ArticlePDF Available

Ingratiation and Self‐Promotion in the Selection Interview: The Effects of Using Single Tactics or a Combination of Tactics on Interviewer Judgments

Authors:

Abstract

This paper investigates the relative effectiveness of the use of 2 impression-management tactics—ingratiation and self-promotion—on interviewers' evaluations of an applicant in a laboratory setting. It was suggested that the use of a single tactic would be better than the use of no tactic; that the use of self-promotion would be more successful than the use of ingratiation; and, finally, that the use of a combination of tactics would lead to the best evaluations. Results were largely in line with our hypotheses. Interviewer ratings and action recommendations were more positive in the combination condition, followed by the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and the neutral condition. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Ingratiation and Self-Promotion in the Selection Interview:
The Effects of Using Single Tactics or a Combination of
Tactics on Interviewer Judgments
Karin Proost1
Hogeschool-Universiteit Brussel
Brussels, Belgium
and
Open University of the Netherlands
Heerlen, The Netherlands
Bert Schreurs
Hogeschool-Universiteit Brussel
Brussels, Belgium
Karel DeWitte
University of Leuven
Leuven, Belgium
Eva Derous
Ghent University
Ghent, Belgium
This paper investigates the relative effectiveness of the use of 2 impression-
management tactics—ingratiation and self-promotion—on interviewers’ evaluations
of an applicant in a laboratory setting. It was suggested that the use of a single tactic
would be better than the use of no tactic; that the use of self-promotion would be
more successful than the use of ingratiation; and, finally, that the use of a combina-
tion of tactics would lead to the best evaluations. Results were largely in line with our
hypotheses. Interviewer ratings and action recommendations were more positive in
the combination condition, followed by the self-promotion condition, the ingratia-
tion condition, and the neutral condition. Theoretical and practical implications are
discussed.jasp_654 2155..2169
Over 50 years ago, Goffman (1955) drew attention to the fact that people
consciously manage the impressions they convey to others in interpersonal
interactions. Especially in high-stakes situations, people will try to convey
a positive self-image by employing impression-management behaviors. This
also applies to the selection interview, which, unlike some other selection
devices (e.g., cognitive ability test, personality inventory), is characterized by
these social dynamics (Judge, Higgins, & Cable, 2000). Both the interviewer
and the applicant will attempt to create positive images relative to each other,
for the applicant to get the best job and for the organization to attract the
best applicant (Gilmore & Ferris, 1989).
1Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Karin Proost, Hogeschool-
Universiteit Brussel, Stormstraat 2, 1000 Brussels, Belgium. E-mail: Karin.proost@hubrussel.be
2155
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2010, 40, 9, pp. 2155–2169.
©2010 Copyright the Authors
Journal of Applied Social Psychology ©2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Past research has investigated applicants’ use of impression-management
(IM) tactics and how these tactics influence interviewer decisions. It was
found that IM tactics, and more specifically ingratiation and self-promotion,
were frequently used by applicants (Stevens & Kristof, 1995) and that these
tactics can be employed successfully in employment interviews (for a review,
see Higgins, Judge, & Ferris, 2003).
In current research on IM, however, IM tactics are often studied in
isolation, without considering the relative effectiveness of different tactics.
Therefore, the present study examines the relative effectiveness of the two
most frequently used tactics; namely, ingratiation and self-promotion
(Stevens & Kristof, 1995), with respect to personnel selection outcomes.
Although Kacmar and colleagues (Kacmar & Carlson, 1999; Kacmar,
Delery, & Ferris, 1992) already found that ingratiation was more effective
than self-promotion, these studies did not compare the use of a single tactic
to a neutral condition in which the applicant uses no IM tactics. Therefore,
the present study extends this research by comparing the effectiveness of
these tactics with a neutral condition, in which no IM tactics are employed.
The effects of combining different tactics have largely been ignored
(Higgins et al., 2003). Therefore, the current study also investigates the effect
of combining ingratiation and self-promotion tactics on personnel selection
outcomes. More specifically, it is investigated whether the combination of
these tactics will lead to more positive selection outcomes than the use of a
single tactic or no use of IM tactics at all.
Types of Impression-Management Tactics
Impression management refers to the activity of controlling information in
an attempt to steer the impression others form of oneself in the service of
personal or social goals (Schlenker & Pontari, 2000). IM tactics were classi-
fied by Tedeschi and Melburg (1984) as either assertive or defensive. Whereas
assertive IM tactics are used to bolster one’s image (e.g., self-enhancement,
other-enhancement), defensive tactics are employed to protect or repair one’s
image (e.g., accounts, excuses, apologies). Tactical assertive behaviors, con-
trary to defensive behaviors, seem particularly salient for applicants to use in
an employment interview (Gilmore & Ferris, 1989) and, therefore, are the
focus of the current study.
The two most frequently used assertive IM tactics in employment inter-
views are ingratiation and self-promotion (Stevens & Kristof, 1995). Ingra-
tiation tactics are used to evoke interpersonal attraction or liking, while
self-promotion tactics are intended to draw attention to the positive qualities
of oneself, one’s future plans, or one’s past accomplishments.
2156 PROOST ET AL.
Although both IM tactics can be classified as assertive tactics, they differ
in where they focus the conversation (i.e., on the applicant or on the inter-
viewer). Where ingratiation is other-focused, used to increase interpersonal
attraction or liking by employing subtle mechanisms of influence (i.e., ver-
bally praising the other person, conforming with the opinion of the other
person), self-promotion is self-focused, used to highlight one’s positive quali-
ties or to draw attention to past accomplishments (Kacmar et al., 1992;
Stevens & Kristof, 1995).
Relative Effectiveness of IM Tactics
Both ingratiation and self-promotion tactics have been positively related
to interviewer evaluations (Ellis, West, Ryan, & DeShon, 2002) and hiring
recommendations (Gilmore & Ferris, 1989; Kacmar et al., 1992) and have
been found to significantly predict whether applicants later obtained onsite
visits from the organization in a real personnel selection context (Stevens &
Kristof, 1995). However, ingratiation (belonging to the category of other-
focused tactics) and self-promotion (belonging to the category of self-focused
tactics) have been shown to have differential effectiveness with respect to
different outcomes. In general, it has been found that self-focused IM tactics
are more effective in employment interviews than are other-focused IM
tactics (Dipboye & Wiley, 1977; Kacmar & Carlson, 1999; Tullar, 1989).
More specifically, Kacmar et al. found that applicants who used self-focused
tactics received higher ratings and were given more job offers and fewer
rejections, but were not given more second interview offers.
Ferris and Judge (1991) developed a framework on political influence in
personnel/human resources management in which they proposed three medi-
ating processes by which applicant behaviors can influence employment
interview outcomes; namely, affect or liking, perceived fit, and assessment of
competence. Ingratiation is mainly focused on increasing liking and affect
through stimulating perceived similarity between the applicant and the inter-
viewer, leading to higher levels of perceived person–organization (P-O) fit
(Chen, Lee, & Yeh, 2008) and overall fit (Higgins & Judge, 2004). Self-
promotion, on the other hand, is mainly directed toward increasing the
assessment of competence (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Rudman, 1998), which
has been found to lead to higher levels of perceived person–job fit (Kristof-
Brown, Barrick, & Franke, 2002), but to lower levels of perceived similarity
(Howard & Ferris, 1996). As such, the differential orientation of ingratiation
and self-promotion, and the related outcomes, might explain why ingratia-
tion has been found to be more effective in employment interviews than
self-promotion.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2157
However, research on IM tactics has not allowed us to make any state-
ments on the extent to which the use of IM tactics (i.e., ingratiation, self-
promotion) improves interviewer decisions, compared to a neutral condition
in which applicants use no IM tactics. Therefore, the current study compares
a neutral condition in which no IM tactics are employed to two IM condi-
tions: one in which the applicant employs ingratiation tactics and one in
which the applicant employs self-promotion tactics.
Further, this study includes a combination condition in which applicants
use both tactics together. Although Baron (1986) found that the combination
of two nonverbal IM tactics in the employment interview induced a “too-
much-of-a-good-thing” effect, and thus led to lower interviewer evaluations.
Higgins et al. (2003) suggested that certain combinations of tactics may be
particularly successful in obtaining desirable outcomes. Evidence for this idea
was provided in a study by Falbe and Yukl (1992), who found that influence
attempts in which a pair of tactics was used had more favorable outcomes
than when a single tactic was used. Bolino and Turnley (2003) suggested
more specifically that the combination of self-promotion with ingratiation
might be an especially successful recipe. If ingratiation leads to higher per-
ceived similarity between the applicant and the interviewer (Chen et al.,
2008), while self-promotion increases the perception of competence (Jones &
Pittman, 1982; Rudman, 1998), combining both tactics might, indeed, be a
good strategy to influence interviewer evaluations.
In line with this stream of research, the following hypotheses are
formulated:
Hypothesis 1. Participants in the combination condition will
rate the applicant most positively, followed by participants in
the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and
the neutral condition.
Hypothesis 2. Participants in the combination condition will be
the most likely to offer a job, followed by participants in the
self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and the
neutral condition.
Hypothesis 3. Participants in the combination condition will be
the least likely to reject the applicant, followed by participants
in the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and
the neutral condition.
Hypothesis 4. Participants in the combination condition will be
the most likely to invite the applicant for a second interview,
followed by participants in the self-promotion condition, the
ingratiation condition, and the neutral condition.
2158 PROOST ET AL.
Method
Participants
Data were collected from 160 psychology students (76 men, 84 women) at
a large public university. Students were randomly assigned to one of four
experimental conditions. Participants’ average age was 19.0 years (SD =1.0).
The students participated in the study to receive credit points.
Procedure
Participants in each experimental condition were asked to imagine
themselves working in a company’s human resources department. After
receiving a short introduction on the experiment and some oral information
about the company, the participants were assigned the task of hiring an
assistant IT manager, who specializes in the design of Internet and Intranet
software. The participants were told that applicants for the job had already
been attracted and now were invited for a brief selection interview. They
received a short job description and a curriculum vitae (CV) of one fictive
applicant.
The experiment consisted of rating the applicant on the basis of a video-
taped selection interview. A between-persons design was used such that each
participant viewed only one condition. After having seen the videotape,
participants were asked to rate the applicant by completing a questionnaire.
Development of Videotapes
Videotaped interviews of an applicant for the job of assistant IT manager
and an interviewer were constructed to be approximately 5 min long. The
interviewer was near the camera, so it appeared that the applicant was
speaking to the research participants, or looking toward the camera. The
research participants could hear the interviewer ask the questions, but did not
see him on the video. The fictive applicant was sitting behind a table and was
visible from his waist up.
The fictive applicant was an assistant in the university’s Department of
Personnel Psychology, who had experience with conducting selection inter-
views. Before starting, the fictive applicant carefully studied the different
experimental conditions so that his answers would appear to be fluent and
natural.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2159
Experimental Conditions
Every interview consisted of eight questions of which two questions—
and, more specifically, their answers—were kept constant across the experi-
mental conditions. Answers on the other six questions differed between the
experimental conditions, but only with respect to the IM tactic(s) employed.
This means that the answers only differed in one or two sentences that were
added, depending on the experimental condition.
In the first, neutral condition, the applicant answered in a neutral way,
without employing IM tactics. The second condition was the ingratiation
condition in which the applicant used verbal tactics to make the interviewer
feel good about himself or herself. For example, in this condition, the appli-
cant complimented the interviewer on the way he conducted the selection
interview. The third condition was the self-promotion condition, in which the
applicant directed attention to his positive qualities. For example, the appli-
cant emphasized different extracurricular activities in which he participated
and that could benefit him in this job. The fourth condition was the high IM
condition and combined the IM tactics used in the ingratiation condition and
self-promotion condition.
Job description, CV, content and structure of the interview were kept
constant across the four experimental conditions. Also, nonverbal behavior
was controlled for by having the same male person playing the four condi-
tions, dressed identically and being interviewed in the same office setting. If,
during the taping, the interviewer or the fictive applicant had the feeling that
the nonverbal behavior was not kept constant, the experimental condition
was redone. Finally, three independent judges from the department looked
at all videotapes in order to evaluate the standardized use of nonverbal
behavior across conditions.
Measures
Interviewer rating. In line with Kacmar et al.’s (1992) study, participants
were asked to rate the applicant on 12 qualities. These qualities are flexibility,
motivation,enthusiasm,initiative,presence,past experience,communication
skills,confidence,technical skills,analytical ability,conceptual ability, and
knowledge of Internet and Intranet applications. The items were rated on
a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to4(strongly agree).
Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .84.
Action recommendation. In line with Kacmar et al. (1992), three items
were formulated with respect to action recommendation and were used
separately in the analyses. The three questions are “Would you invite the
2160 PROOST ET AL.
applicant for a second interview?”; “Would you offer the applicant a job?”;
and “Would you send the applicant a rejection letter?”. All items were rated
on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to4(strongly agree).
Manipulation check. In order to verify whether IM had been manipulated
adequately, three items were added to the questionnaire. Items questioned
the degree to which the participants felt that the applicant had answered in a
neutral way, used ingratiation tactics, and used self-promotion tactics. All
items were rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to4
(strongly agree).
In order to ensure that the four conditions were constant with respect to
job-relevant information and in line with Kacmar et al.’s (1992) study, par-
ticipants were asked to indicate when during the interview they arrived at
their decision (i.e., after reading the CV, after the interview; 0 =no,1=yes)
and which characteristics, based on the 12 qualities on which they rated the
applicant, were important in their decisions. The items were framed as “The
following qualities of the applicant have led to my decision... and were
answered on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree)to4(totally
agree).
Results
Manipulation Checks
In order to verify whether IM had been manipulated adequately, a
MANOVA was computed, using the three items that measured the extent to
which the applicant engaged in IM tactics as the dependent variables, and IM
as the independent variable. The MANOVA shows an overall significant
effect for the independent variable, F(9, 353) =14.52, p<.001. Also, the three
univariate tests show significant effects in line with the IM manipulations in
the different experimental conditions. Descriptive statistics are presented in
Table 1. In the combination condition, respondents rated higher on self-
promotion tactics than on ingratiation tactics (i.e., 3 out of 4 paired ttests
were significant at .01), which makes the experimental condition in line with
that of Stevens and Kristof’s (1995) field study.
In order to ensure that the four conditions were constant with respect to
job-relevant information and in line with Kacmar et al. (1992), a second
MANOVA was performed, with the importance of each of the 12 character-
istics in the decision-making process and the moment of decision making as
dependent variables, and IM as the independent variable. The results show
no significant effect for importance of the characteristics in the decision-
making process and the moment of decision making, F(42, 280) =0.84,
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2161
p=.75. These results suggest that the four experimental conditions were,
indeed, equal with respect to job-relevant information.
Hypotheses
A MANOVA was conducted in order to test Hypotheses 1 through 4,
with interviewer rating and each of the three action recommendation items as
dependent variables, and IM as the independent variable. The results show
an overall significant main effect for IM, F(12, 379) =2.65, p=.002, h2=.07.
Consequently, four univariate ANOVAs were conducted to explore the
effects at the level of each dependent variable. Also, follow-up paired com-
parisons (using Tukey’s HSD correction) were performed to explore the
mean differences between the conditions.
Hypothesis 1 predicted that participants in the combination condition
would give the applicant the highest rating, followed by participants in the
self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and finally the neutral
condition. A one-way ANOVA was significant for interviewer ratings, F(3,
146) =8.79, p=.000, h2=.15. Inspection of the means shows that the highest
Table 1
Mean Values on Items Measuring Adequacy of the Experimental
Manipulations
Item Neutral Ingratiation Self-promotion Combination
In general, the
applicant
responded in a
neutral way to
my questions.
2.85 2.38 2.66 2.28
The applicant tried
to flatter me during
the interview.
1.95 3.38 2.17 3.49
The applicant
promoted himself
during the
interview.
3.18 3.35 3.91 3.85
Note. Entries in boldface show that impression management had been manipulated
adequately in the different conditions.
2162 PROOST ET AL.
rating was given in the combination condition (M=35.56, SD =4.50),
followed by the self-promotion condition (M=34.08, SD =0.08), the ingra-
tiation condition (M=32.50, SD =6.26), and finally the neutral condition
(M=29.36, SD =29.36). Follow-up paired comparisons, however, only
reveal a significant difference between the neutral condition and both the
self-promotion condition and the combination condition. The ingratiation
condition did not differ significantly from all other conditions.
Hypothesis 2 predicted that participants in the combination condition
would be the most likely to offer a job, followed by participants in the
self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and finally the neutral
condition. A one-way ANOVA was significant for job offers, F(3, 146) =3.57,
p=.02, h2=.07. Inspection of the means shows that the highest rating was
given in the combination condition (M=2.82, SD =0.51), followed by the
self-promotion condition (M=2.70, SD =0.52), the ingratiation condition
(M=2.53, SD =0.76), and finally the neutral condition (M=2.39, SD =0.65).
Follow-up paired comparisons, however, only reveal a significant difference
between the neutral condition and the combination condition. The ingratia-
tion condition and the self-promotion conditions did not differ significantly
from each other, nor did they differ significantly from the neutral and the
combination conditions.
Hypothesis 3 predicted that participants in the combination condition
would be the least likely to reject the applicant, followed by participants in
the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition, and finally the
neutral condition. A one-way ANOVA was not significant, F(3, 146) =1.64,
p=.18; and follow-up paired comparisons show no significant differences
between the conditions. Inspection of the means, however, shows that the
results were in line with Hypothesis 3. The lowest rating was given in the
combination condition (M=2.18, SD =0.72), followed by the self-promotion
condition (M=2.27, SD =0.56), the ingratiation condition (M=2.45, SD =
0.76), and finally the neutral condition (M=2.50, SD =0.81).
Hypothesis 4 predicted that participants in the combination condition
would be the most likely to invite the applicant for a second interview,
followed by participants in the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation
condition, and finally the neutral condition. A one-way ANOVA was mar-
ginally significant, F(3, 146) =2.45, p=.07, h2=.05. Inspection of the means
again shows that the results were mainly in line with Hypothesis 4. However,
the highest rating was given in the self-promotion condition (M=3.05, SD =
0.62), followed by the combination condition (M=2.92, SD =0.48), the
ingratiation condition (M=2.87, SD =0.81), and finally the neutral condition
(M=2.64, SD =0.72). Follow-up paired comparisons only reveal a signifi-
cant difference between the neutral condition and the self-promotion
condition.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2163
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the relative effectiveness of
applicants’ use of IM tactics in the selection interview. This study contributes
to the current literature in the sense that the effectiveness of ingratiation and
self-promotion were studied, relative to each other and relative to a neutral
condition in which the applicant used no IM tactics. A combination con-
dition, in which the applicant combined both tactics, was compared to
the neutral condition and the single use conditions (i.e., ingratiation and
self-promotion).
All results were in the expected direction. In general, interviewers’ rating
and action recommendations were more positive in the combination condi-
tion, followed by the self-promotion condition, the ingratiation condition,
and the neutral condition. However, although the results were in line with the
Kacmar et al.’s (1992) finding that self-promotion tactics are more effective
than ingratiation tactics, the conditions did not differ significantly from each
other.
The current design allows us to reach clearer conclusions on the
effectiveness of different IM tactics, as a result of the inclusion of a neutral
and a combination condition. For example, the ingratiation condition
was less effective than was the self-promotion condition in the sense that
it did not differ significantly from the neutral condition for any of the
dependent variables, while the self-promotion condition and the combina-
tion led to higher interviewer ratings. The self-promotion condition was
also more effective with respect to being offered a second interview, while
the combination condition was more effective with respect to getting a job
offer.
The results with respect to inviting the candidate for a second interview
are less clear. The highest mean level for this variable was found in the
self-promotion condition, with a slight decrease in the combination condi-
tion. These results were in line with Kacmar et al. (1992), who found dif-
ferent results with respect to this dependent variable (i.e., second-interview
offer), as compared to the other dependent variables in this study (i.e.,
interviewer ratings, job offer, rejection letter). This finding might be
explained by the double message that is included in inviting someone for a
second interview. On the one hand, this might mean that the interviewer
finds the applicant a good candidate for the job and, therefore, wants the
candidate to proceed through the rest of the selection procedure. On the
other hand, this might mean that the interviewer was not able to collect
enough information from the applicant and wants to be able to elaborate
more on certain aspects in a second, follow-up interview, as was the case in
Kacmar et al.’s study.
2164 PROOST ET AL.
Strengths and Limitations
A key problem with field studies is determining when the applicant uses
IM and when the applicant responds in an honest and objective way
(Peeters & Lievens, 2006). Conducting a lab study enabled us to manipulate
IM use and to disentangle rival explanations for the results. This scenario
research methodology has been used effectively in the past and provides
several benefits (Fandt & Ferris, 1990; Liden, Ferris, & Dienesch, 1988).
Scenarios provide respondents with standardized stimuli, thus eliminating
potential sources of interpretation error. Further, great effort was taken to
ensure that the job-related information presented was held constant across
the four scripts. Therefore, it is reasonable that observed differences between
the four conditions are a result of the manipulations in our study (i.e., level
of IM).
On the other hand, this methodology limits the external validity of the
current study. The fact that interviewers evaluated a videotaped candidate
and had no face-to-face contact might have affected our results. For example,
Van Iddekinge, Raymark, and Roth (2003) showed that ratings of video-
taped interviews are more resistant to interviewee response distortion, which
means that the effect sizes in our study might have been an underestimation
of the real effect sizes.
Another limitation with respect to the external validity of this study is the
use of one applicant and one job. Using a male applicant—being evaluated
by both male and female interviewers—did not allow us to check for differ-
ences in evaluating applicants of the same sex/different sexes. For example,
Graves and Powell (1995) found that interviewers give higher ratings to
applicants of the opposite sex. Therefore, further research could be con-
ducted with both a male and a female applicant in each condition. Van
Vianen and Willemsen (1992) found that for higher level technical jobs, the
ideal candidate is described by job interviewers as having more masculine
traits than feminine traits. Since the job that was used in the present study
could be categorized as a higher level technical job and a male applicant was
used, this might have inflated ratings on the dependent variables. Therefore,
future research with other job descriptions and with male and female appli-
cants is warranted.
The use of one job also limits the generalizability of the results. Previous
research has shown that IM tactics can detract from, improve, or have no
impact on the image observers have of an individual, depending on the
characteristics of the situation in which the tactic was demonstrated (Giaca-
lone, 1985; Kacmar & Carlson, 1999; Tedeschi & Melburg, 1984). In this
study, a back-office job was used, whereas the use of a more commercial job
with the same applicant could have shown very different results.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2165
Directions for Future Research
Further research is necessary to develop a deeper understanding of the
underlying processes that cause IM tactics to have an impact on interviewers’
evaluations. For example, little is known about how individual-difference
variables (e.g., interviewer experience, personality characteristics, gender)
moderate the way IM tactics are evaluated. For example, Baron (1986)
suggested that men base their evaluation more on external related factors
than do women and are less adapted to ignore certain aspects than are
women. Therefore, men would need more time to decide whether behavior is
either situational or dispositional than would women, leading to a more
negative evaluation when there are many external, distracting factors (e.g.,
IM behavior). Research on antecedents of the frequency and types of
IM tactics applicants use is rather scarce and has rarely considered the
joint influence of situational and dispositional variables (Van Iddekinge,
McFarland, & Raymark, 2007).
One meaningful way to move forward with this research is to develop
and test more comprehensive models of decision making in the employment
interview, considering simultaneously the influence of interviewer charac-
teristics, applicant behaviors, and situational characteristics, as well as
mediating processes (see Gilmore, Stevens, Harrell-Cook, & Ferris, 1999).
Several studies have already considered the influence of mediating vari-
ables, such as applicant similarity to the interviewer and interviewer affect
toward the applicant (e.g., Gallois, Callan, & Palmer, 1992; Howard &
Ferris, 1996). An even broader model of decision making was tested by Van
Iddekinge et al. (2007), who showed that interviewee personality affects the
use of IM tactics, which in turn affects interview performance, depending
on the situational strength of the context (i.e., getting a performance incen-
tive or not). Further research should continue in this direction since the
study of mediating and moderating influences is important in order to dis-
cover explanatory mechanisms through which IM tactics influence inter-
view outcomes.
Other selection tools besides the selection interview deserve additional
attention. For example, Varma, Toh, and Pichler (2006) considered how
these same IM tactics may be used in job applicant letters, and their results
were in line with the results on the selection interview, in the sense that
self-focused tactics were more effective than were other-focused tactics.
Further research could be conducted in order to deepen our understanding of
how IM tactics may influence the decisions of recruiters who rely on written
applications, or a combination of written applications and selection
interviews.
2166 PROOST ET AL.
Practical Implications
The results of the current study suggest that it is better to use any type of
IM tactics in the interview than use no tactic at all. However, using only
ingratiation was not effective enough to differentiate oneself from the
applicant using no IM tactics. Using self-promotion or a combination of
self-promotion and ingratiation led to higher interviewer ratings. Using
self-promotion alone also led to more second-interview offers, whereas
using a combination of tactics also led to more job offers.
The current results were obtained with respect to selection interview
outcomes. One should be careful, however, in translating the results to other
contexts. In line with Higgins et al.’s (2003) meta-analytic review, it was
found that ingratiation—especially self-promotion—works well in an inter-
view. These results must be somewhat differentiated, since self-promotion
in particular appears to backfire in performance evaluations provided by
supervisors (Gordon, 1996). Jones and Pittman (1982) suggested that self-
promotion may be less successful when claims of competence can be verified.
References
Baron, R. A. (1986). Self-presentation in job interviews: When there can be
“too much of a good thing.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology,16,
16–28.
Bolino, M. C., & Turnley, W. H. (2003). More than one way to make an
impression: Exploring profiles of impression management. Journal of
Management,29, 141–160.
Chen, C. H. V., Lee, H. M., & Yeh, Y. J. Y. (2008). The antecedent and
consequence of person–organization fit: Ingratiation, similarity, hiring
recommendations, and job offer. International Journal of Selection and
Assessment,16, 210–219.
Dipboye, R. L., & Wiley, J. W. (1977). Reactions of male raters to inter-
viewee self-presentation style and sex: Extensions of previous research.
Journal of Vocational Behavior,13, 192–203.
Ellis, A. P. J., West, B. J., Ryan, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). The use of
impression management tactics in structured interviews: A function of
question type? Journal of Applied Psychology,87, 1200–1208.
Falbe, C. M., & Yukl, G. (1992). Consequences for managers of using single
influence tactics and combinations of tactics. Academy of Management
Journal,35, 638–652.
Fandt, P. M., & Ferris, G. R. (1990). The management of information and
impressions: When employees behave opportunistically. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes,45, 739–749.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2167
Ferris, G. R., & Judge, T. A. (1991). Personnel/human resource management:
A political influence perspective. Journal of Management,17, 447–488.
Gallois, C., Callan, V. J., & Palmer, J. A. M. (1992). The influence of
applicant communication style and interviewer characteristics on hiring
decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,22, 1041–1060.
Giacalone, R. A. (1985). On slipping when you thought you had put your
best foot forward: Self-promotion, self-destruction, and entitlements.
Group and Organizational Studies,10, 61–80.
Gilmore, D. C., & Ferris, G. R. (1989). The effects of applicant impression
management tactics on interviewer judgments. Journal of Management,
15, 557–564.
Gilmore, D. C., Stevens, C. K., Harrell-Cook, G., & Ferris, G. R. (1999).
Impression management tactics. In R. W. Eder & M. M. Harris (Eds.),
The employment interview handbook (pp. 321–336). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Goffman, E. (1955). On face work: An analysis of ritual elements in social
interactions. Psychiatry,22, 225–237.
Gordon, R. A. (1996). Impact of ingratiation on judgments and evaluations:
A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,71, 54–70.
Graves, L. M., & Powell, G. N. (1995). An investigation of sex discrimination
in recruiters’ evaluations of actual applicants. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy,73, 20–29.
Higgins, C. A., & Judge, T. A. (2004). The effect of applicant influence tactics
on recruiter perceptions of fit and hiring recommendations: A field study.
Journal of Applied Psychology,89, 622–632.
Higgins, C. A., Judge, T. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2003). Influence tactics and
work outcomes: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior,24,
89–106.
Howard, J. L., & Ferris, G. R. (1996). The employment interview context:
Social and situational influences on interviewer decisions. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology,26, 112–136.
Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self
presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (pp.
231–262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., & Cable, D. M. (2000). The employment
interview: A review of recent research and recommendations for future
research. Human Resource Management Review,10, 383–406.
Kacmar, K. M., & Carlson, D. S. (1999). Effectiveness of impression man-
agement tactics across human resource situations. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology,29, 1293–1315.
2168 PROOST ET AL.
Kacmar, K. M., Delery, J. E., & Ferris, G. R. (1992). Differential effective-
ness of applicant impression tactics on employment interview decisions.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology,22, 1250–1272.
Kristof-Brown, A., Barrick, M. R., & Franke, M. (2002). Applicant
impression management: Dispositional influences and consequences for
recruiter perceptions of fit and similarity. Journal of Management,28,
27–46.
Liden, R. C., Ferris, G. R., & Dienesch, R. M. (1988). The influence of causal
feedback on subordinate reactions and behavior. Group and Organization
Studies,13, 348–373.
Peeters, H., & Lievens, F. (2006). Verbal and nonverbal impression manage-
ment tactics in behavior description and situational interviews. Interna-
tional Journal of Selection and Assessment,14, 206–222.
Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs
and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,74, 629–645.
Schlenker, B. R., & Pontari, B. A. (2000). The strategic control of informa-
tion: Impression management and self-presentation in daily life. In A.
Tesser, R. Felson, & J. Suls (Eds.), Perspectives on self and identity (pp.
199–232). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Stevens, C. K., & Kristof, A. L. (1995). Making the right impression: A field
study of applicant impression management during job interviews. Journal
of Applied Psychology,80, 587–606.
Tedeschi, J. T., & Melburg, V. (1984). Impression management and influence
in the organization. In S. B. Bacharach & E. J. Lawler (Eds.), Research in
sociology of organizations (Vol. 3, pp. 31–58). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Tullar, W. L. (1989). Relational control in the employment interview. Journal
of Applied Psychology,74, 971–977.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., McFarland, L. A., & Raymark, P. H. (2007). Ante-
cedents of impression management use and effectiveness in a structured
interview. Journal of Management,33, 752–773.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Raymark, P. H., & Roth, P. L. (2003, April). Live
versus videotaped selection interviews: Are they comparable? Paper pre-
sented at the 18th annual conference of the Society for Industrial and
Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL.
Van Vianen, A. E. M., & Willemsen, T. M. (1992). The employment inter-
view: The role of sex stereotypes in the evaluation of male and female job
applicants in The Netherlands. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,22,
471–491.
Varma, A., Toh, S. M., & Pichler, S. (2006). Ingratiation in job applications:
Impact on selection decisions. Journal of Managerial Psychology,21,
200–210.
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT AND INTERVIEWER JUDGMENTS 2169
... The literature reviewed is somewhat mixed with respect to how effective IM is in job selection. Some research has indicated that ingratiation leads to significantly higher ratings of applicants (Proost et al., 2010;Waung et al., 2017), and increases their likelihood of receiving an offer (Zhao & Liden, 2011). On the other hand, Higgins and Judge (2004) found the effect of selfpromotion on hiring recommendations to be generally weak and non-significant. ...
... The finding that the use of self-promotion led to highest selection recommendation is consistent with previous research (for instance, Lievens & Peeters, 2008;Proost et al., 2010;Varma et al., 2006). Self-promotion statements increase the perceived level of competence (Jones & Pittman, 1982) and selfconfidence (Knous et. ...
... The first limitation of the study emerges from the use of college students as participants. While previous research has reliably used inexperienced students as recruiters in lab investigations (e.g., Proost et al., 2010;Varma et al., 2006), it does reduce the generalizability of the study to real-life selection situations. Future researchers can use actual personnel professionals as participants. ...
... When individuals apply for a new job or a promotion, they try to present themselves in the best possible light. During personnel selection or promotion processes, most applicants use impression management strategies to improve their professional attractiveness (Jones & Pittman, 1982;Proost et al., 2010;Swider et al., 2011). One of the presentation strategies they use to increase their status and attractiveness consists in attributing achievements internally, rather than externally (Bolino et al., 2008;Jones & Pittman, 1982;Proost et al., 2010;Rosenfeld et al., 1995;Rudman, 1998). ...
... During personnel selection or promotion processes, most applicants use impression management strategies to improve their professional attractiveness (Jones & Pittman, 1982;Proost et al., 2010;Swider et al., 2011). One of the presentation strategies they use to increase their status and attractiveness consists in attributing achievements internally, rather than externally (Bolino et al., 2008;Jones & Pittman, 1982;Proost et al., 2010;Rosenfeld et al., 1995;Rudman, 1998). When individuals have to explain a negative event, research on impression management shows that the use of defensive tactics (e.g., apologies, justifications, and excuses) has a positive effect (Tsai et al., 2010). ...
Article
This study was carried out within the context of an assessment for promotion to a high-status position. It aimed to determine the conditions in which the backlash effect occurs in a group characterized by negative stereotypes owing to their ethnicity: North African males in France. One hundred twenty-eight recruitment professionals assessed the probability of promoting one of eight fictitious male applicants with different causal attributions (internal or external) and levels of technical competence (high or average), and of different ethnicities (European or North African). Internal attribution, one of the dimensions of self-promotion, was regarded as a counterstereotypical behavior for a North African applicant compared with a European applicant. Backlash was only observed in a high-threat context.
... Methodological steps are shown in Figure 1. However, we acknowledge that it is not always possible to distinguish clearly between individual categories (Proost et al., 2010). Given the exploratory nature of our analysis and the aim to identify patterns in relation to impression management tactics within our sample, we believe that this approach is fully appropriate. ...
... Among the assertive impression management tactics, self-promotion appears to be most widely applied. Especially for self-promotion and ingratiation, it can be difficult to distinguish clearly between these two given that both of them are positive tactics that are related to stakeholders' evaluations (Proost et al., 2010). Along similar lines, self-promotion can contain elements of exemplification, enhancement and entitlement tactics, and it can aim to promote attributions of competence. ...
Article
In this study, we construct a classificatory scheme of impression management tactics and apply it to a sample of sustainability report and annual report CEO statements issued by large Turkish companies. We find a wide range of impression management tactics applied in both types of CEO statements. However, impression management in sustainability report CEO statements emerges as more limited and uniform, revealing clearer prioritizations of impression management tactics when compared to annual report CEO statements. Sustainability report CEO statements emphasize tactics that allow the company to highlight carefully selected, isolated aspects of their sustainability engagement. At the same time, they rarely employ tactics that are geared towards more systematic (quantitative) performance evaluation. We attribute these differences to the way in which corporate sustainability performance is commonly conceptualized and evaluated, emphasising qualitative performance dimensions rather than the reporting of quantitative performance metrics. This results in the application of a different impression management tactics mix and limits the usefulness of sustainability reports as an accountability mechanism when compared to corporate annual reports. In addition, we provide tentative evidence for the context‐specific nature of impression management, with the choice of impression management tactics reflecting the institutional environment a company is based in.
... Ingratiation Frontiers in Psychology 02 frontiersin.org tactics can arouse interpersonal attraction and leaders' preferences (Proost et al., 2010;Gross et al., 2021). Previous studies have shown that impression management has an important impact on all aspects of organizational work (Barrick et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Why do people who seem to be doing well in the workplace occasionally behave badly? Because these employees may be using impression management tactics to create an image. Existing studies have focused on comparing the differences in the use of impression management among different individuals, but cannot explain why a well-behaved individual sometimes behaves badly. Based on the theory of self-control resources, we adopts the survey method of job logs and collects the data of 121 employees. The results show that: (1) the use of self-promotion tactics and ingratiation tactics will promote the depletion of self-control resources. (2) The depletion of self-control resources will encourage employees to engage in counterproductive work behaviors. (3) The effect of self-promotion tactic and ingratiation tactic on the depletion of self-control resources was moderated by emotional intelligence. In the case of high emotional intelligence, this effect is attenuated. And vice versa. (4) Under high emotional intelligence and low emotional intelligence, the indirect effects of self-promotion tactic and ingratiation tactic on employees’ counterproductive work behaviors are significantly different. Our research breaks through the between-individual perspective and illustrates the double-edged sword effect of self-promotion tactic and ingratiation tactic on employee counterproductive work behavior and its mechanism from the internal perspective, which is highly innovative.
... The rejection of H2 could have contributed to H3 not finding any support for the hypothesised interaction effect of inspirational appeal and double-tap on purchase intentions. Unlike the combinations of influence strategies that act together to achieve the desired goal in different settings (Bande et al. 2019;Proost et al. 2010), the combination of inspirational appeal and ingratiation did not prove to be effective in achieving the desired increase in purchase intentions. Hence, the lack of support for H2 and H3 could be due to the operationalisation of ingratiation in the study. ...
Article
The research investigates the effects of chatbot-delivered verbal (inspirational appeals) and non-verbal (ingratiation) cues on customers’ purchase intentions on social commerce pages (study one). The research using a multi-method approach also investigates the role of social presence and ulterior motives in explaining the effects of the chatbot-delivered human-like cues on customers’ purchase intentions (study two). Study one employs an experiment, followed by a structural equation model in study two. The results of study two enable the researchers to triangulate the results of study one and to understand the underlying causes of those results. The data was collected through an online experiment and an online survey for the two studies. The research reports that: (1) inspirational appeals delivered by chatbots positively influence purchase intentions; (2) ingratiation delivered using double-tap by chatbots does not significantly affect purchase intentions. The current research represents one of the first attempts to explore the influence of inspirational appeal and ingratiation delivered through chatbots on s-commerce purchase intentions. The findings of the study provide managerial insights to social commerce pages that could help engage customers and circumvent the attribution of ulterior motives while making a sale.
... It is an important impression management technique in professional communication. Prior studies have found that self-promotion, when combined with other impression management techniques such as ingratiation for likeability, resulted in better interview evaluations (Proost et al., 2010). However, selfpromotion was also found to be a risk factor for women-those who self-promoted may have encountered backlash for violating gender stereotypes (Rudman, 1998). ...
... Although prior literature suggests that flattery largely benefits job candidates (Higgins et al., 2003), what remains less clear is whether organizations benefit from hiring candidates who use flattery during the hiring process. On the one hand, candidate flattery may be interpreted as a job-relevant signal of a candidate's motivation and capacity to get along with others (Proost et al., 2010). On the other hand, if flattery has a biasing influence on hiring managers, such as causing them to overlook a candidate's lower qualifications, then such errors could be costly to organizations (Marlowe et al., 1996). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite decades of research on job candidate ingratiation tactics, few studies have examined if hiring manager personality traits might influence the effectiveness of candidate tactics. Undergraduates (n = 254) played the role of a hiring manager in a low-fidelity simulation and completed a measure of non-clinical narcissism. Candidate flattery and qualifications were experimentally manipulated. Contrary to predictions, results showed that candidate flattery demonstrated strong negative effects on perceptions of candidate fit and the likelihood of being invited for an interview during preliminary recruitment stages. Although hiring manager narcissism was found to attenuate the negative effects of candidate flattery, these findings broadly suggest that jobseekers have more to lose than gain by flattering hiring managers.
Chapter
Full-text available
Bu bölümde örgütsel politika kavramını oluşturan ana kavramlar, örgüt içinde bireylerin sergilediği politik eylemler, bireysel politik özellikler ve bireylerin algıladıkları örgütsel politika üzerinde temel bir yazın taraması yapılmıştır. Takiben, örgütsel politika altındaki bu kavramların ve ilgili arattırmaların günümüzün dijitalleşen dünyasında ve dijitalleşen örgütlerdeki karşılıkları üzerine bir yorum getirilmiştir.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has shown that evaluators react negatively to intense, high levels of self-promotion during the interview, in particular when displayed by female candidates, presumably because these behaviors violate the female gender stereotype of being modest and putting others first. We expand this focus on a single social category and examine the joint effects of gender and age on reactions to high self-promotion/low modesty, as both gender and age stereotypes contain normative expectations regarding assertiveness and humility. Results of our experimental study point out two groups at risk of backlash, older women and younger men. While both older female and younger male candidates engaging in high self-promotion and showing less modesty were seen as competent, they were regarded as less interpersonally warm, received lower interview performance ratings and were less likely to be hired. These results provide evidence for the importance of applying an intersectional lens on the effects of self-promotion at hiring. Their implications for theory and practice as well as recommendations for future research are discussed.
Article
Using an experiment, we investigate whether job candidates' noncontractible effort promises increase their actual effort in the work relationship when the labor market is competitive. Due to promise-keeping preferences, individuals tend to keep promises even if doing so is costly. However, when promises can be made strategically to influence hiring decisions, it is unclear whether workers are less likely to keep their promises. We develop theory to predict that making effort promises matters even more when labor markets are competitive. We find workers promise higher effort levels when competing for a job than when they do not, but do not keep promises to a lesser extent although the costs of promise-keeping increase with the promise size, thereby increasing the total effort provided. The results enhance our understanding of the effects of worker-employer communication during hiring, particularly in a competitive setting in which such communication is most likely to occur.
Article
Full-text available
Although differential item functioning (DIF) theory traditionally focuses on the behavior of individual items in two (or a few) specific groups, in educational measurement contexts, it is often plausible to regard the set of items as a random sample from a broader category. This article presents logistic mixed models that can be used to model uniform DIF, treating the item effects and their interaction with groups (DIF) as random. In a similar way, the group effects can be modeled as random instead of fixed, if the groups can be considered a random sample from a population of groups. The models can, furthermore, be adapted easily for modeling DIF over individual persons rather than over groups, or for modeling the differential functioning of groups of items instead of individual items. It is shown that the logistic mixed model approach is not only a comprehensive and economical way to detect these different kinds of DIF, it also encourages us to explore possible explanations of DIF by including group or item covariates in the model.
Article
This study investigated impression management tactic use during structured interviews containing both experience-based and situational questions. Specifically, the authors examined whether applicants' use of impression management tactics depended on question type. Results from 119 structured interviews indicated that almost all of the applicants used some form of impression management. Significantly more assertive than defensive impression management tactics were used, and among assertive tactics, applicants tended to use self-promotion rather than ingratiation. However, different question types prompted the use of different impression management tactics. Ingratiation tactics were used significantly more when applicants answered situational questions, whereas self-promotion tactics were used significantly more when applicants answered experience-based questions. Furthermore, the use of self-promotion and ingratiation tactics was positively related to interviewer evaluations.
Article
Two issues were explored: (a) which impression management (IM) tactics applicants use during actual interviews and (b) whether there is a relationship between applicants' IM tactics and their interview outcomes. The study also examined convergence across different methods and raters when measuring IM. Postinterview survey measures were obtained from applicants and interviewers regarding applicant IM behavior during a specific interview; in addition, a subset (n = 24) of interviews was audiotaped and analyzed for the presence of IM. Analyses revealed low to moderate convergence across methods and raters, suggesting that IM tactics may be multidimensional constructs. Across methods and raters, there was consistent evidence of greater applicant self-promotion than ingratiation. Similarly, IM tactics significantly predicted interviewers' evaluations and whether applicants later obtained site visits. Implications for future research are discussed.
Article
This study investigated whether sex discrimination existed in recruiters' evaluations of applicants in real employment interviews and, if so, whether other variables could explain it. The variables examined were the perceived similarity of the applicant to the recruiter, the interpersonal attraction felt by the recruiter toward the applicant, and the applicant's objectively measured and subjectively measured qualifications. Applicant sex did not directly or indirectly affect recruiters' evaluations of applicants in campus interviews, providing no evidence of sex discrimination. Instead, subjective qualifications had a significant effect on evaluations and also mediated the effects of perceived similarity, interpersonal attraction, and objective qualifications on evaluations. Recruiters saw stronger subjective qualifications in applicants with high scholastic performance whom they viewed as similar to themselves and whom they liked. Perceptions of these qualifications, in turn, were the primary determinant of evaluations.
Article
The study involved analyses of incidents described from the perspective of the targets of influence attempts. We coded influence behavior in the incidents into nine tactics and classified outcomes as commitment, compliance, or resistance. Hypotheses were developed to explain the outcome of each tactic used alone and in combination. The findings supported most of the hypotheses. The most effective tactics were inspirational appeals and consultation. The least effective were pressure, legitimating, and coalition tactics. Intermediate in effectiveness were rational persuasion, ingratiation, personal appeals, and exchange tactics. "Hard" tactics were generally less effective than "soft" tactics. The outcomes of tactic combinations depended mostly on the potency of the component tactics.
Article
This study investigates how applicant characteristics influence the use of impression management (IM) tactics in interviews, and how these behaviors affect interviewer perceptions of person–job fit (P–J fit) and applicant–interviewer similarity. Results from 72 applicants demonstrated that extraverted applicants made greater use of self-promotion during their interviews, while agreeableness was associated with non-verbal cues. Self-promotion was the IM tactic most strongly related to interviewers’ perceptions of P–J fit, whereas non-verbal IM influenced perceived similarity. The practical implications of these findings for applicant preparation are discussed, as well as concerns regarding the long-term effects of IM use on selection decision making.
Article
In an earlier experiment, college recruiters evaluated a video tape of an interviewee who was either male or female and who displayed either a moderately aggressive or passive self-presentation. In the present paper, two studies are presented which replicated and extended the findings of the original experiment (R. Dipboye and J. Wiley, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 1977, 10, 1–12.). As in the original study, moderately aggressive candidates were evaluated more favorably than passive candidates for a supervisory position and no differences were found in the ratings of male and female candidates. In addition, a short job description was found to result in less favorable ratings than a long job description and passive candidates tended to be rated more favorably than a moderately aggressive candidate for an editorial assistant position.
Article
Applicants in employment interviews use a variety of impression management techniques such as emphasizing positive traits, conforming to the opinions of the interviewer, and claiming responsibility for positive events. A field experiment was conducted on 62 employment interviewers who viewed videotaped interview segments in which either high or low levels of impression management techniques were depicted. Applicant credentials were also manipulated. Results indicated that interviewers were influenced by impression management techniques regardless of applicant credentials. The implications of these results are discussed with respect to theory, research and practice of the employment interview.