ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Product design has been recognized as an opportunity for differential advantage in the market place. The appearance of a product influences consumer product choice in several ways. To help product development managers in optimizing the appearance of products, the present study identified the different ways in which the appearance of a product plays a role in consumer product evaluation and, hence, choice. In addition, the implications for product design of each role are listed, and managerial recommendations for optimizing the appearance of products are given. Based on a literature review, six different roles of product appearance for consumers are identified: (1) communication of aesthetic, (2) symbolic, (3) functional, and (4) ergonomic information; (5) attention drawing; and (6) categorization. A product's appearance can have aesthetic and symbolic value for consumers, can communicate functional characteristics and give a quality impression (functional value), and can communicate ease of use (ergonomic value). In addition, it can draw attention and can influence the ease of categorization of the product. In a large qualitative study (N=142) it was tested whether these roles indeed exist in consumers' process of product choice and whether they are sufficient to describe the way in which product appearance plays a role for consumers. In addition, qualitative insight into these roles was gained. After making a choice between two answering machines, subjects were interviewed about the reasons for their choice and the product information they used to form the judgments underlying their choice reasons. The six appearance roles indeed proved relevant for consumers and were sufficient to describe the influence of product appearance on product choice. The number of ways in which appearance played a role for consumers differed between 0 and 5; most subjects mentioned two different ways in which appearance influenced their product choice. The aesthetic and symbolic roles were mentioned most often. The preferred shape (e.g., rounded or angular), color, or size were found to differ depending on the way in which product appearance played a role for subjects. For example, bright colors may be valued from an aesthetic point of view but may diminish the impression of quality (i.e., functional value). This makes it difficult to optimize all roles and illustrates that the product value that is most important for consumers when purchasing a specific kind of product should be the starting point in the design of the product appearance. Furthermore, the influence of shape, color, or size on a certain kind of product value—aesthetic, symbolic, ergonomic, or functional—differed between subjects. One person may like a rounded shape, while another may prefer a rectangular shape. This means that the value of guidelines indicating how the perception of a specific kind of product value can be engendered by means of shape, color, and size is limited. This is especially the case for aesthetic and symbolic product value, which are very personal. Therefore it is recommended to test the performance of the appearance of a newly developed product on these six roles with the target group of consumers. Insight into the different ways in which appearance characteristics, such as form and color, may influence consumer choice will increase managers' awareness about how to use product appearance as a marketing tool. In addition, distinguishing these six appearance roles will help product development managers to optimize the product appearance better to market needs, as the roles have different and sometimes even conflicting implications for the design of the product appearance.
1
The Different Roles of Product Appearance in
Consumer Choice
Mariëlle E.H. Creusen and Jan P.L. Schoormans*
This is a preprint version of an Article published in the Journal of Product Innovation
Management © 2005, the Product Development and Management Association
* Mariëlle E.H. Creusen is Assistant Professor of Consumer Research and Jan P.L. Schoormans
is Professor of Consumer Research at Delft University of Technology, Faculty of Industrial
Design Engineering.
Address:
Address correspondence to Mariëlle E.H. Creusen, Delft University of Technology, Faculty of
Industrial Design Engineering, Department of Product Innovation and Management,
Landbergstraat 15, 2628 CE Delft, The Netherlands. E-mail: m.e.h.creusen@tudelft.nl.
2
The Different Roles of Product Appearance in Consumer Choice
Abstract
Product design has been recognized as an opportunity for differential advantage in the market
place. The appearance of a product influences consumer product choice in several ways. To help
product development managers in optimizing the appearance of products, we identified the
different ways in which the appearance of a product plays a role in consumer product evaluation,
and hence, choice. In addition, we list the implications for product design of each role, and give
managerial recommendations for optimizing the appearance of products. ‘
Based on a literature review, six different roles of product appearance for consumers are
identified: communication of aesthetic, symbolic, functional and ergonomic information,
attention drawing and categorization. A product’s appearance can have aesthetic and symbolic
value for consumers, communicate functional characteristics and give a quality impression
(functional value), and communicate ease of use (ergonomic value). In addition, it can draw
attention and influence the ease of categorization of the product. In a large qualitative study (N =
142) we tested whether these roles indeed exist in consumers’ process of product choice, and
whether they are sufficient to describe the way in which product appearance plays a role for
consumers. In addition, we gained qualitative insight into these roles. After making a choice
between two answering machines, subjects were interviewed about the reasons for their choice
and the product information they used to form the judgments underlying their choice reasons.
The six appearance roles indeed proved relevant for consumers and were sufficient to
describe the influence of product appearance on product choice. The number of ways in which
appearance played a role for consumers differed between zero and five, most subjects mentioned
two different ways in which appearance influenced their product choice. The aesthetic and
symbolic roles were mentioned most often.
The preferred shape (e.g., rounded or angular), color or size were found to differ depending
on the way in which product appearance played a role for subjects. For example, bright colors
may be valued from an aesthetic point of view, but may diminish the impression of quality (i.e.,
functional value). This makes it difficult to optimize all roles and illustrates that the product
value that is most important for consumers when purchasing a specific kind of product should be
the starting-point in the design of the product appearance. Furthermore, the influence of shape,
color, or size on a certain kind of product value aesthetic, symbolic, ergonomic or functional
differed between subjects. One person likes a rounded shape, while another prefers a rectangular
shape. This means that the value of guidelines indicating how the perception of a specific kind of
product value can be engendered by means of shape, color and size is limited. This is especially
the case for aesthetic and symbolic product value, which are very personal. Therefore it is
recommendable to test the performance of the appearance of a newly-developed product on these
six roles with the target group of consumers.
Insight into the different ways in which appearance characteristics, such as form and color,
may influence consumer choice will increase managers’ awareness about how to use product
appearance as a marketing tool. In addition, distinguishing these six appearance roles will help
product development managers to better optimize the product appearance to market needs, as the
roles have different and sometimes even conflicting implications for the design of the product
appearance.
3
Introduction
Product design is an opportunity for differential advantage in the market place (e.g., Hammer,
1995; Kotler and Rath, 1984; Löbach, 1976; Lorenz, 1986; Pilditch, 1976; Veryzer, 1995). A
number of companies successfully focus on product design as a competitive tool (see e.g.,
Dumaine, 1991; Nussbaum, 1993; Smith, 1994). Several studies indicate the influence of good
product design on commercial success (e.g., Black and Baker, 1987; Bruce and Whitehead,
1988; Gemser and Leenders, 2001; Roy, 1994; Thackara, 1997). Yamamoto and Lambert (1994)
showed that even for industrial products, appearance has an influence on product preference.
But what does this mean in practice? Which product design will lead to commercial success?
To be able to define some guidelines that can be used in new product development, it is
necessary to look at the role of product design in consumer evaluation. First, it must be
recognized that this role is complex and diverse. There are a number of ways in which product
design influences consumer preference (Bloch, 1995). The design of a product determines
consumers’ first impression of the product and can quickly communicate product advantage. In
addition, the design of a product will generate consumer inferences regarding several product
attributes (Berkowitz, 1987; Bloch, 1995; Pilditch, 1976). Furthermore, product appearance can
provide value in itself; many people like to buy a product that looks aesthetically pleasing. As
the influence of product design on consumer evaluation is often complex, it is difficult to decide
upon during the product development process. For example, a product with bright colors may be
valued aesthetically, but these same colors may give consumers the idea that the product is of
low quality.
To be able to give guidelines for design following from its influence on consumer product
evaluations, it is necessary to first answer the question of what exactly constitutes the value of a
product design for consumers. In order to answer this question, we shall start with an overview
of the different roles of the product design in the purchase decision of consumers. More
precisely, we will describe the influence of what consumers see of the product, that is, its
exterior, in making a purchase decision. We therefore use the term ‘product appearance’ instead
of ‘product design’, as the design of a product also refers to product parts that consumers cannot
see (i.e., the interior of the product). On the basis of a literature review and a large qualitative
study, we describe the implications of these roles for product design and product development.
Product Appearance and Consumer Product Evaluation: A literature Review
In this section we will describe the roles of product appearance in the process of consumer
evaluation and choice. For this aim, we have searched literature in the fields of product
development, product design, consumer behavior, marketing and human factors. The literature
shows that the visual appearance of a product can influence consumer product evaluations and
choice in several ways. Several authors considered the role of product or package appearance in
consumer product evaluation or choice (Bloch, 1995; Garber, 1995; Garber et al., 2000; Veryzer,
1993; Veryzer, 1995). However, they did not explicitly discuss the different ways in which
appearance influences consumer choice and their respective implications for product design. In
addition to these more recent contributions to the literature, the functions of a product in
consumer-product interaction are described in earlier industrial design literature (Löbach, 1976;
Pilditch, 1976; Schürer, 1971). Several of these functions concern product appearance. There are
differences between authors in the number of roles (i.e., functions) of product appearance that
4
they distinguish and the terms they use. For example, communication of ease of use is mentioned
by Bloch (1995) and described as part of the aesthetic function by Löbach (1976), while Veryzer
(1995) calls it the communicative function of a product appearance. If we take all the roles
mentioned in the literature together, the following six roles of product appearance for consumers
can be distinguished: communication of aesthetic, symbolic, functional, and ergonomic product
information, attention drawing and categorization. We will describe these six roles and their
implications for product design below.
Product Appearance and Aesthetic Product Value
The aesthetic value of a product pertains to the pleasure derived from seeing the product, without
consideration of utility (Holbrook, 1980). A consumer can value the ‘look’ of a product purely
for its own sake, as looking at something beautiful is rewarding in itself. When product
alternatives are similar in functioning and price, consumers will prefer the one that appeals the
most to them aesthetically (see for example Figure 1). Aesthetic responses are primarily
emotional or feeling responses and, as such, they are very personal (Bamossy et al., 1983).
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 1 about here
-------------------------------
Several researchers have tried to determine properties of products that are related to aesthetic
appreciation. Innate preferences are proposed for visual organization principles, such as unity
(i.e., congruence in elements), proportion (e.g., ‘the Golden Section’), and symmetry (Hekkert,
1995; Muller, 2001; Veryzer, 1993; Veryzer and Hutchinson, 1998), and an inverted U-shaped
relation is proposed between aesthetic preference and complexity (Berlyne, 1971). Another
property influencing aesthetic judgments is color. The desirability of a color will change
according to the object to which it is applied (e.g., a car or a table), and with the style of the
object (e.g., ‘Modern’ or ‘Georgian’) (Whitfield and Wiltshire, 1983).
In addition to (innate) preferences for certain properties of stimuli, prototypicality is found to
influence the aesthetic response. Prototypicality is the degree to which something is
representative of a category (see also the section about categorization). In several studies,
evidence is found for a positive influence of visual prototypicality on aesthetic preference
(Hekkert, 1995; Veryzer and Hutchinson, 1998; Whitfield and Slatter, 1979). According to
Hekkert et al. (2003), products with an optimal combination of prototypicality and novelty are
aesthetically preferred.
As well as the product-related characteristics mentioned above, there are cultural, social and
personal influences on design taste. For example, color preferences differ between cultures and
in time (Whitfield and Wiltshire, 1983). In addition, personal factors, such as design acumen,
prior experience and personality influence the design taste of consumers (Bloch, 1995).
The influence of an aesthetic judgment on product preference can be moderated by the
perceived aesthetic fit of the product with other products that the consumer owns, or his or her
home interior (Bloch, 1995). Consumers may like a product’s appearance per se, but not buy it
because it does not aesthetically fit into their home interior.
5
Product Appearance and Symbolic Product Value
Consumer goods carry and communicate symbolic meaning (McCracken, 1986). Symbolic value
can even be the key determinant for product selection (Hirschman and Holbrook, 1982), and
account for the selection of products that are clearly inferior in their tangible characteristics
(Levy, 1959). An example of the latter is Philippe Starck’s Juicy Salif lemon squeezer (Lloyd
and Snelders, 2003). The choice for a specific product or brand may convey the kind of person
you are or want to be; consumers use products to express their (ideal) self-image to themselves
and to others (Belk, 1988; Landon, 1974; Sirgy, 1982; Solomon, 1983). Symbolic meaning can
be coupled to a product or brand on the basis of, amongst other things, advertising (McCracken,
1986), country of origin, or the kind of people using it (Sirgy, 1982). But the product itself can
also communicate symbolic value in a more direct way, namely by its appearance.
A product’s appearance communicates messages (Murdoch and Flurscheim, 1983), as it may
look ‘cheerful’, ‘boring’, ‘friendly’, ‘expensive’, ‘rude’, or ‘childish’ (see, for example, Figure
2). -------------------------------
Insert Figure 2 about here
-------------------------------
In addition, a certain style of appearance may evoke associations with a certain time or place
(e.g., the ‘fifties’). Furthermore, the product or package appearance can reinforce the image of a
brand, as the identity of a brand is visually expressed in the appearance of products (Schmitt and
Simonson, 1997). Consumers may couple the meaning of a brand to elements of the physical
appearance of products. In this way, a brand image may transfer to different kinds of products
(see the section about categorization). Many companies therefore make consistent use of certain
design elements, such as a color combination, a distinctive form element or style. For example,
car manufacturers often try to keep different car models recognizable as belonging to the same
brand. The distinctive radiator grill of BMW automobiles is an example of a recognizable design
element. The linking of brand meaning to elements of the product appearance will be easier
when the associations that these elements engender by themselves (e.g., because they are innate
or determined by culture) correspond to the desired brand image. For example, use of bright
colors and a large size, which is associated with aggression (Murdoch and Flurscheim, 1983),
will make it easier to position a car brand as aggressive.
Although there are large individual and time specific differences in the experience of color
and form, there are certain associations that seem to be relatively constant. Overviews of the
influence of form and color on consumer perception of symbolic value (but also ergonomic and
aesthetic value) can be found in Muller (2001), Murdoch and Flurscheim (1983), Schmitt and
Simonson (1997), and Whitfield and Wiltshire (1983). For example, angular forms are associated
with dynamism and masculinity, while roundness evokes softness and femininity (Schmitt and
Simonson, 1997).
Culture is an important determinant of the interpretations that consumers give and the
associations that they have with certain factors of a product’s appearance. For example, color
associations vary from culture to culture (Whitfield and Wiltshire, 1983). In America and
Europe, white stands for purity and brides traditionally are dressed in white, while in Japan it is a
color of mourning. Furthermore, meaning is context dependent. The impression that colors give
may change completely by combining certain colors (Muller, 2001). Also, the meaning of forms
6
and colors may change in time, as meanings are continuously transformed by movements in art,
fashion, etcetera (Muller, 2001).
There is some debate about whether symbolic interpretation is part of the aesthetic
experience. In most literature, aesthetic value is mentioned as both a hedonic impression and a
result of interpretation and representation (Schmitt and Simonson, 1997; Vihma, 1995). We
acknowledge that whether a product is conceived of as beautiful is affected by what it represents
(Vihma, 1995). The same style can be considered ‘good taste’ at one point in time, while being
considered ‘bad taste’ ten years later, because the connotations associated with it or the
interpretations given to it have changed. For example, orange was a modern color for clothes,
furniture and (plastic) products in the seventies, was generally perceived as old-fashioned and
ugly in the eighties, and became used in products and clothing again in the nineties. However,
we feel that aesthetic and symbolic value should be distinguished, as they may have opposite
influences on preference. For example, someone who likes a colorful design may not buy it
because it looks ‘too childish’.
Product Appearance and Functional Product Value
The functional value of a product pertains to the utilitarian functions that a product can perform,
to what you can use it for (Löbach, 1976; Veryzer, 1995). Products differ in the degree to which
they are suited to perform their basic utilitarian function, such as communication or
transportation, but also in quality (e.g., by the technology or materials used) and in features. For
example, you can buy telephones with a ‘re-dial’ and a ‘hands-free’ option. The presence of such
options influences the functional value of the product for consumers. As well as reading verbal
product information or asking others, consumers may form an impression about utilitarian
functions and product quality on the basis of a product’s appearance (Bloch, 1995; Dawar and
Parker, 1994).
The utilitarian functions of a product can be directly obvious from its appearance. When you
see a handle, you know the product is portable. In addition, product appearance can be used as a
cue to infer more important but less readily accessible product attributes (Berkowitz, 1987;
Dawar and Parker, 1994). For example, subjects may infer on first sight that a larger hairdryer
has more power than a smaller one (see Figure 3). Or the appearance as a whole may
communicate quality by looking reliable or solid (Srinivasan et al., 1997; Yamamoto and
Lambert, 1994). Physical product appearance is an important quality signal for consumers
(Dawar and Parker, 1994). As Dickson (1994, p.263) notes: “There is also something intangible
about quality. It resides in the feel, the look, the sound of an item. We may not be able to explain
it, but we know it when we see it.” So product appearance can be proactively used in order to
give consumers a certain impression about the functional product value.
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 3 about here
-------------------------------
Product Appearance and Ergonomic Product Value
The ergonomic value of a product (see Löbach, 1976; Schürer, 1971; Veryzer, 1995) entails the
adjustment of a product to human qualities. Product ergonomics or ‘human factors’ concerns the
7
comprehensibility and usability of a product, the suitability to perform and correctly
communicate its utilitarian functions. Technical functions can be implemented in a product in a
more or less easy to use manner. Usability entails cognitive aspects of use, such as how logical a
product is to operate, as well as emotional aspects, in that it is not frustrating in operation and
gives an enjoyable usage experience (March, 1994). Consumers may form an impression about
the ease of use on the basis of the product appearance (e.g., Norman, 1988).
Consumers have to experience the operation of a product in order to adequately judge it. As
consumers often cannot try out products in a shop or when buying on the Internet, they will use
the product appearance to form an indication of the ergonomic product value (see also Bloch,
1995). By seeing the product, people form an impression about whether handles are easy and
pleasant to hold, and whether buttons will be easy to use (see Figure 4). In order to influence
consumer preference positively, it is not sufficient that a product be simply easy to use.
Consumers must also perceive the product to be easy to use. The appearance of the product
influences consumer perception of aspects such as ease of operation, weight and stability, which
affect the perceived ease of use of a product. For example, an upright-shaped product may be
designed in such a way that it cannot fall over in normal use, but consumers may conclude that it
is not stable after seeing it (see Murdoch and Flurscheim, 1983). Based on this first impression,
they may discard the product. Another example is that a small number of controls (such as
buttons) makes a product look easy to use (Norman, 1988).
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 4 about here
-------------------------------
Attention Drawing Ability of the Product Appearance
Gaining attention is an important first step in enabling consumer product purchase. Attention is
the allocation of information processing capacity to a stimulus (Engel et al., 1995). When a
product ‘stands out’ visually from competitive products, chances are higher that consumers will
pay attention to the product in a purchase situation, as it ‘catches their eye’. For food products,
the attention-drawing ability of a package has been found to heighten the probability of purchase
(Garber, 1995; Garber et al., 2000).
In general, the attention-drawing ability of a product can be enhanced by increasing its size
and by using bright colors. Furthermore, people attend to stimuli that contrast with their
background and are novel, that is, unusual or unexpected (see Engel et al., 1995). Garber (1995)
emphasizes that the visual effect of a product package is relative to a background comprised of
competitor alternatives. For example, the Philips ‘Billy’ handmixer (see Figure 5, second one
from the left) draws attention by its bright colors, that differ from the typical white and other
light colors used in this product category. So in order to design an eye-catching appearance,
product alternatives available on the market and perhaps even the purchase environment
should be taken into account.
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 5 about here
-------------------------------
8
Product Appearance and Categorization
Consumers may use product appearance for categorization (Bloch, 1995; Veryzer, 1995). The
appearance of a product can influence the ease with which a product is categorized, and the
category to which it will be assigned. Product identification will be easier when a product
resembles other products in the same category, that is, when it is more prototypical of the
category (Loken and Ward, 1990). With respect to product appearance, this means that it should
be more visually typical. Garber (1995) defines visual typicality as “the look or appearance that
most consumers would associate with a product category, and by which they identify brands that
belong to the category”. When a product is difficult to categorize based on its appearance,
consumers may not regard the product as a purchase alternative. For example, there might be
some consumers who do not notice that the Philips Alessi coffee maker, with its atypical
appearance, is a coffee maker (see Figure 6, right picture).
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 6 about here
-------------------------------
Using verbal product descriptions, Meyers-Levy and Tybout (1989) found that products that
differ slightly from the prototype are evaluated more positively than products that are either very
typical or very atypical. Schoormans and Robben (1997) confirmed this for package
appearances; a slightly atypical appearance catches attention from consumers, while remaining
acceptable to them. So in general, an appearance that differs slightly from the prototype will be
preferred. In some cases, however, strong differentiation from or strong similarity to the
prototype or another product alternative will be a beneficial strategy. We will treat these cases
below.
For products for which prestige, exclusiveness or novelty are important, an atypical
appearance is advisable. For such products, preference declines when it becomes more widely
available and thus more typical, as uniqueness is valued (Ward and Loken, 1988). An atypical
appearance is also advisable when a product has to be differentiated from other products in the
category, for example when there are many competing alternatives. Strong differentiation may
even lead consumers to first consider the product as a member of its own individual class (Rosch
et al., 1976, p.434). Also, new functional attributes are better communicated by an atypical
appearance. Differentiation from the category decreases comparison with other products from the
category. As a result, distinguishing features are better noticed and found more important (Sujan
and Bettman, 1989). For example, the Dyson vacuum cleaner differs in its appearance from the
prototypical vacuum cleaner, so that consumers more easily perceive its unique mechanism (see
Figure 7). -------------------------------
Insert Figure 7 about here
-------------------------------
On the other hand, when consumers do not find the purchase important or interesting, a
typical appearance is advisable (Alba and Hutchinson, 1987). Typical members of a category
tend to be classified more quickly and accurately (see Loken and Ward, 1990). Therefore,
9
consumers tend to buy typical category members in low-involvement purchases, as they want to
minimize their effort (Hoyer, 1984). One can also design the appearance of a product to resemble
another, well-known and positively valued product alternative. This heightens the probability
that people evaluate the product based on knowledge about, or affect towards, the product it
resembles, which is called exemplar based categorization (Cohen and Basu, 1987). This strategy
may be beneficial when there is one dominant brand in the category with which it is difficult to
compete.
Similarity to a category prototype or a known exemplar may provide consumers with
expectations about certain product attributes, and thereby about the functional, ergonomic,
aesthetic and/or symbolic value of the product. Based on previous experience with Sony
products, one may for example assume that new Sony products are easy to operate, without
evaluating the ease of operation of the specific product at hand. However, category-based
evaluations occur less often for durables than for fast-moving consumer goods (Olshavsky and
Spreng, 1996). So for durables, consumers will tend to process the available information, instead
of deriving a judgment from categorization only.
Product Appearance and Consumers’ Choice Reasons: An Extensive Qualitative Study
In a qualitative study we investigated whether the six roles of product appearance for consumers
that we distinguished on the basis of the literature review do indeed exist in consumers’ product
choices. In addition, we assessed whether these six roles sufficiently describe the way in which
consumers use product appearance in making a product choice, or whether additional roles
should be distinguished. Furthermore, we gained qualitative insight into these roles, as we looked
at the inferences consumers make from aspects of the product appearance, the extent of
difference in consumer product perceptions based on product appearance, and the extent to
which these roles are interrelated. As we used a relatively large number of respondents in this
qualitative study, we were able to quantify results.
Research Method
In a laboratory setting, subjects made a choice between two product alternatives, after which they
were interviewed about the reasons underlying their choice. These choice reasons, and the
information on which they are based, are the focus of the study.
Subjects. Subjects (N = 146) were selected from a consumer household panel. About half of
them are male, ages range from 18 to 65.
Stimuli. The products used were telephone answering machines. Three answering machines
were bought, of which a subset of two was presented to each subject. In this way there were three
different choice sets; each of them was presented to about one-third of the sample. The products
themselves were placed in front of subjects, with accompanying cards showing textual
information about four functional product characteristics derived from product catalogues and in-
store information (see Figure 8). This agrees with the way in which durable products are
generally presented in a purchase situation.
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 8 about here
-------------------------------
10
Procedure. First, subjects read a description of the basic functionality of an answering
machine. This ensured that they at least knew what the product basically can be used for, as can
be expected of a person buying one. Two answering machines were placed in front of the
subject, who was asked not to touch (and thus possibly open) the product. The subject made a
product choice by indicating the answering machine that he or she would be most likely to buy.
After that, an interview into the choice reasons was conducted and recorded on audiotape.
The interviewer first gathered all the subject’s choice reasons, after which she probed further into
each separate reason to be clear about the subject’s precise meaning, why he or she valued the
product attribute concerned, and what information he or she used to make the attribute judgment.
Because of the large number of interviews, two interviewers were used (one of them performed
about two-thirds of the total number of interviews).
The total procedure took about 20 minutes, after which each subject received a written
debriefing and a small monetary compensation.
Analysis and Results
The interviews into the choice reasons were fully transcribed. These transcriptions were the basis
for data-analysis. Data of 142 subjects were used (a total of four subjects either possessed one of
the products from their choice set, had handled the products against instructions, or did not
understand the basic use of an answering machine). Two judges (of which one was also an
interviewer) independently categorized each subject’s considerations that played a role in the
product choice and were based on the product appearance (i.e., were not based on the card
information). Choice reasons that did not fit into one of the categories were assigned to a
‘remaining’ category. There were not many differences between the judges, and these were
discussed until an agreement was reached.
The six roles of the product appearance communication of aesthetic, symbolic, functional,
and ergonomic product information, attention drawing and categorization proved sufficient to
categorize all choice reasons that were based on product appearance. There were a few reasons
that did not fit into the six categories of appearance roles. However, these reasons concerned
textual information drawn from the appearance, such as brand name or the language of the words
underneath the buttons (i.e., in Dutch versus in English).
The relative importance of the appearance roles differed between subjects. Some subjects
valued aesthetics the most, while others found functionalities or quality far more important. Age
did not influence the frequency of mentioning a specific appearance role, gender only influenced
concern about whether the product fit with the home interior and attention drawing (see the
aesthetic role and attention drawing role below). A number of subjects considered one product
alternative superior with respect to one kind of product value (e.g., aesthetic value), and the other
alternative with respect to another kind of product value (e.g., ergonomic value). As a result, they
had to choose between two or more kinds of product value, most often functional and aesthetic
value. For example, one subject chose the digital product although she liked the appearance of
the other product better. Some subjects had to choose between aesthetic value and ergonomic
value, and for example chose the attractive-looking product although the other product looked
easier to operate. In total, 19 subjects chose product alternative D, 49 subjects chose F, and 74
subjects preferred G (see Figure 8).
We will now describe the results for each of the six roles separately. The quotations are
11
translated from Dutch by the authors. Keep in mind that one subject can mention several
different choice reasons, and also several choice reasons belonging to one category. The number
of appearance roles mentioned by subjects varies from one to five, the Mode is two. Figure 9
shows the percentage of subjects mentioning each number of product appearance roles. Subjects
mentioning zero appearance roles based their choice on the card information. Figure 10 shows
the percentage of subjects that mentioned each of the different appearance roles.
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 9 about here
-------------------------------
-------------------------------
Insert Figure 10 about here
-------------------------------
Aesthetic role. This role was mentioned most often: sixty-five percent of the subjects (92)
mentioned an attractive product appearance as a choice reason. An additional ten percent
mentioned the attractiveness of the appearance, but did not base their choice on it because other
aspects were more important to them. Aspects mentioned to play a role in the aesthetic
attractiveness are overall roundedness, size, color and specific details. Several subjects found it
difficult to indicate why they found a specific product more aesthetically attractive, as it was an
instinctive judgment.
Fourty-five percent of all subjects (64) liked a rounded product, that is, alternative F or G,
which looked modern to them. They perceived this as suiting the contemporary design trend, as
many modern products are rounded (e.g., cars, car stereos). For example, one subject said: “And
you also see that with a lot of audio equipment. All that has a more rounded design than
previously, really.” Only very few subjects mentioned disliking a rounded shape; one of them
said: “I personally don’t like rounded sides and such things .. a bit trendy.” Many subjects
disliked alternative D because it is rectangular and straight. Only three subjects liked such a
shape.
Many subjects (12) aesthetically preferred a smaller, compact product, as a larger product is
ungainly and obtrusive. Many subjects considered alternative D to be too big. For example, one
subject said: “I really liked the shape of that small one. I found the other one a bit .. yes, if you
place it in your room, not so .. a bit crude. … I do not like all those big things in my room.”
Several (8) subjects mentioned the ‘closed’ impression of alternative F. Alternative G looks
more ‘open’, as it has a display and a bigger button. Half of these subjects liked this ‘closed’
look, as it makes the product a compact whole. As one subject said: “I found that ‘G’ still has
something cosy about it, also because of the display, I think, and that other one was such a closed
whole …”. The other half disliked a closed impression, because it looks less friendly and
sympathetic. One subject for example said: “Well, that appliance totally was a closed box, such a
black box, and that other one made at least a, yes .. a bit more of a friendly impression, it seemed
to look a bit more open.”
Subjects (32) preferred a certain color because they liked it better or because it fits into their
home environment. One subject for example said: “so I picture it next to the black couch and the
black telephone, then that thing completely fits in”. Several subjects wanted a dark or black
color, some a neutral or soft color. Also, several subjects (10) preferred a product in one color as
12
opposed to multiple colors (referring to alternative D with its two-colored casing and white
buttons), as this makes the product a unified whole and looks more tranquil. For example, one
subject said about alternative D: “The buttons also attracted a little attention, in my opinion,
because they had a different color. It just isn’t a whole”.
Some subjects mentioned visual organization principles as a basis for their aesthetic
preference. Unity was mentioned by a few subjects, referring to the use of one instead of more
colors (see the quotation in the paragraph above). Symmetry was mentioned by one subject, who
said: “and in addition it was symmetrical, say, in its length. I also always like that a bit myself”.
Many subjects (33, or 23.2%) mentioned details that played a role in their aesthetic
judgments, often only after the interviewer probed for this. Some (6) liked the little purple button
on alternative F, as it is funny and playful. Nine subjects disliked the buttons on alternative D, as
they ‘lie on top of the product’, while integrated buttons (on alternative F and G) make a product
smoother. The obtrusiveness of the buttons on alternative D is reinforced by their contrasting
white color. Some subjects liked or disliked the presence of a display from an aesthetic point of
view, or chose the product with a more attractive display. For example, one subject said: “such a
display with numbers, I don’t need that .. that doesn’t look nice in my opinion.” Some
idiosyncratic reasons concerned, for example, the size of the holes in the grid in front of the
loudspeaker, and the material.
Twenty-four subjects (16.9%) mentioned that the product had to fit aesthetically into their
home environment or with other products they already own (such as their telephone). For this
reason, many of these subjects valued a dark or neutral-colored product, and a modern-looking
product (only two subjects found that a less modern answering machine suited their homes
better). For example, one subject said that “the other answering machine would fit less into my
home interior” (referring to alternative D). She further explained: “because we have a modern
interior design with black furniture”. Females more often mentioned the aesthetic fit into their
home as a choice reason than males (χ² = 4.68, p < .05).
Symbolic role. Almost half of all subjects (68, or 47.9%) mentioned that the symbolic
meaning or associations of the product appearance played a role in their product choice.
Additionally, some subjects considered symbolic aspects, but found other aspects more
important on which to base their choice. Almost all subjects mentioning symbolic aspects
mention it as part of an aesthetic judgment; only some mentioned a modern, friendly or serious
look as a choice reason without explicitly calling it aesthetically attractive. So symbolic and
aesthetic value were often intertwined.
Symbolic associations that were mentioned by several subjects are expensive or cheap,
playful, friendly, businesslike, soft, sympathetic, boring and hi-tech. Several subjects (6) mention
that alternative D gives a cheap impression, because of its crude and simple shape. One subject
notes: “Look, obviously straight shapes are easier to manufacture. Therefore I interpret them as
cheaper.” Many subjects (38) mentioned a modern or contemporary (alternative F or G) versus
an old-fashioned or even obsolete impression (alternative D) as a choice reason. A lot of these
subjects mentioned that roundedness or a streamlined shape brings about this modern look. This
roundedness also made alternative F and G look friendly, sympathetic and soft. In contrast, the
rectangular straight product (alternative D) looked old-fashioned, ungainly, bombastic, harsh and
cheap to subjects. They associated alternative D with an old cassette player, a cigar-box, a box of
bricks and a bread-tin. Many mention alternative F as resembling a portable CD player or
Discman, which some find funny and which gives it a contemporary look. For example, one
subject mentioned that alternative D reminds him of “an old-fashioned cassette player”, while
13
alternative F “reminds me much more of a modern car radio”. In addition to the associations
mentioned above, other associations evoked by alternative F are: playful, female, cute with that
sweet little purple button, elegant, refined, chic, more sexy, yuppie-like, flowing, more funny,
more hi-tech-like, but also business-like and boring. Specific remarks referring to alternative G
were: cosier, less boring, more flair, playful, more serious, common.
Functional role. Many subjects (49.3%) based their product choice on the textual
information about functionalities that was presented with the products on cards. However, for 18
subjects (12.7%) the appearance influenced the perceived functional product value. Five subjects
(3.5% of the total sample) derived information about functionalities from the product
appearance, namely the presence of a display or a small indication light. One subject based her
choice on the fact that she saw a ‘rewind’ button on alternative G, which she did not see on
alternative F. In addition, two subjects explicitly mentioned wanting as few features as possible.
According to them, these ‘bells and whistles’ make the product more vulnerable so that it will
break down more easily, while you often do not use them anyway.
Eleven subjects (7.8% of the sample) derived an impression about the reliability and
durability of the product from its appearance. They chose the product that looks more solid or
reliable (most often alternative G), so that it would last longer. Some subjects found it difficult to
specify the characteristics responsible for this; a few mentioned that it was their first impression
or something instinctive. Nevertheless, several subjects mentioned elements that engendered this
impression, such as a flap or display that could easily break, a turning instead of sliding volume
button, a large size, or the roundedness or rather the squareness of the product. For example, one
subject said about alternative G: “It looked more reliable, a bit more solid. It was a bit larger.”
He explained why it looked more solid: “The size was decisive .. maybe the shape, it was
broader than the other one.” A few subjects inferred from its modern styling (referring to the
more rounded shape of alternative F or G) that the product was technologically superior, as it had
been designed more recently. The following part of an interview illustrates why one subject
prefers a modern-looking answering machine: “Yes, maybe it will last longer that way, [it] looks
more reliable .. the other one [alternative D] looks as if it is prehistoric, as if it is out of date or
something, that is the impression it makes .. old.” However, another subject preferred alternative
D for its “more functional appearance”, as he thought that “most often with these futuristic
products, they look slick and finished, but they are usually not really solid.” (referring to
alternative F).
Ergonomic role. About one-third of the subjects (51, which is 35.9%) mentioned reasons
concerning usability as a basis for their choice. Of these, 34 subjects mentioned operational
aspects, such as the visibility of the display, or the size, number, clarity or placing of the buttons.
For example, one subject who chose alternative G instead of F, said: “… and also the buttons on
it, they were just a bit more clear, just one button to play and rewind etcetera. The other one, it
had one button, but it did not exactly say what it was for… .” Several subjects found the buttons
of alternative D clear and the button of alternative F too small, although several others preferred
alternative G or F to D because it had one instead of two buttons on top. Nine subjects wanted a
product with as few buttons as possible; according to them, more buttons are only confusing and
heighten the likelihood of making mistakes. As one subject said: “… the more simple the design,
the less easily it will break down .. and a lot of buttons, that is simply confusing.” A few subjects
preferred a separate button for each function instead of one button having several functions.
Seven subjects preferred alternative G because you could immediately see how it works; it had
clear buttons that were clearly labeled so that the possibilities were clear, while alternative F was
14
‘closed’ so that how it works was not obvious from the way it looks (see Figure 8). Two subjects
preferred the product alternative that operates similar to their own answering machine.
In addition to operational aspects, more general aspects of use were mentioned. Such general
aspects are not related to the direct operation of the product, but to more indirect consequences
of use, such as the space needed by the product (e.g., whether it fits on a table), the ease of
cleaning, or the likelihood of accidentally hurting someone. Fourteen subjects valued a small size
(i.e., alternative G or F instead of D) because a small product needs less space and is easier to
hide in a drawer. Four subjects chose alternative D because it is square instead of rounded, and
therefore fits more easily into a corner or in-between other things. In contrast, two subjects
valued a rounded product (alternative F or G) as it is easier to handle and is less likely to hurt
someone (i.e., no sharp edges). One subject mentioned that buttons that are integrated into the
surface make the product easier to handle, and another subject found this easier to clean. They
therefore did not choose alternative D with its protruding buttons.
Attention drawing role. Of the fourteen subjects (9.9% of the total sample) who mentioned
the attention-drawing ability of one of the product alternatives, thirteen preferred the less
attention-drawing alternative (alternative F or G, see Figure 8). Almost all of them found
alternative D to be too conspicuous because it was too big and its buttons were in a contrasting
color. They preferred a product that would be less conspicuous in their home, or as one subject
called it: “harmless in the interior of my home”. Another subject stated that an answering
machine is a functional product that “strictly speaking, you do not want to see”. Therefore, these
subjects preferred a smaller product, that can be easily put away, and has a more neutral color.
Only one subject chose the product that drew her attention by its design. She explained her
preference for the appearance of alternative G as follows: “Well, it is more like a whole, the
impression it makes on me, does it attract my attention and does it satisfy my wishes …”.
Another subject stated that although she chose the less attention-drawing product, she might buy
a specially designed product that draws a lot of attention but looks very attractive at the same
time. Females significantly more often mention ‘attention drawing’ as a choice reason than males
² = 8.80, p < .01).
Categorization role. Eleven subjects (7.8%) mentioned visual categorization as playing a
role in their product choice. Categorization may also have a subconscious influence, and thus
may have played a role for more subjects, but the remarks of these eleven subjects give us some
insight into how visual categorization plays a role for consumers.
A few subjects mentioned prefering alternative G because it is more recognizable as an
answering machine, that is, easy to categorize, but found it difficult to explain why. For example,
one subject explained why she liked the appearance of alternative G better than F: “I found it
more recognizable, the other one, that looked like .. what is it called? .. a CD player. … Well, in
my eyes it looks more like an answering machine.”
Others preferred something different from a standard box, something more special that does
not look ordinary (i.e., is less prototypical). Two subjects explicitly mentioned prefering a
product that is less recognizable as an answering machine, less plain. Subjects valuing a
nontypical product chose alternative F or G. One subject said: “.. I would in first instance
consider the one I chose as a portable CD player instead of an answering machine, so in that
sense somewhat less recognizable as an answering machine.” When asked whether and why he
prefers this, he said: “Well, I just like to.. in everything I buy.. to not pick the ordinary.”
Four subjects preferred alternative F or G because it reminded them of another product,
namely a portable CD player or modern car radio. They found it difficult to explain why, but
15
thought that it was a kind of recognition; they were used to this look. For example, one subject
said: “That rounded one appeals to me, yes, I don’t know why, maybe because it also looks a bit
like a portable CD player or something, .. that appeals more to me.” When asked why, she said:
“Maybe because it is a bit more familiar, I don’t know .. . ” One subject disliked alternative D
because it reminded him of an old-fashioned cassette player.
Interrelations. In several cases, some roles were interrelated. Attention drawing and
aesthetic value were often linked: subjects find an attention drawing product less aesthetically
attractive. Indeed, the correlation between attention drawing and aesthetic choice reasons is
significant (Spearman’s rho = .25, p < .01). Furthermore, symbolic and aesthetic value were
often intertwined. Subjects mention symbolic associations in explaining why they find the
product aesthetically attractive, which agrees with Vihma (1995). Indeed, correlation analysis
shows that aesthetic and symbolic reasons often co-occur (Spearman’s rho = .54, p < .001). Also,
for some subjects, symbolic and functional value were linked, as they felt that a modern-looking
answering machine would be technologically superior. As this concerned only a small number of
subjects, this is not expressed in a significant correlation between functional and symbolic choice
reasons. However, there was a significant correlation between categorization and symbolic
choice reasons (Spearman’s rho = .20, p < .05), which is probably due to the fact that several
subjects found that answering machine F looked modern or contemporary because it resembles a
portable CD player (i.e., another product category). In addition, the correlation between
categorization and aesthetic choice reasons was on the border of significance (Spearman’s rho =
.16, p = .05). This can be explained by the fact that subjects liked an appearance that looked or
did not look like a typical answering machine.
Conclusion and Discussion
We distinguished six roles of product appearance for consumers on the basis of a literature
review, and showed in a qualitative study that these roles are relevant for consumers and are
sufficient to describe the influence of product appearance in consumer choice. In addition, we
gained insight into the information that consumers use and the inferences they make from the
appearance of a product. We give an overview of the roles and their influence on consumers in
Table 1. -------------------------------
Insert Table 1 about here
-------------------------------
Aesthetic value will often be important to consumers for durable products, as these products
are often used for many years and visible in consumer’s homes or to other people. Indeed, the
majority of subjects in our study considered aesthetic value in their product choice and several
subjects considered whether the product fitted aesthetically into their home. We even saw some
subjects giving up functionalities in favor of aesthetic value. Our subjects mentioned
roundedness, size, color and specific details as a basis for their aesthetic judgment, although
some subjects found it difficult to verbalize why precisely a specific product alternative looked
more attractive to them. In general, a small, rounded answering machine in one neutral, dark
color was aesthetically preferred, although some subjects had different preferences. The fact that
only very few subjects mentioned visual organization principles as causing their aesthetic
preference is not surprising, as the influence of such principles will be largely unconscious
16
(Veryzer, 1999). Although prototypicality was not explicitly mentioned, many subjects preferred
a rounded product because it suits the contemporary design trend, and as such is prototypical for
contemporary products.
Symbolic value was mentioned as a choice reason by almost half of our sample. Subjects
mentioned several associations, such as expensive, friendly or businesslike. A modern or
contemporary look was important to more than a quarter of the subjects. Aesthetic and symbolic
value were often intertwined. For example, many subjects liked a rounded appearance because it
looks modern and friendly. It may, however, be good to acknowledge the difference between
these two kinds of product value. Someone might like a certain appearance, but not purchase it
because the symbolic associations are not suited to his or her person (e.g., a childlike appearance
for an adult) or to the occasion (think of use at home versus at work).
A few subjects derived functionalities from the product appearance. In addition, several
subjects derived an impression about the functional quality of the product from its appearance.
They chose the product alternative that looks the most reliable or solid, but found it difficult to
indicate the characteristics responsible for this impression. This agrees with the literature, where
it is noted that the global impression of the product appearance can communicate quality
(Srinivasan et al., 1997; Yamamoto and Lambert, 1994). Whether and what inferences are
formed on the basis of the product appearance will differ between consumers. A knowledgeable
and interested consumer will be able and willing to assess the value of most technical product
functions. However, other consumers may use heuristics such as ‘more buttons mean more
functions’. Information about how subjects form judgments about functional product value on
the basis of product appearance can be used proactively to attune product appearance to
consumer perception. This increases the likelihood that consumers make accurate judgments
about the functional product value, for “It is not enough to bury quality in a product, it must be
seen and experienced to be recognized and believed” (Dickson, 1994, p. 263).
More than one-third of our sample mentioned choice reasons concerning usability. Two-
thirds of them mentioned operational aspects, of which half referred to the number or size of the
buttons. Almost one-third of the subjects that mentioned usability wanted a small number of
buttons on an answering machine, as they believe this makes it more simple to operate. They
consider more buttons to be simply confusing. This agrees with the notion that simplicity of
operation will be a more dominant sales argument than variety of functional characteristics
(Hammer, 1995; Nussbaum, 1988). Clear operation will be especially important for
technologically complex products. Many electronic products are so complex that they are almost
unusable, and many consumers even find high-tech products intimidating (Feldman, 1995). In
addition to parts for consumer-product interaction, such as buttons and displays, we found that
overall aspects of the appearance, such as size, roundedness and material, influence the
(perceived) ergonomic product value. These aspects influence more indirect consequences of
use, such as the space needed by the product (e.g., whether it fits on a table), the ease of handling
the product, or the ease of cleaning. So in investigating the usability of a product, attention has to
be paid not only to (the perception of) operational aspects, but also to these more indirect
consequences of use, as these also play a role in product choice.
While for food products, a positive relation is found between the ability of a package to draw
attention and product choice, all but one of the subjects in our study that mentioned attention
drawing as playing a role in their product choice chose the less attention-drawing product
alternative. The reason for this was that they did not want the product to be conspicuous in their
home. Indeed, products that draw attention in-store are often conspicuous and may not be the
17
same ones that are found aesthetically attractive. Aesthetic considerations will be more important
to consumers for durable products than for fast-moving consumer goods, as durable products are
used for a long(er) period and are often visible in one’s home and for other people. So although
an atypical product appearance can be a suitable way of attracting attention for durable products,
care has to be taken to ensure that this atypical look is aesthetically acceptable for consumers.
Concerning visual categorization, several subjects preferred the most typical looking
answering machine, but found it difficult to explain why. Others preferred an atypical, and
thereby less common and ordinary, answering machine. This confirms that the preference for
(a)typicality differs between consumers. The choice whether to develop a typical, a slightly
atypical or a very atypical appearance will depend on the target group of consumers and the kind
of product. In the literature review section, cases are listed in which it is beneficial to develop a
very typical or atypical appearance.
The aesthetic and symbolic appearance roles were far more salient to consumers, and the
appearance influenced perceived ergonomic value for one-third of the subjects (see Figure 10).
The functional role of the appearance is mentioned less. This does not mean that functionalities
were not important: 57.7% of the sample based their choice on functionalities. However, most of
these were derived from the textual information that was presented with the products themselves,
and only 12.7% of the subjects mentioned the appearance as a basis for a judgment about the
functional product value. The attention-drawing and categorization roles were mentioned less
often. It may be that consumers are not always conscious of their influence (see the section about
future research). The relative importance of the appearance roles was not the focus of our study.
As we used a small number of product alternatives, the influence of the appearance roles in our
study may not be indicative for answering machines in general. For example, the answering
machines in this study had one or two buttons and a volume slider; an alternative with more
buttons would have increased the incidence with which subjects mention ease of operation as a
choice reason. However, it is striking that aesthetic value played a role for so many subjects,
while the answering machines we used do not differ that much in their appearance (they are all
dark-colored, flat shapes). There were more subjects that (partly) based their choice on aesthetics
than on functionalities. This may indicate the importance of aesthetics in consumers’ product
selection. However, the relative importance of the appearance roles will differ between product
categories and consumers (see the section about future research).
We found several examples of interrelations between appearance roles. Significant
correlations are found between aesthetic and symbolic product value, aesthetic value and
attention drawing, and categorization and aesthetic as well as symbolic value. No correlations of
functional or ergonomic value with other appearance roles were significant. However, for some
subjects symbolic and functional value were linked, and some relations between roles might not
have surfaced in our research (e.g., because of the small number of product alternatives used).
Consumers may, for example, derive an impression about the functional or ergonomic product
value from categorization of the product appearance; similarity to a well-known product category
exemplar of high technical quality may lead consumers to infer that the product at hand is also of
good quality.
As some roles can be interrelated, changes in one role may influence other roles. In addition,
the preferred shape (e.g., rounded or angular), color or size, were found to differ depending on
the way in which product appearance played a role for subjects. For example, a small size is
valued from an aesthetic point of view, but a larger size is chosen by some subjects because it
looks more solid and reliable (i.e., functional value). So when something is changed in the
18
product appearance in order to improve its performance on one role, this has implications for the
performance on other roles.
Managerial implications
The appearance of a product can influence consumer choice in different ways. Distinguishing
these different appearance roles will help managers to make better use of product appearance as
a marketing tool.
Focus on the most important appearance roles
To fully use the potential of product appearance in influencing consumer choice, the appearance
should communicate the central consumer advantage to consumers and fit the product’s market
positioning (see also Just and Salvador, 2003). To make optimal use of product appearance, the
marketing department or product development team should explicitly consider the impression
they want the appearance to communicate. The value that is most important to consumers in
purchasing a specific kind of product should be the starting-point in the design of the product
appearance (Bruce and Whitehead, 1988). Therefore, it is recommendable that product designers
know in an early stage whether aesthetics, ease of use, technical quality or features are most
important in the brand choice for the target group of consumers. For the product shape, colors,
materials and configuration that are preferred - or that engender positive product perceptions -
depend on the product value that is important to the consumer. For example, a larger size may
make a product look more old-fashioned and crude, more solid and stable, less easy to store,
easier to operate (as buttons are bigger or further apart) and heavier in weight. Whether a larger
size is preferable will therefore depend on whether aesthetic value, technical quality, or ease of
use is more important to consumers.
Different appearances can be made for groups of consumers that differ in the product value
that is most important in their choice. For example, people who need glasses may prefer an alarm
clock with buttons that have a bright contrasting color as opposed to the casing, so they can
better locate the buttons in a dusky bedroom. Other people may dismiss such a product on
aesthetic grounds.
Are design guidelines valuable?
Several influences of appearance characteristics, such as color and form, on the perception of
certain kinds of product value have been mentioned in the literature, or are intuitive. Subjects in
our study also mentioned such influences. For example, a bigger product looks more solid, bright
colors may diminish a quality impression and a large number of buttons decreases the impression
of ease of use (Norman, 1988). What is the value of such design guidelines? Is it useful to
investigate such influences?
Although product designers will intuitively feel how to engender a certain impression, we
think that research into the influence of specific appearance elements on the perception of certain
kinds of product value may help them in this. However, the intuition of the designer remains
essential, as the effect of combining separate characteristics into a whole cannot be predicted.
Furthermore, the value of such guidelines differs for different kinds of product values. The
influence of appearance characteristics on the perception of utilitarian aspects, such as quality,
19
ease of use and functionality, will probably be similar over product categories, persons, and
countries. People will agree that larger buttons are easier to operate and that a product with a
display looks more functionally complex than one without a display. So for functional and
ergonomic value, such guidelines are reliable and general research into the influence of specific
appearance characteristics on their perception will be useful. However, there will be more
difference between consumers in aesthetic and symbolic perception, as such matters of taste and
experience are more subjective. A large size makes a certain product look modern according to
one consumer, and old-fashioned according to another. In our study, many subjects mentioned
that roundedness looks modern and friendly, and that angularity looks old-fashioned and cheap.
However, this may be specific for the product category, the year, or the country in which the
study is conducted. One has to keep in mind that the aesthetic and symbolic value of a product
may differ between cultures and in time, and even depending on the context (the available
product alternatives, or the store surroundings). General design guidelines will therefore be less
reliable for the aesthetic and symbolic roles of the product appearance. Testing with consumers
is therefore even more important for aesthetic and symbolic value, especially as these roles
seemed to be the most influential, at least in our study.
Testing the appearance with consumers
To make sure that the appearance of a new product has a positive influence on product choice,
this should be tested with consumers. One should assess whether consumer perceptions of the
functional, ergonomic, aesthetic and symbolic value of a new product on the basis of its
appearance are positive and correct. This can be done by asking consumers to judge the
functionalities, quality, ease of use, aesthetic and symbolic value of the product on the basis of
its appearance only. As there are cultural, social, and personal influences on design taste (Bloch,
1995), it is important to use the correct target group in such a test.
If a design does not engender the right impression on one of these aspects, one might ask
consumers how to improve it (e.g., ‘why do you think the quality of this product is low?’).
Consumers are able to do this for functional and ergonomic aspects; they are able to indicate that
a display is too small, that buttons are too close together, or that certain features are unwanted.
But consumers have more difficulty in indicating how aspects such as quality impression and
aesthetic and symbolic value can be improved, as these concern the overall impression of the
appearance. The effect of changes in appearance characteristics on the whole product impression
is difficult to imagine for consumers. For the value of certain characteristics, such as color, for a
consumer may change when the rest of the product changes (cf. Holbrook and Moore, 1981).
With one product style, blue may be the most attractive, while with another style, green may suit
better. So consumers have to see a change in appearance in order to adequately judge it.
Furthermore, a consumer will often be unable to specify why he or she likes or dislikes a certain
appearance, which is descriptive of holistic judgments (see Kemler Nelson, 1989; Mittal, 1988).
For example, most people are probably unaware of the influence of visual organization principles
on their judgments (see Veryzer, 1993; Veryzer, 1999). A possible solution to this problem is
showing consumers a lot of pictures of products that they can use to point out what they mean or
which products fit an intended impression. This may give the design team clues about how to
better engender a specific impression.
Future research
20
The research method we used only gave insight into the conscious use of information by
consumers. The influence of attention drawing and categorization, and perhaps the impression of
quality, may also take place subconsciously. This may explain why only few subjects mentioned
these appearance roles. The influence of these roles on consumer product choice may therefore
have been underestimated. Future research may give a more accurate insight into the influence of
these roles in consumer product choice.
In addition, it will be interesting to investigate the relative importance of the appearance roles
in different product categories. Aesthetics will be relatively important for some types of
products, such as lamps and furniture, while for other types of products, for example appliances,
ease of use will be more important. But for appliances that are visible in one’s home, aesthetics
are probably also important to consumers, as was illustrated for the answering machines in our
study. Attention drawing and categorization (i.e., visual typicality) are probably more influential
for food products than for durables. Future research may give more insight into this issue.
We mentioned that the value that is most important to consumers should be the starting-point
in the design of the product appearance. It may be possible to distinguish groups of consumers
that differ in the importance they attach to each kind of product value in general. Although our
study was not especially focused on this, we found that females pay more attention to whether
the product fits into their home than males. This might not only be the case for answering
machines, but also for other product categories. In addition, it is often assumed that older people
pay more attention to ease of use. We could not find any research that supports this assumption,
and the question remains from what age on people start paying more attention to ease of use.
Also, the question remains to what extent the perceptions that subjects mentioned on the
basis of product appearance generalize to other products (which may differ for utilitarian and
expressive product value, see above). Examples are that a rounded product looks more modern, a
square product looks more solid but also old-fashioned, a larger product looks more solid, and a
modern rounded shape looks technologically superior (i.e., newer). The same goes for
consumers’ preferences. Many want a small number of buttons, as many buttons are confusing.
Furthermore, subjects liked a product in one color as opposed to more colors, and integrated
buttons, which make it a unified whole. As mentioned before, these perceptions and preferences
may differ in time, between groups of consumers and between countries. The extent to which
this is the case is also an issue for further research.
Acknowledgement
We are grateful to the editor of this article, Abbie Griffin, as her comments significantly changed
and improved this article.
21
References
Alba, Joseph W. and Hutchinson J. Wesley. (1987). Dimensions of Consumer Expertise. Journal
of Consumer Research 13:411454.
Bamossy, Gary, Scammon, Debra L. and Johnston, Marilyn. (1983). A Preliminary Investigation
of the Reliability and Validity of an Aesthetic Judgment Test, In: Advances in Consumer
Research, Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice M. Tybout (eds.). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for
Consumer Research, 685690.
Belk, Russell W. (1988). Possessions and the Extended Self. Journal of Consumer Research
15:139168.
Berkowitz, Marvin. (1987). Product Shape as a Design Innovation Strategy. Journal of Product
Innovation Management 4:274283.
Berlyne, David E. (1971). Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Black, Caroline D. and Baker, Michael J. (1987). Success through Design. Design Studies
8(4):207215.
Bloch, Peter H. (1995). Seeking the Ideal Form: Product Design and Consumer Response.
Journal of Marketing 59:1629.
Bruce, Margaret and Whitehead, Maureen. (1988). Putting Design into the Picture: the Role of
Product Design in Consumer Purchase Behaviour. Journal of the Market Research Society
30(2):147162.
Cohen, Joel B. and Basu, Kunal. (1987). Alternative Models of Categorization: Toward a
Contingent Processing Framework. Journal of Consumer Research 13:455472.
Dawar, Niraj and Parker, Philip. (1994). Marketing Universals: Consumers' Use of Brand Name,
Price, Physical Appearance, and Retailer Reputation as Signals of Product Quality. Journal of
Marketing 58:8195.
Dickson, Peter R. (1994). Marketing Management. Orlando, FL: The Dryden Press.
Dumaine, Brian. (1991). Design that Sells and Sells and… . Fortune 11:5661 (March).
Engel, James F., Blackwell, Roger D. and Miniard, Paul W. (1995). Consumer Behavior.
Orlando, FL: The Dryden Press.
Feldman, Laurence P. (1995). Increasing the Usability of High-Tech Products through Design
Research. Design Management Journal Fall:2733.
Garber, Lawrence L. Jr. (1995). The Package Appearance in Choice, In: Advances in Consumer
Research, Frank R. Kardes and Mita Sujan (eds.). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
Research, 653660.
Garber, Lawrence L. Jr., Burke, Richard R. and Jones, J. Morgan. (2000). The Role of Package
Color in Consumer Purchase Consideration and Choice. Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science
Institute (Working Paper Series, Rep. No. 00-104).
Gemser, Gerda and Leenders, Mark A. A. M. (2001). How Integrating Industrial Design in the
Product Development Process Impacts on Company Performance. The Journal of Product
Innovation Management 18:2838.
22
Hammer, Norbert. (1995). Testing Design via Eye-Movement Analysis - Perspectives and
Problems, In: Successful Product Engineering: Testing for Optimal Design and Function. Berlin:
ESOMAR, 155172.
Hekkert, Paul P. M. (1995). Artful Judgements: A Psychological Inquiry into Aesthetic
Preference for Visual Patterns. Delft: Delft University of Technology.
Hekkert, Paul, Snelders, Dirk, and van Wieringen, Piet C. W. (2003). ‘Most Advanced yet
Acceptable’: Typicality and Novelty as Joint Predictors of Aesthetic Preference in Industrial
Design. British Journal of Psychology 94:111124.
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. and Holbrook, Morris B. (1982). Hedonic Consumption: Emerging
Concepts, Methods and Propositions. Journal of Marketing 46:92101.
Holbrook, Morris B. (1980). Some Preliminary Notes on Research in Consumer Esthetics, In:
Advances in Consumer Research, Jerry C. Olson (ed.). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for
Consumer Research, 104108.
Holbrook, Morris B. and Moore, William L. (1981). Feature Interactions in Consumer
Judgments of Verbal versus Pictorial Presentations. Journal of Consumer Research 8:103113.
Hoyer, Wayne D. (1984). An Examination of Consumer Decision Making for a Common Repeat
Purchase Product. Journal of Consumer Research 11:822829.
Just, Lily A. and Salvador, Rommel. (2003). Conference Summary: Marketing Meets Design.
Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science Institute (Working Paper Series, Rep. No. 03-001).
Kemler Nelson, Deborah G. (1989). The Nature and Occurrence of Holistic Categorization, In:
Object perception: Structure and process, Bryan E. Shepp and Soledad Ballesteros (eds.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 357386.
Kotler, Philip and Rath, G. Alexander. (1984). Design: A Powerful but Neglected Strategic Tool.
Journal of Business Strategy 5 (2):1621.
Landon, E. Laird. (1974). Self Concept, Ideal Self Concept, and Consumer Purchase Intentions.
Journal of Consumer Research 1:4451.
Levy, Sidney J. (1959). Symbols for Sale. Harvard Business Review 37:117119 (July-August).
Lloyd, Peter and Snelders, Dirk. (2003). What was Philippe Starck Thinking of? Design Studies
24:237253.
bach, Bernd. (1976). Industrial Design: Grundlagen der Industrieproduktgestaltung.
Muenchen: Verlag Karl Thiemig.
Loken, Barbara and Ward, James. (1990). Alternative Approaches to Understanding the
Determinants of Typicality. Journal of Consumer Research 17:111126.
Lorenz, Christopher. (1986). The Design Dimension. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
March, Artemis. (1994). Usability: The New Dimension of Product Design. Harvard Business
Review Sept/Oct:144149.
McCracken, Grant. (1986). Culture and Consumption: A Theoretical Account of the Structure
and Movement of the Cultural Meaning of Consumer Goods. Journal of Consumer Research
13:7184.
23
Meyers-Levy, Joan and Tybout, Alice M. (1989). Schema Congruity as a Basis for Product
Evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research 16:3954.
Mittal, Banwari. (1988). The Role of Affective Choice Mode in the Consumer Purchase of
Expressive Products. Journal of Economic Psychology 9:499524.
Muller, Wim. (2001). Order and Meaning in Design. Utrecht: Lemma.
Murdoch, Peter and Flurscheim, Charles H. (1983). Form, In: Industrial Design in Engineering,
Charles H. Flurscheim (ed.). Worcester, UK: The Design Council, 105131.
Norman, Donald A. (1988). The Psychology of Everyday Things. New York: Basic Books.
Nussbaum, Bruce. (1988). Smart Design: Quality is the New Style. Business Week April 11:80
86.
Nussbaum, Bruce. (1993). Hot Products: How Good Design Pays off. Business Week June 7:40
43.
Olshavsky, Richard W. and Spreng, Richard A. (1996). An Exploratory Study of the Innovation
Evaluation Process. Journal of Product Innovation Management 13:512529.
Pilditch, James. (1976). Talk about Design. Redwood Burn Limited.
Rosch, Eleanor, Carolyn B. Mervis, Wayne D. Gray, David M. Johnson and Penny Boyes-
Braem. (1976). Basic Objects in Natural Categories. Cognitive Psychology 8:382439.
Roy, Robin. (1994). Can the Benefits of Good Design be Quantified? Design Management
Journal 5:917 (Spring).
Schmitt, Bernd H. and Simonson, Alex. (1997). Marketing Aesthetics: The Strategic
Management of Brands, Identity, and Image. New York: The Free Press.
Schoormans, Jan P. L. and Robben, Henry S. J. (1997). The Effect of New Package Design on
Product Attention, Categorization and Evaluation. Journal of Economic Psychology 18:271287.
Schürer, Arnold. (1971). Der Einfluss Produktbestimmender Faktoren auf die Gestaltung.
Clausthal-Zellerfeld: Boenecke-Druck.
Sirgy, M. Joseph. (1982). Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review. Journal of
Consumer Research 9:287300.
Smith, Eric. (1994). Good Design is indeed Good Business. Design Management Journal 5:18
23 (Spring).
Solomon, Michael R. (1983). The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A Symbolic Interactionism
Perspective. Journal of Consumer Research 10:319329.
Srinivasan, V., Lovejoy, William S. and Beach, David. (1997). Integrated Product Design for
Marketability and Manufacturing. Journal of Marketing Research 34:154163.
Sujan, Mita and Bettman, James R. (1989). The Effect of Brand Positioning Strategies on
Consumers' Brand and Category Perceptions: Some Insights from Schema Research. Journal of
Marketing Research 26:45467.
Thackara, John. (1997). Winners: How Successful Companies Innovate by Design. Amsterdam:
BIS.
24
Veryzer, Robert W. (1999). A Nonconscious Processing Explanation of Consumer Response to
Product Design. Psychology & Marketing 16(6):497522.
Veryzer, Robert W. Jr. (1993). Aesthetic Response and the Influence of Design Principles on
Product Preferences, In: Advances in Consumer Research, Leigh McAlister and Michael L.
Rothschild (eds.). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 224229.
Veryzer, Robert W. Jr. (1995). The Place of Product Design and Aesthetics in Consumer
Research, In: Advances in Consumer Research, Frank R. Kardes and Mita Sujan (eds.). Provo,
UT: Association for Consumer Research, 641645.
Veryzer, Robert W. Jr. and Hutchinson, J. Wesley. (1998). The Influence of Unity and
Prototypicality on Aesthetic Responses to New Product Designs. Journal of Consumer Research
24:374394.
Vihma, Susann. (1995). Products as Representations: A Semiotic and Aesthetic Study of Design
Products. Helsinki: University of Art and Design.
Ward, James and Loken, Barbara. (1988). The Generality of Typicality Effects on Preference
and Comparison: An Exploratory Test, In: Advances in Consumer Research, Michael J. Houston
(ed.). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 5561.
Whitfield, Allan and Wiltshire, Tom. (1983). Colour, In: Industrial Design in Engineering,
Charles H. Flurscheim (ed.). Worcester, UK: The Design Council, 133157.
Whitfield, T. W. A. and Slatter, P. E. (1979). The Effects of Categorization and Prototypicality
on Aesthetic Choice in a Furniture Selection Task. British Journal of Psychology 70:6575.
Yamamoto, Mel and Lambert, David R. (1994). The Impact of Product Aesthetics on the
Evaluation of Industrial Products. Journal of Product Innovation Management 11(4):309324.
25
The Different Roles of Product Appearance in Consumer Choice
Biographical Sketches
Mariëlle E.H. Creusen is Assistant Professor of Consumer Research at the Faculty of Industrial
Design Engineering, Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands. She received her M.Sc.
in economic psychology from Tilburg University and her Ph.D. from Delft University of
Technology. She has published in journals such as the International Journal of Research in
Marketing and Advances in Consumer Research. Her current research interests include the
influence of product appearance factors on consumer product preference, and consumer research
methods in product development.
Jan P.L. Schoormans is Professor of Consumer Research at the Faculty of Industrial Design
Engineering, Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands. He received his M.Sc. and Ph.D.
in economic psychology from Tilburg University. He has published in journals such as the
Journal of Product Innovation Management, Design Studies, the International Journal of
Research in Marketing, the Journal of Economic Psychology, and Advances in Consumer
Research. His current research interests include consumer research methods in the product
development process.
26
Table 1. The Six Roles of Product Appearance for Consumers
Appearance Role
Influence on Consumers
Attention Drawing
- Draw consumer attention in-store
Categorization
- Influence ease of categorization
- Offer possibility for differentiation from the product category
Functional
- Show features/functionalities
- Serve as a cue for features/functionalities
- Serve as a cue for technical quality
Ergonomic
- Show parts for consumer-product interaction
- Show consequences of use of overall appearance aspects (e.g.,
size, roundedness)
Aesthetic
- Serve as a basis for aesthetic appreciation
- Fit with home interior and other products owned
Symbolic
- Serve as a basis for symbolic product associations
- Communicate brand image
27
Figure 1. Mobile phones differing in their aesthetic appearance (reprinted with permission
from Nokia Corporation)
Figure 2. Electrical toothbrushes with an appearance symbolizing use by children (right) or
adults (reprinted with permission from Gillette/ Oral-B)
28
Figure 3. A larger hair dryer (left) looks more powerful (both are 1875 Watts) (reprinted
with permission from Conair Corporation)
Figure 4. An easy to operate telephone (left) and a more complex one (reprinted with
permission from Hesdo BV, www.profoon.nl)
29
Figure 5. The ‘Billy’ hand blender draws attention in-store by its differentiating colors
Figure 6. A typical (left) and an atypical (right) coffee maker
30
Figure 7. A differentiating appearance underlines the unique mechanism of the Dyson
vacuum cleaner (reprinted with permission from Dyson BV)
31
Alternative D: selected by 19 subjects (13.4%)
Alternative F: selected by 49 subjects (34.5%)
Alternative G: selected by 74 subjects (52.1%)
Figure 8. Stimulus material
32
Number of appearance roles
543210
Percent
40
30
20
10
0
Figure 9. Percentage of subjects mentioning each number of product appearance roles
categorization
attention draw ing
functional
ergonomic
symbolic
aesthetic
Percent
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Figure 10. Percentage of subjects that mentioned each of the different appearance roles
... In today's highly competitive marketplace, understanding the factors that influence consumer purchase decisions has become increasingly important for product designers and marketers. Among the various elements of product design, visual aspects such as color and packaging play crucial roles in shaping consumers' first impressions and subsequent evaluations of products (Bloch, 1995;Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). These design elements not only contribute to aesthetic appeal but may also convey information about product functionality, quality, and ease of use. ...
... Building on this framework, scholars have investigated various aspects of product design, including shape (Westerman et al., 2012), material (Karana et al., 2009), and size (Raghubir & Krishna, 1999). Creusen and Schoormans (2005) identified six roles that product appearance plays in consumer choice: attention drawing, categorization, aesthetic impression, symbolic association, functional indication, and ergonomic information. These functions highlight the multifaceted nature of design elements in consumer decision-making processes. ...
... Products with more aesthetically pleasing colors and user-friendly packaging were more likely to be associated with higher purchase intention categories. These findings align with previous research suggesting the importance of visual design elements in consumer decision-making (Bloch, 1995;Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This study investigates the relationship between product design elements (color and packaging) and consumer purchase intention, with perceived usability serving as a mediating variable. Data were collected from 287 participants through a structured survey and analyzed using SPSS software, with both chi-square and regression analyses employed to test the hypothesized relationships. Results indicate that both product color and packaging significantly influence purchase intention, with perceived usability partially mediating these relationships. Specifically, more aesthetically pleasing colors and user-friendly packaging positively impact perceived usability, which in turn enhances purchase intention. These findings contribute to the understanding of consumer behavior in product design contexts and provide practical implications for product developers and marketers seeking to optimize design elements to enhance consumer response.
... Introduction (Creusen and Schoormans, 2005). Both designers and researchers should thus be aware of and understand the impact of gender-neutral design on consumer response (Morgenroth et al., 2021).Some design researchers suggest that the designer's role is to design products and packages that are independent from all constructed bias, emphasizing the societal importance of gender-neutral design (Cakiroglu, 2017). ...
... As previously outlined, when it comes to designs, symbolism is not portrayed through roles, but through visual elements applied to a design. Nevertheless, consumers do use product designs as symbols of their identity and tend to prefer designs that reflect their own personalities through the visual elements employed (Creusen and Schoormans, 2005;Govers and Mugge, 2004;van den Hende and Mugge, 2014). Hence, the preference for gender-neutral designs might depend on how much one identifies with one's own gender and thus with the group of people of the same gender. ...
... Prior research focused on gender neutrality in advertising (Åkestam et al., 2021;Chu et al., 2016;Coggiola and Coste-Manière, 2021), but there are important differences between advertising and visual (package) design. Advertising, as a component of promotion, primarily focuses on portraying (gender) roles by using narrative elements, while package design, as a component of product, has aesthetic and symbolic functions (Creusen and Schoormans, 2005). Visual package and product designs have elements (e.g. ...
Article
Purpose While marketers often design their offers to visually appeal to either female or male consumers, the traditional view on gender is changing. As a result, many advocate for gender-neutral marketing visuals. This paper aims to investigate how consumers respond to gender-neutral package designs. Design/methodology/approach Three experiments explore (1) the influence of visual gender neutrality in package design on consumer responses to marketing visuals varying in perceived gender of design and, thus, their congruity with the respondent gender (gender-congruent, gender-incongruent and neutral design) including (2) the moderating roles of consumer gender and respondents’ identification with their gender as well as (3) the underlying mechanism through perceived modernity. Findings This study finds that neutrality in visual design has a positive influence on consumer response. Furthermore, both gender and the degree of respondents’ identification with their gender play moderating roles. The effect on purchase intention is positively mediated through perceived modernity, with this indirect effect being more pronounced for women and individuals with low levels of gender identification. Research limitations/implications To improve the robustness of these findings, future research should explore which specific visual elements are most suitable for portraying gender neutrality. Practical implications The findings offer valuable insights for marketers and designers to enhance consumer engagement by integrating gender-neutral package designs. By understanding the positive consumer response and perceived modernity associated with such designs, marketers can more effectively appeal to a broader audience, particularly among women and those with lower gender identification. Originality/value This work advances the understanding of gender-neutral marketing by empirically exploring consumer responses to gender-neutral visual design. It provides essential insights that support the development of inclusive, forward-thinking marketing strategies designed to engage a broader audience.
... Packaging also plays a critical role in attracting consumer attention, particularly when it deviates from the norm or includes compelling visual elements (Underwood, 2001;Garber et al., 2000). Research on food products suggests that larger sizes, bold colors, and distinct shapes enhance a product's visibility and purchase likelihood (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). ...
... Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). Consumers often favor rounded designs, as angular ones may appear outdated and less appealing (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). ...
Research
Full-text available
This study investigates the factors influencing consumers' bottled water brand choice in Ethiopia using an explanatory research design and a quantitative research approach. Data were collected from 384 bottled water consumers through questionnaires, and multiple linear regression was employed for data analysis. The findings indicate that product quality, price, alternative attractiveness, and brand awareness significantly influence brand choice, with product quality and brand awareness emerging as the most critical factors. Consumers tend to prefer brands that consistently deliver high-quality water, highlighting the importance of stringent quality assurance programs, adherence to industry certifications, and transparent marketing strategies that reinforce trust in the product. Additionally, competitive pricing plays a vital role, suggesting that companies should align their pricing strategies with the perceived value of their products to appeal to price-sensitive customers without compromising quality. Alternative attractiveness also influences consumer decisions, implying that brands must differentiate themselves effectively through unique value propositions. Moreover, brand awareness is a key factor, necessitating targeted marketing efforts that enhance visibility, recall, and consumer loyalty. Therefore, water bottle companies should invest in promotional campaigns that emphasize both quality and affordability while ensuring consistent messaging across various communication channels.
... Packaging also plays a critical role in attracting consumer attention, particularly when it deviates from the norm or includes compelling visual elements (Underwood, 2001;Garber et al., 2000). Research on food products suggests that larger sizes, bold colors, and distinct shapes enhance a product's visibility and purchase likelihood (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). ...
... Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). Consumers often favor rounded designs, as angular ones may appear outdated and less appealing (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This study investigates the factors influencing consumers' bottled water brand choice in Ethiopia using an explanatory research design and a quantitative research approach. Data were collected from 384 bottled water consumers through questionnaires, and multiple linear regression was employed for data analysis. The findings indicate that product quality, price, alternative attractiveness, and brand awareness significantly influence brand choice, with product quality and brand awareness emerging as the most critical factors. Consumers tend to prefer brands that consistently deliver high-quality water, highlighting the importance of stringent quality assurance programs, adherence to industry certifications, and transparent marketing strategies that reinforce trust in the product. Additionally, competitive pricing plays a vital role, suggesting that companies should align their pricing strategies with the perceived value of their products to appeal to price-sensitive customers without compromising quality. Alternative attractiveness also influences consumer decisions, implying that brands must differentiate themselves effectively through unique value propositions. Moreover, brand awareness is a key factor, necessitating targeted marketing efforts that enhance visibility, recall, and consumer loyalty. Therefore, water bottle companies should invest in promotional campaigns that emphasize both quality and affordability while ensuring consistent messaging across various communication channels.
... Packaging also plays a critical role in attracting consumer attention, particularly when it deviates from the norm or includes compelling visual elements (Underwood, 2001;Garber et al., 2000). Research on food products suggests that larger sizes, bold colors, and distinct shapes enhance a product's visibility and purchase likelihood (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). ...
... Furthermore, product appearance reinforces brand identity, with rounded shapes conveying softness and femininity, while angular designs suggest energy and masculinity (Schmitt & Simonson, 1997). Consumers often favor rounded designs, as angular ones may appear outdated and less appealing (Creusen & Schoormans, 2005). ...
Article
This study investigates the factors influencing consumers' bottled water brand choice in Ethiopia using an explanatory research design and a quantitative research approach. Data were collected from 384 bottled water consumers through questionnaires, and multiple linear regression was employed for data analysis. The findings indicate that product quality, price, alternative attractiveness, and brand awareness significantly influence brand choice, with product quality and brand awareness emerging as the most critical factors. Consumers tend to prefer brands that consistently deliver high-quality water, highlighting the importance of stringent quality assurance programs, adherence to industry certifications, and transparent marketing strategies that reinforce trust in the product. Additionally, competitive pricing plays a vital role, suggesting that companies should align their pricing strategies with the perceived value of their products to appeal to price-sensitive customers without compromising quality. Alternative attractiveness also influences consumer decisions, implying that brands must differentiate themselves effectively through unique value propositions. Moreover, brand awareness is a key factor, necessitating targeted marketing efforts that enhance visibility, recall, and consumer loyalty. Therefore, water bottle companies should invest in promotional campaigns that emphasize both quality and affordability while ensuring consistent messaging across various communication channels.
... The study focused on consumers shopping for smart home products from a specific manufacturer, which may limit the applicability of the findings to other products (Polman and Maglio, 2022). Consumer decision-making processes and preferences vary across different product types and contexts, and the evaluative criteria for utilitarian products differ from those for hedonic products (Creusen and Schoormans, 2005). Consequently,the findings of this research invite further investigation across diverse contexts and product categories, offering an avenue for broader theoretical and managerial implications. ...
Article
Purpose The objective of this study is to explore the effect of family and message type interactions on the sales of smart home products. The study hypothesizes that dual-earner parents, a prolific segment of consumers, will indicate a greater willingness to pay for smart home products when exposed to characteristics-related marketing messages. Design/methodology/approach Two quasi-experimental studies were conducted to test the hypothesis. In collaboration with Samsung Electronics, the studies used different smart home product bundles (Smart Air Care and Smart Safety Care), recruited distinct participant groups (parents of children aged three to five and parents of children aged zero to three) and manipulated different types of benefits-related messages (a user review video and a teaser page). Findings In response to smart home product messaging, dual-earner parents exhibited greater willingness to pay when exposed to characteristics-related messages compared to benefits-related messages. This difference was not found among single-earner parents. Originality/value Challenging conventional marketing assumptions, the findings demonstrate that benefits-related messages do not universally appeal to smart home product consumers, while characteristics-related messages can increase willingness to pay among the dual-earner segment. The collaboration with Samsung Electronics in a quasi-experimental setting strengthens the external validity of the results, suggesting that marketers should tailor messaging strategies based on the characteristics of customer segments.
Article
Marketing universals are defined as consumer behaviors within a segment and toward a particular product category that are invariant across cultures. Using several definitions of culture and three different criteria for universality, the authors evaluate whether the use of brand, price, retailer reputation, and physical product appearance as signals of quality are marketing universals for consumer electronics products. Using a sample representing 38 nationalities, they find that there are few differences in the use of quality signals across cultures for a high priority segment of consumers. They draw conclusions for the adaptation versus standardization debate and argue that certain behaviors are likely to be universal, whereas others are not. Understanding such differences is essential to designing international marketing strategies.
Article
This paper defines hedonic consumption as those facets of consumer behavior that relate to the multisensory, fantasy and emotive aspects of product usage experience. After delineating these concepts, their theoretical antecedents are traced, followed by a discussion of differences between the traditional and hedonic views, methodological implications of the latter approach, and behavioral propositions in four substantive areas relevant to hedonic consumption—mental constructs, product classes, product usage and individual differences. Conclusions concern the usefulness of the hedonic perspective in supplementing and extending marketing research on consumer behavior.
Article
The physical form or design of a product is an unquestioned determinant of its marketplace success. A good design attracts consumers to a product, communicates to them, and adds value to the product by increasing the quality of the usage experiences associated with it. Nevertheless, the topic of product design is rarely, if ever, encountered in marketing journals. To bring needed attention to the subject of product design and enable researchers to better investigate design issues, the author introduces a conceptual model and several propositions that describe how the form of a product relates to consumers’ psychological and behavioral responses. After presenting this model, the author describes numerous strategic implications and research directions.
Article
In spite of the fact that product appearance would not seem to bear upon performance, this article provides evidence that the appearance of an industrial product may have an impact on its evaluation. Utilizing a conjoint scaling approach, Mel Yamamoto and David R. Lambert find that industrial product appearance exerts an influence, which in some circumstances exceeds the influence of certain product performance or price attributes. They suggest that attention paid to product aesthetics may have a payoff in terms of sales performance. And further, the impact of product appearance affects people in different organizational functions, across a range of technical orientations.
Article
Even though there is an established body of research concerning nonconscious memory processes and how they may influence preference formation, the importance of this work for understanding consumers' responses to product designs has been largely overlooked. Although evidence that product design can have a significant influence on consumers' reactions to products continues to grow, the process(es) by which responses to designs are formulated has received relatively little attention. The primary purposes of this article are to develop a nonconscious processing explanation of consumer response to product design and to focus attention on the area of design processing. A conceptual basis for understanding product design response formation is provided by research relating to various aspects of nonconscious processing (e.g., mere exposure, implicit learning). The implications of the proposed approach for consumer behavior and marketing are explored. Finally, several suggestions for future research directions are discussed. (C) 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.