ArticlePDF Available

Making Decisions and Judgments on Disability: The Disability Representation of Parents, Teachers, and Special Needs Educators

Authors:
  • University of Perugia. Italy

Abstract and Figures

Representations of disability, shaping the opinions, attitudes, and behaviour of parents, teachers and special needs educators were investigated. Two experiments were carried out: in the first one the disability representations of 90 participants were investigated through both qualitative analyses – consisting of textual interpretation of the data gathered by 17 focus groups, assisted by the Atlas.ti software – and by quantitative analyses, consisting of Chi-square statistical testing. In the second experiment, the disability representations were investigated by administering the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to 36 participants. The results obtained demonstrated that, on an implicit level, disability is strongly and stereotypically associated with a negative and unpleasant dimension of existence, compared to the richness of scripts showed in explicit explanatory disability opinions, attitudes and behaviour of the participants in different contexts.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Making decisions and judgments on disability: the disability representation of parents, teachers,
and special needs educators
Stefano F
EDERICI
Department of Human and Educational Sciences, University of Perugia, Italy
CIRID, Interdisciplinary Centre for Integrated Research on Disability, University of Rome ‘La Sapienza’
Rome – Perugia, Italy
Fabio M
ELONI
CIRID, Interdisciplinary Centre for Integrated Research on Disability, University of Rome ‘La Sapienza’
Rome, Italy
Correspondence: Stefano Federici, Department of Human and Educational Sciences, University of Perugia, Piazza G. Ermini, 1 - 06123
Perugia, Italy. E-mail: stefano.federici@unipg.it
Abstract
Representations of disability, shaping the opinions, attitudes, and behaviour of parents, teachers and special needs educators were
investigated. Two experiments were carried out: in the first one the disability representations of 90 participants were investigated through
both qualitative analyses – consisting of textual interpretation of the data gathered by 17 focus groups, assisted by the Atlas.ti software –
and by quantitative analyses, consisting of Chi-square statistical testing. In the second experiment, the disability representations were
investigated by administering the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to 36 participants. The results obtained demonstrated that, on an implicit
level, disability is strongly and stereotypically associated with a negative and unpleasant dimension of existence, compared to the richness
of scripts showed in explicit explanatory disability opinions, attitudes and behaviour of the participants in different contexts.
K
EYWORDS: models of disability, parental attitudes, ICF, biopsychosocial model, Computer Assisted Qualitative Data Analysis Software,
Atlas.ti, special needs educators, IAT – Implicit Association Test.
INTRODUCTION
The disability models are categorical representations
in which the social relations are understood, built, and given;
frames in which everyone finds his own identity, and also, as
scripts, in which the identities of other individuals are
represented in that complex system of attribution that defines us
and lets us to make decisions and judgments.
Therefore, following our perspective, disability
models should not only be considered as useful access tools to
deal theoretically and practically with a too complex reality, but
as the possibility itself to access that reality called disability.
The idea of disability should be brought back to
etiopathological causes, to social structures, to cultural
discrimination, to divine punishment, pertains to the diversity of
the models that are not a simple interpretation of a
malfunctioning, but the means of experiencing it.
Purpose
In this work we will present the results of two
experiments about a qualitative and a quantitative investigation
on the representations of disability which orient the opinions
and attitudes of people who spend large amounts of their time
with a disabled person on a daily basis. Two reasons led us to
conduct this investigation: first, to study more in depth the
relationship between disability, adaptation, socialization, and
development and, second, to verify the spread of the
biopsychosocial model proposed by the International
Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health [1].
EXPERIMENT
1
Method
Aims of descriptive research and quasi-
experimental hypotheses
The implementation of the first experiment is based
on the following assumption: the disability models that
enlighten the social relations of a disabled student can be
inductively inferred by the representations of disability
orienting the opinions and attitudes of people.
The quasi-experimental results were analysed using
qualitative analyses, text coding and interpretation, and
quantitative software-assisted data analyses. The latter were
carried out, with explorative purposes, starting from two
statistical hypotheses:
1. In the quantitative analyses, the disability model attributed
to each role-homogeneous group, depends on the highest
number of expressions coded as consistent with that
specific model;
2. The disability model, significantly prevailing in each role-
homogeneous group, correlates with the participants’
modalities of expression of their personal experience.
Participants
The enrolment of participants was carried out
exclusively in schools and local educational agencies, or in local
associations and organizations dedicated to care, assist, and
educate disabled people.
A total of 90 participants (69 F; 21 M; mean age: 47
yrs) were involved in the study: 30 (23 F, 7 M) parents of
disabled students, 7 (6 F, 1 M) parents of non disabled students,
20 (18 F, 2 M) non-specialized teachers, 14 (11 F, 3 M)
specialized teachers, 6 (3 F, 3 M) professional special needs
educators, 13 (8 F, 5 M) social-health workers.
Research Tools
The focus group was chosen as the main tool to gather
all the data for this study. Several authors [2-7] highlight the
expediency of the focus group as an investigation tool when the
researcher is concerned about exploring complex ideas,
connected with the dynamics of personality, such as opinions,
emotions, and motivations.
The choice of the tool for the analysis was largely
reliant on the aim of the study itself, in addition to the type of
data we gathered: the assessment of the underlying disability
models, starting from the coding of the main representations
emerging from the focus groups, is a consistent operation to the
approach supported by the scientific foundations of the
Grounded Theory [8]. The software Atlas.ti was designed
within the frame of the Computer Assisted Qualitative Data
Analysis Software (CAQDAS) Networking Project [9-11] at the
beginning of the 1990s. The software analyse data in a
qualitative approach, by means of interpretation, compatible
with the theoretical basis of the Grounded Theory [12].
We carried out 17 focus groups. All the focus groups
conversations were audio recorded for a total of approximately
17 hours. The conversations were then transcribed, analysed,
and interpreted, following methods and procedures described in
the following paragraph.
Qualitative data analysis procedures and results
Manual text analysis procedures
The purpose of the manual analysis of the text was to
verify the perspectives on disability according to the different
roles of the participants.
At first, all the 17 hours of audio recordings from the
focus groups were transcribed. Then, the transcripts were
carefully read and indexed, namely the most significant verbal
expressions were identified and a code/index was assigned to
each one. The code categorised the sentences according to the
three main disability models: medical, social, and
biopsychosocial. Overall, 37 codes emerged from this phase: 17
referring to the social model, 8 to the medical one, and 12 to the
biopsychosocial one.
After the manual coding, we calculated the most
frequent indexes for each focus group, and we assigned to each
educational role the prevailing disability model.
Manual text analysis results
The text analysis manually performed highlighted
different perspectives on disability, according to the educational
role held by each group of participants. In every role-
homogeneous group, except for the special needs educators’
one, the social model appeared to prevail. However in more
than one group the evaluation was not so unambiguous.
Among the parents of disabled and the non-
specialized teachers, the social disability model was evaluated
as the foremost, since the codes/indexes relative to that model
were the most frequent ones in almost all the focus groups.
It was not possible to attribute a single model to the
other groups. Among the parents of non disabled children, even
though the social model seemed to prevail, the presence of the
medical model was detected. Among the specialized teachers
and the social-health workers emerged a fluctuation between the
social and the biopsychosocial models. This would confirm
what is reported in the literature about the appearance of a so-
called transitional model that would account for aspects of both
models [13-14].
Results of the qualitative analysis assisted by
Atlas.ti
The text analysis carried out with the software
partially confirmed the results emerged from manual indexing,
showing some additional nuances within and between the
groups.
The social model is widespread in all the role-
homogeneous groups, except from the specialized teachers that
fluctuate through all the three disability models. The parents of
non disabled students oscillated between the social and the
medical models, confirming what came out from the manual
analysis. However, only the parents of disabled students and the
non-specialized teachers seemed to strongly claim their
adhesion to the social model. The transitional model seems to
spread among the special needs educators and the social-health
workers, with no preference for the biopsychosocial model,
despite the presence of several assumption relative to it.
The parallel analysis developed on a ‘dimensions of
experience’ level, allowed us to discover the existence of a
relationship between disability models and participants’
modalities of expression. Parents, for example, seem to support
some assumptions of the biopsychosocial model, but only at a
level of ‘opinions’, while when they refer to ‘behaviours’ those
assumptions disappear and the social model reappears. In
almost all the groups, the medical model is often expressed with
‘behaviours’, along with emotions like ‘anguish’ and ‘sadness’.
Finally, the social model is expressed through ‘opinions’,
‘behaviours’, and emotions such as ‘anger’ and ‘disgust’.
Quantitative analysis hypothesis
We performed an additional analysis to test if the
qualitative interpretation aiming at assigning a disability model
to the participants of each focus group according to the majority
of the opinions of their components is true. If so, then we
should find a greater number of those codes (attributed to the
texts of each focus group) united in one ‘family’ relative to the
model appearing in each focus group.
Results of quantitative analysis using χ²
distribution
Statistical analysis highlighted significant differences
between the groups regarding the distribution of 2 out of 3
disability models: the social and the medical ones (see
Appendix 1). No significant differences were observed for the
biopsychosocial model, equally spread over the groups divided
by role. In general, significant differences in the distribution of
the 3 models appeared in all the groups, with the exception of
parents of non disabled and social-health workers.
The distribution of the 5 dimensions concerning
personal experience resulted significantly different within all
the groups (see Appendix 2). Parents, teachers, social-health
workers, and special needs educators expressed themselves
mainly showing opinions and behaviours, while emotions like
anger, sadness, and joy appeared only in parents and non-
specialized teachers.
As indicated in Appendix 2, the analysis on the
emotions was carried out only on 6 role-homogeneous groups.
In the groups in which opinions and behaviours showed a
prevalence of the social model, these dimensions were
emotionally expressed as well; in groups in which the
biopsychosocial model prevailed, the participants expressed
mainly opinions, while in groups with preference for the
medical model, the participants expressed both behaviours and
opinions. Lastly, the distribution of opinions, behaviours, and
emotions, resulted significantly different, while the opposite
occurred for the distribution of attitudes and myths/stereotypes.
Discussion of the first experiment results
Results of experiment 1 confirmed our hypothesis that
there is a different perspective on disability according to the
different educational roles.
The results obtained from the qualitative analysis
showed that the horizon of perspectives is wider compared to
the partition into medical, social, and biopsychosocial models.
According to the results obtained we can conclude that:
- The social-health workers and the special needs
educators, as interpreted on the basis of the
qualitative analysis results, refer more to a
transitional model;
- The parents of disabled students, the non-
specialized teachers, and the majority of the
specialized teachers are the groups that adhere the
most to the premises of the social model;
- The parents of non disabled students, as interpreted
on the basis of the qualitative analysis results,
oscillate between the medical and the social model,
although this outcome is not confirmed by the χ²
test.
In the quantitative analysis carried out with the χ² test,
the statistical hypotheses were confirmed: the attribution of a
specific model to each group depends on the greater scores of
the codes united in the ‘family codes’ referring to the model
occurring in each group. The statistical analysis of the personal
experiences distribution in the groups highlighted an interesting
correlation among the foremost disability model in each group
and the modalities of expression of the personal experiences of
the participants. This would explain even more the presence,
within the groups, of different perspectives on disability.
The presence of different models within the groups
leads to some additional conclusions:
- There is a cultural predisposition to the adhesion to
the biopsychosocial model, since the participants
have the tendency to define disability in terms of
individual functioning, rather than using
ability/inability polarities.
- Among the parents of non disabled students, the
presence of a perspective on disability partially
different compared to the one that parents of
disabled students have, let us think over the role
played by the experience of disability on the
parents.
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Aims and hypotheses
As Focus Group methodology is suitable to bring out
judgments and opinions for which participants have a certain
degree of consciousness, in the following experiment we
thought to widen the research question to the so-called
automatic processes.
The aim of experiment 2, was to try to establish the
strength of the association that participants create between the
idea of “disabled” and “non disabled” and a series of
dimensional qualities considered as explanatory of two of the
three disability models: the medical and the social one. The
decision of excluding the biopsychosocial model from this
experiment is due to the fact that, being a complex and non
linear model, it is not possible to “stereotypy” it and to bring it
back to an “aut-aut” logic. On the contrary, both the medical
and the social models can be simplified to their basic
assumptions according to attributes of opposite couples:
healthy/ill for the medical model and outcast/integrated for the
social model. Moreover, we introduced a third couple of
attributes, "good/bad", to better evaluate the strenght of the
association that participants create between the categories
“disabled” and “non disabled” and a more basic dimension of
the human nature. Our hypothesis is that, in a condition of
temporal constrain, participants will rely on the automatic
component of the cognitive elaboration when making
associations, generating less variable and more stereotypical
responses, compared to the ones provided by the focus group.
The aim of this second experiment is to verify if there
is a dominant or a prevailing frame in the automatic processes
to elaborate information concerning disability in a different way
compared to what it is observed for the controlled processes, in
which it appears to be a complex of frames which act as scripts,
(i.e. adapted answers and environmental conditions) both
exogenous and endogenous.
Participants
36 participants (31 F; 5 M; mean age: 42 yrs) were
involved in the study: 8 (8 F) parents of disabled students, 6 (4
F, 2 M) parents of non disabled students, 6 (4 F, 2 M) non-
specialized teachers, 5 (5 F) specialized teachers, 5 (5 F)
professional special needs educators, 6 (5 F, 1 M) social-health
workers.
Research Tools
The IAT [15,16] assess the association between a
target-concept discrimination and an attribute dimension. The
procedure starts with an introduction of the target-concept
discrimination task. The initial discrimination consist in to
distinguish image those who are recognizable as “normal” from
those recognizable as “disabled”. This discrimination task and
the subsequent ones are performed by assigning one category
responding with the left hand and the other responding with the
right hand to the presented stimuli. The second step is the
introduction of the attribute dimensions, also in the form of a
two-category discrimination (good-bad; healthy-ill; outcast-
integrated). After the target discrimination and the attribute
dimension tasks, attributes and categories are superimposed in
the third step, in which stimuli for target and attribute
discriminations appear on alternative trials. In the fourth step,
the respondent has to perform a reversal of response
assignments for the target discrimination, and the fifth (final)
step combines the attribute discrimination (not changed in
response assignments) with the reversed target discrimination. If
the target categories are differentially associated with the
attribute dimension, the subject should find one of the combined
tasks (of the third or fifth step) to be considerably easier than
the other. The measure of difficulty provides the measure of
implicit attitudinal difference between the target categories.
In this study we created 3 different versions of the
IAT: for all the versions the target concept couple was the
normal vs. disabled one, whether in the first test the couples of
attributes were good/ bad, in the second healthy/ill and in the
third outcast/integrated.
The hypothesis underlying the IAT [15] is that
participants’ responses are faster when making associations in
line with their beliefs, emotion and implicit motivations. The
peculiarity of this method consists in the possibility to evaluate
only attitudes without the involuntary tendency of people to
alter their response in order to protect self representation. The
intensity of the association between two concepts is equivalent
to the difference in response time in a classification task
between two blocks. Blocks are defined as compatible if the
experimental task is congruent with the personal association of
the participant, incompatible if the task is not congruent.
In conjunction with the IAT we administered three
scales measuring the semantic differential with the intent to
evaluate, together with the implicit attitudes, the explicit ones
towards disability [17]. Participants are administered a set of
paired attributes which are regarded as a description and a
quantification of the object in a specific dimension. The
semantic differentials used in the present study were constructed
using the same couple of attributes included in each IAT.
Participants were asked to indicate on a 7 point scale, which one
of two extreme was closer to the status of a disabled person.
The mean score indicating a neutral position (“neither…nor”),
was 4. The mean score of the whole scale was 20. All
participants were administered the first 3 IAT followed by the
three semantic differential.
Results
The three implicit association tests (IAT), were
administered with the intent to measure indirectly the individual
differences on the intensity of the association between the
category “disabled” and the attributes couples “good/bad”,
“healthy/ill”, “outcast/integrated”. The computational algorithm
used to measure this association, called “d-biep”, is based on
the calculation of the difference in the means of response times
between the sessions compatible (e.g. non disabled /healthy Vs
disabled/ill) and the session non compatible (e.g. disabled
/healthy Vs non disabled/ill).
For all the IAT, the one sample T-Test (DoF=35,
p<0,000), calculated using the scoring obtained using the “d-
biep” (see Table 1) showed that response latencies for the
compatible sessions are significantly lower compared to the
ones for the non compatible session (see Table 2).
Table 1 – Measures of the d-biep or IAT general effect.
IAT
N
Mean
Standard
deviation
d-biep IAT good/bad 36
0,984
0,301
d-biep IAT healthy/ill 36
0,889
0,287
d-biep IAT outcast /integrated 36
0,704
0,393
Table 2 – One sample T-Test on the d-Biep scoring.
IAT
t DoF p
d-biep IAT good/bad
19,57 35 0,000
d-biep IAT healty/ill
18,54 35 0,000
d-biep IAT integrated/castout
10,75 35 0,000
In order to analyse the correlation between the 3 IAT,
we calculated Pearson’s r coefficient on the d-biep measures for
each IAT. The “good/bad” IAT correlated significantly with the
other two: IAT “healthy/ill” (r = .39, p<.05) and IAT “outcast/
integrated” (r = .41, p<.05). The IAT “healthy/ill” correlates
with IAT “good/bad” ” (r = .39, p<.05) and IAT “outcast/
integrated” (r = .54, p<.01). Participants’ performance on the
“healthy/ill” IAT is weakly associated with performance on the
other two IAT, which are strongly correlated. Regarding the
semantic differentials, the mean scores obtained by participants
for each one of them ( “good/bad”= 21,61; “healthy/ill” =
19,30; “outcast/integrated” = 19,27) does not differ significantly
from the overall mean score. Participants did not show a
significant preference for one item of the couple of attributes,
scoring close to the overall mean values.
A good indicator of the validity of the Implicit
Association Test is the correlation with explicit measures. [16].
In the present study, we found that the semantic differential
“outcast/ integrated” correlates significantly both with the IAT
“healthy/ill” (r = -.37, p<.05), and the “outcast/ integrated” one
(r = -.39, p<.05).
Moreover, participants were divided into two groups:
relatives of a disabled or not. We did so in order to verify the
presence of significant differences on the performance on
implicit and explicit tests as a function of this variable. We
selected 13 participants as relatives of a disabled on a total of
36. An independent sample T Test highlighted the significant
differences between the two groups on the IAT “healthy/ill”
(t(34) = 2,84, p = 0,01), showing a stronger association for the
compatible category and attribute in the group of relatives of a
disabled (“normal”, “healthy”) compare to non relatives.
Discussion of the second experiment results
Results obtained from the administration of the 3 IAT
and the 3 semantic differentials showed that participants tend to
establish preferential associations between the category
“disable” and the attributes “bad”, “ill” and “outcast”. Such
associations, instead, did not emerge clearly from performance
on the explicit tests. Participants’ response is more stereotypical
and less variable on the implicit tests whereas, on an explicit
level, disability in not seen as distinctive of a group of people
with clear boundaries like those demarcated by attributes such
as healthy/ill, limited/able, outcast/ integrated and so on, but is
laid on a continuum.
On an implicit level, the categorisation of the disabled
as “bad” seemed to be the strongest one. D-biep scores are
higher for the dimension “good/bad” compared to the
dimensions “healthy/ill” and “integrated/ outcast”. The bipolar
dimension “good/bad” seem to better discriminate between the
two categories “disabled” and “non disabled” compared to the
dimension “healthy/ill”. The latter appears to discriminate more
compared to the “outcast/integrated” one. Results of the
correlational analysis, appeared to confirm the better capacity of
the dimension “good/bad” to represent the categorical
discrimination between “disabled” and “non disabled”. Among
the implicit tests, the IAT “healthy/ill” and the IAT “outcast/
integrated” showed a robust correlation, while there was a poor
correlation between those dimension with the IAT “good /bad”.
As for the correlation between implicit and explicit
tests, only a small correlation appeared between the two IAT
“healty/ill” and “outcast/ integrated” with the scale of semantic
differential “outcast/ integrated”. This result might be
interpreted as indicative of the degree of separation between a
categorisation executed on an unconscious level and the mental
models, more complex and variegated, that people
unconsciously express when describing their beliefs and
experiences with disability.
Finally, the comparison between the scoring of
participants in both tests – implicit and explicit - as a function of
the presence of a disable person in the family, highlighted the
fact that relatives of a disable tended to associate more then
others disability as a disease. This might indicate that people
with a disabled relative are reinforced in the association of the
category disabled with the attribute “bad” as a function of the
experience of medicalisation reserved from our society to
people with disability, in particular during the first years of their
life.
GENERAL
CONCLUSION
The aim of this study was to investigate if and in
which measure the representations of disability, elaborated by
parents, teachers and educators, reflected the different theorical
models of disability. Some significant data emerged from our
results. First, conscious and unconscious visions do not overlap.
In fact, from a conscious perspective people do not appear to
rely only on a specific model in order to describe and think
disability but they prefer to elaborate more complex points of
view, defined by some authors as “in transit” models between
different theoretical models [14]. From an unconscious
perspective instead, the stereotypical and bipolar dimension
appeared to be the most discriminative between disability and
non disability and the “good/bad” one. In other words, on an
implicit categorization level, the strongest association is the one
between disability and the negative and unpleasant dimension of
existence: disability is considered, regardless the role or
familiarity, as “bad”. It is also interesting to point out that the
association disabled/bad seemed “reinforced” in a different way
on a social environmental level: people with a relative with
disability on an implicit tests tend more then others to relate
disability with the disease, whether on an explicit level in the
focus group, they express with more intensity the “tragic” of the
disabled condition. This information might be interpreted in the
light of the strong medicalisation experience that our society
reserves to disabled people, especially during the first years of
their life.
The correlational results between implicit and explicit
tests found in the second experiment, confirmed a certain degree
of differentiation between the categorisation made at an
unconscious level and the mental models, more complex and
variegated, that people consciously use in describing their
beliefs and experiences with respect to disability. This result is
not necessary negative. In fact, it might be explained as an
index of a cultural predisposition to comply with the
biopsychosocial model, more evolved and complex, compared
to the medical and social ones.
Only a capillary and universal action of information
and education, inspired by the principles of the universal model
of disability, will allow to make salient, for the individual and
the group, the instances of a new “culture of diversity”[18].
REFERENCES
[1] WHO, International Classification of Functioning,
Disability and Health: ICF, Geneva: WHO, 2001.
[2] D. J. Warr, ‘It was fun... but we don’t usually talk
about these things’: Analyzing Sociable Interaction in Focus
Groups, Qualitative Inquiry, 11, 2005, pp. 200-225.
[3] S. Wilkinson, Focus Groups in Health Research.
Exploring the Meanings of Health and Illness, Journal of
Health Psychology, 3, 1998, pp. 329-348.
[4] D. M. Linhorst, A Review of the Use and Potential of
Focus Groups in Social Work Research, Qualitative Social
Work, 1, 2002 pp. 208-228.
[5] L. F. Chiu, Transformational Potential of Focus Group
Practice in Participatory Action Research, Action Research, 1,
2003, pp. 165-183.
[6] D. L. Morgan, Focus groups as qualitative research,
London: Sage, 1998.
[7] V. L Zammuner, I focus group, Bologna: Il Mulino,
2003.
[8] A. L. Strauss, & J. Corbin, Basics of qualitative
research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques,
London: Sage, 1990.
[9] N. G. Fielding, & R. M. Lee, Computer analysis and
qualitative research, London: Sage, 1998.
[10] N. G. Fielding, & R. M. Lee, Computing for
qualitative research: Options, problems and potential, in: N. G.
Fielding, & R. M. Lee (Eds.) Using computers in qualitative
research, London: Sage, 1991.
[11] A. Coffey, B. Holbrook, & P. Atkinson, Qualitative
data analysis: Technologies and representations, Sociological
Research Online, 1, 1996.
[12] T. Muhr, Atlas.ti - A prototype for the support of text
interpretation, Qualitative Sociology, 14, 1991, pp. 349-371.
[13] J. Brett, The experience of disability from the
perspective of parents of children with profound impairment: Is
it time for an alternative model of disability?, Disability &
Society, 17, 2002, pp. 825-843.
[14] E. B. Jenks, Explaining Disability: Parents’ Stories of
Raising Children with Visual Impairments in a Sighted World.
Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, Special Issue:
Ethnography and Disability Studies, 34, 2005, pp. 143-169.
[15] A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K.
Schwartz, Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit
Cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1998, pp. 1464-
1480.
[16] A. G. Greenwald, B. A. Nosek, & M. R. Banaji,
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An
improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 2003, pp. 197-216.
[17] C. E. Osgood, G. J. Suci, & P. R. Tannenbaum, The
Measurement of Meaning. Illinois: University of Illinois
Press, 1971.
[18]
S. Federici, & M. Olivetti Belardinelli, Un difficile
accordo tra prevenzione e promozione, Psicologia Clinica dello
Sviluppo, 10, 2006, pp. 330-334.
Appendix 1. Table of distribution of observed and expected frequencies (among and within) of the 3 disability models in the groups and relative significance scores,
applying the χ² test (p < .05).
Disability models
Medical model Social model Biopsychosocial model
Expected value Expected value Expected value
Focus Group [FG]
Observed
value
among within
Observed
value among within
Observed
value among within
Sig.
FG1
5
11.18 9.33
16
26.00 9.33
7
10.94 9.33
0.03
FG13
10
11.18 22.33
45
26.00 22.33
12
10.94 22.33
0.00
FG14
7
11.18 18.00
35
26.00 18.00
12
10.94 18.00
0.00
Lazio
FG15
16
11.18 18.33
33
26.00 18.33
6
10.94 18.33
0.00
Apulia
FG7
9
11.18 22.00
41
26.00 22.00
16
10.94 22.00
0.00
Parents of
disabled students
Umbria
FG17
5
11.18 11.00
19
26.00 11.00
9
10.94 11.00
0.01
Parents of non-
disabled students
Lazio
FG2
18
11.18 16.33
21
26.00 16.33
10
10.94 16.33
0.14
Lazio
FG3
8
11.18 12.00
20
26.00 12.00
8
10.94 12.00
0.02
Molise
FG8
22
11.18 17.33
21
26.00 17.33
9
10.94 17.33
0.05
Non-specialized
teachers
Umbria
FG9
22
11.18 25.67
44
26.00 25.67
11
10.94 25.67
0.00
Lazio
FG4
9
11.18 7.33
7
26.00 7.33
6
10.94 7.33
0.73
Molise
FG10
9
11.18 13.67
27
26.00 13.67
5
10.94 13.67
0.00
Specialized
teachers
Umbria
FG11
8
11.18 19.33
37
26.00 19.33
13
10.94 19.33
0.00
Apulia
FG5
16
11.18 20.33
26
26.00 20.33
19
10.94 20.33
0.27
FG12
8
11.18 12.00
19
26.00 12.00
13
10.94 12.00
0.10
Social-health
workers
Umbria
FG16
11
11.18 13.33
9
26.00 13.33
16
10.94 13.33
0.34
Special needs
educators
Molise
FG6
7
11.18 14.33
22
26.00 14.33
14
10.94 14.33
0.02
Sig. 0.00 0.00
0.11
Appendix 2. Table of distribution of observed and expected frequencies (among and within) of the 5 dimensions of personal experience in the groups and relative significance scores,
applying the χ² test (p < .05).
Dimensions of personal experience
Opinions Attitudes Behaviours Myths and stereotypes Emotions
Expected value Expected value Expected value Expected value Expected value
Focus Group [FG]
Observed
value
among within
Observed
value among within
Observed
value among within
Observed
value among within
Observed
value among within
Sig.
Parents of
disabled
students
FG1
18
40.76 6.60
1
0.35 6.60
9
6.65 6.60
0
0.24 6.60
5
3.50 6.60
0.00
Parents of non
disabled
students
FG2
34
40.76 11.80
1
0.35 11.80
18
6.65 11.80
1
0.24 11.80
5
3.50 11.80
0.00
Non-specialized
teachers
FG3
13
40.76 9.40
0
0.35 9.40
23
6.65 9.40
0
0.24 9.40
11
3.50 9.40
0.00
Specialized
teachers
Lazio
FG4
15
40.76 4.60
0
0.35 4.60
8
6.65 4.60
0
0.24 4.60
0
3.50 4.60
0.00
Social-health
workers
Apulia
FG5
60
40.76 12.20
0
0.35 12.20
1
6.65 12.20
0
0.24 12.20
0
3.50 12.20
0.00
Special needs
educators
Molise
FG6
41
40.76 8.60
0
0.35 8.60
2
6.65 8.60
0
0.24 8.60
0
3.50 8.60
0.00
FG13
59
40.76 16.25
0
0.35 16.25
5
6.65 16.25
1
0.24 16.25
0.00
FG14
46
40.76 13.50
0
0.35 13.50
8
6.65 13.50
0
0.24 13.50
0.00
Lazio
FG15
49
40.76 14.00
0
0.35 14.00
6
6.65 14.00
1
0.24 14.00
0.00
Apulia
FG7
62
40.76 16.25
1
0.35 16.25
2
6.65 16.25
0
0.24 16.25
0.00
Parents of
disabled st.
Umbria
FG17
26
40.76 8.00
0
0.35 8.00
6
6.65 8.00
0
0.24 8.00
0.00
Molise
FG8
47
40.76 13.25
1
0.35 13.25
5
6.65 13.25
0
0.24 13.25
0.00
Non-specialized
teachers
Umbria
FG9
69
40.76 19.50
1
0.35 19.50
8
6.65 19.50
0
0.24 19.50
0.00
Molise
FG10
35
40.76 9.00
0
0.35 9.00
1
6.65 9.00
0
0.24 9.00
0.00
Specialized
teachers
Umbria
FG11
52
40.76 14.25
1
0.35 14.25
4
6.65 14.25
0
0.24 14.25
0.00
FG12
39
40.76 9.75
0
0.35 9.75
0
6.65 9.75
0
0.24 9.75
0.00
Social-health
workers
Umbria
FG16
28
40.76 9.00
0
0.35 9.00
7
6.65 9.00
1
0.24 9.00
n.a.
0.00
Sig. 0.00
0.81
0.00
0.67
0.00
... Implicit attitudes toward general disability using methods other than D-score interpretation were measured in nine studies. For example, using latencies to interpret IAT results, participants were significantly quicker to associate "disabled" with "bad" and "nondisabled" with "good," suggesting an implicit bias toward disability or a preference for nondisabled (Federici & Meloni, 2009). Implicit bias was also reported as a comparison between groups or time points. ...
... Stereotypes toward PWD were investigated in seven studies in relation to general disability, blindness or low vision, physical disability, and intellectual disability. Of the studies investigating implicit stereotypes toward general disability, all reported IAT results as mean latencies, which found participants were significantly quicker to implicitly associate PWD as ill rather than healthy (Federici & Meloni, 2009;Park et al., 2003), "outcasts" rather than integrated within society (Federici & Meloni, 2009), and "child-like" (Robey et al., 2006). Robey et al. (2006) also reported D-scores for the same sample on two administrations of their specific IAT, finding the significant IAT effect showed slight association of PWD as "child-like" on the first administration (D = 0.2), and a neutral effect on the second administration (D = 0.04). ...
... Stereotypes toward PWD were investigated in seven studies in relation to general disability, blindness or low vision, physical disability, and intellectual disability. Of the studies investigating implicit stereotypes toward general disability, all reported IAT results as mean latencies, which found participants were significantly quicker to implicitly associate PWD as ill rather than healthy (Federici & Meloni, 2009;Park et al., 2003), "outcasts" rather than integrated within society (Federici & Meloni, 2009), and "child-like" (Robey et al., 2006). Robey et al. (2006) also reported D-scores for the same sample on two administrations of their specific IAT, finding the significant IAT effect showed slight association of PWD as "child-like" on the first administration (D = 0.2), and a neutral effect on the second administration (D = 0.04). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: People with disabilities (PWD) experience barriers to accessibility and employment, harming their quality of life. Strategies aimed at reducing disparity for PWD have not changed key statistics such as unemployment rates. Previous research has focused on explicit attitudes, generally found to be positive, prompting investigation into factors such as implicit bias. This systematic review and meta-analysis investigated implicit bias toward PWD and associated factors. Method: Forty-six peer-reviewed studies published between January 2000 and April 2020 utilizing the Implicit Association Test were included. Of these, 12 studies met the inclusion criteria for meta-analysis. Results: A significant moderate pooled effect (mean difference = 0.503, 95% CI [0.497-0.509], p < .001) was found, suggesting moderate negative implicit attitudes toward general disability. Negative implicit attitudes were also found toward physical, and intellectual disability. PWD were implicitly stereotyped as incompetent, cold, and "child-like." Findings were inconsistent for factors associated with bias including age, race, sex, and individual differences. Contact with PWD may be associated with implicit bias; however, measures were inconsistent. Conclusion: This review has found moderate negative implicit biases toward PWD, however, factors associated with this bias remain unclear. Further research should investigate implicit bias toward specific disability groups and strategies to shift such biases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
... representations that organize experience and help people to identify and explain the social reaction to human, biological, and social diversity (Meloni, Federici, and Dennis 2015), which can be the result of both cultural learning processes and the outcome of evolved psychological mechanisms. As cognitive organizers, they offer social frames (Goffman 1963) where expected behaviors and social identities are represented, helping people to make decisions and judgments (Federici and Meloni 2009). ...
... These models influence the education of children from the time children enter primary school. While the medical/individual model, much like creationism, seems intuitively plausible (Meloni, Federici, and Bracalenti 2012;Federici and Meloni 2009;Shakespeare 2006;Meloni, Federici, and Dennis 2015), the social model, developed as a reversal of the medical/individual model, is as counterintuitive as evolution (Meloni, Federici, and Dennis 2015;Evans et al. 2012;Lundie 2009;Couser 1997;Dawkins 1987). ...
... As previously reported (Federici et al. 2017;Meloni, Federici, and Dennis 2015;Meloni, Federici, and Bracalenti 2012;Federici and Meloni 2009;, our studies confirm the assumption that the medical/individual model is predominant in explaining disability for all age groups, the social model was second in order of importance, and the biopsychosocial model was the least common. ...
Chapter
Dis/ability research has often been done on dis/abled subjects, positioning them beneath the researcher as something to be studied. Through this commentary, we share our background as a research lab within an academic institution and propose an emancipatory educational action research (EEAR) (Kinsler 2010) framework for academic leaders to utilize to include dis/abled students in an anti-oppressive research model. We also share the unique successes and challenges we face as training drama therapists, preparing to enter a field dominated by medical models that far too often override or invalidate our experiences. In line with participatory action research (PAR) and EEAR, the framework offers steps toward creating a designated space for dis/abled students at the collegiate level to become researcher-participants and develop more presence in dis/ability research.
... impacts on their futures, both in terms of identity and material security (Federici & Meloni, 2009;Hornstra et al., 2010). These are just a handful of the many examples indicating how implicit disability attitudes impact disabled people immensely, especially when the groups holding these attitudes have power over disabled people. ...
Chapter
How disability attitudes operate have shifted over time, becoming more and more subtle, and, thus, more difficult to recognize. The aim of this chapter is to describe modern disability attitudes. First, using a social psychology approach, this chapter explains how modern explicit (conscious) and implicit (unconscious) disability attitudes operate, with negative explicit disability attitudes becoming less common, while negative implicit disability attitudes remain widespread. In fact, this misalignment between people’s explicit and implicit disability attitudes provides critical information about what people feel, and how they go on to behave. For these reasons, this chapter outlines the two-dimensional model of disability prejudice, which helps map the ways explicit and implicit attitudes interact by organizing prejudice types into four categories: symbolic ableism; principled conservatives; aversive ableism; and truly low prejudiced. Next, the chapter covers ways implicit disability attitudes can harm disabled people. Implicit attitudes influence individual behavior, which can have significant implications for interactions between people, including, for example, related to health care. When aggregated, implicit attitudes also highlight systemic discrimination and the bidirectional relationship between systems and attitudes, which are each shaped by the other. Finally, the chapter concludes by interrogating implicit interventions, with limited evidence for effective implicit interventions, especially related to disability. Negative implicit disability attitudes are pervasive and insidious, and impact disabled people immensely.
... They are daily naturalized acts that sometimes even targets have difficulty identifying due to their ambiguity or subtleness, thus frustrating any possible or adequate corrective response. Though still limited, research seeking to understand the implications ableism has in the everyday lives of people with disabilities is burgeoning (Bogart and Dunn 2019;Dirth and Branscombe 2019;Evans et al. 2017;Federici and Meloni 2009;Moscoso Pérez and Arnau Ripollés 2016;Nario-Redmond, Kemerling, and Silverman 2019;Olkin 1999b; Otala Barranquero and Huete García 2019;Toboso Martín 2017;Toboso Martín and Guzmán Castillo 2010). Moreover, whereas some scholars have explored the development of disability identity and how these identities form (Forber-Pratt et al. 2017;Gill 1997), others have shown the consequences of ableism on their targets and their various forms (Dirth and Branscombe 2019;Dunn 2019;Friedman 2017;Harder, Keller, and Chopik 2019). ...
Article
Counterspaces are fields of social action and activism in which people organize themselves to change oppressive social structures. Ableism, or discrimination based on disability, is expressed in everyday life. Its expressions may take the form of ableist microaggressions. This paper analyses a Twitter campaign, through a sample of 335 tweets published from August to December 2018 under the hashtag MeCripple, that exposed ableism and the daily microaggressions people with disabilities endure in Spain. It examines exposed ableist microaggressions, tests if the domains of microaggressions proposed by other scholars apply to Spain, and explores whether the campaign generated a space for coping and responding to ableist discrimination. It also shows that practices of online visibility generate heterotopic online counterspaces leading to cooperation and mutual support to counter ableist microaggressions.
... Such attitudes and stereotypes are the results of two pervasive and interrelated misconceptions (myths), which very often influence human thinking and behaviour: bodily perfection [6] and asexuality [7][8][9][10]. These two myths arise from a disability model that is often known as the medical model of disability [11,12], whereby people are deemed disabled due to their medical condition or impairment [13,14]. Therefore, disability is understood as an individual inability to conform to a standard of normality, namely when the abnormality occurs within the person [15], making him/her different from the majority of people [16]. ...
... The various studies that have analyzed the contribution of social representations of disability in the educational setting (Savarese, 2009;Consiglio, Guarnera and Magnano, 2015) and in relation to the teachers' perceptions (Federici & Meloni, 2009;Samsel & Perepa 2013;Ramel, 2014;Savarese & Cuoco, 2015;St-Onge & Lemyre, 2016). Teachers' representations and the attitudes that derive from them carry out a key role for the realization of a culture of inclusion at school. ...
... Such attitudes and stereotypes are the results of two pervasive and interrelated misconceptions (myths), which very often influence human thinking and behavior: bodily perfection [6] and asexuality [7][8][9][10]. These two myths arise from a disability model that is often known as the medical model of disability [11,12], whereby people are deemed disabled due to their medical condition or impairment [13,14]. Therefore, disability is understood as an individual inability to conform to a standard of normality, namely when the abnormality occurs within the person [15], making him/ her different from the majority of people [16]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
People with paraplegia have to fight their own and societal attitudes and stereotypes that reduce sexuality to the physiological functions of genitalia. These psychological and social limitations arise from cultural and disability models that focus sexual pleasure on phallocentric primacy, and sexual attractiveness of perfect bodies. In this chapter, we evaluate the impacts of a psychoeducational intervention in a personal growth group on the sexual life of two groups of people with spinal cord injury (SCI) and their partners, throughout their sexual interest and satisfaction , depression, and anxiety. In the first study, nonparametric statistical tests were used to compare pre-and post-outcome measures for all participants. In the second study, the grounded theory was used to explore dialogs and activities that were audiotaped during the group meetings. The participants in both groups were patients and their partners. The psychoeducational intervention was clearly effective in increasing sexual interest and satisfaction as well as the motivation and ability to enjoy sexuality. Anxiety was minimized for all participants, although it may not have been associated with the psychoeducational intervention. In addition, the intervention significantly improved the partner and patient group's opportunity and ability to enjoy sexuality.
... A growing body of research has uncovered prominent implicit attitudes using a disabilityspecific version of the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). For example, negative implicit attitudes toward people with Down syndrome were observed among a general sample of college students (Enea-Drapeau, Carlier, & Huguet, 2012) and for a more general classification of ID among a diverse group of participants (Federici & Meloni, 2008). Similarly, when exploring implicit measures through a recent literature review of attitudes toward people with ID, moderate to strong negative attitudes toward people with ID were often observed (Wilson & Scior, 2014). ...
Article
People with intellectual disability (ID) experience negative consequences as a result of stigmas held by the public. Students with ID involved in inclusive postsecondary education (IPSE) programs demonstrate positive outcomes. This study examines the impact of an IPSE program on typically matriculating student attitudes toward ID. Explicit and implicit attitudes were measured at the start and end of a semester among IPSE volunteer peer mentors (n = 17) and an uninvolved student group (n = 14). Findings indicate that volunteers demonstrated lower discomfort after their volunteer experience, as measured by the Attitudes Toward Intellectual Disability Questionnaire (ATTID). Volunteers also demonstrated higher knowledge of causes and preference for interaction with people with ID than nonvolunteers. This demonstrates that volunteer involvement in IPSE positively impacts attitudes toward people with ID among typically matriculating college students.
... In questa direzione, diversi studi hanno analizzato il contributo delle rappresentazioni sociali della disabilità in ambito formativo (Consiglio, Guarnera & Magnano, 2015;Savarese, 2009;Vadalà, 2013), proponendo anche uno specifico affondo sulle rappresentazioni sociali degli insegnanti (Federici & Meloni, 2009;Ramel, 2014;Samsel & Perepa, 2013;Savarese & Cuoco, 2015;St-Onge & Lemyre, 2016). Tra le rappresentazioni della disabilità e dell'inclusione esiste, infatti, un legame molto stretto, reciprocamente condizionante (Lamontagne-Müller & Gygax, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Η Περιβαλλοντική Εκπαίδευση (ΠΕ) λαμβάνει χώρα σε πολλά δημοτικά σχολεία. Κατά τη διάρκεια της καραντίνας και εν μέσω πανδημίας του Κορωνοϊού, έπρεπε τα περιβαλλοντικά προγράμματα που είχαν ξεκινήσει στην αρχή της σχολικής χρονιάς να αλλάξουν μορφή και να ενσωματώσουν σχέδια εργασίας εξ αποστάσεως ΠΕ. Τα οφέλη τόσο της ΠΕ, όσο και της εξ αποστάσεως ΠΕ, είναι πολλαπλά σε ένα παιδί αλλά και στην κοινωνία γενικότερα, αφού η αλλαγή στάσεων, αντιλήψεων και συμπεριφορών των μαθητών (Ράπτης, όπως αναφέρεται στη Σκαναβή, 2004) έχει βραχυπρόθεσμα και μακροπρόθεσμα αποτελέσματα. Επίσης, προωθείται η υπεύθυνη περιβαλλοντική συμπεριφορά και αναπτύσσεται στους μαθητές η δεξιότητα να συμμετέχουν ενεργά στην λήψη αποφάσεων. Μαθητές της Ε’ τάξης Δημοτικού Σχολείου της Νίκαιας, πήραν μέρος όλη την περίοδο του εγκλεισμού στο σπίτι σε καινοτόμες δράσεις και έδειχναν υπερβάλλων ζήλο στη συμμετοχή τους. Environmental Education (EP) takes place in many primary schools. During the quarantine and in the midst of its pandemic Covid 19, needed the environmental programs that had started at the beginning of the school year to change form and incorporate distance work plans IP. The benefitsof both EP and and distance EP, are multiple in a child but also in society in general, since the change of attitudes, perceptions and behaviors of students (Raptis, as mentioned in Skanavi, 2004) have in short and long-term results. Responsible environmental behavior is also promoted and the skill is developed in the students to be actively involved in decision-making. Elementary school students of Nikaia, took part throughout his period inclusion at home in innovative actions and showed excessive eagerness in their participation.
Article
Full-text available
In Participatory Action Research (PAR), group processes are central in facilitating change and focus groups are widely used by action researchers. However, the epistemological basis upon which focus group practice is grounded has not been closely examined and its transformational role has been taken for granted. This might impede the development of the focus group as a distinctive research practice and the realization of its transformational potential. Against the background of three participatory action research projects carried out in the National Health Service of the UK, the author shares her experience of using a variety of focus group methods for facilitating change in health promotion practice. Practical examples are used to explore the relationships between focus group practice and its epistemological grounding. It is suggested that the transformational potential of the focus group lies in the equal value placed on different ways of knowing by an extended epistemological framework and the dynamic of knowing and doing embedded in the PAR process.
Article
Full-text available
Over the last 15 years, social scientists increasingly have used focus groups as a method for collecting qualitative data. With focus groups, a moderator uses the group process with a small number of participants to stimulate discussion and obtain information on the beliefs, attitudes, or motivations of participants on a specific topic. Based upon a review of a sample of social work articles, researchers used focus groups to study a wide range of topics, with diverse and vulnerable populations, and in combination with other qualitative or quantitative methods. Although focus groups have limitations, they have the potential to be an important qualitative data collection method to capture information about the variety of issues and populations dealt with by the social work profession. In addition, social work researchers have the potential to further develop focus groups as a qualitative research method.
Article
Full-text available
An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute. The 2 concepts appear in a 2-choice task (2-choice task (e.g., flower vs. insect names), and the attribute in a 2nd task (e.g., pleasant vs. unpleasant words for an evaluation attribute). When instructions oblige highly associated categories (e.g., flower + pleasant) to share a response key, performance is faster than when less associated categories (e.g., insect & pleasant) share a key. This performance difference implicitly measures differential association of the 2 concepts with the attribute. In 3 experiments, the IAT was sensitive to (a) near-universal evaluative differences (e.g., flower vs. insect), (b) expected individual differences in evaluative associations (Japanese + pleasant vs. Korean + pleasant for Japanese vs. Korean subjects), and (c) consciously disavowed evaluative differences (Black + pleasant vs. White + pleasant for self-described unprejudiced White subjects).
Article
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 85(3) of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (see record 2007-16878-001). The article contained several errors. On page 203, the data lines in Figure 2 are incorrectly labeled. As in Figure 1, the line with filled squares as data points should be labeled MEAN, the line with filled diamonds as data points should be labeled MEDIAN, and the line with unfilled squares as data points should be labeled RECIPROCAL.] In reporting Implicit Association Test (IAT) results, researchers have most often used scoring conventions described in the first publication of the IAT (A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998). Demonstration IATs available on the Internet have produced large data sets that were used in the current article to evaluate alternative scoring procedures. Candidate new algorithms were examined in terms of their (a) correlations with parallel self-report measures, (b) resistance to an artifact associated with speed of responding, (c) internal consistency, (d) sensitivity to known influences on IAT measures, and (e) resistance to known procedural influences. The best-performing measure incorporates data from the IAT's practice trials, uses a metric that is calibrated by each respondent's latency variability, and includes a latency penalty for errors. This new algorithm strongly outperforms the earlier (conventional) procedure.
Article
Focus groups are a useful method for sociological research because the sociable interaction that is generated can yield rich insight into people’s life worlds. This is because the nature of the talk that is generated in focus groups is a mixture of personal beliefs and available collective narratives that are further flavored by the local circumstances of participants’ lives. The interactions between participantsin focus group discussions characteristically comprise layers of talk and present researchers with complex analytical tasks. In addition to what participants say about themselves, analysis and interpretation of focus group data must pay attention to the content and form of interaction between participants. This article discusses and illustrates how sociable interactions from focus groups were analyzed for insights into classed contexts for romantic relationships.
Article
This article is a comparison of the narratives told by and about parents of children who are blind or visually impaired. The narratives in this article come from the author’s own experience raising a child who is visually impaired, from conversations the author has had with other parents of blind and visually impaired children, and from published accounts of parents’ experiences of raising visually impaired children. In addition, this article synthesizes the metanarrative told in the social scientific literature about parents of visually impaired children. This narrative comparison demonstrates that disability is located in the interplay between individuals’ physical bodies and society’s constructed meanings of difference. This article posits that the lived experiences of parents who are raising visually impaired children in a sighted world do not mirror either the medical or the social models of disability, but lie somewhere in between.
Article
This article considers the significance of a model of disability in relation to the child with profound impairment and examines the individual and social models of disability in connection with this. It is argued that although these two models cannot be completely dismissed, the models largely ignore the experience and perspective of the profoundly impaired child, their parent(s) and family. Following in-depth interviews with four parents I attempted to elicit the experience of disability from this perspective. I invited the parents to tell me the ways in which they experience disability, how they viewed the nature and cause of disability in their lives and the relationship between themselves and the various professionals involved in the care of their child. This article endeavours to implement the interview 'findings' to inform an alternative model of disability, the 'Alliance' model, which is briefly discussed and critiqued.
Article
At the core of ATLAS/ti is an explorative approach to theory-building. With linear textual data, such as transcribed interviews, as a starting point, segmentation and coding (textual phases) of the text alternates with the building of conceptual networks and hypertextual structures (conceptual phase). The researcher may draw actual maps, consisting of boxes and connecting lines, that depict the linkages among concepts as a network. Memos can be written for any entity at any stage in the process. For proceeding to a coherent text-outcome, features like the compilation of text units, and cut & paste operations between different text windows are available. All interaction with the program is through a graphic user interface containing windows, menus and icons, since special emphasis is placed in this program on its readiness-at-hand.
Article
Focus group method is becoming increasingly popular among qualitative researchers. After introducing focus group method and briefly overviewing its use in health research, this article shows that the distinctive (and under-used) feature of focus group method is its generation of interactive data. Illustrating my argument with examples from health- related focus group research (including my own data on breast cancer), I argue that this feature makes focus groups an ideal method for gaining access to research participants' own meanings. Interactive data result in enhanced disclosure, improved access to participants' own language and concepts, better understanding of participants' own agendas, the production of more elaborated accounts, and the opportunity to observe the co-construction of meaning in action. Focus groups are, then, an ideal method for exploring people's own meanings and understandings of health and illness.
Article
In this paper we address a number of contemporary themes concerning the analysis of qualitative data and the ethnographic representation of social realities. A contrast is drawn. On the one hand, a diversity of representational modes and devices is currently celebrated, in response to various critiques of conventional ethnographic representation. On the other hand, the widespread influence of computer-assisted qualitative data analysis is promoting convergence on a uniform mode of data analysis and representation (often justified with reference to grounded theory). We note the ironic contrast between these two tendencies, the heterodox and the orthodox, in contemporary qualitative research. We go on to suggest that there exist alternatives that reflect both the diversity of representational approaches, and the broader possibilities of contemporary computing. We identify the technical and intellectual possibilities of hypertext software as offering just one such synthesis.