ArticlePDF Available

Neural Correlates of Religious Experience

Authors:
  • Evangelisches Krankenhaus Duisburg-Nord / Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf

Abstract and Figures

The commonsense view of religious experience is that it is a preconceptual, immediate affective event. Work in philosophy and psychology, however, suggest that religious experience is an attributional cognitive phenomenon. Here the neural correlates of a religious experience are investigated using functional neuroimaging. During religious recitation, self-identified religious subjects activated a frontal–parietal circuit, composed of the dorsolateral prefrontal, dorsomedial frontal and medial parietal cortex. Prior studies indicate that these areas play a profound role in sustaining reflexive evaluation of thought. Thus, religious experience may be a cognitive process which, nonetheless, feels immediate.
Content may be subject to copyright.
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Neural correlates of religious experience
Nina P. Azari,
1,2
Janpeter Nickel,
1
Gilbert Wunderlich,
1,3
Michael Niedeggen,
4
Harald Hefter,
1
Lutz Tellmann,
3
Hans Herzog,
3
Petra Stoerig,
4
Dieter Birnbacher
5
and Ru
Èdiger J. Seitz
1
1
Department of Neurology, University Hospital Du
Èsseldorf, Germany
2
The Iliff School of Theology/University of Denver, 2201 S, University Blvd, Denver, Colorado 80210, USA
3
Institute of Medicine, Research Center, D-52407 Ju
Èlich, Germany
4
Department of Physiological Psychology, Heinrich-Heine-University, D-40225 Du
Èsseldorf, Germany
5
Philosophical Institute, Heinrich-Heine-University, D-40225 Du
Èsseldorf, Germany
Keywords: brain, human, neuroimaging, PET, religion
Abstract
The commonsense view of religious experience is that it is a preconceptual, immediate affective event. Work in philosophy and
psychology, however, suggest that religious experience is an attributional cognitive phenomenon. Here the neural correlates of a
religious experience are investigated using functional neuroimaging. During religious recitation, self-identi®ed religious subjects
activated a frontal±parietal circuit, composed of the dorsolateral prefrontal, dorsomedial frontal and medial parietal cortex. Prior
studies indicate that these areas play a profound role in sustaining re¯exive evaluation of thought. Thus, religious experience
may be a cognitive process which, nonetheless, feels immediate.
Introduction
For more than a century, the nature of religious experience has been a
topic of considerable scholarship and debate, yet virtually nothing is
known about its biological foundations. The commonsense view of
religious experience is that it is a preconceptual, immediate affective
event (James, 1902; Schleiermacher, 1958. It should be noted that
James in The Principles of Psychology (1890) was reluctant to
contrast thought and feeling). Work in the philosophy of mind and
psychology of religion have challenged this view, suggesting that
religious experience may be a cognitive attributional phenomenon
(Proudfoot & Shaver, 1975; Shaver, 1975), constituted not simply by
the felt immediacy, but by a causal claim regarding a religious source
for the personal experience. Therein resides the authority and `force'
of the experience (Proudfoot, 1985), rendering religious experience a
unique and persistent personal event.
We studied a group of self-identi®ed religious subjects, who
attributed their religious experience to a biblical psalm, in order to
explore for the ®rst time using functional neuroimaging the brain
areas involved in religious experience. While the view that religious
experience is a preconceptual feeling would predict the activation of
limbic brain areas engaged by emotion, attribution theory would
predict brain areas mediating reasoning to be activated. We show
that, during religious recitation, religious subjects activated a frontal-
parietal circuit composed of the dorsolateral prefrontal, dorsomedial
frontal and medial parietal cortex, suggesting that religious experi-
ence may be a cognitive process.
Methods
Subjects
Twelve right-handed, healthy, native-German-speaking adults par-
ticipated in this study. All had given informed consent to their
participation. Six were self-identi®ed as religious (2F, 4M; age,
31 63 years), and six were self-identi®ed as non-religious (2F, 4M;
age 26 63 years). Values are given as mean 6SD throughout. The
two groups of subjects were matched on education level (2±5 years
university). The religious subjects were members of a `Free
Evangelical Fundamentalist Community' in Germany, all of whom
reported having had a documented conversion experience, and
interpreted biblical text literally as the word of God. For the
conversion experience and, hence, for their personal religious
experience, the ®rst verse of biblical Psalm 23 was essential. The
religious subjects were teachers at a private secondary school (run by
their same faith community), having been selected for their posts on
the basis of rigorous faith-based criteria. In this regard, they were a
religiously homogeneous group, having religious experience in a
personal reproducible manner which could be standardized for the
purpose of this study. The non-religious subjects were students at the
University of Du
Èsseldorf, who were studying various subjects in the
natural sciences. Interaction with the subjects during all study phases
was conducted in German by native German speakers. The religious
and non-religious subjects did not differ signi®cantly on imaginability
and verbal traits (Imaginability: religious subjects, 27 67; non-
religious subjects, 25 67, group differences P> 0.5; Verbal:
religious subjects, 27 68; non-religious subjects, 26 66, group
differences P> 0.6) (Paivio & Harshman, 1983; this questionaire was
translated into German for the purposes of this study), nor did they
differ signi®cantly on personality or life satisfaction measures
(P> 0.1, groups different) (Fahrenberg et al., 1984; Eysenck &
Eysenck (1975).
Correspondence: Dr Nina P. Azari,
2
The Iliff School of Theology, as above.
E-mail: nazari@du.edu
Received 12 December 2000, revised 8 February 2001, accepted 13 February
2001
European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 13, pp. 1649±1652, 2001 ãFederation of European Neuroscience Societies
Stimuli
The texts used for the different tasks conditions were: `religious': the
®rst verse of biblical Psalm 23 (Die Bibel, 1975); `happy': a well-
known German children's nursery rhyme (Lange & Stephan, 1984);
and `neutral': instructions on using a phone card from the Du
Èsseldorf
telephone book (Deutsche Telekom, 1996). The texts for all stimuli
were matched on length, and the rhyme was specially chosen because
it was familiar to all subjects and was not associated with music. All
subjects were able to recite from memory both the psalm and rhyme
at the time of the PET session. According to their responses in
prestudy interviews, the religious subjects regarded the induction of
repeated, transient religious states in a single scanning session as
antithetical to religious experience (and disrespectful to their faith).
Thus, they found it acceptable (and were asked) to induce in
themselves and then sustain for the duration of a given scanning
session a unique religious state.
PET scanning
Positron-emission tomography (PET) imaging was approved by the
Ethics Committee of Heinrich-Heine-University Du
Èsseldorf
(Application #1301). The PET-regional cerebral blood ¯ow (rCBF)
data were acquired as described elsewhere (Herzog et al., 1996;
Duncan et al., 2000). For this study we used a 24-ring ECAT
EXACT-HR camera (resolution 4 mm in-plane, 9 mm FWHM, slice
distance 2.4 mm). The session for each subject included six scans
separated by 10 min. For each scan, 555 MBq of
15
O butanol were
injected into the right brachial vein, ¯ushed with saline. PET
scanning began at the moment of the injection and lasted for 60 s.
The 40 s of dynamically recorded head uptake were used for
calculation of the rCBF data. PET image slices were reconstructed
using a Hanning ®lter to an effective image resolution of 9 mm
(FWHM) with a slice distance of 2.425 mm. Image analysis was done
using SPM97d (http://www.®l.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).
Subjects were PET-scanned in six conditions, inclusive of `reli-
gious' and appropriate control conditions: reading silently or reciting
(eyes covered) biblical Psalm 23 (1, `religious-read'; 2, `religious-
recite'); reading silently or reciting (eyes covered) the children's
nursery rhyme (3, `happy-read'; 4, `happy-recite'); reading silently the
set of instructions (5, `neutral-read'); and while lying quietly (eyes
covered) (6, `rest'). In the recite conditions (eyes covered), subjects
started the task at injection time. In the read conditions, presentation of
the stimuli were on a ¯at-screen monitor; horizontal display extent was
»12 degrees. For the 30-s preinjection engagement period of the task,
the ®rst half of the text was displayed on the screen and the subject read
the text aloud. At the time of the injection and the start of the scanning,
the second half of the text was displayed on the screen and the subject
read the text silently. These conditions ensured that the subject was
fully engaged in the task by the start of the scan, and that all subjects
were engaged in the same stimulus material during a given task
condition. Task order was pseudo-randomized (`rest' was always ®rst,
and for the religious and happy conditions, `read' was always before
`recite'). The felt-quality of each target state was assessed with the
Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988)
before and after each PET scan.
Results
During recitation of this psalm, the religious subjects successfully
attained the religious state (Table 1). And, as predicted from previous
TABLE 1. Self-assessment ratings for the target state in religious and non-religious subjects after the PET session
Happy-
read
Happy-
recite
Religious-
read
Religious-
recite
Neutral-
read
Religious subjects
Achieve 5.8 61.9 5.7 61.4 6.3 61.4 8.5 60.8* ² 8.17 62.6*
Sustain 6.7 61.8 6.2 61.2 6.7 61.9 8.3 61.6* ² 8.17 62.6*
Non-religious subjects
Achieve 6.8 61.3 8.7 60.8* ² 4.2 62.2 5.3 62.6 9.0 60.6*
Sustain 6.8 61.2 8.2 61.2* ² 4.8 62.5 5.0 63.0 8.8 61.0*
Values are mean 6SD for the target state (scale of 1±10, max is 10) in religious and non-religious subjects. *P< 0.05, paired t-test, different from other task
conditions;
²
P< 0.05, two-tailed, independent t-tests, groups different. Note that only the religious subjects achieved the religious state, and only the non-religious
subjects achieved the happy state; both groups achieved the neutral state.
FIG. 1. Signi®cant activations for the contrast `religious-recite' vs. `rest' in
religious subjects, rendered onto canonical T1-weighted image of SPM97d
(P< 0.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) (see also Table 2).
Shown are the left, dorsal and right view of the brain. Scans for each
subject were realigned and spatially normalized onto the PET template, and
smoothed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel with FWHM set at 20 mm.
The SPM grey matter threshold was set to its default value. For task
comparisons, an ANCOVA (analysis of covariance) model was ®tted to the
data for each voxel.
1650 N. P. Azari et al.
ã2001 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1649±1652
studies in psychology of religion, demonstrating that religious
attributions are commonly directed toward countering or improving
negative or `crisis' situations (Spilka et al., 1992), the religious
subjects showed a trend toward decreased negative affect during the
religious state [PANAS scores for religious subjects in the `religious-
recite' condition: negative postscan (14.0 64.5) < negative prescan
(15.8 66.1), P< 0.09. No other task condition showed changed
PANAS values in these subjects, and the non-religious subjects
showed no changes in PANAS values (P> 0.5)]. Note that religious
experience must be described from the perspective of the subject
(Proudfoot, 1985), because the subject's interpretation thereof is
central to (constitutive of) the experience. Moreover, there is no
acceptable, subject-independent de®nition of religion or religious
(Sharpe, 1983). Thus, attainment of a target state can be determined
solely on the basis of subjective self-assessment. The subject
achieved and sustained the target state if and only if the subject
reported that this was the case (and not, if the reverse).
The PET images showed a speci®c, signi®cant, activation of the
right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the religious subjects during the
religious state as compared with non-religious subjects. Also as
compared to a happy state and a neutral read condition these areas
were observed (Fig. 1, Table 2). During the religious state, the
religious subjects showed few additional activations, including the
dorsomedial frontal cortex and the right precuneus. These activations
involved areas that are engaged in cognitive processes, while limbic
areas (including the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex) activated by
emotional stimuli (Morris et al., 1996; Paradiso et al., 1997) did not
show rCBF changes. As expected, however, the non-religious
subjects showed a speci®c left amygdala activation during the
happy state (x= ±20, y= ±8, z = ±22; P< 0.001). Therefore, our
results strongly support the view that religious experience is a
cognitive attributional phenomenon.
Discussion
Recent neuropsychological and functional imaging studies suggest
that the prefrontal cortex holds representations of knowledge
structured in the form of cognitive schemas (Partiot et al., 1995).
Cognitive schemas are mental representations containing organized
prior knowledge about speci®c domains, inclusive of speci®cations of
the causal relations among the attributes therein (Taylor & Crocker,
1981). Religious attributions are made in accordance with religious
schemas, which consist in organized knowledge about religion and
religious issues, and include reinforced structures for inferring
religiously related causality of experienced events (Spilka &
McIntosh, 1995). Prefrontally localized schemas manage such
memories stored as representations in posterior brain areas
(Grafman & Hendler, 1991), and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
is critical for memory retrieval and conscious monitoring of thought
(Fletcher et al., 1998; McIntosh et al., 1999; Duncan et al., 2000;
Gallagher et al., 2000). The medial parietal area activated in the
religious subjects was the precuneus, which has strong anatomical
connections to prefrontal cortex (Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Petrides &
Pandya, 1984), and plays a key role in visual memory (Fletcher et al.,
1995). The dorsomedial frontal area we observed corresponds to the
presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) (Rizzolatti et al., 1998).
Pre-SMA receives strong inputs from the prefrontal cortex, as well as
from the cerebellum (Luppino et al., 1993), the latter of which now is
understood to participate in a wide variety of both cognitive and
noncognitive processes (Allen et al., 1997). Converging evidence
from both animal and human studies indicates that pre-SMA is
important in automatically controlling the readiness for action
processed by lateral parieto-frontal circuits (Deiber et al., 1991;
Tanji et al., 1996), and sustaining the preparation to act based upon
the current contents of working memory (Jahanshahi et al., 1995;
Haxby et al., 2000).
Thus, we suggest that religious experience may be a cognitive
process, mediated by a pre-established neural circuit, involving
dorsolateral prefrontal, dorsomedial frontal and medial parietal
cortex. Because religious attributions are made in anomalous or
ambiguous situations, when a person does not know what to expect or
what to do, yet actively and persistently seeks a solution, a persistent,
internally generated `readiness' emerges, which subsequently serves
to re-activate the religious schema in the presence of salient religious
cues (Proudfoot, 1985). For the religious subjects of this study,
biblical Psalm 23 was the salient religious cue. There is substantial
evidence from the psychology of religion to suggest that people are
`prepared' for religious experiences (Spilka et al., 1996). This
`readiness' is probably mediated by the dorsomedial frontal cortex,
leading to the commonly reported felt immediacy of religious
experience. The experience, however, becomes religious when the
subject has consciously identi®ed it as consistent with the subject's
own religious schema (Proudfoot, 1985). This cognitive process most
probably involves the dorsolateral prefrontal and medial parietal
cortex (Fletcher et al., 1995; Partiot et al., 1995). Consequently,
subsequent self-initiated religious actions will reinforce the subject's
personal religious schema (Proudfoot, 1985; Spilka & McIntosh,
1995).
Notably, the religious experience studied here did not involve
activation of limbic areas, although the religious subjects were
TABLE 2. Peak activations for `religious-recite' in religious subjects
Brain region
Coordinates Z-scores
xyzVs. `rest' Vs. non-religious Vs. `happy-recite'
R dorsolateral prefrontal (BA 9) 24 50 34 3.74** 3.29** 3.78**
L dorsolateral prefrontal (BA 9) ±28 36 32 3.62** ± ±
dorsomedial frontal (pre-SMA) (BA 6) 7 10 68 3.58** 2.74* 3.32**
R medial parietal (precuneus) (BA 7) 14 ±82 46 3.57** ± 3.32*
L cerebellum ±2 ±48 ±20 3.31** ± 2.99*
The Z-scores are shown for the comparisons `religious-recite' vs. `rest' in religious subjects (vs. `rest'); religious vs. non-religious subjects in `religious-recite'
(vs. non-religious); and `religious-recite' in religious subjects vs. `happy-recite' in non-religious subjects, i.e. group3task interaction effect, (vs. `happy-recite') at
**P< 0.001, *P< 0.01, uncorrected. Coordinates (x,y,zin space of Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template) and selection of foci according to
conventions of SPM97d. Brain regions (approximate Brodmann areas ± BAs) estimated from Talairach & Tournoux, 1988), after adjustment as appropriate for
differences between MNI and Talairach coordinates (http://www.mrc cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/mnispace.html).
Brain acitivity and religious experience 1651
ã2001 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1649±1652
positively affected by the recital of Psalm 23. Thus, the religious
experience was not an emotional experience, nor an arousal
comparable to that of the happy state which we observed in the
non-religious subjects. Admittedly we used highly standardized
stimuli to identify a neurobiological correlate of the concept of
`religious experience' in this initial study. A challenge for future
work will be to explore transient religious states and the evolution of
other varieties of religious experience. It may turn out that `religious
experience' can be divided eventually into a variety of subprocesses,
as has been, for example, the case with memory.
Acknowledgements
Supported by grants to N.P. Azari from the Alexander von Humboldt
Foundation and the Deutscher Akademischer Austausch Dienst (DAAD), and
SFB 194 of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to R.J.S. and H.H.
Abbreviations
PANAS, Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale; PET, positron emission
tomography; pre-SMA, presupplementary motor area; rCBF, regional cerebral
blood ¯ow.
References
Allen, G., Buxton, R.B., Wong, E.C. & Courchesne, E. (1997) Attentional
activation of the cerebellum independent of motor movement. Science,275,
1940±1943.
Deiber, M., ±P., Passingham, R.E., Colebatch, J.G., Friston, K.J., Nixon, P.D.
& Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1991) Cortical areas and the selection of movement:
a study with positron emission tomography. Exp. Brain Res.,84, 393±402.
Deutsche Telekom, A.G. (1996) Telefonbuch (1996/97) fu
Èr Du
Èsseldorf.
Dienstleistungen rund ums Telefon: T-Card. Deutsche Telekom AG,
Germany, p. 3.
Die Bibel (1975) Der gute Hirte, revised text [oder die ganze Heilige Schrift
des alten und neuen Testaments nach der U
Èbersetzung Martin Luthers].
Deutsche Bibelstiftung, Stuttgart, pp. 581±519.
Duncan, J., Seitz, R.J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., Newell,
F.N. & Emslie, H. (2000) A neural basis for general intelligence. Science,
289, 457±460.
Eysenck, H.J. & Eysenck, S.B.G. (1975) Manual of the Eysenck Personality
Questionaire. [Hodder & Stoughton, London. (EPQ-R, German translation:
Ruch, W. (1999).] Z Differentielle Diagnostische Psychologie,20, 1±24.
Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R. & Selg, H. (1984). Das Freiburger
Persoenlichkeitsinventar FPI, 4 Rev. Au¯. Hogrefe, Go
Èttingen.
Fletcher, P.C., Frith, C.D., Baker, S.C., Shallice, T., Frackowiak, R.S.J. &
Dolan, R.J. (1995) The mind's eye ± Precuneus activation in memory-
related imagery. Neuroimage,2, 195±200.
Fletcher, P.C., Shallice, T., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J. & Dolan, R.J.
(1998) The functional roles of the prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. II.
Retrieval. Brain,121, 1249±1256.
Gallagher, H.L., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P.C. & Frith, C.D. (2000)
Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of `theory of mind'
in verbal and non-verbal tasks. Neuropsychologia,38, 11±21.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1988) Topography of cognition: parallel distributed
networks in primate association cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.,11, 137±156.
Grafman, J. & Hendler, J. (1991) Planning and the brain. Brain Behav. Sci.,
14, 563±564.
Haxby, J.V., Petit, L., Ungerleider, L.G. & Courtney, S.M. (2000)
Distinguishing the functional roles of multiple regions on distributed
neural systems for visual working memory. Neuroimage,11, 380±391.
Herzog, H., Seitz, R.J., Tellmann, L. & Mu
Èller-Ga
Èrtner, H.-W. (1996)
Quantitation of regional cerebral blood ¯ow using an autoradiographic-
dynamic approach in positron emission tomography. J. Cereb. Blood Flow
Metab.,16, 645±649.
Jahanshahi, M., Jenkins, I.H., Brown, R.G., Marsden, D., Passingham, R.E. &
Brooks, D.J. (1995) Distinguishing the functional roles of multiple regions
on distributed neural systems for visual working memory. Brain,118,
913±933.
James, W. (1902) Varieties of Religious Experience. Longs, Greens, New
York.
Lange, S. & Stephan, W. (1984) Das ist der Daumen. In Lange, S. & Stephan,
W. (eds), Das Grosse Buch der Kinderreime. Diogenes-Verlag, Zu
Èrich,
p. 38.
Luppino, G., Matelli, M., Camarda, R. & Rizzolatti, G. (1993) Corticocortical
connections of area F3 (SMA-proper) and area F6 (pre-SMA) in the
macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol.,338, 114±140.
McIntosh, A.R., Rajah, M.N. & Lobaugh, N.J. (1999) Interactions of
prefrontal cortex in relation to awareness in sensory learning. Science,
284, 1531±1533.
Morris, J.S., Frith, C.D., Perrett, D.I., Rowland, D., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J.
& Dolan, R.J. (1996) A differential neural response to fearful and happy
facial expressions. Nature,383, 812±815.
Paivio, A. & Harshman, R. (1983) Factor analysis of a questionaire on imagery
and verbal habits and skill. Can. J. Psychol,37, 461±483.
Paradiso, S., Robinson, R.G., Andreasen, N.C., Downhill, J.E., Davidson, R.J.,
Kirchner, P.T., Watkins, L., Ponto, L. & Hichwa, R.D. (1997) Emotional
activation of limbic circuitry in elderly normal subjects in a PET study. Am.
J. Psychiatry,154, 384±389.
Partiot, A., Grafman, J., Sadato, N., Wachs, J. & Hallett, M. (1995) Brain
activation during the generation of non-emotional and emotional plans.
Neuroreport,6, 1269±1272.
Petrides, M. & Pandya, D.N. (1984) Projections to the frontal cortex from the
posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp Neurol.,228,
105±116.
Proudfoot, W. (1985). Religious Experience. University of California Press,
Berkeley.
Proudfoot, W. & Shaver, K.G. (1975) Attribution theory and the psychology of
religion. J. Scienti®c Study Religion,14, 317±330.
Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G. & Matelli, M. (1998) The organization of the
cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
Neurophysiol.,106, 283±296.
Schleiermacher, F.D.E. (1958). On Religion: Speeches to its Cultured
Despisers. [translation Oman, J.]. Harper & Row, New York.
Sharpe, E.J. (1983). Understanding Religion. St. Martin's Press, Inc., New
York, pp. 33±48.
Shaver, K.G. (1975) An Introduction to Attribution Theory. Winthrop,
Cambridge, MA.
Spilka, B., Brown, G.A. & Cassidy, S.A. (1992) The structure of religious
mystical experience in relation to pre- and post-experience lifestyles. Int. J.
Psychol. Religion,2, 241±257.
Spilka, B., Ladd, K.L., McIntosh, D.N. & Milmoe, S. (1996) The content of
religious experience: The roles of expectancy and desirability. Int. J.
Psychol. Religion,6, 95±105.
Spilka, B. & McIntosh, D.N. (1995) Attribution Theory and Religious
Experience. In Hood, R.W. (ed.), Handbook of Religious Experience.
Religious Education Press, Birmingham, Alabama, pp. 421±445.
Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-Planar Atlas of the Human Brain.
Thieme, Stuttgart.
Tanji, J., Shima, K. & Mushiake, H. (1996) Multiple cortical motor areas and
temporal sequencing of movements. Cogn. Brain Res.,5, 117±122.
Taylor, S.E. & Crocker, J. (1981) Schematic bases of social processing. In
Higgins, E.T., Herman, C.P. & Zanna, M.P. (eds), Social Cognition.
Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A. & Tellegren, A. (1988) Development and validation
of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J.
Personality Social Psychol.,54, 1063±1070.
1652 N. P. Azari et al.
ã2001 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1649±1652
... The studies mainly reported significant results for religious task > secular task and vice versa, from Christian subjects. The studies mainly used fMRI as their imaging modality, except for one PET study (Azari et al., 2001) and one structural MRI study (Kober et al., 2017). The extracted brain coordinates are provided in Supplementary File 2. ...
... In fact, the experimental limitations of inducing religiosity during fMRI scans have been addressed by some of the included studies in the current meta-analysis. For example, some Christian subjects commented that to induce repeated, transient religious states within a single scanning session was antithetical and disrespectful to authentic religious experience; instead, they agreed to induce and sustain the religious state for the entire duration of a scanning session by reciting Psalm 23:1 (Azari et al., 2001). In another study, some Carmelite nuns commented that "God cannot be summoned at will" so that they agreed to recall a mystical experience rather than actually attempting to achieve one, during fMRI scan (Beauregard and Paquette, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Multiple neuroimaging studies have been published to report brain processing of religious behavior related to Christianity, such as prayer and recitation of the Bible. This meta-analysis aimed to pool data across studies to identify brain regions consistently activated in response to such religious tasks. Methods Web of Science, Scopus, and PubMed were queried to identify relevant studies. Brain coordinates and sample size were manually extracted from the identified studies, and entered into a dedicated software called GingerALE to conduct meta-analysis. Results Meta-analytic results based on 11 studies showed that brain processing of Christian behavior was associated with the right middle frontal gyrus and superior frontal gyrus, with a peak location (at 44, 38, 26; cluster size = 760 mm³) preferentially associated with working memory, cognitive task, and executive function according to Neurosynth data. Sub-analyses on Christian subject data revealed no significant results at the pre-defined threshold. With a more liberal threshold, Christian tasks > non-Christian tasks showed activation in the anterior cingulate and medial frontal gyrus (peak at 4, 48, −4; cluster size = 256 mm³) that were frequently associated with reward, self-referential, and reinforcement learning, whereas non-Christian tasks > Christian tasks showed activation in the right middle frontal gyrus (peak at 48, 36, 24; cluster size = 472 mm³) that frequently associated with working memory, executive function, arithmetic, and calculation. Conclusion This study has revealed the relevance of frontal and limbic regions to Christian behavior.
... 3. Further, Nina P. Azari and Dieter Birnbacher (2004) argued against the use of attribution theory and defended an approach informed by the above-mentioned discussions in the philosophy of mind. Their argument is built on neuroimaging studies conducted by Azari and colleagues (Azari et al. 2001), in which religious experience was not associated with arousal, yet experience was felt uniquely religious. The feeling aspect of the experience in these experiments was bound up with the thinking aspect: religious experience, they concluded, emerges as "thinking that feels like something"-without a temporal or logical order of feeling and thinking (Azari and Birnbacher 2004, 915). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter addresses the neuroscientific background of ancient tours of heaven. McNamara’s work on religious experience is used in the context of other philosophical, cognitive, and neuroscientific research. After discussing the definition and phenomenology of subjective experience, the chapter outlines three possible understandings of religious experience in the cognitive research tradition. The chapter then situates ancient tours of heaven in the context of flights and ascents in religious traditions and discusses how neuroscientific evidence sheds light on various aspects of such experiences. In particular, McNamara’s work is combined with Michael Marsh’s study of extracorporeal experiences to account for the two-phase narrative sequence of the tours. The model is applied to the tour of heaven in the Ascension of Isaiah, followed by a brief discussion of other early Jewish and Christian apocalypses.
... The iconography is also quite different, as classical art is generally associated with Roman Catholic churches. Architecture is in fact very relevant for visitors to sacred spaces such as churches and cathedrals in order to produce a more emotional and spiritual response [37]- [39]. It would be interesting and it might be possible to actually crossover the device to an entirely different culture, in which this type of sacred art is not common. ...
... Recently, some neuroscientist tried developing a neuroethology scholarship. There is some evidence to suggest that the dorsomedial frontal cortex mediates the readiness for religious experiences (Azari et al. 2001). However, some studies suggested that although there is a neuropsychological basis of spirituality, but it is not directed to a specific area of the brain. ...
Article
Full-text available
Positive changes or transformations have been the subject of study within spiritual traditions as well as humanistic and transpersonal psychology. The aim of the current study is to understand the process of transformation among Moslems in Indonesia, who follow spiritual practices, to achieve the nafs al-muṭma ínnah [tranquil self]. Ten participants in Yogyakarta province were involved in this study. They were recruited using nafs al-muṭmaʾinnah scale developed by the authors. In-depth interviews of both the participants and their significant others were conducted. To analyse the data, an interpretative phenomenological analysis was employed. Five themes emerged from the data, including deviation from the basis of religious environment, spiritual discipline, transformative experiences, new characters and a continuous process. We conclude that participants experience spiritual-religious transformation and achieve the state of nafs al-muṭma ínnah [tranquil self]. The process of transformation occurs gradually after a sudden experience, and it continuously happens throughout the course of life. Three factors contribute to the transformation, including the role of predisposition, the role of a spiritual guide and the role of spiritual experiences.Contribution: As the study of spiritual-religious transformation is mainly conducted within a Judea-Christian tradition, this study significantly contributes to the literature by providing spiritual-religious transformation from an Islamic-Sufi perspective.
... It has been used to identify the neurological foundations of religious belief (Kapogiannis et al., 2009) religious behavior (Hayward et al., 2011) and personal prayer. (Schjoedt et al., 2009) Neuroscience has been especially helpful in the study of meditation (Fox et al., 2016), mysticism (Newberg & Waldman, 2017), spiritual experience (Beauregard & Paquette, 2006),and personal religious experiences (Azari et al., 2001). Recently, there has been a spate of studies of the psychedelic state (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012) and ayahuasca (Palhano-Fontes et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Neuroscience has become a well-accepted methodological modality in the study of religion, especially of religious behavior, personal prayer, meditation, mysticism, spiritual experience, and personal religious experiences. However, such studies have been performed on individuals only; none have helped scholars understand the neuro-physiological correlates of religious communities, religious interactions, collective liturgical action, or the like. This article introduces the new field of social neuroscience, showing how its primary tool, hyperscanning, is revealing surprising levels of “brain-to-brain synchrony.” Though there are no hyperscanning studies of religious communities yet, the authors suggest that findings about shared attention, interpersonal coordination, and feelings of closeness all have clear parallels in and implications for religious communities. The authors then suggest both directions and cautions for future research.
... NDEs have also been speculatively linked to a number of neurotransmitters in the brain, most frequently endorphins (Carr, 1982), although other models have implicated serotonin, adrenaline, vasopressin, and glutamate (Jansen, 1997;Morse, Venecia, & Milstein, 1989;Saavedra-Aguilar & Gómez-Jeria, 1989). NDEs have also been speculatively linked to a number of anatomic locations in the brain, most often the right temporal lobe (Blanke, Ortigue, Landis, & Seeck, 2002) or the left temporal lobe (Britton & Bootzin, 2004), although other neuroscientists have argued for involvement of the frontal lobe attention area, the parietal lobe orientation area, the thalamus, the hypothalamus, the amygdala, and the hippocampus (Azari et al., 2001;Fenwick, 2001;Newberg & d'Aquili, 1994). Th ese putative neurological mechanisms, for which there is little empirical evidence, may suggest brain pathways through which NDEs are expressed or interpreted, but they do not necessarily imply causal mechanisms. ...
Chapter
Postmaterial spiritual psychology posits that consciousness can contribute to the unfolding of material events and that the human brain can detect broad, non-material communications. In this regard, this emerging field of postmaterial psychology marks a stark departure from psychology's traditional quantum measurements and tenets. The Oxford Handbook of Psychology and Spirituality codifies the leading empirical evidence in the support and application of postmaterial psychological science. Sections in this volume include: personality and social psychology factors and implications; spiritual development and culture; spiritual dialogue, prayer, and intention in Western mental health; Eastern traditions and psychology; physical health and spirituality; positive psychology; and scientific advances and applications related to spiritual psychology. With articles from leading scholars in psychology, medicine, physics, and biology, this text is an interdisciplinary reference for a rapidly emerging approach to contemporary science.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Neurophysiological correlates of religious experiences in worship were analyzed. Method: Sixty evangelical worshippers were recruited for recording religious worship experiences using electroencephalogram. Experimental conditions included resting state, religious and secular songs (both participant selected and researcher provided), a nonmusic condition, and a disturbing 12-tone song. Participants continuously rated their sense of God’s presence using a bar slider. Results: Three hypotheses were confirmed: (1) Religious songs induced greater neurophysiological activation than secular ones, (2) activation was stronger when participants selected the religious song, and (3) stronger religious experiences correlated with enhanced attentional mechanisms and emotional reactions. The study found notable activation in occipital regions but did not support the prevalent temporal involvement and executive inhibition hypotheses. Findings suggested a distinction between religious experience (subjective divine perception) and the religious element (content). Religious experiences were stronger with self-selected songs. Secular songs showed more upper δ and lower θ waves during stronger experiences, indicating relaxation. In the religious condition, upper θ and lower α bands were more active, suggesting heightened focus and awareness, akin to meditation. Conclusions: Results align with the Feedback Loop Model for Religious Experiences, which posits that religious elements and self-selection enhance experiences. Generally, religious conditions showed more upper β activations and less lower α frequencies compared with secular ones, indicating sustained attention and active thinking under religious influence. The study’s unique contribution lies in its novel experimental approach, distinguishing between experiential aspects and religious content and the unexpected involvement of the occipital lobe.
Article
Religious fundamentalism, characterized by rigid adherence to a set of beliefs putatively revealing inerrant truths, is ubiquitous across cultures and has a global impact on society. Understanding the psychological and neurobiological processes producing religious fundamentalism may inform a variety of scientific, sociological, and cultural questions. Research indicates that brain damage can alter religious fundamentalism. However, the precise brain regions involved with these changes remain unknown. Here, we analyzed brain lesions associated with varying levels of religious fundamentalism in two large datasets from independent laboratories. Lesions associated with greater fundamentalism were connected to a specific brain network with nodes in the right orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and inferior parietal lobe. This fundamentalism network was strongly right hemisphere lateralized and highly reproducible across the independent datasets ( r = 0.82) with cross-validations between datasets. To explore the relationship of this network to lesions previously studied by our group, we tested for similarities to twenty-one lesion-associated conditions. Lesions associated with confabulation and criminal behavior showed a similar connectivity pattern as lesions associated with greater fundamentalism. Moreover, lesions associated with poststroke pain showed a similar connectivity pattern as lesions associated with lower fundamentalism. These findings are consistent with the current understanding of hemispheric specializations for reasoning and lend insight into previously observed epidemiological associations with fundamentalism, such as cognitive rigidity and outgroup hostility.
Article
Full-text available
The study of human biology includes exploration of all the genetic and environmental influences on human variation and life history, including impacts of sociocultural and physical environments. Religious practice and spirituality may be one of these influences. There are more than 5.8 billion religiously affiliated adults and children, accounting for 84% of the world's 6.9 billion people. Furthermore, 70% of Americans consider themselves spiritual in some way, including 22% who do not consider themselves religious, and the numbers for Europe are lower but proportionally similar. Such a high rate of religious affiliation and spiritual belief suggests that religion and spirituality could be sociocultural influences on human variation, but human biologists have scarcely attended to their impacts, as indicated by the limited numbers of relevant articles in the two flagship human biology journals. In this article, we discuss why human biologists may have overlooked this important force for human variability and highlight foundational work from human biology and other disciplines that can give our colleagues directions forward. We review the impacts of religion and spirituality at population and individual levels and call for human biologists to attend to the many aspects of religion and spirituality that can impact human biology and are much more than simply influences of denominational affiliation.
Article
We investigated the functional anatomy of self-initiated and externally triggered movements. Six patients with Parkinson's disease off medication and six age-matched normals were assessed All subjects had regional cerebral blood pow (rCBF) measurement with PET and recording of movement-related cortical potentials (MRPs) from frontal (F), fronto-central (FC), central (C) and parietal (P) sites to obtain measures of the Bereitschaftspotential (BP). The tasks were (i) self-initiated extension of the right index finger on average once every 3 s, (ii) externally triggered finger extension with the rate yoked to the self-initiated task, and (iii) rest condition with tones presented at a rate yoked with the self-initiated task. For the self-initiated movements, the amplitude of the early and peak BP were lower in Parkinson's disease relative to normals. For the externally triggered movements, the patients and the normals did not differ on any of the measures of cortical negativity prior to movement For both groups, the late and peak BP components, but not the early component, had a lower amplitude in the externally triggered than the self-initiated movements. In normals, the left primary sensorimotor cortex, the supplementary motor area bilaterally anterior cingulate, the lateral premotor cortex bilaterally the insular cortex bilaterally, the left thalamus and the left putamen, parietal area 40 bilaterally and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) were significantly activated during the self-initiated movements relative to rest. For the normals, greater activation of the right DLPFC during the self-initiated movements was the only area that significantly differentiated them from the externally triggered movements. When Parkinson's disease patients and normals were compared for the self-initiated movements relative to rest, normals showed greater activation of the supplementary motor area and anterior cingulate, left putamen, left insular cortex, right DLPFC and right parietal area 40. When the groups were compared for the externally triggered movements relative to rest the global pattern of blood pow and rCBF change in the two groups did not differ, confirming the absence of group differences in BPs for the externally triggered movements. During the self-initiated movements, the lower amplitude of the early BP in patients with Parkinson's disease as well as the underactivation of the supplementary motor area relative to normals support the premises that (i) the supplementary motor area contributes to the early BT: and (ii) the deficit in self-initiated movements in Parkinson's disease is due to supplementary motor area underactivation. The DLPFC is activated in situations requiring non-routine decision making as in the self-initiated movements.
Article
Recent anthropological and sociological studies have emphasized the cognitive functions of religion, but few have drawn on relevant work by cognitive social psychologists. One current social psychological approach, attribution theory, is especially promising for the study of religion because it deals directly with individuals' interpretations of their own experiences and behavior. Three lines of attribution research are reviewed and each is shown to be useful for understanding certain religious phenomena. A classic conversion experience discussed by William James, the experiences of the apostles at Pentecost, an experimental study of mystical experience, and several examples from an American Nichiren Shoshu group are considered in the light of attribution theory. Possibilities for experimental research are briefly considered.
Article
Functional neuroimaging studies of memory retrieval show consistent activation of the right prefrontal and superior parietal cortex. We examined the specific role of the prefrontal cortex during retrieval with the hypothesis that this region mediates monitoring processes necessary for optimal recall. During functional neuroimaging with PET, subjects retrieved verbal material under two conditions. In the first, an organizational structure had been provided, prior to scanning, and this formed the basis for a monitored memory search while the scan took place. A comparison condition did not require a monitored search because recall was externally cued. In both conditions, when compared with baseline tasks prefrontal cortex and medial parietal activation was observed. Within the right prefrontal cortex activation an anatomical dissociation was seen between the dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex. The dorsal region showed greater activation when monitoring demands were emphasized, while the ventral region showed greater activation when external cueing was emphasized. An unpredicted dissociation within the superior parietal activation was also observed, a dorsal region showing activation during the monitored search task and a more ventral region showing activation under the externally cued condition. The results provide evidence for functional specialization of the right prefrontal cortex for discrete cognitive processes during episodic memory retrieval.
Article
WE STUDIED 12 normal volunteers who were asked to imagine and plan their behavior in emotional and nonemotional situations while their regional cerebral blood flow was measured with positron emission tomography. The dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior temporal cortex were more activated during the nonemotional situation whereas the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior temporal cortex were more activated during the emotional situation. These results demonstrate that distinctive regions of the prefrontal and temporal cortex used to imagine and plan behavior are activated during the expression of emotional and non-emotional plans.