and took blood samples on designated days.
2.5. Bark stimulus
The bark stimulus was an unfamiliar dog walked on leash in front of the run of the test dog. A different
dog was presented at each of the three presentations during the 30 min session (at 5, 15 and 25 min), for 30 s
per presentation. The unfamiliar dogs were selected to be dogs which themselves did not bark during testing.
These dogs were housed in another part of the kennel.
2.6. Test collars
Test collars were a lemon spray bark control collar (Model SBC100 Spray bark control collar containing
lemon spray [Lemon Spray RFA-164, Petsafe Corp., Knoxville TN]) and an electronic bark control collar
(Deluxe Bark Collar Model DBC100 electrical stimulation collar; Radio Systems Corp., Knoxville, TN).
The spray collar emitted a lemon-scented spray but did not contain citronella. Vibration detection triggered
the spray. The electronic collar used a combination of vibration and sound detection techniques such that the
device activated when both the sound and vibration of a bark were detected simultaneously. The
approximate time from detection of the bark to correction is 152 ms for the electronic collar, and
67 ms for the spray collar.
Activated electronic collars were set at the ‘‘off’’ position for wk 0 acclimation, and ‘‘low’’ intensity
setting on wks 1 and 2. The electronic collar was positioned high on the neck, immediately below the jaw.
The lemon spray collar was positioned 2–4 cm lower, so that the lower jaw did not block the spray outlet of
the canister. Collars were checked each week to ensure proper functioning. During most of the study, each
dog wore its own test collar.
Half the controls wore an electronic collar that was turned off, and the other half wore a lemon spray
collar that had previously been filled with spray but was empty and turned off.
2.7. Barking and activity measurements
The observers measured barking duration with a stop watch for the entire 30 min, at each recording
session. Barking data were verified by review of the videotapes at a later time. Measurements included
whining as well as barking.
An activity indicator was obtained by counting the number of times the left front foot of the dog crossed
one of the grid lines painted on the floor. The left front foot was chosen arbitrarily for ease of counting, and
as a consistent standard of measure. Grid lines, approximately 2.5 cm wide, had been painted on the floor of
each run. Grid lines were oriented from left–right and front–back, resulting in 0.6 m 0.6 m squares. The
criterion for crossing a line was that the entire foot print crossed the line. The activity data were determined
by the evaluators by reviewing the videotapes at a later time.
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106 99
2.8. Cortisol and ACTH measurements
Blood samples were taken within 4 min of opening the door to the run at the end of the 30 min recording
sessions. Blood was drawn from the cephalic vein into a 6 ml syringe. Each dog was returned through the
guillotine door to the other side of its run immediately after blood sampling. The blood was divided into two
EDTA vacutainer tubes (3 ml volume) and stored on ice for transport. Several hours later, the tubes were
centrifuged (4000 rpm 10 min) and aliquots of plasma were frozen at 28 8C. Previous studies indicated
that samples could be kept on ice for at least 6 h with no change in cortisol (Steiss, unpublished data). The
frozen samples were assayed within 4 days.
The exception to the protocol of drawing blood within 4 min of approaching the dog was during the
initial screening (wk 1), when some dogs were walked on leash from their run to the treatment room and
were subjected to the stress of manual restraint for physical examination before drawing blood. Therefore,
wk 0 values are considered more accurate indicators of baseline than wk 1.
Assays were performed by the Endocrinology Laboratory, College of Veterinary Medicine, Auburn
University. Cortisol and ACTH concentrations were assayed in duplicate using commercial radio-immu-
noassay kits (Coat-A Count Cortisol, Diagnostic Products Corp., Los Angeles, CA; ACTH, Nichols Institute
Diagnostics, San Clemente, CA). The methodology for the cortisol assay (Kemppainen et al., 1983) and
ACTH assay (Gould et al., 2001) have been reported previously. The cortisol assay sensitivity is 5 nmol/l.
The only steroids which have been shown to have significant (>3%) cross reaction are prednisolone and 11-
deoxy-17-hydrocorticosterone (Kemppainen et al., 1983). The limit of detection of the ACTH assay is 1 pg/
ml (Gould et al., 2001). The manufacturer states that related peptides such as alpha MSH and beta endorphin
show no cross reaction.
2.9. Statistics
The experiment was evaluated as a repeated measures over time for the two treatments (electronic and
lemon spray collars) and the control group. Data were analyzed by the general linear model (GLM)
procedure of SAS (SAS Institute, 1990) with treatments and time as the main effects. Barking, plasma
cortisol, ACTH and activity were measured repeatedly over time. Significantly different main effects
(treatments and time) for various response measurements were further classified using the all pairs Tukey–
Kramer honestly significant difference (HSD) test (Tukey, 1953). Within the main effect of time, the data
were also analyzed for treatment effect by the GLM procedure of SAS (SAS Institute, 1990) and Scheffe’s
test for multiple comparisons.
3. Results
3.1. Barking
Starting the 2nd day wearing either an activated electronic or lemon spray collar (wk 1 d 2),
barking time was reduced to a mean of 1.8 s or less during the 30 min recording period, P<0.05.
The values on wk 1 d 3 were not significantly different compared to controls probably due to the
large standard deviation in the control group on that day. There was no significant difference
between collar types. We did not notice any barking that could be attributed to interaction
between the dogs (Table 1).
3.2. Number of collar corrections
The mean number of corrections during the 30 min recording sessions decreased to 0.0 for
both collar types on the 3rd day wearing an activated collar (wk 1 d 3) (Table 2).
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106100
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106 101
Table 1
Summary of barking (measured in s, mean S.E.M.)
Group
a
Wk 0 d 3
b
(D)
acclimation to
inactivated collar
Wk1d1(EJ)
1st day wearing
active collar
Wk 1 d 2 (FJ)
2nd day wearing
active collar
Wk1 d 3 (GJ)
3rd day wearing
active collar
Wk 2 d 1 (HJ)
4th day wearing
active collar
Wk 2 d 2 (IJ)
5th day wearing
active collar
Wk 2 d 3 (J)
6th day wearing
active collar
Control
collar (A)
(n=7)
48.1 11.0 (100%)
c
32.7 9.8 (69%) 26.0 7.9 (54%) 40.9 23.3 (85%) 55.4 18.4 (115%) 38.4 16.1 (79%) 71.6 27.4 (150%)
Electronic
collar (BC)
(n=6)
102.4 18.1 (100%) 16.0 5.6 (16%) 1.8
d
1.3 (2%) 1.0 1.0 (1%) 1.3
d
1.0 (1%) 0.7 0.7 (1%) 0.7
d
0.7 (0%)
Lemon spray
collar (C)
(n=8)
65.4 20.4 (100%) 12.8 4.3 (20%) 0.9
d
0.4 (1%) 0.2 0.2 (0%) 0.0
d
0.0 (0%) 0.0
d
0.0 (0%) 0.4
d
0.2 (0%)
Barking was measured over 30 min. The bark stimulus was a strange dog walked in front of the run for 30 s on three separate occasions during the 30 min test period. Overall
comparisons within columns are designated by letters A–C. Treatment groups bearing the same letter are not significantly different, P>0.05. Overall comparisons within rows are
designated by letters D–J. Times bearing the same letter are not significantly different, P>0.05.
a
Treatments were significantly different, P<0.05 (d.f. = 2; F= 16.71).
b
Time was significantly different, P<0.05 (d.f. = 6; F= 8.28).
c
The percentage in parentheses represents the mean on that day divided by the mean of the acclimation period (wk 0 d 3).
d
Interaction between treatment and time was significantly different, P<0.05 (d.f. = 12; F= 2.81). Significantly different from control at this time point, P<0.05.
3.3. Activity
There was no significant difference between the lemon spray and electronic collar groups or
electronic and control groups. The lemon spray group was significantly less active than the
control group throughout the study, P<0.05. The time periods were not significantly different,
P>0.05 (Table 3).
3.4. Cortisol
During the initial screening prior to admission to the study, the mean cortisol level for all 21
dogs was 83.6 nmol/l (S.E.M., 13.4 nmol/l). Mean plasma cortisol values remained within the
reference range for all groups throughout the study. On the 1st day wearing an activated bark
collar, mean plasma cortisol increased to 169% of acclimation baseline levels for both collar
types, but this was not statistically significant (P>0.05). There was no significant difference
between the control, electronic collar and lemon spray collar groups, P>0.05. There was a
significant overall time effect (P<0.05) (Table 4).
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106102
Table 2
Summary of number of corrections per dog from bark control collars during the first 3 days of wearing activated collars
(mean S.E.M.)
Group Wk 1 d 1 1st day
wearing active collar
Wk 1 d 2 2nd day
wearing active collar
Wk 1 d 3 3rd day
wearing active collar
Control collar (n= 7) Not applicable Not applicable Not applicable
Electronic collar (n= 6) 4.0 0.55 0.0 0.05 0.0 0.05
Lemon spray collar (n= 8) 2.0 0.53 1.0 0.21 0.0 0.04
Observations were made over 30 min.
Table 3
Summary of activity (crossing grid lines on floor, mean S.E.M.) for dogs in bark collar study
Group
a
Wk 0 d 3 acclimation
to inactivated collar
Wk 1 d 1 1st day
wearing active collar
Wk 1 d 3 3rd day
wearing active collar
Wk 2 d 3 6th day
wearing active collar
Control
collar (AB)
(n=7)
399.7 193.1 (100%)
b
378.9 108.4 (95%) 374.0 110.0 (94%) 337.3 119.8 (84%)
Electronic
collar (BC)
(n=6)
283.3 58.2 (100%) 217.0 54.4 (77%) 236.7 68.6 (84%) 285.5 95.8 (101%)
Lemon spray
collar (C)
(n=8)
262.2 86.5 (100%) 193.6 49.9 (74%) 166.1 48.7 (63%) 155.5 29.3 (60%)
Activity was measured over 30 min. Measurements were calculated by an observer counting the number of times the dog’s
left front foot crossed over a grid line during the 30 min test period. Grid lines (2 cm wide) were painted on the floor of
each run resulting in a 2 ft. 2 ft. square pattern. Overall comparisons within columns are designated by letters A–C.
Treatments groups bearing the same letter are not significantly different, P>0.05.
a
Treatments were significantly different, P<0.05 (d.f. = 2; F= 3.84). Time, and treatment time interactions were
not significant, P>0.05.
b
The percentage inparentheses represents the mean on that day divided by the mean of the acclimation period (wk 0 d 3).
3.5. ACTH
Groups showed no significant difference in plasma ACTH levels, P>0.05. The plasma
ACTH values (pg/ml, mean S.E.M.) for the control group were 14.3 1.8 (wk 0 d 3) and
15.0 1.7 (wk 1 d 1). The corresponding values for the electronic collar group were 20.2 5.6
(wk 0 d 3) and 28.2 9.8 (wk 1 d 1); values for the lemon spray collar group were 13.5 1.3 (wk
0 d 3) and 16.7 1.7 (wk 1 d 1). The reference range for resting ACTH (Endocrine Diagnostic
Service Laboratory, Auburn University) is 10–80 pg/ml.
4. Discussion
In this study, barking was significantly decreased starting the 2nd day of wearing either an
electronic or lemon spray collar, with no significant difference between the collar types.
Evaluations of bark control collars have been reported previously. Investigators in Australia
randomly surveyed people who had used a collar mounted electronic training device (Coleman
and Murray, 2000). They reported that 97% of respondents were satisfied with the product, and
30% considered their dogs to be calmer after using the electronic collar. Juarbe-Diaz and Houpt
(1996) conducted an in-home study comparing citronella spray collars and electronic shock
collars for efficacy and user satisfaction after a 2 week trial. They found that citronella spray
collars decreased barking 89% whereas electronic collars decreased barking 44%. There was no
explanation for this difference between collar types. Most owners in that study expressed a
preference for the citronella spray collar and perceived it as more humane. Moffat et al. (2003)
used citronella collars on dogs in a veterinary hospital and a kennel; out of 62 dogs, barking
ceased in 40 and decreased in 17 dogs with the collar. In a colony of Beagles, citronella collars
reduced barking noise from a mean of 106 to 70 dB (Moffat et al., 2003). The results of our study
cannot be compared to a more recent report on electronic (‘‘shock’’) collars because that study
examined electronic collars during guard dog training, not as bark deterrents (Schilder and van
der Borg, 2004).
Elevation in blood cortisol concentration has long been used as an indicator of stress in
mammals (Beerda et al., 1998). Rushen (1991) has cautioned about attempts to make claims
concerning animal welfare based on physiological data, given the complexity of the pituitary–
adrenocortical axis. Corticosterone levels have been reported to fluctuate systematically in
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106 103
Table 4
Summary of canine plasma cortisol concentrations (nmol/l) in bark collar study (mean S.E.M.)
Group Wk 0 d 3
a
(AB)
acclimation to
inactivated collar
Wk 1 d 1 (A)
1st day wearing
activated collar
Wk 1 d 3 (B)
3rd day wearing
activated collar
Wk 2 d 3 (B)
6th day wearing
activated collar
Control collar (n= 7) 33.4 5.0 (100%)
b
31.6 7.4 (97%) 22.0 2.3 (67%) 27.3 4.5 (82%)
Electronic collar (n= 6) 61.3 19.1 (100%) 103.3 31.8 (169%) 35.0 7.1 (57%) 39.3 9.1 (64%)
Lemon spray collar (n= 8) 36.0 4.7 (100%) 61.1 14.9 (169%) 37.2 9.0 (103%) 33.9 5.0 (94%)
Reference range for canine baseline cortisol, Endocrine Diagnostic Service Laboratory, Auburn University: 10–160 nmol/
l. Mean plasma cortisol concentration for all 21 dogs at screening prior to admission to the study was 83.6 nmol/l (S.E.M.,
13.4 nmol/l). Overall comparisons within rows are designated by letters A and B. Times bearing the same letter are not
significantly different, P>0.05.
a
Time was significantly different, P<0.05 (d.f. = 3; F= 7.36). Treatments, and treatment time interactions were not
significant, P>0.05.
b
The percentage inparentheses represents the mean on that day divided by the mean of the acclimation period (wk 0 d 3).
response to changes in emotional state (Hennessy et al., 1979). In the present study, on the 1st day
of wearing an activated bark collar, mean plasma cortisol increased to 169% of acclimation
baseline levels for both collar types, but this was not statistically significant (P>0.05).
Throughout this study, the average cortisol level in dogs wearing either bark collar remained
within normal range. In a study which examined dogs in an animal shelter, Hennessy et al. (2002)
reported plasma cortisol concentrations ranging from approximately 10 to 20 ng/ml (equivalent
to 27.5–55 nmol/L), comparable to the values in this study.
Concern has been expressed that the stress of blood sampling limits the usefulness of cortisol
measurement (Wielebnowski, 2003). However, there is an interval after a stressful stimulus
before cortisol rises. Hennessy et al. (1979) documented increases in corticosterone and ACTH in
rats after 10 min, but not 2.5 min, of exposure to a stressful stimulus. In later studies, Hennessy
collected samples from dogs within 4 min after removing the dog from its cage or the test area
(Hennessy et al., 1997, 1998, 2002). In this study, in order to avoid a rise in plasma cortisol which
could be associated with the stress of restraint and venipuncture, blood samples on wks 0–2 were
collected within 4 min of the operator opening the door to the dog’s run.
Mean plasma cortisol levels in this study were higher on wk 1 (initial physical examination)
than wk 0 (acclimation). On wk 1, some dogs were moved from their run to a treatment room
and were restrained for physical examination before drawing blood. This change in handling the
dogs was not due to any difference in the dogs. Transfer to a different room and/or handling and
restraint of the dogs likely account for the higher mean cortisol levels. Therefore, wk 0 values
were considered more accurate indicators of resting baseline than wk 1. Hennessy et al. (1998)
found that handling a dog and taking a venous blood sample raised plasma cortisol. Some
investigators have expressed concern that dogs housed long term in shelters or laboratories are
unable to increase their cortisol levels due to chronic stress (Hennessy et al., 2002). The findings
on wk 1 show that the dogs were able to elevate their blood cortisol.
Several factors in the experimental design were controlled in order to avoid extraneous causes
of elevated plasma cortisol concentrations. Dogs have been shown to have higher plasma cortisol
concentrations during their first 3 days of confinement in an animal shelter compared to dogs
housed in the shelter for longer periods (Hennessy et al., 1997). In our study, dogs were housed a
minimum of 7 days in the kennel in which the study was conducted. Also, 10 empty runs
separated each pair of dogs, thereby avoiding the possibility of dogs in adjacent runs being
exposed to lemon spray, or being influenced by the behavior of the dog in a neighboring run. To
minimize diurnal variations in cortisol, dogs were studied within a time frame of 11:00–16:00 h.
Blood sampling was scheduled more than 2 days apart to allow for recovery of the adrenal axis
between sampling. The dogs were screened by age (approximately 1–4 years of age), breed and
behavior. Breeds that might be considered to have a higher threshold for pain, such as Pit Bulls
and Jack Russell terriers, were not studied. Dogs that demonstrated aggressive tendencies on the
behavioral screening were not used.
Measurements of plasma cortisol and behavior may be preferable to measurement of either
one alone. Our study reports one behavioral measure of activity, namely, the number of times
dogs moved across sectors (grid lines) marked on the floor of each run (Beerda et al., 1998). The
citronella group was less active than the other groups at all time points in this study. That result is
likely due to the small number of animals and individual variation.
Some studies have reported barking as ‘‘episodes per minute’’ (Moffat et al., 2003). However,
distinguishing a single bark from episodes of multiple short barks can be difficult and could yield
unacceptable inter-rater reliability. Therefore, in this study, vocalization time, measured with a
stop watch, has been reported instead.
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106104
Some studies indicate that dogs may habituate to the citronella spray collar (Moffat et al.,
2003; Wells, 2001) although habituation has not always been found (Beaudet, 2001). No
evidence of habituation was seen in this study with dogs wearing bark collars intermittently over
a 2 week period. Juarbe-Diaz and Houpt (1996) commented that dogs quickly learned not to bark
when they wore citronella spray collars and to bark when the collar was not worn. The possibility
of habituation to the collars over a longer time period than the present study would be a relevant
issue to investigate in future studies.
5. Conclusions
Dogs wore a lemon spray bark collar, electronic bark collar or an inactivated bark collar (n=8
per group) for 30 min/day, 3 days per week for 2 weeks, after an acclimation period the previous
week. Both electronic and lemon spray bark collars significantly reduced barking, with no
significant difference between the two types of collars. The amount of barking was significantly
reduced starting the 2nd day that the dogs wore a bark collar. The mean number of corrections
decreased to 0 by the 3rd day for both collar types. Mean plasma cortisol levels were within the
reference range for all groups throughout the study. Plasma cortisol, as well as ACTH levels, did
not differ among groups (P>0.05). Activity did not change significantly over time (P>0.05).
The findings of this study may contribute additional information in the animal welfare debate
regarding whether the use of bark control collars is humane. In the present study, with dogs
wearing bark control collars intermittently over a 2 week period, the collars effectively deterred
barking without statistically significant elevations in plasma cortisol, compared to controls, at
any of the time points measured.
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by Radio Systems Corporation (10427 Electric Ave, Knoxville, TN
37932) and the Department of Health and Human Services’ Health and Services Administration,
Bureau of Health Professions under Tuskegee University’s Center of Excellence Grant.
Veterinary students, S. Chandler, J. Johnson, J. Persaud and P. Reisdorff, Tuskegee University
School of Veterinary Medicine, are acknowledged for their contributions. Dr. R. Kemppainen
(Endocrinology Laboratory, College of Veterinary Medicine, Auburn University) provided
cortisol and ACTH assays. The authors thank Dr. James Wright, Auburn University, for helpful
discussion on statistical analyses.
References
Beaudet, R., 2001. Comparing the effectiveness of citronella with unscented odours in the anti-barking spray collar. In:
Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Veterinary Behaviour Medicine, Vancouver, BC, August 7–8, pp.
190–191.
Beaver, B.V., 1994. Owner complaints about canine behavior. JAVMA 204, 1953–1955.
Beerda, B., Schilder, M.B.H., van Hooff, J.A.R.A.M., de Vries, H.W., Mol, J.A., 1998. Behavioural, saliva cortisol and
heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 58, 365–381.
Campbell, W.E., 1986. The prevalence of behavioral problems in American dogs. Mod. Vet. Pract. 28–31.
Coleman, T., Murray, R., 2000. Collar mounted electronic devices for behaviour modification in dogs. In: Proceedings of
the Urban Animal Management Conference, http://www.ava.com.au/UAM/proc00/murray.htm.
Gould, S.M., Baines, E.A., Mannion, P.A., Evans, H., Herrtgae, M.E., 2001. Use of endogenous ACTH concentration and
adrenal ultrasonography to distinguish the cause of canine hyperadrenocorticism. J. Small Anim. Pract. 42, 113–121.
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106 105
Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A., 1985. Activity, barking, and destructiveness problems in dogs. In: Canine and Feline Behavioral
Therapy, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia, pp. 70–81.
Hennessy, M.B., Davis,H.N., Williams, M.T., Mellott, C., Douglas, C.W., 1997. Plasma cortisol levels of dogs at a public
animal shelter. Physiol. Behav. 62, 485–490.
Hennessy, M.B., Heybach, J.P., Vernikos, J., Levine, S., 1979. Plasma corticosterone concentrations sensitively reflect
levels of stimulus intensity in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 22, 821–825.
Hennessy, M.B., Voith, V.L., Hawke, J.L., Young, T.L., Centrone, J., McDowell, A.L., Linden, F., Davenport, G.M., 2002.
Effects of a program of human interaction and alterations in diet composition on activity of the hypothalamic–
pituitary–adrenal axis in dogs housed in a public animal shelter. JAVMA 221, 65–71.
Hennessy, M.B., Williams, M.T., Miller, D.D., Douglas, C.W., Voith, V.L., 1998. The influence of male and female petters
on plasma cortisol and behaviour: can human interaction reduce the stress of dogs in a public shelter? Appl. Anim.
Behav. Sci. 61, 63–77.
Juarbe-Diaz, S.V., 1997. Assessment and treatment of excessive barking in the domestic dog. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Small
Anim. Pract. 27, 515–532.
Juarbe-Diaz, S.V., Houpt, K.A., 1996. Comparison of two antibarking collars for treatment of nuisance barking. J. Am.
Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 32, 231–235.
Kemppainen, R.J., Thompson, F.N., Lorenz, M.D., 1983. Use of a low dose synthetic ACTH challenge test in normal and
prednisone-treated dogs. Res. Vet. Sci. 35, 240–242.
Moffat, K.S., Landsberg, G.M., Beaudet, R., 2003. Effectiveness and comparison of citronella and scentless spray bark
collars for the control of barking in a veterinary hospital setting. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 39, 343–348.
Patronek, G.J., Glickman, L.T., Beck, A.M., McCabe, G.P., Ecker, C., 1996. Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an
animal shelter. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 209, 572–581.
Rudolph, J.K., Myers, L.J., 2004. Is the bark worse than the bite? Vet. Forum 26–43.
Rushen, J., 1991. Problems associated with the interpretation of physiological data in the assessment of animal welfare.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28, 381–386.
SAS Institute, 1990. SAS
1
Institute User’s Guide: Statistics, 1990 ed. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC.
Schaffer, C.B., Schaffer, D.D., 1996. Dogs uplifting people’s spirits. In: Ackerman, L., Landsberg, G., Hunthausen, W.
(Eds.), Dog Behavior and Training: Veterinary Advice for Owners. TFH Publications, Neptune City, NJ, pp. 253–
269.
Schilder, M.B.H., van der Borg, J.A.M., 2004. Training dogs with help of the shock collar: shortand long term behavioral
effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 85, 319–334.
Tukey, J., 1953. The problem of multiple comparisons. Mimeographed, Princeton University, Math. Dept. Princeton, NJ.
Villalobos, A., 2005. Poisonings linked to barking dogs. Vet. Pract. News 17 (3), 31.
Wells, D., 2001. The effectiveness of a citronella spray collar in reducing certain forms of barking in dogs. Appl. Anim.
Behav. Sci. 73, 299–309.
Wells, D.L., Hepper, P.G., 2000. Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal
rescue shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 69, 55–65.
Wielebnowski, N., 2003. Stress an distress: evaluating their impact for the well-being of zoo animals. JAVMA 223, 973–
977.
J.E. Steiss et al. / Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106 (2007) 96–106106
... Respondents questioned the impact on the mental and physical wellbeing of elephants in response to electric shocks. Previous studies conducted on other species [63,[91][92][93][94][95] and with captive Asian elephants [76] showed that the expected increase in acute stress levels measured using behavioural (e.g., aversive or anxiety/stress related behaviours) and physiological (e.g., cortisol hormone levels, heart rate, body temperature) responses to electric shock rapidly returned to baseline levels soon after experiencing them. Further, by ensuring that stimuli are delivered only when the elephant moved in the 'wrong' direction and not based on its location, will permit elephants to learn accurately and move in the 'right' direction to avoid the shock [96]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aversive geofencing devices (AGDs) or animal-borne satellite-linked shock collars might become a useful tool to mitigate human-elephant conflict (HEC). AGDs have the potential to condition problem elephants to avoid human-dominated landscapes by associating mild electric shocks with preceding audio warnings given as they approach virtual boundaries. We assessed the opinions of different stakeholders (experts, farmers, and others who have and have not experienced HEC; n = 611) on the potential use of AGDs on Asian elephants. Most respondents expressed positive opinions on the potential effectiveness of AGDs in managing elephant movement (62.2%). About 62.8% respondents also provided positive responses for the acceptability of AGDs if pilot studies with captive elephants have been successful in managing their movements. Some respondents perceived AGDs to be unacceptable because they are unethical or harmful and would be unsuccessful given wild elephants may respond differently to AGDs than captive elephants. Respondents identified acceptability, support and awareness of stakeholders, safety and wellbeing of elephants, logistical difficulties, durability and reliable functionality of AGDs, and uncertainties in elephants’ responses to AGDs as potential challenges for implementing AGDs. These issues need attention when developing AGDs to increase support from stakeholders and to effectively reduce HEC incidents in the future.
... It can be expected that during early stages, as animals learn to relate the audio warning from the AGD with the electric shock that follows, they would show acute stress responses, but after learning to avoid the electric shock, stress levels to be no different from baseline levels Lee and Campbell, 2021). Welfare impact of shock collars on dogs and livestock has been assessed by studying their behavioural and physiological stress responses, demonstrating negligible effects of using them on the welfare of study animals (Campbell et al., 2019(Campbell et al., , 2017Kearton et al., 2020Kearton et al., , 2019Schalke et al., 2007;Steiss et al., 2007). Conducting such assessments during preliminary investigations of AGDs with captive elephants will likewise help understand how AGDs could affect the wellbeing of wild elephants if used as an HEC mitigation tool. ...
Article
Full-text available
Animal-borne aversive geofencing devices (AGDs, or satellite-linked shock collars) are commercially available and used on livestock to restrict their movement within a virtual boundary. This technology has potential application as a human-wildlife conflict mitigation tool, where problem animals might be conditioned to avoid human-dominated habitats by associating an audio warning with a subsequent electric shock, which is delivered if the audio warning is ignored. Ensuring that high standards of animal welfare are maintained when implementing such tools is important for acquiring manager and community acceptance of such approaches. We conducted two pilot experiments with eight captive Asian elephants using mild electric shocks from a modified dogtraining collar fitted around the neck, as part of an ongoing effort to develop AGDs suitable for mitigating human-elephant conflict. As part of these experiments, we assessed elephants' behavioural and physiological stress before, during and after our experiments. During the experiments elephants wore collars up to nine consecutive days and received a small number of electric shocks on 1–3 consecutive days. Bootstrapped principal component analysis showed that daily activity budgets of individual elephants on experiment days were not different to the pre-experiment days. Generalised linear mixed-effect model (GLMM) showed that anxiety/stress behaviours increased on the first day of acclimatising to the collar and on testing days (i.e. days they received shocks) of the first experiment, but not during the second experiment relative to pre-experiment days. Analysis of faecal cortisol metabolite (FCM) concentrations using GLMM showed that FCM concentrations were higher in samples collected ~24 hrs and ~48 hrs after testing days compared to baseline levels as expected given the lag time for excretion of cortisol metabolites. These elevated anxiety/stress behaviours and FCM concentrations returned to baseline levels shortly after the experiment. Therefore, we conclude that AGDs did not produce lasting behavioural or physiological stress effects in elephants during this short term study but recommend further studies with a larger sample of elephants to confirm the transferability of these findings.
... From this point of view, blood is currently considered a method of detecting cortisol for revealing certain endocrine problems and not assessing stress. In a percentage of 30%, authors support the idea that although there is a risk of having uncertain results, if the animal, in this case, the dog, is desensitized to the sampling method before the beginning of the experiment, the inter-and intraindividual values have a much lower variability compared to the experiments in which the animal is subjected to contention without it being accustomed to the procedure (14,22,23,25). The other authors cannot agree or oppose any of the theories, having experimental variability as motivation supports the idea that the environment, animal and man, involved in the experiment are the factors that determine the outcome of the experiment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cortisol is quantitatively the major glucocorticoid product of the adrenal cortex. Cortisol is often used as a stress indicator in animal behaviour research. This hormone can be measured using several different mediums like blood, saliva, urine, hair and even nail clippings. These parameters provide an acute, time-point measurement of cortisol concentration. When using invasive methods such as blood sampling, the measured cortisol concentration can be affected by several factors, such as circadian rhythms, handling, restraint, and degree of habituation or acclimation. In practical medicine, the aim of determining cortisol levels dictates the method used by the practician. In recent years, this hormone was up to debate whether measuring its levels can truly be an objective method of determining stress. Investigations in many species often rely exclusively on non-invasive sampling methods, because commonly applied analyses using plasma would be either not possible or simply inappropriate because their high invasiveness can cause stress responses. Initially principally measured in blood, cortisol measurement methods are now evolving towards lower invasiveness and allowing repeated measurements over time. In the case of small animals, especially dogs, in practical medicine, cortisol is often used as an indicator of hormonal diseases and salivary cortisol has been mainly used as a measure of the stress response, because saliva collection is less invasive than blood sampling, resting cortisol concentrations are routinely measured in dogs with chronic gastrointestinal signs to rule out hypoadrenocorticism. Initially principally measured in blood, cortisol measurement methods are now evolving towards lower invasiveness and allowing repeated measurements over time. Using non-invasive methods may be the key to measuring cortisol levels objectively, without external factors intervening in changing the cortisol level thus, having inconclusive results (7). Non-invasive measures of glucocorticoid (GC) hormones and their metabolites, particularly in faeces and hair, are gaining popularity as wildlife management tools, but species-specific validations of these tools remain rare. Identification of severe stress in hospitalized veterinary patients may improve treatment outcomes and welfare (21).
... Regarding animal education, 13.8% of respondents believed that animals should be trained through special training classes taught by an animal trainer. It aligns with previous research, showing that dogs housed in animal shelters can learn new behaviours, inhibiting problem behaviour [109,110]. It can provide bene ts to the prospective owner in making the animal much easier to handle and thus minimizing the risk of relinquishment. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Strays and companion animal management is a sensitive issue in Malaysia that incites solid and conflicting views. Through structured questionnaires administered to a random sample of 704 respondents, this study explored public opinion on a) causes of the stray animal population, b) the management of the stray animal population, and c) the national strategy on strays and companion Animal Management. The results show that 70.3% of respondents agreed that a lack of public awareness regarding animal care was the major contributor to the stray animal population. In addition, 58.1% of respondents felt that treating and vaccinating animals exposed to zoonotic diseases is a viable approach that could be instituted as a conceivable measure in stray animal population management. Finally, developing animal protection areas through a multi-stakeholder partnership strategy initiative recorded the highest support (48.4%) for intervention planning for stray animal management at a national level. Notably, a significant percentage of public responses were implicitly influenced by demographic variables. These findings provide valuable insights into public opinion regarding stray and companion animal management in Malaysia. These findings could inform the development of future legislation aimed at reducing the unfavorable effects of stray animal populations on humans and the ecology of Malaysia.
... It might be expected that animals would show acute stress responses during early stages of learning, but after learning has occurred and animals know how to avoid the stimuli effectively, chronic stress levels should be no different from normal baseline levels [186,197]. Several studies have explored physiological stress levels using cortisol hormone and behavioural responses to understand the welfare of animals in relation to aversive conditioning [154,172,173,193,[198][199][200]. If animals continue to show chronic stress responses and inability to learn, the experiment may need to be modified or discontinued with those animals [197]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Asian elephants are a principal cause of human-wildlife conflict. This results in the death/injury of elephants and humans and large-scale crop and property damage. Most current human-elephant conflict (HEC) mitigation tools lack the flexibility to accommodate the ecological needs of elephants and are ineffective at reducing HEC in the long-term. Here we review common HEC mitigation tools used in Asia and the potential of Aversive Geofencing Devices (AGDs) to manage problem elephants. AGDs can be configured to monitor animal movements in real-time and deliver auditory warnings followed by electric stimuli whenever animals attempt to move across user-specified virtual boundaries. Thus, AGDs are expected to condition elephants to avoid receiving shocks and keep them away from virtually fenced areas, while providing alternative routes that can be modified if required. Studies conducted using AGDs with other species provide an overview of their potential in conditioning wild animals. We recommend that the efficacy and welfare impact of AGDs be evaluated using captive elephants along with public perception of using AGDs on elephants as a means of addressing the inherent deficiencies of common HEC mitigation tools. If elephants could be successfully conditioned to avoid virtual fences, then AGDs could resolve many HEC incidents throughout Asia.
... Each stress indicator has advantages or challenges in the given study design, and the use of multiple indicators, such as a combination of more than one indicator, is recommended [2][3][4]. Historically, in stress response studies of dogs, cortisol in peripheral samples, which primarily reflects HPA responses, has been commonly measured as a stress marker [5][6][7][8][9][10]. However, since stress responses are complicated and cannot be evaluated based on one component or one marker, the need to explore other markers, especially to reflect SAM responses, via noninvasive sample collection, is needed [1]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Since salivary chromogranin A (CgA) is one of the known sympathetic adrenomedullar system (SAM) stress markers in humans and pigs, this study aimed to investigate whether salivary CgA in dogs reflects SAM activation. Our hypothesis was that salivary CgA would increase when central noradrenaline was pharmacologically induced. A selective noradrenaline transporter blocker, atomoxetine, was orally administered without causing any aversive responses in nine laboratory dogs to see if it would increase salivary CgA. Three treatment groups (i.e., atomoxetine, placebo, and pre-administration of a selective alpha-2 adrenoreceptor agonist (dexmedetomidine) followed by atomoxetine) were prepared with a randomized crossover design. Saliva sample collection, heart rate measurement and behavior observation were performed at Time 0 (baseline) and at 30, 60, 90 and 150 min after each treatment administration. The results demonstrated that salivary CgA significantly increased at 90 and 150 min in the atomoxetine treatment (p < 0.05), whereas it was not observed in the other two treatments. The present study showed that salivary CgA was increased by atomoxetine-induced SAM activation. However, this increase was blocked if dexmedetomidine was pre-administered. Overall, the results indicate that salivary CgA is a potential candidate for SAM-mediated stress markers in dogs. Further study to determine the dynamics of salivary CgA will be helpful in its practical use.
... Schalke et al (2007) [29] found significant differences in the cortisol levels of three groups of laboratory dogs trained using shock collars with different degrees of shock predictability (the lower the predictability, the higher the cortisol levels). However, studies comparing aversive-and reward-based training methods have found either no significant differences or the opposite pattern: the effect on cortisol from shock collar and lemon-spray bark collars did not differ from the control treatment [9,30], and a negative punishment training method (a quitting signal) resulted in higher levels of cortisol than the use of a pinch collar (aversive-based technique) [31]. Hence, the present study is the first to report a significant increase in cortisol levels in dogs trained with aversive-based methods as compared to dogs trained with reward-based methods. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dogs play an important role in our society as companions and work partners, and proper training of these dogs is pivotal. For companion dogs, training helps preventing or managing dog behavioral problems—the most frequently cited reason for relinquishing and euthanasia, and it promotes successful dog-human relationships and thus maximizes benefits humans derive from bonding with dogs. For working dogs, training is crucial for them to successfully accomplish their jobs. Dog training methods range widely from those using predominantly aversive stimuli (aversive methods), to those combining aversive and rewarding stimuli (mixed methods) and those focusing on the use of rewards (reward methods). The use of aversive stimuli in training is highly controversial and several veterinary and animal protection organizations have recommended a ban on pinch collars, e-collars and other techniques that induce fear or pain in dogs, on the grounds that such methods compromise dog welfare. At the same time, training methods based on the use of rewards are claimed to be more humane and equally or more effective than aversive or mixed methods. This important discussion, however, has not always been based in solid scientific evidence. Although there is growing scientific evidence that training with aversive stimuli has a negative impact on dog welfare, the scientific literature on the efficacy and efficiency of the different methodologies is scarce and inconsistent. Hence, the goal of the current study is to investigate the efficacy and efficiency of different dog training methods. To that end, we will apply different dog training methods in a population of working dogs and evaluate the outcome after a period of training. The use of working dogs will allow for a rigorous experimental design and control, with randomization of treatments. Military (n = 10) and police (n = 20) dogs will be pseudo-randomly allocated to two groups. One group will be trained to perform a set of tasks (food refusal, interrupted recall, dumbbell retrieval and placing items in a basket) using reward methods and the other group will be trained for the same tasks using mixed methods. Later, the dogs will perform a standardized test where they will be required to perform the trained behaviors. The reliability of the behaviors and the time taken to learn them will be assessed in order to evaluate the efficacy and efficiency, respectively, of the different training methods. This study will be performed in collaboration with the Portuguese Army and with the Portuguese Public Security Police (PSP) and integrated with their dog training programs.
Article
Full-text available
Strays and companion animal management is a sensitive issue in Malaysia that incites solid and conflicting views. Through structured questionnaires administered to a random sample of 704 respondents, this study explored public opinion on a) causes of the stray animal population, b) the management of the stray animal population, and c) the national strategy on strays and companion Animal Management. The results show that 70.3% of respondents agreed that a lack of public awareness regarding animal care was the major contributor to the stray animal population. In addition, 58.1% of respondents felt that treating and vaccinating animals exposed to zoonotic diseases is a viable approach that could be instituted as a reasonable measure in stray animal population management. Finally, developing animal protection areas through a multi-stakeholder partnership strategy initiative recorded the highest support (48.4%) for intervention planning for stray animal management at a national level. Notably, a significant percentage of public responses were implicitly influenced by demographic variables. These findings provide valuable insights into public opinion regarding stray and companion animal management in Malaysia. These findings could inform the development of future legislation aimed at reducing the unfavorable effects of stray animal populations on humans and the ecology of MalaysiaPlease check if affiliations [is/are] captured correctly.
Article
In this paper I outline the drawbacks with the two main behavioural approaches to animal behaviour problems and argue that each alone is insufficient to underpin a field of clinical animal behaviour. Applied ethology offers an interest in an animal’s spontaneous behaviour in natural contexts, understood within an ecological and evolutionary framework but lacks an awareness of mechanisms that can be manipulated to modify the behaviour of individual animals. Behaviourism in the form of Applied Behavior Analysis offers a toolkit of techniques for modifying the behaviour of individual animals, but has seldom been applied to non-human species, and often overlooks phylogenetic aspects of behaviour. Notwithstanding the historical animosities between the two fields of animal behaviour they are philosophically highly compatible — both being empiricist schools stemming ultimately from Darwin’s insights. Though each individually is incomplete, I argue that an integrated approach that synthesizes the strengths of each holds great promise in helping the many animals who need our assistance to survive and thrive in human-dominated environments.
Article
Full-text available
During their second or third day in public animal shelter, juvenile/adult dogs were exposed to a venipuncture procedure. Then the dogs were either not petted or were petted in a prescribed manner by either a man or a woman; 20 min later, a second blood sample was collected. There was a clear increase in cortisol levels 20 min after the first venipuncture in juvenile/adult dogs that were not petted, but not in dogs that were petted by either a man or a woman. Additional comparisons showed that the petting procedure also inhibited the cortisol response following venipuncture in puppies. However, petting did not reduce the cortisol response to housing in the shelter per se. During petting, dogs made few attempts to escape, frequently were observed in a relaxed posture, and panting was common in juvenile/adult dogs. When dogs were petted immediately following removal from the living cage, those petted by women yawned more often and spent more time in a relaxed, head-up posture. Together, these results indicate that a previously observed sex difference in the effectiveness of petters in reducing the cortisol response was not due to some difference in odor or other nonbehavioural stimulus quality of men and women. Subtle aspects of petting technique appear to have pronounced effects on physiological and possibly behavioural responses of dogs confined in a shelter. Petting may be an effective means of reducing the cortisol responses of dogs to other common aversive situations, such as routine medical examinations and vaccination procedures at veterinary clinics as well as shelters.
Article
Behavioural effects of the use of a shock collar during guard dog training of German shepherd dogs were studied. Direct reactions of 32 dogs to 107 shocks showed reactions (lowering of body posture, high pitched yelps, barks and squeals, avoidance, redirection aggression, tongue flicking) that suggest stress or fear and pain. Most of these immediate reactions lasted only a fraction of a second. The behaviour of 16 dogs that had received shocks in the recent past (S-dogs) was compared with the behaviour of 15 control dogs that had received similar training but never had received shocks (C-dogs) in order to investigate possible effects of a longer duration. Only training sessions were used in which no shocks were delivered and the behaviour of the dogs (position of body, tail and ears, and stress-, pain- and aggression-related behaviours) was recorded in a way that enabled comparison between the groups. During free walking on the training grounds S-dogs showed a lower ear posture and more stress-related behaviours than C-dogs. During obedience training and during manwork (i.e. excercises with a would-be criminal) the same differences were found. Even a comparison between the behaviour of C-dogs with that of S-dogs during free walking and obedience exercises in a park showed similar differences. Differences between the two groups of dogs existed in spite of the fact that C-dogs also were trained in a fairly harsh way. A comparison between the behaviour during free walking with that during obedience exercises and manwork, showed that during training more stress signals were shown and ear positions were lower. The conclusions, therefore are, that being trained is stressful, that receiving shocks is a painful experience to dogs, and that the S-dogs evidently have learned that the presence of their owner (or his commands) announces reception of shocks, even outside of the normal training context. This suggests that the welfare of these shocked dogs is at stake, at least in the presence of their owner.
Article
Claims about animal welfare based on data regarding the pituitary-adrenocortical axis should be viewed with scepticism because of the lack of consistency between the results of different studies. Occasional sampling of blood does not give an accurate description of the episodic nature of corticosteroid secretion. Too little is known about how chronic stress affects the activity of the pituitary-adrenocortical axis, or whether changes in mean daily level or in the nature of the secretory episodes are most important. More attention should be paid to basic research to understand the nature of this biological system rather than to premature attempts to “measure” animal welfare by corticosteroid values.
Article
This research project was designed to draw some conclusions from the observations of people who have used electronic collars for dog behaviour modification. The Innotek1 electronic collars studied for this paper included three types: bark collars, boundary collars and remote trainers. If used correctly, punishment works best when used least and when the aversive stimulus is as moderate as possible while still achieving the desired effect. The electronic collars used by respondents in this study are designed to do this. They are, according to the distributor, when used properly (in accordance with manufacturer's instructions) neither able to injure nor to cause pain. The data gathered from this survey showed that electronic training collars can be an effective remedial measure for some types of problem behaviour in dogs.
Article
This study examined the effectiveness of a citronella spay collar in reducing barking in 30 dogs which wore the collar continuously, i.e. every day for 30 min, or intermittently, i.e. every other day for 30 min, for a period of 3 weeks. Owners rated the frequency of their dog’s barking on a scale ranging from 1 (very infrequent) to 5 (very frequent) before the study began (pre-treatment condition) and at the end of every week that the collar was worn (treatment condition). A further evaluation was made at the end of week 4, following 7 days of collar deprivation (post-treatment condition). Dogs exhibited a significantly lower frequency of barking during the treatment and post-treatment conditions compared to the pre-treatment condition. Barking was most effectively reduced when dogs wore the collar intermittently. However, barking increased over the period of time the collar was worn, particularly in those dogs which wore the device every day. Barking continued to increase when the dogs stopped wearing the collar, albeit at frequencies lower than pre-treatment, particularly in those animals which had worn the collar continuously. The collar was more effective at reducing travel related barking than television or traffic related barking. Findings indicate that dogs generally habituate to the citronella spray collar irrespective of how the animal is exposed to the device. A longer period of efficacy occurs, however, if the collar is worn intermittently. This may appeal to owners who are concerned about their pet having to wear the collar for long periods of time.