Article

Depressive realism: A meta-analytic review

Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA.
Clinical psychology review (Impact Factor: 7.18). 05/2012; 32(6):496-509. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.05.004
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT

The current investigation represents the first meta-analysis of the depressive realism literature. A search of this literature revealed 75 relevant studies representing 7305 participants from across the US and Canada, as well as from England, Spain, and Israel. Results generally indicated a small overall depressive realism effect (Cohen's d=-.07). Overall, however, both dysphoric/depressed individuals (d=.14) and nondysphoric/nondepressed individuals evidenced a substantial positive bias (d=.29), with this bias being larger in nondysphoric/nondepressed individuals. Examination of potential moderator variables indicated that studies lacking an objective standard of reality (d=-.15 versus -.03, for studies possessing such a standard) and that utilize self-report measures to measure symptoms of depression (d=.16 versus -.04, for studies which utilize structured interviews) were more likely to find depressive realism effects. Methodological paradigm was also found to influence whether results consistent with depressive realism were found (d's ranged from -.09 to .14).

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: David M Fresco
  • Source
    • "In fact, overall, their control judgments were closer to the objective contingency programmed by the experimenter. This finding was labeled " depressive realism " with the implication that the depressed recognized their lack of control because it was consistent with motivational and negative bias aspects of depression (Ackermann and DeRubeis, 1991; Haaga and Beck, 1995; Moore and Fresco, 2012). Thus, illusory control was conceived as evidence of healthy biases and optimism, serving as a protective factor for mental healthiness (Taylor and Brown, 1988). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Perceived control in contingency learning is linked to psychological wellbeing with low levels of perceived control thought to be a cause or consequence of depression and high levels of control considered to be the hallmark of mental healthiness. However, it is not clear whether this is a universal phenomenon or whether the value that people ascribe to control influences these relationships. Here we hypothesize that values affect learning about control contingencies and influence the relationship between perceived control and symptoms of mood disorders. We tested these hypotheses with European university samples who were categorized as endorsing (or not) values relevant to control-individualist and collectivist values. Three online experimental contingency learning studies (N 1 = 127, N 2 = 324, N 3 = 272) were carried out. Evidence suggested that individualist values influenced basic learning processes via an effect on learning about the context in which events took place. Participants who endorsed individualist values made control judgments that were more in line with an elemental associative learning model, whilst those who were ambivalent about individualist values made judgments that were more consistent with a configural process. High levels of perceived control and individualist values were directly associated with increased euphoric symptoms of bipolar disorder, and such values completely mediated the relation between perceived control and symptoms. The effect of low perceived control on depression was moderated by collectivist values. Anxiety created by dissonance between values and task may be a catalyst for developing mood symptoms. Conclusions are that values play a significant intermediary role in the relation between perceived control and symptoms of mood disturbance.
    Full-text · Article · Sep 2015 · Frontiers in Psychology
  • Source
    • "It is also interesting to relate the free-energy approach to the literature on Depressive Realism, a claim that depressed people are sometimes better at evaluating instrumentality than nondepressed people (Alloy and Abramson, 1979; Alloy et al.). The claim appears robust, if small: a recent meta-analysis of 75 studies indicated a small overall depressive realism effect, although both depressed and non-depressed individuals showed a substantial " optimism bias " (Moore and Fresco, 2012). Some compelling model-driven research suggests the effect may be driven by contextual processing differences, rather than depressed individuals having consistently low expectation of control (Msetfi et al., 2005). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Major Depressive Disorder is a debilitating and increasingly prevalent psychiatric condition (Compton et al., 2006; Andersen et al., 2011). At present, its primary treatments are antidepressant medications and psychotherapy. Curiously, although the pharmacological effects of antidepressants manifest within hours, remission of clinical symptoms takes a number of weeks-if at all. Independently, support has grown for an idea-proposed as early as Helmholtz (von Helmholtz, 1924)-that the brain is a prediction machine, holding generative models for the purpose of inferring causes of sensory information (Dayan et al., 1995; Rao and Ballard, 1999; Knill and Pouget, 2004; Friston et al., 2006; Friston, 2010). If the brain does indeed represent a collection of beliefs about the causal structure of the world, then the depressed phenotype may emerge from a collection of depressive beliefs. These beliefs are modified gradually through successive combinations of expectations with observations. As a result, phenotypic remission ought to take some time as the brain's relevant statistical structures become less pessimistic.
    Full-text · Article · Feb 2015 · Frontiers in Psychology
  • Source
    • "On the contrary, women with body image discordance (i.e., those with a perceived actual silhouette smaller than actual) were less likely to report high CES-D scores; this finding, however, was not statistically significant (see Table 4). The concept of " depressive realism, " which states that depressed persons lose the positive bias that allows them to see themselves in an unjustifiably positive light (Moore and Fresco 2012), suggests that depressed women would also experience less body image discordance (that is, they would not perceive their body size to be smaller than actual). This concept could explain why women with body image discordance also reported lower levels of depressive symptoms. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: With aging, women's bodies undergo changes that can affect body image perception, yet little is known about body image in midlife. The purpose of this study was to examine associations between body image and depressive symptoms in Caucasian and African-American midlife women from the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) Chicago site. Body image was measured using the Stunkard Adult Female Figure Rating Scale, and a clinically significant level of depressive symptoms was defined as Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) score of ≥16 (N = 405; N = 63 (15.6 %) with clinically significant levels of depressive symptoms). Differences between perceived actual, perceived ideal, and actual body size and responses to questions concerning weight satisfaction and attractiveness were examined using logistic regression for associations with a CES-D score of ≥16. Women with body image dissatisfaction (odds ratio (OR) = 1.91; p = 0.04) or who perceived themselves as "unattractive" (OR = 7.74; p < 0.01) had higher odds of CES-D of ≥16. We found no significant difference by race. Our results were not confounded by BMI. These results suggest that midlife women with poor body image may be more likely to have clinically significant levels of depressive symptoms. Larger prospective studies are needed to better understand this association.
    Full-text · Article · Mar 2014 · Archives of Women s Mental Health
Show more