ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

This study examined cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilingual children. One hundred and thirty-seven first to fourth graders participated in the study. The children were tested on parallel measures of compound awareness, vocabulary, word reading and reading comprehension in Chinese and English. They also received measures of English derivational awareness, English phonological awareness and nonverbal reasoning. Structural equation modelling was used to compare a baseline model with only within-language paths to a model with cross-language paths. The cross-language model fit significantly better than the within-language model, suggesting transfer of morphological awareness between English and Chinese. In particular, we observed a bidirectional relationship between English compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary. Furthermore, English compound awareness was a significant predictor of Chinese reading comprehension. The conditions that support transfer of morphological awareness and the impact of transfer on literacy development in Chinese and English are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Cross-language transfer of
morphological awareness in
Chinese–English bilinguals
Adrian Pasquarella, Xi Chen, Katie Lam and Yang C. Luo
Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology, Ontario Institute for
Studies in Education, University of Toronto, Canada
Gloria Ramirez
Thompson Rivers University, Kamloops, Canada
This study examined cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in
Chinese–English bilingual children. One hundred and thirty-seven first to fourth
graders participated in the study. The children were tested on parallel measures of
compound awareness, vocabulary, word reading and reading comprehension in
Chinese and English. They also received measures of English derivational
awareness, English phonological awareness and nonverbal reasoning. Structural
equation modelling was used to compare a baseline model with only within-language
paths to a model with cross-language paths. The cross-language model fit
significantly better than the within-language model, suggesting transfer of
morphological awareness between English and Chinese. In particular, we observed
a bidirectional relationship between English compound awareness and Chinese
vocabulary. Furthermore, English compound awareness was a significant predictor of
Chinese reading comprehension. The conditions that support transfer of
morphological awareness and the impact of transfer on literacy development in
Chinese and English are discussed.
Morphological awareness refers to the ability to reflect upon and manipulate morphemes,
the smallest phonological unit that carries meaning, and to use word formation rules to
construct and understand morphologically complex words (Kuo & Anderson, 2006).
Recent research has identified morphological awareness as an important predictor of
literacy constructs including vocabulary, word reading and reading comprehension in
alphabetic languages (e.g. Carlisle, 1995, 2000; Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Nagy, Berninger,
Abott, Vaughan & Vermeulen, 2003), as well as in Chinese (e.g. Ku & Anderson, 2003;
McBride-Chang, Shu, Zhou, Wat & Wagner, 2003; Wang, Cheng & Chen, 2006; Wang,
Yang & Chen, 2009) among monolingual and bilingual children. Only a relatively small
number of studies, however, have examined the cross-language impact of morphological
awareness on literacy development in bilingual children. The present study investigated
Journal of Research in Reading, ISSN 0141-0423 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01484.x
Volume 34, Issue 1, 2011, pp 23–42
Copyright r2011 UKLA. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ,
UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA
Journal of
Research in Reading
Journal of
Research in Reading
the cross-language contribution of morphological awareness to literacy skills in bilingual
Chinese- and English-speaking children.
Morphology in English and Chinese
There are three types of morphology: inflection, derivation and compounding (Kuo &
Anderson, 2006). Inflectional morphemes are word endings that denote case, verb tense,
gender or syntax. They mark syntactic or semantic relations between different words
without altering the meaning or the part of speech of the root word (e.g. explain !
explained). Derivations, on the other hand, change either the part of speech or the
meaning, or both, of a base morpheme (root word) by adding a prefix or a suffix (e.g.
friendly !unfriendly,explain !explanation). Finally, compounding creates new
words by combining two or more root words (e.g. sunshine). Important differences exist
between English and Chinese morphology. Inflections and derivations are the main word
formation methods in English, whereas in Chinese over 75% of words are formed through
compounding (Kuo & Anderson, 2006; Sun, Sun, Huang, Li & Xing, 1996). The salience
of morphological features appears to influence the development of specific morphological
skills. In a comparative study, Ku and Anderson (2003) observed that Taiwanese children
had more advanced compound awareness than American children, whereas American
children had more advanced derivational awareness than their Taiwanese counterparts.
To the extent that compounding is more prominent in Chinese, Chinese and English are
rather similar in terms of rules for the formation and meaning of compound morphology.
Compounds in both languages are right-headed, which means that the right morpheme
specifies the category and the left morpheme modifies the meaning and identifies the
subcategory (Clark, Gelman & Lane, 1985). For example, in English ‘classroom’ refers to a
room where a class takes place. The corresponding concept in Chinese is ‘ ’. The left
morpheme ‘ /jiao4/’ means instruction and right morpheme ‘ /shi4/’ means room.
Chinese compounds tend to be relatively transparent (Yuan & Huang, 1998). In transparent
compounds, the meaning of the compound is predictable from the meaning of the
constituent morphemes, for example birthday. In opaque compounds, the meaning of the
compound is not closely related to one or both constituent morphemes, for example jailbird.
English also has many transparent compounds (Frisson, Niswander-Klement & Pollatsek,
2008; Libben, 1998), though opaque compounds do exist in both English and Chinese.
Within-language contribution of morphological awareness to literacy
The role of morphological awareness in reading has been examined in both English and
Chinese. Studies in English have generally supported that inflectional awareness and
derivational awareness are positively associated with vocabulary, word reading and
reading comprehension in monolingual as well as bilingual children (e.g. Carlisle, 2000;
Carlisle & Fleming, 2003; Carlisle & Stone, 2005; McBride-Chang, Cho et al., 2005;
Roman, Kirby, Parrila, Wade-Woolley & Deacon, 2009). Given the prominence of
compounds in Chinese, research involving Chinese children has focused on compound
awareness. This body of research has convincingly demonstrated that compound
awareness is a significant predictor of literacy development among children who are
native speakers of Chinese (e.g. Chen, Hao, Geva, Zhu & Shu, 2009; Chung & Hu, 2007;
24 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Ku & Anderson, 2003; Li, Anderson, Nagy & Zhang, 2002; McBride-Chang, Cheung,
Chow, Chow & Choi, 2006; McBride-Chang, Shu, Ng, Meng & Penney, 2007; McBride-
Chang et al., 2008; Shu, McBride-Chang, Wu & Liu, 2006). Among Chinese–English
bilinguals, Wang et al. (2009) found that Grade 1 children’s English compound awareness
explained a small but significant amount of variance (4%) in their Chinese word reading.
Although the association between morphological awareness and literacy outcomes is
generally interpreted as evidence that morphological awareness is important for literacy
development, researchers acknowledge that the relationship is likely to be reciprocal (Kuo
& Anderson, 2006; McBride-Chang, Cho et al., 2005, 2008; Nagy, Berninger & Abbott,
2006; Wang et al., 2006). McBride-Chang et al. (2008) observed that in Chinese and
Korean kindergarteners, the ability to produce novel compound words using familiar
morphemes predicted unique variance in their vocabulary knowledge 1 year later;
vocabulary knowledge measured at pre-test also predicted unique variance in subsequent
morphological awareness. A number of intervention studies have found that morphological
awareness training led to improved literacy skills (e.g. Carlo et al., 2004; Chow, McBride-
Chang, Cheung & Chow, 2008; Nunes, Bryant & Olsson, 2003). On the other hand, there is
little doubt that vocabulary forms the foundation for developing morphological awareness
because knowledge of root words is required to understand and construct compounds
(Chung & Hu, 2007; M. Hao, X. Chen, V. Dronjic, H. Shu & R. Anderson, unpublished
data; Kuo & Anderson, 2006). In fact, morphological awareness is often considered to
represent vocabulary depth (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2008). It offers an effective way for
children to organise morphologically related words in their mental lexicon.
Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness
The purpose of the present study was to investigate cross-language transfer of
morphological awareness in bilingual Chinese–English speaking children. We examined
whether English morphological awareness would predict children’s Chinese literacy
skills and whether Chinese morphological awareness would also predict English literacy
skills. Among different aspects of metalinguistic awareness, transfer of phonological
awareness has been studied extensively. Cross-language effects of phonological
awareness on reading have been reported between many language combinations such
as Spanish and English (Cisero & Royer, 1995; Durgunog
˘lu, Nagy & Hancin-Bhatt,
1993), French and English (Comeau, Cormier, Grandmaison & Lacroix, 1999; LaFrance
& Gottardo, 2005) and between Chinese and English (Chen, Xu, Nguyen, Hong & Wang,
2010; Gottardo, Yan, Siegel & Wade-Woolley, 2001).
Research on transfer of morphological awareness is more limited. Studies involving
bilingual children who are exposed to two alphabetic languages have focused on the
cross-language effects of derivational awareness and inflectional awareness on word
reading (Deacon, Wade-Woolley & Kirby, 2007; Ramı
´rez, Chen, Geva & Kiefer, 2010;
Saiegh-Haddad & Geva, 2008; Schiff & Calif, 2007). Transfer effects of derivational
awareness on word reading have been observed from Hebrew to English in Israeli
children who learned English as a second language (L2) (Schiff & Calif, 2007), and from
Arabic and Spanish to English in English Language Learners in Canada (Ramı
´rez et al.,
2010; Saiegh-Haddad & Geva, 2008). Notably, Arabic and Hebrew have very different
morphological structures from English. In these languages, word formation involves
the simultaneous affixation of a consonantal root (e.g. K-T-B) and a word pattern
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 25
Copyright r2011 UKLA
(e.g. Ca:CeC), both of which are bound morphemes that cannot stand on their own
(Saiegh-Haddad & Geva, 2008). Transfer of inflectional awareness has been found
between French and English in Canadian French immersion children (Deacon et al., 2007).
Taken together, this body of research provides evidence for cross-language transfer of
morphological awareness, both between languages with similar morphological structures
(English and Spanish, English and French), and between languages with considerably
different morphological structures (English and Hebrew, English and Arabic).
Transfer of compound awareness has been investigated in several studies involving
Chinese children (Wang et al., 2006, 2009; Zhang et al., 2010). Zhang et al. (2010)
offered fifth graders in Mainland China, who learned English as a second language, a
brief intervention on either Chinese or English compound structures. The researchers
found that the children who received the Chinese intervention outperformed the control
group on an English morphological awareness task, suggesting transfer of morphological
awareness from Chinese to English. Among children who received the English
intervention, only those with high English proficiency outperformed the children in the
control group on a Chinese morphological awareness task. This intervention study reveals
a causal link between Chinese and English morphological awareness.
Wang et al. (2006, 2009) investigated transfer of compound awareness in two studies
involving Chinese–English bilinguals. Wang et al. (2009) found that English compound
awareness was related to Chinese word reading in Grade 1. Wang et al. (2006) showed that
English compound morphology predicted significant unique variance in Chinese reading
comprehension in a mixed sample containing children from Grades 1 to 5. This study offered
evidence for cross-language transfer of English compound awareness to Chinese reading
comprehension, but it also had several limitations. First, neither Chinese compound
awareness nor Chinese word reading were entered as within-language controls when
examining the cross-language effect of English compound awareness, which might have
inflated the transfer effect observed in this study. Another concern was that English and
Chinese compound awareness tasks were not correlated, making the transfer effect difficult to
interpret. Because of these limitations, the result of Wang et al. (2006) requires replication.
Interestingly, in both studies by Wang and colleagues, transfer of compound awareness was
found from English compound awareness to Chinese reading, but not from Chinese compound
awareness to English reading. Since reading Chinese requires more compound awareness than
reading English, it may be easier to detect transfer of compound awareness from English to
Chinese than from Chinese to English. Zhang et al. (2010) did observe transfer from Chinese
to English. However, their outcome variable was English compound awareness rather than
English reading. Furthermore, Wang et al. (2006) measured both English compound
awareness and English derivational awareness. Only the former transferred to Chinese reading
comprehension. Because of the rare occurrence of derivational morphology in Chinese, it was
unlikely that English derivational awareness would facilitate Chinese vocabulary and reading,
over and above Chinese compound awareness. Thus, it seems that the direction of transfer of
morphological awareness, at least to the reading outcomes, is influenced by the morphological
structure of children’s first and second language.
The present study
The present study evaluated the cross-language effects of morphological awareness in
Chinese–English bilingual children. We measured compound awareness in Chinese and
26 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
English in parallel tasks. We also measured derivational awareness in English, as English
is a language rich in derivation. Our study attempted to present a comprehensive
framework of cross-language transfer by including Chinese and English vocabulary, word
reading and reading comprehension measures in the same structural equation model
(SEM). To our knowledge, this was the first study to examine the cross-language effects
of morphological awareness on vocabulary and only the second study, after Wang et al.
(2006), to examine these effects on reading comprehension. We created a baseline model
with only within-language paths. In the second model, cross-language paths were added
to examine transfer effects. By comparing the two models, we aimed to extend the
findings of previous studies with better controls and rigorous statistical modelling.
We were interested in what aspects of morphological awareness would transfer, and
the direction of transfer. We expected that compound awareness would transfer between
English and Chinese, due to shared compounding structures. It was unlikely that English
derivational awareness would transfer to Chinese reading. With respect to the direction of
transfer, based on Wang et al.’s (2006, 2009) findings, we predicted that English
compound awareness would contribute to Chinese vocabulary and reading.
Method
Participants
In total, 137 children who spoke Mandarin Chinese as their first language (L1) and
English as their L2 participated in this study. They were recruited from 12 schools
located in predominantly middle-class neighbourhoods in a large Canadian city. English
was the only language of instruction in all schools. The sample consisted of 47 first
graders (24 boys, 22 girls, M
age
581.35 months), 53 second graders (27 boys, 26 girls,
M
age
589.06 months) and 37 fourth graders (15 boys, 22 girls, M
age
5114.19 months).
On average the first graders had been living in Canada for 6.35 months (1–28 months,
SD 55.04 months), second graders for 29.82 months (1–99 months, SD 532.04 months)
and fourth graders for 71.83 months (19–120 months, SD 531.80 months). All children
were exposed to Chinese at home, with 81% of the parents reporting speaking Chinese
with their child at home for more than 75% of the time. Approximately 82% of the
students participated in Chinese heritage language programmes for 1.5 hours every week.
Measures
Nonverbal reasoning. Nonverbal reasoning ability was measured using the Raven’s
Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, Raven & Court, 1998). For each item, children
were asked to complete a visual–spatial matrix by choosing the missing piece from six or
eight patterned segments. Children progressed through the test until they gave five
consecutive wrong answers. A raw score was calculated by adding up the total number of
correct items.
English phonological awareness. Children’s phonological awareness was assessed using
the Elision subtest from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP;
Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1999). Children were asked to delete individual sounds
from words and to give the remaining part (e.g. say ‘cat’, say it without ‘/k/’). The six
practice items and 20 test items in this test included initial, middle and last phoneme
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 27
Copyright r2011 UKLA
deletion. The test was discontinued when the child had responded incorrectly on three
consecutive items.
English derivational awareness. The derivational awareness task was adapted from
Carlisle (2000). Children were orally presented with a root word and a sentence with a
word missing by the experimenter, and were asked to produce a derived form of the root
word to complete the sentence. For example, Experimenter: ‘Farm [root word]. My uncle
is a __________’ (farmer). Children were given three practice trials with feedback to
verify that they understood the task. This test contained 27 items.
English compound awareness. This task was adapted from a similar task designed by
McBride-Chang, Wagner, Muse, Chow and Shu (2005b). In each trial, children were
orally presented with the definition of a compound word, and were then asked to create a
novel compound word of similar structure. For example, Experimenter: ‘Early in the
morning, we can see the sun rising. This is called a sunrise. At night, we might also see
the moon rising. What could we call this?’ (moonrise). There were two practice trials and
15 test items.
English word reading. Children’s word reading ability in English was assessed using the
Letter–Word Identification Subtest from the Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery
(WLPB; Woodcock, 1984). This test required children to identify 14 letters and to read
62 words of increasing difficulty. The test was discontinued if the child read six
consecutive words incorrectly. The raw score was the total number of words read
correctly.
English receptive vocabulary. A shortened version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test, Third Edition, Form III A (PPVT-IIIA; Dunn & Dunn, 1997) was administered to
measure children’s receptive vocabulary. To maintain the same progression of item
difficulty as the original task, every third item from the original task was selected to
create the shortened version, for a total of 60 items. The modifications were made to
shorten administration time and to allow for group administration. Four practice items
were administered before the test items. The experimenter read each item twice and the
children circled in response booklets the picture that best described the word presented.
English reading comprehension. Reading comprehension was assessed using the
Reading Comprehension subtest of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test – Revised
(PIAT-R; Markwardt, 1998). To reduce administration time, we created a shortened
version of the original test by including every other item. The adapted test had 36 items.
Each child received booklets containing sentences and short paragraphs of increasing
difficulty, along with stimulus pictures. Children were asked to silently read each
sentence or short paragraph, then turn the page and select out of four stimulus pictures the
one that best represented the text previously read. Three practice trials were administered
to ensure that the children understood the task.
Chinese compound awareness. This task was parallel to the English compound
awareness measure. In each trial, children were orally presented with the definition
of a compound word in Chinese, and were then asked to create a novel Chinese
compound word of similar structure. For example, ‘
28 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
?[Striped horse (Zebra) is a kind of horse with stripes on the
body. What should we call a cow with stripes on the body?]’ ( striped cow). This task
included 15 test items and two practice items.
Chinese word reading. This task required the child to read 135 characters. The task
started with the most frequent characters (e.g. ) and moved to the less frequent ones
(e.g. ). The test was discontinued when the child misread 10 characters
consecutively.
Chinese receptive vocabulary. Similar to the English receptive vocabulary task, every
third item was selected from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition, Form
III A (PPVT-IIIA; Dunn & Dunn, 1997) and was translated into Chinese to measure
children’s Chinese receptive vocabulary. The total number of items was also 60. These
items were distinct from those selected for the English vocabulary task. The
administration procedure was the same as the English vocabulary measure.
Chinese reading comprehension. Since no standardised reading comprehension test was
available in Chinese, we developed a reading comprehension task based on the sentences
presented in the Neilson’s reading test for elementary school children (Shu, Meng, Chen,
Luan & Cao, 2005). The format of the task was the same as the English reading
comprehension measure. The only difference was that each item contained five stimulus
pictures. There were three practice items and 36 test items.
Parent questionnaire. Parents were encouraged to fill out a language and literacy
questionnaire in Chinese or English. A number of items from the questionnaire were used
in the data analysis, including birthplace, age of immigration, number of months lived in
Canada, maternal education, attendance of Chinese heritage language programme and
home language use. Maternal education was rated on a 6-point scale (1 5some high
school,25completed high school,35some university or college,45completed college
or undergraduate,55some graduate studies,65graduated with an advanced degree).
The amount of Chinese and English that parents spoke to the child was coded along a
5-point scale from never (0% of the time) to always (100% of the time).
Procedure
Participants were assessed in a quiet room at their schools within school hours.
Experimenters were trained undergraduate and graduate research assistants who majored
in psychology, linguistics or human biology. All research assistants were Chinese–
English bilinguals. Testing was divided into two sessions of approximately 60 minutes
each for Chinese and English tasks. The children completed the nonverbal reasoning
measure, as well as the Chinese and English vocabulary and reading comprehension tasks
in small groups. The remaining measures were administered individually. For tests that
were administered individually, the order by which the tests were given was
counterbalanced across participants. Instructions for a test were first given in the
language of the test. Explanation in the other language was provided when needed to
facilitate understanding. Practice trials were also used to confirm that the children
understood the tests.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 29
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Results
Means and correlations
Table 1 presents the reliability ratings, maximum scores, means and standard deviations
of the measures. The scores appear to be reasonable and floor or ceiling effects in the data
are not evident. Children’s performance significantly increased with grade level on most
Chinese and English measures (pvalues for ANOVA tests of group difference were
all .009). However, there were a few exceptions. Post hoc tests revealed that there was
no difference between children in Grades 2 and 4 on English phonological awareness,
English derivational awareness and Chinese compound awareness. Children in Grades 1
and 2 also performed similarly on English receptive vocabulary. Furthermore, maternal
education (p5.121) and number of months lived in Canada (p5.475) were not
significantly different across grades.
Table 2 presents a correlation matrix partialling out the influence of age. Generally
speaking, the English reading and language variables were significantly correlated with
each other (rs ranged from .41 to .79). The Chinese reading and language variables were
also significantly correlated with each other (rs ranged from .36 to .69). A number of
significant correlations emerged between languages. In particular, English compound
awareness was related to Chinese compound awareness, Chinese vocabulary and Chinese
reading comprehension (r5.32, .30 and .41, respectively). Chinese compound awareness
was related to English phonological awareness, English word reading, English
derivational awareness and English reading comprehension (r5.36, .24, .22 and .30,
respectively).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the variables under study.
Variable aMaximum Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 4 Total
(n547) (n553) (n537) (n5137)
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Age 81.35 7.98 89.06 3.85 114.19 5.13 93.41 15.54
Months in Canada 74.48 16.44 68.23 25.50 70.61 31.72 71.02 24.77
MomED 6 4.70 1.14 4.91 0.71 4.41 1.36 4.66 1.16
NV 0.90 60 17.15 5.72 32.98 10.87 44.30 8.05 31.45 14.04
EPA 0.92 20 9.91 5.05 12.98 4.70 14.11 4.77 12.36 5.06
EDA 0.78 27 8.36 4.00 10.58 2.46 9.76 3.74 9.59 3.37
ECA 0.91 15 6.83 2.08 9.70 2.70 11.43 2.54 9.50 2.93
EWR 0.95 76 37.45 11.28 46.26 7.79 53.27 10.82 45.50 11.62
EV 0.82 60 32.17 4.57 34.30 4.30 39.49 7.09 35.02 6.03
ERC 0.89 36 14.17 7.18 21.42 5.12 24.86 5.79 20.14 7.55
CCA 0.83 15 7.03 3.32 9.76 3.40 10.41 2.45 9.22 3.47
CWR 0.98 135 19.86 7.44 35.72 19.70 45.92 27.43 35.35 23.47
CV 0.83 60 29.13 6.76 36.30 6.31 39.46 7.44 34.98 8.09
CRC 0.90 36 6.26 6.46 12.60 8.48 20.46 8.66 12.77 9.98
Note: Months in Canada 5Number of months lived in Canada; MomED 5maternal education; NV 5nonverbal
reasoning; EPA 5English phonological awareness; EDA 5English derivational awareness; ECA 5English
compound awareness; EWR 5English word reading; EV 5English vocabulary; ERC 5English reading
comprehension; CCA 5Chinese compound awareness; CWR 5Chinese word reading; CV 5Chinese
vocabulary; CRC 5Chinese reading comprehension.
30 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Construction of SEMs
We constructed SEM models to examine the relationships among the variables. Before
performing the SEM analyses, we first calculated hierarchical regressions to compare the
pattern of relationships across the three grades. Regressions with Chinese reading
comprehension, Chinese receptive vocabulary and English receptive vocabulary as the
dependent variables revealed the same patterns of relationships across the three grades.
The variables predicting English reading comprehension were also very similar for the
three grades. The only difference was that English vocabulary was a marginally
significant predictor of English reading comprehension in Grade 1, while it was a
significant predictor in Grades 2 and 4. Because the relationships among the variables
were almost identical across the three grades, to increase power, we conducted the SEM
analyses on the whole sample. Moreover, because regressions and SEM models yielded
highly similar results, only SEM results are reported to avoid redundancy.
Two SEM models were created with AMOS 17.0 to explore the within- and cross-
language relationships among morphological awareness, word reading, vocabulary and
reading comprehension. The baseline model contained only within-language paths. In the
cross-language model, cross-language paths were added between the measures of
phonological awareness, morphological awareness, word reading, vocabulary and reading
comprehension. The correlations reported in Table 2 were used to inform model
construction. The earlier versions of the models contained all possible paths that
displayed significant correlations. In the final versions, nonsignificant paths were deleted
to create more parsimonious models and to increase the number of degrees of freedom
(df). Age and nonverbal reasoning were significantly correlated with most reading
measures in Chinese and English and were subsequently entered as control variables in
Table 2. Pearson correlation matrix of variables partialling for age.
Months
in
Canada
Mom
ED
NV EPA EDA ECA EWR EV ERC CCA CWR CV CRC
Months in
Canada
.05 .18
*
.19
*
.33
***
.03 .27
**
.39
***
.21
*
.10 .35
***
.40
***
.31
***
MomED – .20
*
.37
***
.41
***
.27
***
.37
***
.37
***
.38
***
.27
**
.15 .24
**
.10
NV – .34
***
.38
***
.61
***
.42
***
.26
**
.51
***
.22
*
.11 .29
***
.40
***
EPA – .54
***
.49
***
.69
***
.47
***
.59
***
.36
***
.16 .17
***
.27
***
EDA – .41
***
.71
***
.57
***
.66
***
.22
*
.08 .06 .12
ECA – .53
***
.44
***
.59
***
.32
***
.14 .30
***
.41
***
EWR – .59
***
.79
***
.24
**
.05 .11 .21
*
EV – .57
***
.13 .19 .04 .03
ERC – .29
***
.07 .22
*
.28
***
CCA – .45
***
.63
***
.36
***
CWR – .53
***
.69
***
CV – .39
***
Note: Correlations with Months in Canada and MomED have not partialled out the influence of age.
Months in Canada 5Number of months lived in Canada; MomED 5maternal education; NV 5nonverbal
reasoning; EPA 5English phonological awareness; EDA 5English derivational awareness; ECA 5English
compound awareness; EWR 5English word reading; EV 5English vocabulary; ERC 5English reading
comprehension; CCA 5Chinese compound awareness; CWR 5Chinese word reading; CV 5Chinese
vocabulary; CRC 5Chinese reading comprehension.
*
p.05;
**
p.01;
***
p.001.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 31
Copyright r2011 UKLA
both models. Maternal education and number of months lived in Canada were entered
initially, but removed from the final models because adding these variables reduced
model fits, and did not produce any significant relationships with word reading,
vocabulary or reading comprehension.
We explored bidirectional paths between morphological awareness and the outcome
variables in the cross-language model. Although longitudinal data are ideal for testing
reciprocal effects, cross-sectional data can be useful for capturing reciprocal relationships
if the model has proper specification (Wong & Law, 1999). Various fields of research
(e.g. industrial, organisation, political sciences) have used cross-sectional data to test
reciprocal relationships in SEM (Benda & Corwyn, 2000; Everland, Hayes, Shah &
Kwak, 2005; Wong, Wong, Hui & Law, 2001). We applied this analytic technique,
following the considerations of Wong and Law (1999), to investigate the reciprocal
relationships in the cross-language model. Notably, our model supported a bidirectional
path between English compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary. That is, English
compound awareness significantly predicted Chinese vocabulary, and Chinese vocabu-
lary also significantly predicted English compound awareness. The final versions of the
within- and the cross-language models are displayed in Figures 1 and 2, respectively.
1
Comparison of within- and cross-language models
The model fit indices for the within- and cross-language models are displayed in Table 3.
All scores were mean centred before analysis to increase interpretability when comparing
parameter values. Multiple indices were used to assess model fit. A w
2
to df ratio o3 and
a CFI 4.95 suggest good fit. RMSEA values .05 suggest good fit, values o.08
suggest satisfactory fit and values .10 suggest poor fit. Deviance statistics were
calculated by taking the difference of the w
2
,df, AIC and BCC values between models. If
the deviance statistics are significant, the model with the lower values (i.e. w
2
, AIC and
BCC) is the better fitting model (Kenny, Kashy & Cook, 2006).
As shown in Table 3, the within-language model produced adequate fit on measures of
w
2
/df and CFI but not RMSEA, w
2
(39) 599.80, po.001, w
2
/df 52.56, RMSEA 5.11,
CFI 5.95. The cross-language model produced excellent fit, w
2
(33) 541.81, p5.140,
w
2
/df 51.27, RMSEA 5.04, CFI 5.99 and it fit significantly better than the within-
language model for all three tests, Dw
2
(6) 557.99, DAIC(6) 545.99, DBCC(6) 544.72,
po.001. These results suggest that the addition of the cross-language paths significantly
improved the model fit. We calculated the required sample size and power for the cross-
language model according to the method outlined in MacCallum, Browne and Cai (2006),
and Kim (2005). Based on the RMSEA fit statistic, alpha value, and df, the cross-
language model required a minimum sample size of 106; our actual sample size
(N5137) was larger than the required sample size. In addition, the cross-language model
had an excellent power rating of .90.
Path analysis for the cross-language model
Table 4 presents standardised (b) parameter estimates and the unique variance explained
(DR
2
) for the cross-language model. The outcome variables are listed above the
corresponding independent variables.
Within-language relationships. The cross-language model detected a number of
significant within-language relationships. In English, phonological awareness was a
32 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
significant predictor of word reading (b5.73; DR
2
5.53, po.001). Word reading
predicted vocabulary (b5.71; DR
2
5.50, po.001), and derivational awareness (b5.69;
DR
2
5.47, po.001). English compound awareness was related to both English word
reading (b5.48; DR
2
5.23, po.001) and English vocabulary (b5.17; DR
2
5.03,
p5.033). English reading comprehension was predicted by five variables. English word
reading was the strongest predictor (b5.51; DR
2
5.26, po.001). The other predictors
included English receptive vocabulary (b5.12; DR
2
5.01, p5.040), derivational
awareness (b5.11; DR
2
5.01, p5.045), compound awareness (b5.13; DR
2
5.02,
p5.026) and nonverbal reasoning (b5.17; DR
2
5.03, p5.003).
In Chinese, word reading significantly predicted vocabulary (b5.54; DR
2
5.29,
po.001). Chinese vocabulary significantly predicted Chinese compound awareness
(b5.61; DR
2
5.37, po.001). With respect to Chinese reading comprehension, Chinese
word reading was the strongest predictor (b5.58; DR
2
5.34, po.001). Age and
nonverbal reasoning also contributed significant variance to Chinese reading comprehen-
sion, b5.13, p5.041 and b5.24, po.001, respectively.
English
Reading
Comprehension
Chinese
Reading
Comprehension
English
Compound
Awareness
Chinese
Compound
Awareness
English
Vocabulary
Chinese
Vocabulary
English
Word Reading
Chinese
Word Reading
English
Derivational
Awareness
English
Phonological
Awareness
.73***
.71***
.69***
.15*
.58***
.53***
.14*
.51***
.62***
.11*
.67***
.12*
Figure 1. The within-language structural model, showing standardised regression coefficients.
*
p.05;
**
p.01;
***
p.001.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 33
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Cross-language relationships. The most important results of the present study lie in the
cross-language relationships. We found that English phonological awareness predicted
Chinese compound awareness (b5.27; DR
2
5.07, po.001), as well as Chinese word
reading (b5.24; DR
2
5.06, p5.003). Notably, English compound awareness was
significantly predicted by Chinese receptive vocabulary (b5.30; DR
2
5.09, p5.004).
Chinese receptive vocabulary was also predicted by English compound awareness
(b5.31; DR
2
5.10, p5.004). The bidirectional paths suggest a reciprocal relationship
between English compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary in Chinese–English
English
Reading
Comprehension
Chinese
Reading
Comprehension
English
Compound
Awareness
Chinese
Compound
Awareness
English
Vocabulary
Chinese
Vocabulary
English
Word Reading
Chinese
Word Reading
English
Derivational
Awareness
English
Phonological
Awareness
.73***
.24**
.71***
.54***
.31**
.17*
.48***
.30**
.69***
.51***
.12*
.11*
.13*
.27*
.61***
.58***
.18**
Figure 2. The cross-language structural equation model, showing standardised regression coefficients.
*
p.05;
**
p.01;
***
p.001.
Table 3. Comparison of model fit indices.
Model w
2
df p w
2
/df AIC BCC RMSEA CFI Dw
2
DAIC DBCC Ddf Dp
1. Within-
language
99.80 39 o.001 2.56 201.80 212.58 .11 .95
2. Cross-
language
41.81 33 .140 1.27 155.81 167.86 .04 .99 57.99 45.99 44.72 6 po.001 for Dw
2
,
DAIC, DBCC
Note: Critical w
2
value (df 55) 12.59 for p.05 and 22.46 for p.001.
34 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
bilinguals. Another important finding was that English compound awareness was
significantly related to Chinese reading comprehension (b5.18 DR
2
5.03, p5.005).
Thus, English compound awareness transferred to both Chinese receptive vocabulary and
Chinese reading comprehension.
Discussion
The present study investigated cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in
Chinese–English bilingual children from the first to fourth grade. We compared a model
with cross-language paths with a baseline model that included within-language paths
only. The cross-language model produced significantly better fits, providing clear
evidence for cross-language transfer.
Table 4. Standardised coefficients (b) and unique variance (DR
2
) explained in the cross-language model.
Variable bDR
2
English
English word reading
EPA .73 .53
***
English vocabulary
EWR .71 .50
***
English compound awareness
EWR .48 .23
***
EV .17 .03
*
CV .30 .09
**
English derivational awareness
EWR .69 .47
***
English reading comprehension
NV .17 .03
**
EWR .51 .26
***
EV .12 .01
*
EDA .11 .01
*
ECA .13 .02
*
Chinese
Chinese word reading
EPA .24 .06
**
Chinese vocabulary
CWR .54 .29
***
ECA .31 .10
**
Chinese compound awareness
EPA .27 .07
*
CV .61 .37
***
Chinese reading comprehension
Age .13 .02
*
NV .24 .06
***
CWR .58 .34
***
ECA .18 .03
**
Note: NV 5nonverbal reasoning; EPA 5English phonological awareness; EDA 5English derivational
awareness; ECA 5English compound awareness; EWR 5English word reading; EV 5English vocabulary;
CWR 5Chinese word reading; CV 5Chinese vocabulary. The dependent (outcome) variables are listed above
the independent (predictor) variable(s) and the associated standardised regression coefficient (b) and amount of
unique variance explained (DR
2
).
*
p.05;
**
p.01;
***
p.001.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 35
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Cross-language relationships between morphological awareness and vocabulary
To our knowledge, this was the first study to examine transfer of morphological awareness
to vocabulary in bilingual children. Consistent with our prediction, English compound
awareness was a significant predictor of Chinese vocabulary for the Chinese–English
bilinguals. This cross-language transfer is likely due to the fact that Chinese and English
have similar compounding structures. As previously discussed, compounds in both
languages are right-headed, and tend to be transparent in meaning. The similarities explain
why compound awareness developed in English transfers to Chinese and enhances Chinese
vocabulary development. By contrast, English derivational awareness was not related to
Chinese vocabulary. Because Chinese has very few derived words, it is not surprising that
English derivational morphological awareness does not facilitate Chinese vocabulary.
Notably, transfer of compound awareness was only observed from English to Chinese.
Chinese compound awareness, on the other hand, was not significantly related to English
vocabulary. This finding suggests that the direction of morphological awareness transfer
is influenced by the morphological structure of the language of the outcome variable.
Since Chinese has a larger number of compounds than English, compound awareness
plays a more prominent role in developing Chinese vocabulary. In contrast, compound
awareness is less important for developing English vocabulary, thus there is little need for
cross-language transfer to occur. Previous research on transfer of morphological
awareness has indicated that the direction of transfer is influenced by relative proficiency
levels of L1 and L2 (Deacon et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2010). Our findings, together with
those of Wang et al. (2006, 2009), suggest that another determining factor is the structure
of the languages and writing systems involved.
The present study provides preliminary evidence for a reciprocal relationship between
English compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary. English compound awareness was
predicted by Chinese as well as English vocabulary, while it also predicted Chinese
vocabulary. It is particularly interesting that Chinese vocabulary was linked, not only
with English compound awareness, but also with English vocabulary through English
compound awareness. The pattern of relationships demonstrated in our model, in line
with Cummins’ (1981) interdependent hypothesis, underscores mutual facilitation
between L1 and L2 oral proficiency in bilingual children. A reciprocal relationship
between compound awareness and vocabulary has been previously reported in
monolingual children (e.g. McBride-Chang et al., 2008). Our study is the first to extend
this relationship to cross-language variables. Admittedly, the reciprocal relationship was
tentative, as it was based on cross-sectional data, but it points to the possibility that a
‘true’ relationship exists between the variables. Of course, this finding needs to be
replicated by future longitudinal studies.
It should also be noted that we did not observe a reciprocal relationship between
compound awareness and vocabulary within either English or Chinese. In English,
vocabulary was a significant predictor of compound awareness, whereas compound
awareness did not predict vocabulary. Because English vocabulary contains compound
words, it is not surprising that vocabulary development leads to more advanced
compound awareness. Growth in root words also facilitates compound awareness,
because compounds are formed by root words. On the other hand, because only a small
percentage of English words are compound words, the impact of compound awareness on
overall English vocabulary is limited. It is not entirely clear why a reciprocal relationship
was not found between Chinese compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary. One
36 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
possibility is that the Chinese and English compound awareness tasks tapped the same
underlying construct (Zhang et al., 2010), as suggested by the significant correlation we
observed between the two measures. The underlying construct might have been better
captured by the English compound awareness measure in our data. As a result, the
English measure was a significant predictor of Chinese vocabulary, while the Chinese
measure was not due to shared variance. Of course, the unidirectional relationship
observed in our cross-sectional data does not rule out the possibility that a reciprocal
relationship between Chinese compound awareness and Chinese vocabulary can emerge
in longitudinal data, as demonstrated in McBride-Chang et al. (2008). In any case, the
explanations we offer for the lack of within-language reciprocal relationships can only be
speculative due to the limitation of the cross-sectional design, and have to be confirmed
with future longitudinal and interventional research.
Cross-language contribution of morphological awareness to reading comprehension
Another important finding of our study was that English compound awareness explained
individual differences in Chinese reading comprehension, after considering the effects of
Chinese word reading, Chinese vocabulary and Chinese compound awareness. Our
finding, confirming that of Wang et al. (2006), suggests that English compound
awareness transfers to Chinese reading comprehension. As discussed previously, this
transfer is likely based on the shared compounding structure between Chinese and
English. Importantly, we extended Wang and colleagues’ study by including Chinese
word reading and Chinese compound awareness as control variables in our analysis, both
of which have been shown to predict Chinese reading comprehension in previous
research involving monolingual Chinese children (e.g. Ku & Anderson, 2003; Leong,
Hau, Tse & Loh, 2007; Leong & Ho, 2008; Leong, Tse, Loh & Hau, 2008; Li et al., 2002;
Shu et al., 2006). Including these two within-language control variables strengthens the
evidence of cross-language transfer. Furthermore, Chinese and English compound
awareness tasks were significantly correlated in our study, suggesting that these tasks
may tap the same underlying construct. The shared variance between these tasks may
explain why the English compound awareness task, but not the Chinese compound
awareness tasks, was significantly related to Chinese reading comprehension.
Again, we observed unidirectional transfer from English compound awareness to
Chinese reading comprehension, but not from Chinese compound awareness to English
reading comprehension. The direction of transfer from morphological awareness to reading
comprehension is similar to that from morphological awareness to vocabulary. This
remarkable similarity further supports the notion that the direction of transfer is influenced
by the morphological features of the language of the outcome variable. Because
compounding is the most salient feature of Chinese morphology, compound awareness
developed in English can transfer to Chinese and facilitate both Chinese vocabulary and
reading comprehension. In contrast, the best predictors of English reading comprehension
were English word reading and English morphological awareness. Chinese compound
awareness made little contribution after controlling for these within-language variables.
Conclusion
To recapitulate, the present study produced several important findings. First, we found
that English compound awareness, but not English derivational awareness, transferred to
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 37
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Chinese and enhanced the development of Chinese vocabulary and reading comprehen-
sion. In addition, there was a reciprocal relationship between English compound
awareness and Chinese vocabulary. These findings provide strong evidence for the
transfer of morphological awareness between Chinese and English. The aspect of
morphological awareness (compound vs derivational) that transfers, and the direction of
transfer (from L1 to L2 vs from L2 to L1) are both influenced by the morphological
structures of the languages involved. Transfer occurs when the target morphological
structure is similar across children’s L1 and L2. The direction of transfer seems to be
determined particularly by the morphological structure of the language of the outcome
variable – because reading Chinese requires more compound awareness, cross-language
transfer of compound awareness is more likely to be observed from English to Chinese.
The results of this study expand our understanding of cross-language transfer of
metalinguistic awareness. It seems that the nature of morphological awareness is different
from that of phonological awareness. Phonological awareness transfers across languages,
regardless of the nature of the script used to represent them (e.g. Chen et al., 2010;
Durgunog
˘lu et al., 1993; Gottardo et al., 2001). For this reason, it is considered to be a
common underlying competence (e.g. Genesee, Geva, Dressler & Kamil, 2006; Koda,
2007). In contrast, different aspects of morphological awareness exhibit different transfer
patterns. Derivational awareness and inflectional awareness have been observed to
transfer between two alphabetic languages that are rich in derivation and inflection
(Deacon et al., 2007; Ramı
´rez et al., 2010; Saiegh-Haddad & Geva, 2008; Schiff & Calif,
2007). Our study and Wang et al. (2006, 2009) demonstrate that awareness of compounds
transfers between Chinese and English due to shared compounding structures. Taken
together, available evidence supports the notion that the transfer of morphological
awareness is conditioned by the morphological features of the languages involved.
Furthermore, evidence of transfer observed between Chinese and English also provides
insights beyond that observed between closely related languages. Since Chinese and
English do not share any cognates, what transfers is not concrete vocabulary knowledge,
but abstract metalinguistic understanding. It could be understanding of compounding
rules or the ability to derive compound meaning from constituent morphemes or both.
2
The present study has several limitations. First, the study adopted a cross-sectional
design. Although cross-sectional data offer useful information about reciprocal relation-
ships among variables, rigorous longitudinal studies, especially studies with experimental
intervention, are needed to better understand these relationships. Second, our analyses
were based on a mixed sample combining children from Grades 1 to 4. Power restrictions
constrained our ability to performance grade-level analyses. Future studies should
conduct such analyses to examine the development of literacy skills in bilingual children
and compare the pattern of cross-language transfer across different grades. Finally, in our
study, English vocabulary was measured with a shortened version of PPVT. While PPVT
is a widely used standardised vocabulary measure, it contains few compound words. This
may have reduced the chance of observing transfer from Chinese compound awareness to
English vocabulary, as well as the chance of observing a significant impact of English
compound awareness on English vocabulary. It will be beneficial to include an
experimental measure of English compound vocabulary in future studies.
Despite the limitations, results of this study offer convincing evidence for cross-language
transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Our study has
important educational implications. Even though Chinese and English are two languages
represented by very different writing systems, developing English proficiency may have a
38 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
positive impact on Chinese vocabulary and reading comprehension through the transfer of
compound awareness. However, teachers should closely monitor the development of
English derivational awareness in Chinese children, especially in those who immigrated
recently, as this aspect of morphological awareness is not supported by their L1.
Notes
1. The control variables are not displayed in the figures of the within- and cross-language models in an attempt
to simplify the diagrams. The direct effects of the control variables on reading comprehension are displayed
in Table 4.
2. We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out.
References
Benda, B.P. & Corwyn, R.F. (2000). A theoretical model of religiosity and drug use with reciprocal
relationships: A test using structural equation modeling. Journal of Social Service Research, 26, 43–67.
doi:10.1080/01488370009511336.
Carlisle, J.F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L.B. Feldman (Ed.),
Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carlisle, J.F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on
reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190. doi:10.1023/A:1008131926604.
Carlisle, J.F. & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary
years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239–254. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_3.
Carlisle, J.F. & Stone, C.A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research
Quarterly, 40, 428–449. doi:10.1598/RRQ.40.4.3.
Carlo, M.S., August, D., Mclaughlin, B., Snow, C.E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D.N. et al. (2004). Closing the gap:
Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms.
Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215. doi:10.1598/RRQ.39.2.3.
Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, J. & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children’s
vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 615–
631. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9127-9.
Chen, X., Xu, F., Nguyen, T.–K., Hong, G. & Wang, Y. (2010). Effects of cross-language transfer on first-
language phonological awareness and literacy skills in Chinese children receiving English instruction.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 712–728. doi:10.1037/a0018802.
Chow, B.W.–Y., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H. & Chow, C.S.-L. (2008). Dialogic reading and morphology
training in Chinese children: Effects on language and literacy. Developmental Psychology, 44, 233–244.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.233.
Chung, W. & Hu, C. (2007). Morphological awareness and learning to read Chinese. Reading and Writing: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 441–461. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9037-7.
Cisero, C.A. & Royer, J.M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 275–303. doi:10.1006/ceps.1995.1018.
Clark, E.V., Gelman, S.A. & Lane, N.M. (1985). Compound nouns and category structure in young children.
Child Development, 56, 84–94. doi:10.2307/1130176.
Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E. & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological
processing in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.29.
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language
minority students. In California State Department of Education (Ed.), Schooling and language minority
students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3–49). Los Angeles, CA: National Dissemination and Assessment
Center.
Deacon, S.H. & Kirby, J.R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just ‘more phonological awareness’? The
roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25,
223–238. doi:10.1017.S0124716404001117.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 39
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Deacon, S.H., Wade-Woolley, L. & Kirby, J. (2007). Crossover: The role of morphological awareness in French
immersion children’s reading. Developmental Psychology, 43, 732–746. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.732.
Dunn, L. & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test. (3rd edn). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Service.
Durgunog
˘lu, A.Y., Nagy, W. & Hancin-Bhatt, B.J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonemic awareness.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.453.
Everland, W.P., Hayes, A.F., Shah, D.V. & Kwak, N. (2005). Understanding the relationship between
communication and political knowledge: A model comparison approach using panel data. Political
Communication, 22, 423–446. doi:10.1020/10584600500311345.
Frisson, S., Niswander-Klement, E. & Pollatsek, A. (2008). The role of semantic transparency in the
processing of English compound words. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 87–107. doi:10.1348/00071
2607X181304.
Genesee, F., Geva, E., Dressler, D. & Kamil, M. (2006). Synthesis: Cross-linguistic relationships. In D. August
& T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel
on language-minority children and youth (pp. 153–174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gottardo, A., Yan, B., Siegel, L. & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in
children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-linguistic transfer of phonological
processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 530–542. doi:10.1348/000712607X181304.
Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A. & Cook, W.L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
Kieffer, M.J. & Lesaux, N.K. (2008). The role of morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking
English Language Learners. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 783–804. doi:10.1007/
s11145-007-9092-8.
Kim, K.H. (2005). The relation among fix indexes, power, and sample size in structural equation modeling.
Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 368–390. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1203_2.
Koda, K. (2007). Reading and language learning: Crosslinguistic constraints on second language reading
development. Language Learning, 57(Suppl 1), 1–44. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00411.x.
Ku, Y. & Anderson, R.C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 399–422. doi:10.1023/A:1024227231216.
Kuo, L. & Anderson, R.C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-linguistic perspective.
Educational Psychologist, 41, 161–180. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
LaFrance, A. & Gottardo, A. (2005). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills and reading in
bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 559–578. doi:10.1017/S0142716405050307.
Leong, C.K., Hau, K.T., Tse, S.K. & Loh, K.Y. (2007). Component skills of text comprehension
in less competent Chinese comprehenders. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 75–97. doi:10.1007/s11881-007-
0004-z.
Leong, C.K. & Ho, M.K. (2008). The role of lexical knowledge and related linguistic components in typical and
poor language comprehenders of Chinese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 559–586.
doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9113-7.
Leong, C.K., Tse, S.K., Loh, K.Y. & Hau, K.T. (2008). Text comprehension in Chinese children: Relative
contribution of verbal working memory, pseudoword reading, rapid automated naming, and onset-rime
phonological segmentation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 135–149. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.
100.1.135.
Li, W., Anderson, R.C., Nagy, W. & Zhang, H. (2002). Facets of metalinguistic awareness that contribute to
Chinese literacy. In W. Li, J.S. Gaffney & J.L. Packard (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading acquisition:
Theoretical and pedagogical issues (pp. 87–106). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Press.
Libben, G. (1998). Semantic transparency in the processing of compounds: Consequences for representation,
processing, and impairment. Brain and Language, 61, 30–44. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.135.
MacCallum, R.C., Browne, M.W. & Cai, L. (2006). Testing differences between nested covariance structure
models: Power analysis and null hypotheses. Psychological Methods, 11, 19–35. doi:10.1037/1082-
989X.11.1.19.
Markwardt, F.C. Jr (1998). Peabody Individual Achievement Test – Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American
Guidance Service Inc.
McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., Chow, B.W.–Y., Chow, C.S.–L. & Choi, L. (2006). Metalinguistic skills and
vocabulary knowledge in Chinese (L1) and English (L2). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,
19, 695–716. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-5742-x.
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J., Liu, H., Wagner, R.K., Shu, H., Zhou, A. et al. (2005). Changing models across
culture: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure with vocabulary and word
40 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.009.
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Ng, J.Y.W., Meng, X. & Penney, T. (2007). Morphological structure awareness,
vocabulary, and reading. In R.K. Wagner, A.E. Muse & K.R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition:
Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 104–122). New York: Guilford Press.
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C.P. & Wagner, R.K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely
predicts young children’s Chinese character reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95, 743–751.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.743.
McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J., Shu, H., Fletcher, P., Stokes, S.F. et al. (2008). What’s in a word?
Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29,
437–462. doi:10.1017/S014271640808020X.
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R.K., Muse, A., Chow, B.W. & Shu, H. (2005b). The role of morphological
awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415–435.
doi:10.1017/S014271640505023X.
Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abott, R., Vaughan, K. & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and
other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730.
Nagy, W.E., Berninger, V.W. & Abbott, R.D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to
literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,
134–147. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. & Olsson, J. (2003). Learning morphological and phonological spelling rules: An
intervention study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 289–307. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_6.
Ramı
´rez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E. & Kiefer, H. (2010). Morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English
language learners: Within- and cross-language effects on word reading. Reading and Writing: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 337–358. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9203-9.
Raven, J., Raven, J.C. & Court, J.H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s advanced progressive matrices. (1998 edn).
Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.
Roman, A.A., Kirby, J.R., Parrila, R.K., Wade-Woolley, L. & Deacon, S.H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive
view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
102, 96–113. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004.
Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in
English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.
doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x.
Schiff, R. & Calif, S. (2007). Role of phonological and morphological awareness in L2 oral word reading.
Language Learning, 57, 271–298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00409.x.
Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S. & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia:
Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.122.
Shu, H., Meng, X., Chen, X., Luan, H. & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese:
Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11, 311–329. doi:10.1002/dys.310.
Sun, H.L., Sun, D.J., Huang, J.P., Li, D.J. & Xing, H.B. (1996). The description on the corpus system of modern
Chinese studies. In Z.S. Luo & S.L. Yuan (Eds.), Studies of Chinese and Chinese character in the computer
era. (pp. 459–466). Tsing Hua: Tsinghua University Publisher.
Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K. & Rashotte, C.A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin,
TX: PRO-ED.
Wang, M., Cheng, C. & Chen, S. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese–
English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 542–553. doi:10.1037/0022-
0663.98.3.542.
Wang, M., Yang, C. & Chen, C. (2009). The contribution of phonology, orthography, and morphology in
Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 291–314. doi:10.1017/
S0142716409090122.
Wong, C.-S. & Law, K.S. (1999). Testing reciprocal relations by nonrecursive structural equation models using
cross-sectional data. Organizational Research Methods, 2, 69–87. doi:10.1177/109442819921005.
Wong, C.-S., Wong, Y.-T., Hui, C. & Law, K.S. (2001). The significant role of Chinese employees’
organizational commitment: Implications for managing employees in Chinese societies. Journal of Word
Business, 36, 326–340. doi:10.1016/S1090-9516(01)00058-X.
Woodcock, R.W. (1984). Woodcock language proficiency battery. Allen, TX: DLM.
TRANSFER OF MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 41
Copyright r2011 UKLA
Yuan, C.F. & Huang, C.N. (1998). [Chinese morphemes
and compounds: A corpus study]. [Teaching Chinese as a Second
Language, 3], 7–12.
Zhang, J., Anderson, R.C., Li, H., Dong, Q., Wu, X. & Zhang, Y. (2010). Cross-language transfer of insight into
the structure of compound words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 311–336.
doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9205-7.
Xi Chen is an associate professor at OISE/University of Toronto. She has extensive research
experience in monolingual and bilingual Chinese children’s language and literacy development.
She has been the principal investigator of an SSHRC standard research project that examines the
development of morphological, orthographic, phonological and visual skills in monolingual and
bilingual Chinese children and the contribution of these skills to reading.
Adrian Pasquarella, Katie Lam and Yang C. Luo are PhD students under Xi Chen’s supervision
at OISE/University of Toronto. All of them have played a key role in the SSHRC-funded research
on Chinese children’s literacy.
Gloria Ramirez is an assistant professor at Thompson Rivers University. Her research has
examined the development and transfer of morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking ELLs
(English Language Learners) and Chinese-speaking ELLs.
Received 30 September 2010; revised version received 19 November 2010.
Address for correspondence: Xi Chen, Department of Human Development and
Applied Psychology, Ontario Institute of Studies in Education, University of Toronto,
252 Bloor Street West, 9th Floor, Toronto, ON, Canada M5S 1V6. E-mail:
xchenbumgardner@oise.utoronto.ca
42 PASQUARELLA, CHEN, LAM, LUO and RAMIREZ
Copyright r2011 UKLA
... Given the different characteristics of each language's morphology, one might expect that morphological awareness in Chinese and English would be relatively independent among Chinese-English bilinguals. However, contrary to this expectation, some studies have indicated a positive association between morphological awareness in the two languages, at least when operationalized with the same aspect of morphology (i.e., compounding; Ke & Xiao, 2015;Pasquarella et al., 2011;Wang et al., 2006;Yang et al., 2017). For example, one study with a Chinese-English bilingual sample in Canada suggested that English compounding awareness explained 21% of the variance in Chinese compounding awareness, and Chinese compounding awareness explained 18% of English compound awareness (Luo et al., 2014). ...
... RQ2: Do any of the cognitive skills have a cross-language effect on morphological BILINGUAL MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS 9 awareness in L1 Chinese and L2 English? We hypothesized that phonological awareness (a proxy for phonology in the triangle model) and vocabulary knowledge (a proxy for semantics) would have independent cross-language effects on bilingual morphological awareness because there may be shared processes in phonology and semantics across languages (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2012;Pasquarella et al., 2011). On the other hand, orthographic awareness (a proxy for orthography) would have limited or no cross-language effect due to the non-shared orthographic representations for the two languages. ...
... Since all measures were correlated with age, we focus on the partial correlations and summarize the results here. In line with the findings from previous studies with Chinese-English bilingual children (e.g., Ke & Xiao, 2015;Pasquarella et al., 2011;Yang et al., 2017), morphological awareness in Chinese and English were moderately correlated (r = .51) after controlling for age. ...
Preprint
We examined the within- and cross-language cognitive predictors of morphological awareness in L1 Chinese and L2 English in Hong Kong Chinese children. Three hundred twenty-seven Chinese-English bilingual children (Mage = 87.29 months; 50.4% female) in Grades 1 to 4 were assessed on nonverbal IQ, phonological awareness, vocabulary knowledge, orthographic awareness, and morphological awareness in both languages. Results of path analyses showed that Chinese phonological awareness, vocabulary knowledge, and orthographic awareness all predicted Chinese morphological awareness after controlling for the effects of age and nonverbal IQ. English vocabulary knowledge and orthographic awareness predicted English morphological awareness. In addition, Chinese vocabulary knowledge and morphological awareness predicted English morphological awareness over and above the effects of English vocabulary knowledge and orthographic awareness. These findings suggest that morphological awareness in L1 Chinese and L2 English may share semantic processes, providing evidence for the cross-linguistic associations of bilingual morphological awareness.
... They found that learners' compound awareness, but not derivational awareness, is a significant predictor of reading comprehension. Different results were detected in Pasquarella et al. (2011), wherein both derivational and compound awareness were important predictors of English reading comprehension for Chinese immigrant children in Canada. A similar finding was reported by Zhang and Koda (2013). ...
... Sign language morphology, in terms of its visual-spatial representation of morphemes and its simultaneity, differs significantly from spoken language morphology (Aronoff et al., 2005;Valli et al., 2011). Nevertheless, metalinguistic insights at the morphological level, such as awareness of lexical compounding, might be transferable between sign language and spoken language as in the case of some typologically distant spoken language (Pasquarella et al., 2011;Zhang, 2013). Potential transfer of MA warrants to be tested on signing d/Deaf readers in the future. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents the first systematic review and meta‐analysis on cross‐linguistic correlations between sign language and spoken/written language competences in bilingual d/Deaf learners. A total of 202 effect sizes were identified based on 70 independent samples that appeared in 52 studies from 1981 to 2023 (N = 3570). The effect sizes involved seven sign language correlates (e.g., fingerspelling, phonological awareness [PA], lexico‐semantic knowledge) and eight spoken/written language correlates (e.g., PA, lexico‐semantic knowledge, word reading, reading comprehension). Estimated mean correlations of 26 cross‐linguistic relationships ranged from r = 0.322 (p < 0.001) for sign language PA and reading comprehension to r = 0.645 (p < 0.001) for fingerspelling and word reading. Among other moderators, age/grade (elementary, secondary, vs. university/adult), signer status (native vs. non‐native), program type (bilingual vs. Total Communication/SimCom), and task type (passage vs. sentence comprehension) either showed significant moderation effects or resulted in differing size (small, medium vs. large) in the correlation coefficients among subgroups of primary studies. The meta‐analytic findings lend support to the legitimate application of linguistic interdependence, common underlying proficiency, cross‐linguistic transfer facilitation, and their associated constructs to the educational development of signing/bimodal bilingual learners. The moderator analysis results, in particular, have shed light on the conditioning of linguistic interdependence and transfer and expanded current understandings about the complexity of the issue and its practical implications in bilingual deaf education.
... Therefore, in the bilingual transfer of lexical morphological skills, one must consider both direct and indirect types of crosslinguistic influence. The direct influence is often measured through associations between literacy skills in languages A and B. For instance, cross-linguistic transfer of morphological awareness is robustly observed between Spanish and English (Kuo et al., 2015;Ramírez et al., 2013), whereas such transfer between Chinese and English is predominantly limited to compound awareness (Pasquarella et al., 2011;Zhang et al., 2010). Cross-linguistic similarity effects play a role here as Spanish and English are two Indo-European languages with shared grammatical affixation rules and common lexical items (e.g., de + scribe), whereas Chinese is structurally and lexically distinct. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cross-linguistic interactions are the hallmark of bilingual development. Theoretical perspectives highlight the key role of cross-linguistic distances and language structure in literacy development. Despite the strong theoretical assumptions, the impact of such bilingualism factors in heritage-language speakers remains elusive given high variability in children's heritage-language experiences. A longitudinal inquiry of heritage-language learners of structurally distinct languages – Spanish–English and Chinese–English bilinguals ( N = 181, M age = 7.57, measured 1.5 years apart) aimed to fill this gap. Spanish–English bilinguals showed stronger associations between morphological awareness skills across their two languages, across time, likely reflecting cross-linguistic similarities in vocabulary and lexical morphology between Spanish and English. Chinese–English bilinguals, however, showed stronger associations between morphological and word reading skills in English, likely reflecting the critical role of morphology in spoken and written Chinese word structure. The findings inform theories of literacy by uncovering the mechanisms by which bilingualism factors influence child literacy development.
... Research has illuminated various factors that influence reading comprehension among Chinese learners. For example, the significance of morphological awareness, especially among bilingual individuals, was emphasized by studies such as those conducted by Pasquarella et al. (2011) and Qiao et al. (2021), highlighting its substantial contribution to Chinese reading comprehension. Specifically, Qiao et al. (2021) emphasized the contribution of morphological awareness to reading comprehension in both English and Chinese, indicating that the inclusion of morphological awareness in reading instruction could enhance vocabulary, word reading, and reading comprehension. ...
Article
Full-text available
Language educators and researchers have shown keen interest in enhancing second language (L2) learners’ reading skills, and recently, more emphasis has been placed on multimodal learning. Multimodal learning theories posit that the confluence of auditory and visual stimuli facilitates learners to establish robust referential connections, and thus integrating audio cues has emerged as a strategy to enhance reading performance. While prior research has affirmed the advantages of incorporating audio cues for fostering reading fluency and comprehension in general, the findings are inconsistent. These investigations have predominantly focused on learners of English as a second language. Considering the differences between English and Chinese, especially their relationship between written scripts and oral language, investigating the reading experiences of Chinese language learners can contribute to a fuller understanding of the effect of audio inclusion on L2 learners. This study compared reading comprehension and reading experiences in reading-only and reading-while-listening modes for intermediate-low and intermediate-high-level Chinese language learners. After learners read in two different modes, a T-test was conducted on their comprehension performance followed by a qualitative analysis of their perspectives on their reading experiences. The findings showed a significant influence of audio integration on enhancing reading comprehension among intermediate-low-level Chinese language learners engaged in multimodal Chinese reading, and these learners displayed a preference for audio inclusion. On the other hand, a lack of significance in the effects of supplementary audio cues on reading comprehension was identified for intermediate-high-level learners, who manifested a mixed attitude towards audio integration with reading. This study complements the existing understanding of multimodal reading based on alphabetic language, particularly in English. Moreover, the findings carry pertinent implications for Chinese language reading pedagogy.
... Numerous studies have shown the role of prior linguistic and cognitive abilities in predicting behavioural outcomes of second language learning (Dörnyei, 2014;Ganschow et al., 1998;Koda, 2009;Morgan-Short et al., 2014;Zafar & Meenakshi, 2012). More specifically, morphological awareness, which is the ability to consciously recognize and manipulate morphemes (Carlisle & Feldman, 1995;Ke et al., 2021) has been shown to play a critical role in L2 literacy (Ramirez et al., 2010;Wang et al., 2006) and vocabulary development (Pasquarella et al., 2011). A study of L2 English Grade 4 students showed a strong relation between derivational morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2012). ...
Article
We examined neural mechanisms associated with the learning of novel morphologically derived words in native-Hebrew speakers within the Complementary Learning Systems (CLS) framework. Across four sessions, 28 participants were trained on an artificial language, which included two types of morphologically complex words: linear (root + suffix) with a salient structure, and non-linear (root interleaved with template), with a prominent derivational structure in participants' first language (L1). A third simple monomorphemic condition, which served as baseline, was also included. On the first and fourth sessions, training was followed by testing in an fMRI scanner. Our behavioural results showed decomposition of both types of complex words, with the linear structure more easily learned than the non-linear structure. Our fMRI results showed involvement of frontal areas, associated with decomposition, only for the non-linear condition, after just the first session. We also observed training-related increases in activation in temporal areas specifically for the non-linear condition which was correlated with participants' L1 morphological awareness. These results demonstrate that morphological decomposition of derived words occurs in the very early stages of word learning, is influenced by L1 experience, and can facilitate word learning. However, in contrast to the CLS framework, we found no support for a shift from reliance on hippocampus to reliance on cortical areas in any of our conditions. Instead, our findings align more closely with recent theories showing a positive correlation between changes in hippocampus and cortical areas, suggesting that these representations co-exist and continue to interact with one another beyond initial learning.
... The following assessments were administered in the order described, for all participants: The Sight Word Efficiency and Phonemic Decoding sub-tests from the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE-2; Torgesen et al., 1999); Word Identification and Passage Comprehension subtests from the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised (WRMT-R; Woodcock, 1998); Elision subtest of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP-2; Wagner et al., 2013); orthographic and semantic knowledge tests (adapted from Olson et al., 1985); a shortened version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (M-PPVT-3; Dunn and Dunn, 2007;Wang et al., 2009;Pasquarella et al., 2011); the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999); the Digit Span subtest . Ortho-semantic learning task . . ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: As children become independent readers, they regularly encounter new words whose meanings they must infer from context, and whose spellings must be learned for future recognition. The self-teaching hypothesis proposes orthographic learning skills are critical in the transition to fluent reading, while the lexical quality hypothesis further emphasizes the importance of semantics. Event-related potential (ERP) studies of reading development have focused on effects related to the N170 component—print tuning (letters vs. symbols) and lexical tuning (real words vs. consonant strings)—as well as the N400 reflecting semantic processing, but have not investigated the relationship of these components to word learning during independent reading. Methods: In this study, children in grade 3 independently read short stories that introduced novel words, then completed a lexical decision task from which ERPs were derived. Results: Like real words, newly-learned novel words evoked a lexical tuning effect, indicating rapid establishment of orthographic representations. Both real and novel words elicited significantly smaller N400s than pseudowords, suggesting that semantic representations of the novel words were established. Further, N170 print tuning predicted accuracy on identifying the spellings of the novel words, while the N400 effect for novel words was associated with reading comprehension. Discussion: Exposure to novel words during self-directed reading rapidly establishes neural markers of orthographic and semantic processing. Furthermore, the ability to rapidly filter letter strings from symbols is predictive of orthographic learning, while rapid establishment of semantic representations of novel words is associated with stronger reading comprehension.
... In addition, similarities between the morphological structures of children's home language and the societal language they are acquiring can facilitate learning of specific morphological regularities through cross-language influences, or transfer (Pasquarella et al., 2011;Sun et al., 2023). In the current study, all children had the same linguistic background, with Russian being their home language. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study mapped the trajectory of developing derivational morphological knowledge in Hebrew monolingual and Russian–Hebrew bilingual children. We investigated 2 nd and 4 th graders, using a two-by-two structure along the dimensions of modality (comprehension, production) and type of word (real-word, pseudo-word). Performance in the morphological analogies comprehension tasks improved with grade, and monolingual and bilingual children performed equally well. A different pattern was evident in production tasks. In real-word production, monolingual children were more accurate than bilingual children, but this group difference narrowed with age. In pseudo-word production, monolingual children used more morphological elements than bilingual children, and there was also a tendency towards group differences narrowing with age. Detailed error analyses across all tasks revealed that monolingual children recruited more morphological elements than bilingual children. We present implications for assessment of morphological knowledge, and suggest that morphological intervention is a promising avenue for promoting bilingual children’s success.
Article
The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of implicit and explicit morphological analysis instruction in Spanish, a language characterized by high morphological complexity and relatively consistent letter–sound correspondences. For 3 days, 94 Grade 3 Spanish monolingual students (43 girls; Mage = 8.9 years) were trained on target words containing experimenter-designed suffixes consistent in form and meaning (e.g., the suffix -isba refers to a factory in words such as ‘‘botisba ” [a boot factory] and ‘‘cajisba ” [a box factory]). Explicit and implicit instruction differed in the attention given to the co-occurrence of the suffixes in the target words. One day (immediate posttest) and 1 week (delayed posttest) after training concluded, participants were tested on their learning of the suffixes’ form using a suffix identification task and meaning using a word definition and a multiple-choice task. Results of mixed effects models showed that explicit instruction yielded better results for the learning of the form of the suffixes. Regarding meaning, across-condition differences were detected only in the word definition task; explicit instruction produced better results for both trained and transfer words. We discuss our findings in the context of the grain-size unit theory and examine the interplay between the language’s orthographic and morphological characteristics, considering their impact on classroom instruction.
Article
Full-text available
Parallel measures of phonological, syntactic, and orthographic processing skill and reading were administered in English and in Chinese to 65 children whose 1st language (LI) was Cantonese and whose 2nd language (L2) was English. Phonological skill was correlated across LI and L2. Phonological skill in both LI and L2 was correlated with L2 reading and contributed a unique variance to L2 reading, even though the children's LI was not written in an alphabetic orthography, whereas the 2nd language had an alphabetic orthography. This research adds to a growing body of evidence for cross-language transfer of phonological processing in L2 learning of English-as-a-Second-Language students. Phonological processing skill is strongly related to reading performance in monolingual English speakers (e.g.
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACTS Gaps in reading performance between Anglo and Latino children are associated with gaps in vocabulary knowledge. An intervention was designed to enhance fifth graders' academic vocabulary. The meanings of academically useful words were taught together with strategies for using information from context, from morphology, from knowledge about multiple meanings, and from cognates to infer word meaning. Among the principles underlying the intervention were that new words should be encountered in meaningful text, that native Spanish speakers should have access to the text's meaning through Spanish, that words should be encountered in varying contexts, and that word knowledge involves spelling, pronunciation, morphology, and syntax as well as depth of meaning. Fifth graders in the intervention group showed greater growth than the comparison group on knowledge of the words taught, on depth of vocabulary knowledge, on understanding multiple meanings, and on reading comprehension. The intervention effects were as large for the English‐language learners (ELLs) as for the English‐only speakers (EOs), though the ELLs scored lower on all pre‐ and posttest measures. The results show the feasibility of improving comprehension outcomes for students in mixed ELL‐EO classes, by teaching word analysis and vocabulary learning strategies.
Article
Full-text available
TWO STUDIES were designed to investigate the role of morphemic structure on students' word reading. The first study asked whether familiar morphemes in words facilitate word reading for elementary students. Results showed that lower and upper elementary students read words with two morphemes (derived words with a base word and one suffix, such as shady) faster than words with one morpheme (e.g., lady). The second study was designed to investigate the effects of phonological transparency on middle and high school students' reading of derived words. Results showed that phonologically transparent words, words in which a base form is intact in the pronunciation of the derived word (e.g., classic in classical), were recognized and read faster than derived words that are less phonologically transparent (e.g., colonial), pinpointing an aspect of morphemic structure that affects reading derived words. The results indicate that reading derived words is not accomplished solely by familiarity with letter-sound associations or syllables; morphemes also play a role. Results also suggest value in emphasizing morphemic structure in models of word-reading acquisition.
Article
Full-text available
We looked at the effects of teaching 7- and 8-year-old children morphological and phonological distinctions. Some of those given morphological training and some of those given phonological training were also taught how to represent these distinctions in writing. All 4 intervention groups did better than the control group in a standardized test of reading after the intervention. There were gains in children's use of morphological spelling rules but not in their use of conditional phonologically based spelling rules. The improvement in the use of morphological rules in spelling was confined to groups trained in morphology. Training in phonology also had a beneficial effect on the use of morphology in reading. The results are interpreted within the framework of a dual-route model of learning to read and spell.
Article
Full-text available
This study explores emerging lexical processes that may be the foundation for children's acquisition of morphological knowledge and the relation of these processes to reading comprehension. First and third graders were given two tasks involving lexical analysis of morphologically complex words. Two years later, they were given a measure of processing derived words in sentence contexts and a test of reading comprehension. The results support the view that the development of morphological processing in the elementary years might depend on access to representations of full forms, base forms, and affixes. Further, semantic and syntactic knowledge of morphemes was related to morphological processing of sentences and contributed to reading comprehension in the late elementary years.
Chapter
This study evaluated the role of metalinguistic awareness in learning to read Chinese. Reading ability depends not only on children’s basic abilities to comprehend and use language (i.e., linguistic knowledge) but also on metalinguistic knowledge. According to our theory, two facets of metalinguistic awareness important for learning to read Chinese are phonological awareness and morphological awareness. To evaluate this theory, batteries of test were developed and administered to representative samples of first- and fourth grade students. The tests assessing phonological awareness consisted of syllable reversing, onset deletion, final deletion and tone judgment. The tests to assess morphological awareness were radical awareness, morpheme awareness and homographic awareness. The tests to assess reading comprehension were character knowledge, syntactic awareness, cloze completion, sentence reading, pinyin reading, and story reading. Altogether data was collected from 400 first-grade and 400 fourth grade students in ten average-level primary schools. Structural equation models and communality and uniqueness analysis confirmed that morphological awareness and phonological awareness are important in learning to read Chinese, and second, that morphological awareness is the more important of the two
Article
Given the morphophonemic nature of the English orthography, surprisingly few studies have examined the roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading. This 4-year longitudinal study (Grades 2-5) compared these two factors in three aspects of reading development: pseudoword reading, reading comprehension. and single word reading. Morphological awareness contributed significantly to pseudoword reading and reading comprehension, after controlling prior measures of reading ability. verbal and nonverbal intelligence, and phonological awareness. This contribution was comparable to that of phonological awareness and remained 3 years after morphological awareness was assessed. In contrast, morphological awareness rarely contributed significantly to single word reading. We argue that these results provide evidence that morphological awareness hits a wide-ranging role in reading development, one that extends beyond phonological awareness.
Article
This study of 1093 adolescents from 6 public high schools was designed to test a hypothesized model formulated by theoretical elaboration of control theory with elements from social learning theory using structural equation modeling procedures. The primary purpose was to establish that religion is relevant to illicit drug use when its direct, indirect and reciprocal effects are tested within a more complete system of relationships than found in existing studies. Most aspects of the model were supported by data and religion had direct inverse effects on illegal drug use for all adolescents studied. However, the feedback effects of drug use on religion were significant only among younger adolescents and females. Other age and gender differences were observed when the hypothesized model was tested with structural equation modeling procedures. The implications of these findings were discussed in regard to future conceptual work and intervention.