Content uploaded by Diana M. Hechavarria
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Diana M. Hechavarria on Apr 01, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
The nascent entrepreneurship hub: goals, entrepreneurial
self-efficacy and start-up outcomes
Diana M. Hechavarria •Maija Renko •
Charles H. Matthews
Accepted: 1 December 2010 / Published online: 14 August 2011
ÓSpringer Science+Business Media, LLC. 2011
Abstract Entrepreneurship involves human agency.
The entrepreneurial process occurs because people are
motivated to pursue and exploit perceived opportuni-
ties. It is rooted in the theory that action is the result of
motivation and cognition. Therefore, this paper applies
elements of goal theory and social cognitive theory to
develop a motivational model of nascent entrepreneur-
ial start-up outcomes. The objective of this model is to
renew attention on motivational constructs in entre-
preneurship research. Additionally, it provides predic-
tive value for the likelihood of new firm founding
among nascent entrepreneurs. Results suggest that
motivational antecedents among nascent entrepreneurs
significantly influence the likelihood of quitting the
start-up process versus continuing nascent entrepre-
neurial start-up efforts.
Keywords Nascent entrepreneurship
Entrepreneurial self-efficacy Business planning
Goal setting
JEL Classifications L25 L26 M13
1 Introduction
The importance of human motivation in entrepre-
neurship is gathering increasing interest. However,
relatively little of this motivation research in entrepre-
neurship has considered the effects of motivation on
specific steps in the entrepreneurial process (Shane et al.
2003). This is a limitation in existing research since
starting up a new venture is a dynamic process, and is
likely to involve a variety of motivational factors at
various stages of the process. For example, an engineer
might be highly motivated to invent a new medical
device with a potential commercial application, but may
lack the motivation to assemble the required financial
resources for a new venture. In this paper, we employ a
process approach to study motivation and new firm
formation among nascent entrepreneurs. A nascent
entrepreneur is a person who initiates actions that are
intended to culminate in a viable new firm (Reynolds
1994). Gartner (1988) has proposed that entrepreneur-
ship ends when organizational creation is over. Conse-
quently, we limit our focus to individuals in the
earliest stages of the start-up process to identify those
motivational factors that may differentiate nascent
D. M. Hechavarria (&)
Department of Management, University of Cincinnati,
Lindner Hall 516, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0020, USA
e-mail: hechavda@mail.uc.edu
M. Renko
Department of Managerial Studies, University of Illinois
at Chicago, 2211 University Hall, 601 S Morgan Street,
Chicago, IL 60607, USA
e-mail: maija@uic.edu
C. H. Matthews
Center for Entrepreneurship Education & Research,
University of Cincinnati, 510 Carl H. Lindner Hall,
Cincinnati, OH 45221-0165, USA
e-mail: charles.matthews@uc.edu
123
Small Bus Econ (2012) 39:685–701
DOI 10.1007/s11187-011-9355-2
entrepreneurs who actually start new operative firms
from those who quit, and from those ‘‘hobbyists’’ who
continue in their start-up efforts for extended periods of
time. Our study is a response to a call for research that
would incorporate motivations into a dynamic, evolu-
tionary perspective on entrepreneurship by using moti-
vations to distinguish those individuals who continue to
pursue opportunities fromthose who abandon the effort
(Shane et al. 2003).
An examination of recent individual level entre-
preneurship research reveals a range of theoretical
and empirical approaches to explain new firm births,
often from a process approach (Gelderen et al. 2006;
Dimov 2007; Gruber 2007; Teece 2007; Brush et al.
2008; Harper 2008). This overall process approach,
however, does not discount the additional importance
of individual level entrepreneurial factors. There have
been a number of individual level factors that have
been studied in the entrepreneurship field. Some of
the more common include the need for achievement
(McClelland 1965; Hansemark 2003), risk-taking
propensity (Brockhaus 1980; Brockhaus and Horwitz
1986; Corman et al. 1988), and internal locus of
control (Rotter 1966; Borland 1974; Hansemark
1998; Kaufman et al. 1995). However, there are
limited discussions of entrepreneurial motivation in
the process literature.
When motivation has been studied, it has often
been for the purpose of discussing why people enter
the start-up process from the labor force. Indeed,
Gatewood et al. (2002) provide evidence that moti-
vations prove most powerful in understanding the
determinants of organizational creation. Herron and
Sapienza (1992, p. 49) state, ‘‘motivation plays an
important part in the creation of new organizations,
theories of organization creation that fail to address
this notion are incomplete.’’ The concept of motiva-
tion is used to explain the direction, effort, and
persistence of action (Kanfer 1990). Focusing on the
direction of action, this study proposes that entrepre-
neurs are motivated to accomplish the goals that they
set for themselves (Naffziger et al. 1994). A goal is
what an individual is trying to accomplish; it is the
object or aim of an action (Locke et al. 1981). In the
case of nascent entrepreneurs, goals may vary from
cashing out quickly to pursuing one’s intrinsic goals.
The common denominator for these and other goals
of nascent entrepreneurs is that they all involve
establishing a new firm as a first step.
The model developed in this paper suggests that
entrepreneurial start-up outcomes are, in part, driven
by an individual’s motivation. Theoretically, we base
our proposed model on concepts borrowed from goal
setting (Locke and Latham 1990) and social cognitive
theory (Bandura 2001). Goal setting theory is not
limited to but focuses primarily on motivation in
work settings, and the focus of goal setting theory is
on the fundamental properties of an effective goal.
Social cognitive theory and the research that under-
lies it are primarily focused on self-efficacy. Both
theories agree about what is considered important in
performance motivation (Locke and Latham 2002).
As a result, we apply goal setting and social cognitive
theory as motivational mechanisms (although cogni-
tive elements are necessarily involved) to help
understand new firm emergence. We posit that the
coalescence of self-efficacy and goal specificity
provide a robust individual level motivational model
for understanding the outcomes of the start-up
process. Such a model essentially answers the
research question of ‘‘Why do individuals start new
businesses?’’, and consequently has implications for
entrepreneurship policy and education, in addition to
academic research.
The following two sections of this paper will
review the key theories on which our arguments are
based. Subsequently, we will explicate our operal-
ization of key constructs and overview our findings in
the ‘‘Analysis and results’’ section. Finally, we will
discuss the implications of out findings and summa-
rize our conclusions.
2 A starting point: the motivation hub
Our work draws from Locke’s (1991)motivation
sequence which attempts to understand human, and
especially work, motivation. In his motivation
sequence model, Locke first profiles needs, which
directly feed into the motivation core (values and
motives). Secondly, the motivation core directly
impacts the motivation hub, which then leads to
rewards and, finally, satisfaction.
The motivation hub is the core of action (Locke
1991). The motivation hub includes linkages between
goals, self-efficacy and performance. As such, the
motivation hub is the central component of the model
(Locke 1991), and self-efficacy is depicted as having
686 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
direct relationships to goals and performance.
According to Locke’s (1991) model, what people
do is powerfully (though not solely) influenced by
their goals or intents and by their perceived confi-
dence in being able to take the actions in question.
Subsequent empirical findings in goal setting and task
motivation research have also found that self-efficacy
operates as a moderator between goals and perfor-
mance (Bass 1985; Bandura 1997; White and Locke
2000; Locke and Latham 2002).
What Locke (1991) called the motivation hub,
meaning where the action is, consists of personal
goals and self-efficacy. These variables are often,
though not invariably, the most immediate, con-
scious motivational determinants of action (Locke
and Latham 2002). Building and expanding on the
motivation hub, the nascent entrepreneurship hub is
aimed at understanding how goals, self-efficacy, and
start-up outcomes (i.e. ‘performance’ in Locke’s
model) are linked in the context of new firm
creation.
2.1 The nascent entrepreneurship hub: goal
setting and social cognitive theory
as motivational mechanisms
A nascent entrepreneur is defined as someone who
initiates activities that are intended to culminate in a
viable new firm (Reynolds 1994). Operationally,
nascent entrepreneurs (1) consider themselves as
starting a business, (2) have engaged in start-up
activities within the past year, (3) expect to own all or
part of the new business, and (4) have not experienced
more than three months positive cash flow (Reynolds
2007). A benefit of utilizing nascent entrepreneurs as
units of observation in entrepreneurial motivation
research is the explication of the cognitive sequence
for individuals who enter the start-up process. It is
advantageous because it provides an opportunity for
scholars to identify cognitive differences among
individuals who are subsequently (1) successful in
implementing a new firm, (2) unsuccessful in their
efforts, but keep on trying, and/or (3) unsuccessful and
quit (Reynolds 2007). Few studies to date have
investigated the role of entrepreneurial motivation
and its subsequent translation into action aimed at
organizational creation in general, and focused on
nascent entrepreneurial activity in particular. In this
manuscript, we introduce the elements of the ‘‘nascent
entrepreneurship hub,’’ based on Locke’s motivation
hub, and formulate hypotheses for empirical testing.
2.2 Goal specificity
Following Locke and Latham’s (1990) goal theory,
we know that goals direct attention and action to
goal-related activities. Second, goals have an ener-
gizing function. Harder goals lead to greater effort
than easier goals (Locke et al. 1981; Locke and
Latham 2002). Third, goals affect persistence. When
participants are allowed to control the time they
spend on a task, hard goals prolong effort (LaPorte
and Nath 1976). Fourth, goals affect action indirectly
by leading to the arousal, discovery, and/or use of
task-relevant knowledge and strategies (Wood et al.
1987). However, despite this obvious relevance for
entrepreneurship research, there is little extant
research that applies goal theory’s predictions to
entrepreneurial situations, with the exception of
Shane and Delmar’s (2004) study on business
planning.
In the context of nascent entrepreneurship, goal
setting theory suggests that undertaking business
planning before acting will enhance the start-up
performance of new ventures (Shane and Delmar
2004). Plans are particularly useful when tasks are
fuzzy or uncertain, and the decision maker cannot
rely on previous experience (Campbell 1988), which
is often the case in new business start-ups. Goal
setting theory also suggests that written planning
improves human action. Writing a plan clarifies goals
and permits people to set more specific objectives,
which facilitate the achievement of those goals
(Locke and Latham 1990; Shane and Delmar 2004).
In a retrospective empirical study of a limited sample
of entrepreneurs, van Gelder et al. (2007) found that
entrepreneurs running surviving businesses had set
more specific goals than entrepreneurs, whose busi-
nesses had ceased to exist.
In general, we posit that entrepreneurs have a set
of goals they seek to accomplish when they decide to
enter nascent entrepreneurship (e.g., Naffziger et al.
1994). In most goal setting studies, the term goal
generally refers to attaining a specific standard of
proficiency on a task, usually within a specified time
limit (Locke et al. 1981). These goals may vary for
each nascent entrepreneur: some may seek to rapidly
grow a firm and cash out while others may seek to
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 687
123
grow and build their venture over time. Despite
variability in goals, goal setting theory states that
specific and difficult goals lead to higher performance
than vague or easy goals (Locke and Latham 2002).
For example, Wiese et al. (2002) found that individ-
uals who report setting difficult work goals showed
stronger progress towards their goals than individuals
who perceived their goals as less difficult. Indeed,
Shane and Delmar (2004) find that this prediction of
goal setting theory holds with respect to the value of
business planning. Completing business plans before
undertaking marketing activities (i.e., specifying and
formalizing one’s goals) reduces the hazard of
termination of new ventures (Shane and Delmar
2004). While not specifically focused on goal setting
theory, other research suggests that business plan
formalization
1
is an antecedent to venture organizing
activity (Delmar and Shane 2003; Honig and Karls-
son 2004; Liao and Gartner 2006).
In sum, goals seem to regulate performance most
predictably when they are expressed in specific
quantitative terms (or as specific intentions to take a
certain action, such as quitting a job) rather than as
vague intentions to ‘‘try hard’’ or as subjective
estimates of task or goal difficulty. Given the findings
of prior research, as well as extant classroom and
community entrepreneurship pedagogy, which often
has an emphasis on planning, we hypothesize that
more specific goals (i.e., more formal plans) will
benefit nascent entrepreneurs who aim to establish a
new firm. Since entrepreneurs self-select their goals,
a business plan would serve as a proxy to measure
how specifically they have formalized that self-
selected goal. Therefore, we hypothesize that:
H1 There is a positive relationship between goal
specificity and the new firm outcome status. Specif-
ically, nascent entrepreneurs with more formalized
and specific quantified goals are more likely to start
new ventures
In Locke’s (1991) motivation sequence model, as
well as in the findings of Shane and Delmar (2004),
goals are directly related to performance (see also
Baum and Locke 2004). However, especially in the
context of nascent entrepreneurship, goals by them-
selves may not be sufficient to lead to start-up
performance outcomes unless the goals are actually
being pursued by individuals who at a minimum feel
that they are capable of starting such a venture.
Individuals must have the ability to attain or at least
approach their goals. Therefore, self-efficacy
undoubtedly plays a critical role in directing behavior
aimed at goal attainment.
2.3 Self-efficacy
Self-efficacy refers to the extent to which a person
believes that he/she can organize and effectively
execute actions to produce given attainments (Ban-
dura 2001). It is one of the single best predictors of an
individual’s performance in general (Locke and
Latham 2002). It is considered a state-like character-
istic that generally increases with experience and is
highly related to actual ability (Phillips and Gully
1997). This mechanism of personal agency is most
central to social cognitive theory, a theory of self-
regulation (Bandura 1989,2001).
Self-efficacy beliefs influence an individual’s level
of motivation, as reflected in how much effort he/she
will exert in an endeavor, and how long he/she will
persevere in the face of obstacles (Bandura 1994).
For instance, Cervone and Peake (1986) found that
the higher was the instated perceived self-efficacy,
the longer individuals persevered on difficult and
unsolvable problems before they quit. Therefore,
individuals with a strong sense of self-efficacy will
put forth a high degree of effort in order to meet their
commitments, and attribute failure to things which
are in their control, rather than blaming external
factors (Bandura 1994; Zacharakis 1999). Self-effi-
cacious individuals also recover quickly from set-
backs, and ultimately are likely to achieve their
personal goals (Bandura 1997). Conversely, individ-
uals with low self-efficacy believe they cannot be
successful, and thus are less likely to make a
concerted, extended effort and may consider chal-
lenging tasks as threats that are to be avoided
(Margolis and McCabe 2006).
Self-efficacy has been linked theoretically and
empirically with managerial and entrepreneurial phe-
nomena (Krueger et al. 2000; Markman and Baronb
2003). In organizational research, separate meta-
analyses by both Stajkovic and Luthans (1998) and
Judge and Bono (2001) have demonstrated a robust
positive relationship between employee self-efficacy
1
Business plan formalization is the degree of specificity for
the business plan.
688 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
and performance. Self-efficacy, and particularly entre-
preneurial self-efficacy (McGee et al. 2009), appears to
be an important antecedent to new venture intentions
and creation (Chen et al. 1988; Boyd and Vozikis 1994;
Zhao et al. 2005; Barbosa et al. 2007; Markman et al.
2005; Wilson et al. 2007; Townsend et al. 2010).
Entrepreneurial self-efficacy is a context-specific
measure of self-efficacy. This research focuses on the
belief of individuals in their ability to perform entre-
preneurship-related tasks. For example, Chen et al.
(1988) created a measure of entrepreneurial self-
efficacy comprised of dimensions related to marketing,
innovation, management, risk-taking, and financial
control. Using this measure, Chen et al. (1988) found
entrepreneurial self-efficacy to significantly differen-
tiate entrepreneurs from non-entrepreneurs. In addition
to its effect on entrepreneurial intent and immediate
venture creation, entrepreneurial self-efficacy of the
founder has even been found to influence performance
outcomes on the firm level (e.g., Baum and Locke
2004; Hmieleski and Baron 2008).
Based on prior self-efficacy research, we believe
that nascent entrepreneurs who have a strong belief in
their capabilities—that is, high entrepreneurial self-
efficacy—exert greater effort when they face difficult
or ambiguous challenges in the start-up process. Such
strong perseverance contributes to the outcomes of
the process. Therefore, we hypothesize that:
H2 There is a positive relationship between entre-
preneurial self-efficacy and the new firm outcome
status. Specifically, nascent entrepreneurs with higher
degrees of entrepreneurial self-efficacy are more
likely to start new ventures.
Both goal setting and social cognitive theory
literature suggests that the relationship between
self-efficacy and goal attainment varies as a function
of goal specificity. This would suggest that the
nascent entrepreneurial motivation hub is incomplete
without more fully considering the moderating effect
that self-efficacy has on goal-specificity to start-up
outcome status.
2.4 Goal specificity and entrepreneurial
self-efficacy
Behavior is powerfully influenced by both goals and
by the perceived confidence in being able to take
action (i.e., self-efficacy) (Locke 1991; Latham and
Pinder 2005a,b). Previous research from the goal
setting perspective has found that specific, challeng-
ing (difficult) goals led to higher output than vague
goals such as ‘‘do your best’’ (Locke 1968). As
described earlier, self-efficacy has also been found to
be an antecedent of a variety of positive task
outcomes. Goal setting and social cognitive theories
in general, and self-efficacy in particular, are consid-
ered to be the most direct and immediate motivational
determinants of work performance (Lathan and
Pinder 2005a,b), and thus particularly relevant to
understanding why nascent entrepreneurs engage in
the firm creation process.
Building on previous research that has investigated
the role of self-efficacy as an intervening variable in
entrepreneurial models (Zhao et al. 2005; Hmieleski
and Corbett 2008) and between goals and perfor-
mance (Locke and Latham 2002), we believe that
perceived self-efficacy will also moderate the rela-
tionship between specific goals and positive start-up
process outcomes. Those who have a strong belief in
their capabilities exert greater effort, and when
directed by specific goals, such efforts will result in
desired outcomes. However, individuals who have
doubts about their entrepreneurial skills and knowl-
edge—i.e. their self-efficacy concerning entrepre-
neurial tasks is low—are not likely to fully benefit
from the specific goals that they still may be able to
set for their start-up endeavors. As Bandura (2001)
implies, making a decision is not the same as
implementing the decision, and entrepreneurial self-
efficacy is needed to carry out the implementation
stages of the nascent venture. This suggests that
critical insights can be gained from the interaction of
goals and self-efficacy on start-up outcomes. There-
fore, we hypothesize that:
H3 Entrepreneurial self-efficacy moderates the goal
specificity to new firm outcome status relationship,
such that a higher degree of entrepreneurial self-
efficacy will strengthen the positive relationship
proposed in Hypothesis 1.
3 Methods
Data for the empirical analysis come from the Panel
Study of Entrepreneurial Dynamics I (PSEDI). The
PSEDI is a longitudinal study that involves more than
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 689
123
100 entrepreneurship scholars who came together as
part of the Entrepreneurial Research Consortium
(ERC). Initially, random digit dialing calls was made
to 31,261 individuals in 1998–1999. The study
methodology allowed researchers to identify nascent
entrepreneurs—those individuals in the process of
starting up a new venture—from this pool of
individuals and to longitudinally follow their pro-
gression through data collection periods over time.
The breadth and quality of the PSED data provides a
unique opportunity to avoid the survival bias typical
for studies of young firms. Since the PSED focuses on
nascent entrepreneurs (individuals actively involved
in the start-up process who have yet to experience
three months of positive operating cash flow) and is a
longitudinal study, it also avoids the recollection bias,
typical for cross-sectional surveys. The PSED dataset
and related codebooks are publicly available on the
consortium’s website.
2
A total of 830 nascent entre-
preneurs were identified for this longitudinal study.
These nascent entrepreneurs were then followed up at
about one-year intervals to enquire about the current
status of their start-up efforts. Three such follow-up
waves were completed. Additional detailed descrip-
tions of the methods and sampling used to generate
the PSED can be found in Reynolds and Curtin
(2004).
3.1 Dependent variable
3.1.1 Outcome status
For this assessment, start-up outcome status has three
possibilities for nascent start-ups. A nascent entre-
preneur can quit the start-up process, reach new firm
status, or continue in the start-up process. For our
dependent variable (i.e., new firm, start-up continues,
quit initiative), we utilized a time-lagged measure
from the PSED sample which asks respondents to
categorize the status of their potential venture at each
follow-up wave. These results were cross-checked
with a related question where the respondents
reported the actual year and month the venture began
operation as well as respondent statements on the
cash flow figures for the nascent venture. Conse-
quently, a nascent start-up is classified as a new firm
if the respondent answers that the firm is up and
running, and, furthermore, the venture has experi-
enced at least 3 months of positive cash flow within
the 72-month investigation period. If the nascent
start-up has not experienced at least three months
consecutive positive cash flow, it remains in the
continued start-up category. Finally, all respondents
who self-identified as disengaging from the process
were categorized as ‘‘quits’’.
3.2 Independent variables
3.2.1 Business plan formalization
In order to measure goal specificity, we use PSED
items q114-r/s/t571 to compute the final form of
business planning as an ordinal variable with four
levels (none, unwritten in head, informal, and formal).
Business planning has engendered a lengthy discus-
sion by various scholars regarding its efficacy to
facilitate goal attainment largely around business
planning helping firm founders to undertake guided
venture development activities (Cyert and March
1963; Simon 1964; Locke 1968; Latham and Yukl
1975; Bird 1988; Smith et al. 1990; Timmons 2000;
Baum et al. 2001; Shane and Delmar 2004; Baum and
Locke 2004). Therefore, guided by Shane and Delmar
(2004), we measure goal specificity of nascent
entrepreneurs by looking at their business planning
activities. A realized business plan is defined as
having (formally or informally) identified the current
state and the presupposed future of the fledgling
organization (Honig and Karlsson 2004). Writing a
plan typically clarifies goals while allowing entrepre-
neurs to set more specific goals (Locke and Latham
1990; Shane and Delmar 2004). In the PSED protocol,
the level of business plan formalization is assessed
through two questions. First, the nascent entrepre-
neurs were asked: ‘‘A business plan usually outlines
the markets to be served, the products or services to be
provided, the resources required—including money—
and the expected growth and profit for the new
business. Has a business plan been prepared for this
start-up?’’. If the answer to this question was ‘‘No’’,
the value for business plan formalization is zero. For
those who answered ‘‘Yes’’ to the question above, the
level of formalization was then subsequently queried.
Individuals with unwritten, ‘‘in head’’ plans were
coded 1, individuals with informally written plans
2
http://www.psed.isr.umich.edu/main.php.
690 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
were coded 2, and individuals with formally prepared
written plans were coded 3.
3.2.2 Entrepreneurial self-efficacy
Bandura (1977) has argued that self-efficacy should
be focused on a specific context and activity domain.
The more task-specific one can make the measure-
ment of self-efficacy, the better the predictive role
efficacy is likely to play in research on the task-
specific outcomes of interest (Bandura 1977; McGee
et al. 2009). To measure degree of entrepreneurial
self-efficacy, first interview (Wave Q) responses were
utilized in order to overcome post hoc rationalization
among respondents. A direct approach is taken in
measuring entrepreneurial self-efficacy, based on
responses to Likert scale items from the mail
questionnaire. Cassar and Friedman (2009), validated
the following items as a measure of entrepreneurial
self-efficacy: Qk1a, Qk1d, QK1e and QK1f (see
Table 1for variable descriptions).
3
Exploratory fac-
tor analysis confirmed the unidimensional factor
structure. Some examples of items included in the
entrepreneurial self-efficacy scale are: ‘‘If I work
hard, I can successfully start a business,’’ and ‘‘I am
confident, I can put in the effort needed to start a
business,’’ where 1 indicated completely disagreed
and 5 indicated completely agreed. The items
selected were then averaged to create the entrepre-
neurial self-efficacy scale (Cronbach a=.80).
3.3 Control variables
In the analysis, several control variables were iden-
tified for inclusion. These include educational attain-
ment, income and wealth measures, sex of the lead
respondent, total number of individuals on the team,
degree of firm innovativeness, prior industry experi-
ence, prior start-up experience and conception lag (in
months) for the start-up. Educational attainment is an
ordinal variable (grade school, no high school degree,
high school degree, post-high school no college
degree, college degree, post-college experience). This
measure was used since education is often a factor
reported to influence business planning (Autio et al.
1997; Krueger 1993; Honig and Karlsson 2004).
Income and wealth measures were self-reports of
respondent household income and assets (adjusted for
inflation in 2009). This measure was included because
availability of resources may positively influence new
firm emergence. Sex was self-report from the respon-
dent. Sex was included as a measure because men are
more likely than women to engage in entrepreneurial
activity (Robb and Coleman 2009). Team size was
also controlled for since more team members may
positively influence the availability of human, social,
fiscal and cultural resources. Degree of innovativeness
was controlled for, since degree of innovation may
impact the difficulty of new firm emergence as
routines and competencies are new and often foreign
to the market (Aldrich and Ruef 2006). Prior start-up
and industry experience were included because such
measures have been found to positively influence
operational firm status (Reynolds 2007). Finally,
conception lag was included to control for time, since
individuals who have been in the start-up process
longer may more likely reach some form of resolution
(Reynolds 2007; Reynolds and Curtin 2004).
4 Analysis and results
We apply univariate and bivariate techniques to
describe the sample. Moreover, multivariate statistics
such as multinomial logistic regression will be
utilized to test the hypotheses.
After executing listwise deletion of cases with
missing data, the sample size for this analysis totals
342 nascent entrepreneurs (n=342). Data for this
analysis are re-weighted to represent all nascent
entrepreneurs on which information on our study
variables are available at the end 72-month interview
window. Descriptive statistics for the sample can be
found in Table 2. The data show that, within the
sample, a third of all cases reach new firm status, a
third continue in their efforts, and a thirds quit after
the 72-month follow-up period. Men represent about
51% of the cases and women 49%. The mean for
education is post-high school or some college (about
32% of the cases) followed by college degree (25%
of the cases). The mean household income for the
3
Note that item Qk1a was originally developed as measure of
expectancy (Gatewood 2004) and is used as such by Renko
et al. in this journal issue. Since the theoretical constructs of
expectancy and self-efficacy have similarities (Bandura 1977;
Steel and Konig 2006), item Qk1a has also been used in a
validated scale measuring entrepreneurial self-efficacy by
Cassar and Friedman (2009).
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 691
123
sample is US$70,000, with a mean household net
worth of $177,000. On average, nascent entrepre-
neurs have about 8 years of industry experience and
have participated in about 1.2 other start-up teams.
However, it should be noted that the median for
previous start-up team experience is 0. On average,
most nascent start-ups score low on hi-tech emphasis.
Furthermore, the average team size for the sample is
two, and the time from conception, or first initial
start-up action taken, to the first wave of the PSED
interviews is 25.57 months, or a little over two years.
Furthermore, Table 2presents the bivariate corre-
lations among the variables, assuming compound
symmetry. Initial analysis of Table 2shows that
multicollinearity is not likely to affect our results.
Moreover, Table 2highlights the various relationships
between the control and independent variables. Par-
ticularly, business plan formalization shows a positive,
significant correlation with education (r=.118;
p=.005), household income (r=.149; p=.001),
household net worth (r=.151; p=.001), start-up
experience (r=.140; p=.001), business hi-tech
index (r=.140; p=.001), and team size (r=.180;
p=.004). However, entrepreneurial self-efficacy is
only significantly correlated to the control variable
conception lag (r=-.149; p=.001). Therefore, as
conception lag increases, self-efficacy decreases.
Moreover, business plan formalization and self-
efficacy are significantly correlated (r=.124; p=
.004). Therefore, as business plan formality increases,
self-efficacy increases (see Table 2).
In order to investigate the effects of business plan
formalization and entrepreneurial self-efficacy on new
firm emergence, multinomial logistic regression was
utilized. Multinomial logistic regression breaks the
regression up into a series of binary regressions
comparing each group to a baseline, or referent group.
The referent category for this analysis is continued
start-up status. In order to identify the most parsimo-
nious model, the independent variables were force
entered, and forward stepwise selection was simulta-
neously employed to identify the most significant
control variables. The forward stepwise selection
criteria included control variables with a likelihood
ratio pprobability of pB.05 and removed the variable
if the likelihood ratio pprobability pC.10. This
procedure selected conception lag (in months) and sex
of the respondent as the only significant controls
differentiating continuing start-up efforts from new
firms or quits. All other control variables were not
significant (and therefore excluded from the model).
To assess the goodness of fit for the model, the
deviance statistic was computed. Thus, we can
conclude that the model fits the data relatively well
(v
2
=705.53, df =772, p=.180) (Agresti 1996;
Tabachnick and Fidell 1996). Also, pseudo R square
Table 1 PSED I variables operalizations
Variable Description PSED Item Measurement
level
Educational attainment Educational attainment level Q343 Ordinal
Team size Total number of people on team TEAMSZ Ordinal
Industry experience Total years of same industry experience Q199 Continuous
Start-up experience Total number of other start-up initiatives engaged in Q200 Continuous
Household income Total household income from all sources Q386 Continuous
Household net worth Estimated current net worth of the household Q391 Continuous
Conception lag Months lag from conception to first interview in
months. Computed from difference in first start-up
activity reported to first wave interview.
Start-up characteristics
section, PHDAY,
PHMTH, PHYR,
Continuous
Degree of firm innovativeness Degree of innovativeness of the start-up Q299-Q301 Ordinal
Business plan formalization Degree of formality of business plan Q114-R/S/T571 Ordinal
Entrepreneurial self-efficacy Degree of belief in skills and abilities for
entrepreneurial tasks
Qk1a, Qk1d, Qk1e, Qk1f Ordinal
Outcome status 72-month outcome status based on respondent self-
reports and month and year revenue first exceeded
the expenses
R/S/T502, R/S/T622_my Nominal
Sex Sex of respondent NCGENDER Nominal
692 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations
Variable Mean SD Mode 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Educational attainment 4.210 1.100 4.000 1.000
2. Team size 1.740 0.970 1.000 .079* 1.000
3. Industry experience 8.720 9.980 0.000 .105** -0.051 1.000
4. No. of start-up experiences 1.190 3.240 0.000 .144** .107** .136** 1.000
5. Household income (1,000) 70.120 93.410 59.000 .125** .129** 0.054 0.040 1.000
6. Household net worth
(1,000)
176.920 391.480 234.000 .200** .155** .112** .192** .476** 1.000
7. Conception lag in months 25.570 35.880 10.280 -.077* -0.025 .257** -0.010 -0.030 -0.016 1.000
8. Degree of firm
innovativeness
0.970 0.900 1.000 0.047 .109** 0.029 0.056 0.012 0.013 -0.012 1.000
9. Business plan
formalization
1.410 1.150 0.000 .118** .180** 0.039 .140** .149** .151** -0.014 .140** 1.000
10. Entrepreneurial self-
efficacy
4.230 0.590 4.000 0.024 -0.001 0.074 0.009 -0.007 0.019 -.149** -0.010 .124** 1.000
11. Outcome status 2.040 0.810 3.000
12. Sex 0.510 0.500 1.000
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 693
123
statistics show that about 12% to 6% of the variation
in start-up outcome status is explained by the model
in this analysis. Finally, the overall model fitting
criteria (2LL for the overall model: v
2
=45.02,
df =10, p\.0005) is significant, and therefore we
reject the null hypothesis that business plan formal-
ization, self-efficacy and their interaction effect, as
well as control variables of sex and conception lag (in
months) make no difference in odds for continuing
start-up efforts versus new firms or quitting (see
Table 3).
Examination of Table 3in regards to continued
start-up status versus new firm status shows that none
of the independent variables or control variables
influence the odds of new firm emergence versus
continuing start-up efforts. Hence, we find no support
for our H1–H3. However, comparing nascent entre-
preneurs who continue start-up efforts to those that
quit, significant patterns emerge, different to what we
originally hypothesized.
4
In this model, both concep-
tion lag (p=.0005) and sex (p=.012) are signif-
icant control variables. A one unit increase in
conception lag (months) decreases the odds of
quitting by 2% versus continuing start-up efforts.
Similarly, men are more likely to quit than women
compared to continuing start-up efforts. Moreover,
business plan formalization is significant (p=.062),
such that a one unit increase in business plan
formalization decreases the odds of quitting by
81%, after controlling for other variables in the
model. Similarly, entrepreneurial self-efficacy is also
significant (p=.01), such that a one unit increase in
entrepreneurial self-efficacy among nascents
decreases the odds of being in the quit category by
66%, controlling for other variables in the model.
Finally, the interaction term for business plan
formalization and self-efficacy is significant
(p=.052), such that a one unit increase in the
interaction coefficient increases by 50% the chances
of being in the quit category. Although our hypoth-
eses were not supported, we did find evidence that
coincides with goal theory and social cognitive
theory. The subsequent discussion further explores
the nature of our findings, particularly the interaction
effect of goal specificity and entrepreneurial self-
efficacy found between nascent entrepreneurs who
quit compared to those who continue on the start-up.
4.1 Post hoc analysis
In order to further investigate the nature of the
relationship between the dependent and independent
variables, we will employ cross-classification data
Table 3 Multinomial logistic regression results for the entrepreneurship hub model
Outcome status B SE Wald df p Exp (B) 95% CI of Exp (B)
Lower
bound
Upper
bound
New firm
Intercept -1.733 1.909 .824 1 .364
Entrepreneurial self-efficacy .357 .438 .663 1 .415 1.429 .605 3.374
Business plan formalization .068 .927 .005 1 .941 1.071 .174 6.593
Business plan 9entrepreneurial self-efficacy -.002 .214 .000 1 .993 .998 .656 1.519
Conception lag (months) -.003 .006 .250 1 .617 .997 .985 1.009
Sex .204 .273 .558 1 .455 1.226 .718 2.095
Quit
Intercept 4.603 1.738 7.017 1 .008
Entrepreneurial self-efficacy -1.066 .415 6.584 1 .010 .344 .153 .777
Business plan formalization -1.668 .893 3.485 1 .062 .189 .033 1.087
Business plan 9entrepreneurial self-efficacy .411 .211 3.771 1 .052 1.508 .996 2.282
Conception lag (months) -.038 .009 16.670 1 .000 .962 .945 .980
Sex .730 .289 6.355 1 .012 2.074 1.176 3.658
a
The reference category is start-up continues
4
We set our level of significance at a=.10.
694 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
techniques. As a result, the independent variables
were categorized into dichotomous variables. Busi-
ness plan formalization was recoded to no planning
and yes planning (where unwritten, informal, and
formal were all grouped). And entrepreneurial self-
efficacy was recoded into low and high, where all
cases over the median (MD =4.33) were coded as
high and cases under the median as low. Table 4
shows the frequencies for each classification of self-
efficacy by planning among the three outcome
categories (new firm, quits, and start-up continues).
Initial analysis shows that business planning and
entrepreneurial self-efficacy are significantly posi-
tively correlated (see Table 2). Subsequent chi-
square analysis also shows that business planning
and entrepreneurial self-efficacy are dependent con-
structs (v
2
=11.55, df =1, p=001). Therefore, the
column and row frequencies for entrepreneurial self-
efficacy and business planning are not random.
Therefore, in order to more closely examine
differences in entrepreneurial self-efficacy and busi-
ness planning among nascent outcome categories,
odds ratios were calculated. This procedure will aid
in identifying the nature of the relationship between
business planning and entrepreneurial self-efficacy.
First, we examine the odds ratio, which is the ratio of
two conditional odds. If we look at Table 4, the
question can be raised whether the odds ratio is
homogenous across categories of nascent outcome
start-up status. To test this, we perform the Mantel–
Haenszel test. The Mantel–Haenszel chi-square coef-
ficient tests whether the common odds ratio across the
various start-up outcome categories is 1.0, indicating
no effect of the stratification variable (Agresti 1996).
This test of conditional independence shows that the
odds ratio does vary by category according to the
Mantel–Haenszel test (v
2
=6.70, df =1, p=.01).
Additionally, we see that in each outcome cate-
gory, the odds ratio is greater than 1 (see Table 4).
An odds ratio of 1.0 indicates that there is no
association for the two variables. Moreover, the
further the odds ratio is away from 1.0, the more
different are the conditional odds. Among those who
reach new firm status, the conditional odds of having
a plan are 1.52 as high among respondents with high
entrepreneurial self-efficacy than low entrepreneurial
self-efficacy (see Table 4). Moreover, for those who
continue in their start-up efforts, the conditional odds
of having a plan is 1.29 times as high among those
with high self-efficacy as among those with low
Table 4 Relationship between business plan formalization and entrepreneurial self-efficacy by outcome status
New firm Quit Start-up continues
Low self-
efficacy
High self-
efficacy
Total Low self-
efficacy
High self-
efficacy
Total Low self-
efficacy
High self-
efficacy
Total
No plan 16 21 37 37 15 52 27 26 53
43.20% 56.80% 100% 71.20% 28.80% 100% 50.90% 49.10% 100%
Yes plan 38 76 114 50 55 157 50 62 112
33.30% 66.70% 100% 47.60% 52.40% 100% 44.60% 55.40% 100%
Total 54 97 151 87 70 157 77 88 165
35.80% 64.20% 100% 55.40% 44.60% 100% 46.70% 53.30% 100%
Odds Odds Odds
High self-efficacy and no plan/
low self-efficacy and no plan
High self-efficacy and no plan/
low self-efficacy and no plan
High self-efficacy and no plan/
low self-efficacy and no plan
1.31 0.41 0.96
High self-efficacy and yes plan/
low self-efficacy and yes plan
High self-efficacy and yes plan/
low self-efficacy and yes plan
High self-efficacy and yes plan/
low self-efficacy and yes plan
2.00 1.10 1.24
Odds ratio Odds ratio Odds ratio
1.52 2.71 1.29
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 695
123
entrepreneurial self-efficacy. Finally, we can con-
clude that there is a strong positive association
between entrepreneurial self-efficacy and business
planning, particularly among the quit group, whose
odds ratio =2.71. So among nascents who plan and
quit, the odds of high entrepreneurial self-efficacy are
about 2.71 times greater than those who do not plan.
This implies that, in the quit group, highly efficacious
nascents plan more readily. Therefore, planning and
high entrepreneurial may lead to quitting the start-up
process more readily than continuing in start-up
efforts.
5 Discussion
This assessment has contributed to the understanding
of how motivation influences the outcomes of the
nascent entrepreneurship process from a goal setting
perspective. Goal setting theory purports that more
specific goals, self-efficacy, as well as their interaction
increase task performance. A critical outcome for
nascent entrepreneurs is the establishment of a viable,
new business. Hence, our hypotheses predicted that
having specific goals (H1) and higher self-efficacy
(H2) would be related to the establishment of a new
business among nascent entrepreneurs. Surprisingly,
we found no hypothesized effects for ‘‘new firm status’’
as an outcome category. What we did find, however,
was having a more formalized business plan and higher
self-efficacy contributed to maintaining in a start-up
effort versus quitting among nascent entrepreneurs.
Therefore, the value of planning and entrepreneurial
self-efficacy is that it facilitates the determination that
a given initiative is not economically viable (Reynolds
2007). Moreover, our findings confirm prior findings
by Cassar and Friedman (2009). Although Cassar and
Friedman (2009) found entrepreneurial self-efficacy
positively influenced operational status among nascent
entrepreneurs, they did not examine differences
between quits and continuing start-ups. Therefore,
our study advances our understanding of entrepreneur-
ial self-efficacy, from a goal setting perspective, on
task performance. Particularly, demonstrating that
high entrepreneurial self-efficacy and specific goals
positively influence the likelihood of continuing start-
up efforts versus quitting.
Our study operationalized goal specificity through
formality of business planning which is recognized
precedent in the entrepreneurial literature (Locke
1968; Latham and Yukl 1975; Bird 1988; Smith et al.
1990; Timmons 2000; Baum et al. 2001; Shane and
Delmar 2004; Baum and Locke 2004). Our analysis
provides evidence that entrepreneurial self-efficacy
and business plan formality are dependent constructs.
Moreover, there is also evidence that goal specificity,
operationalized as business plan formality, varies as a
function of entrepreneurial self-efficacy. Particularly,
among quits, there is compelling evidence that
individuals with low self-efficacy are less likely to
plan formally. Although our hypotheses were not
supported, we did find evidence that coincides with
goal theory and social cognitive theory.
These findings suggest that, in the context of nascent
entrepreneurship, goal specificity and entrepreneurial
self-efficacy operate together to cue nascent entrepre-
neurs regarding the feasibility of their prospective
opportunity, thus increasing the likelihood of persist-
ing in continuing start-up efforts versus quitting.
Moreover, the higher levels of entrepreneurial self-
efficacy with more formalized goals (or business
planning) increases the likelihood of quitting start-up
efforts versus persisting. Therefore, it could be inferred
that goal setting in the context of nascent entrepre-
neurship influences start-up outcomes, such that
nascent entrepreneurs who have high entrepreneurial
self-efficacy and formalize goals via business planning
are more likely to identify unworthwile opportunities
more rapidly, and subsequently more likely to exit
efforts than individuals with formalized goals and low
entrepreneurial self-efficacy and individuals with un-
formalized goal and high entrepreneurial self-efficacy.
Thus, when people fail to fulfill a challenging standard,
they lower or change their goals, but others remain
confident and persist in the face of failure and even
raise their goals (Baum and Locke 2004).
Moreover the finding that individuals with high
entrepreneurial self-efficacy who plan are more likely
to quit before those with low entrepreneurial self-
efficacy and no plan is no surprise. Bandura and
Jourden (1991), as well as Stone (1994), found that
high self-efficacy led to overconfidence in one’s
abilities. Instead of high self-efficacy individuals
contributing more of their resources toward the task,
they contributed less. These participants were both
less attentive and effortful than were their low self-
efficacy counterparts. One might conclude that,
although high self-efficacy can motivate individuals
696 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
to adopt high level goals, it may reduce motivation
within a goal level. Hence, high self-efficacy along
with highly formal goals likely lead to predictions of
higher states (i.e., reaching the goals sooner) than
predictions made when self-efficacy is low (Vancou-
ver et al. 2002). The result is that self-efficacy can
lower performance, and in turn explain the interaction
effect found in our analysis. Another reason why
individuals with higher degrees of entrepreneurial
self-efficacy who plan to quit more readily maybe
because they find and use better task strategies to
attain the goal of establishing a new firm based on the
negative feedback they may have obtained from that
initial opportunity. As a result, nascents may shift
their efforts to identify another opportunity to exploit
(Locke and Latham 1990; Seijts and Latham 2001).
Additionally, findings show that the time lag since
conception, the first initial action taken towards
implementing the prospective new firm, is also
significant in predicting the odds of quitting versus
continuing with start-up efforts. The longer nascent
entrepreneurs are engaged in their start-up initiative,
the lower their odds of quitting the start-up process.
According to Reynolds (2007), it takes half a year or
longer to quit the start-up process than it does to
create an operating firm. Previous entrepreneurship
research has shown that entrepreneurs persist with
under-performing firms (DeTienne et al. 2008). On
the firm level, such a phenomenon has been explained
based on threshold theory (Gimeno et al. 1997) and
the escalation of commitment (Staw 1976). However,
the reasons for this increasing commitment to a start-
up effort among nascent entrepreneurs provide an
interesting topic for future research.
Future research should investigate other factors to
improve model fit and to provide a more compre-
hensive test of Locke’s (1991)motivation sequence.
Our research has focused on the motivation hub of
Locke (1991), but future research would benefit from
an analysis of other parts of the motivation sequence.
How, for example, do values and motives influence
nascent entrepreneurs’ goals? Or what are the
perceived rewards and satisfaction that entrepreneurs
achieve after establishing the start-up? Also, within
the motivation hub, additional research could look at
the effects of goal difficulty, goal commitment, and
goal acceptance in addition to goal specificity studied
here. Atkinson (1958) showed that task difficulty,
measured as probability of task success, was related
to performance in a curvilinear, inverse function. The
highest level of effort occurred when the task was
moderately difficult, and the lowest levels occurred
when the task was either very easy or very hard
(Locke and Latham 2002). Although our analysis
included a control measure for degree of innovative-
ness of the start-up, it was a non-significant covariate
in the analysis. We suggest that future studies attempt
to identify ways to operationalize goal difficulty
beyond industry classification.
A question raised by our selected outcome vari-
ables concerns the desirability of these very out-
comes. We have found that having a formalized
business plan combined with high self-efficacy of a
nascent entrepreneur is a recipe for an increased
likelihood of exiting the start-up process. One may
argue that this, after all, may not be such a negative
outcome as one might first think. It is possible that the
mere engagement in a business gestation process
allows the individual to learn for their future career
and possible future start-up efforts. Along the same
lines, continued start-up effort may sometimes be a
signal of wasting resources and unwillingness to face
the market and competitive realities of the economy.
When considering the kind of ‘‘task performance’’
that goal setting theory might predict among nascent
entrepreneurs, one should keep in mind that a variety
of positive outcomes are possible, and some of them,
like quitting, may initially come in disguise.
Business planning is widely encouraged across
various entrepreneurship education programs. Our
results show that, compared to non-planners, nascent
entrepreneurs with formalized business plans perse-
vere longer in the process. Those who do not plan are
more likely to quit trying. As long as discouraging
exits from the firm gestation process is an outcome
sought by various business planning programs, our
results should be welcome news for entrepreneurship
educators. Also, previous research has suggested that
educators can influence students’ entrepreneurial
intentions by improving their entrepreneurial self-
efficacy (e.g. Wilson et al. 2007). Our results suggest
that this heightened self-efficacy with formalized
business planning may improve an individuals’
capabilities to promptly identify those business
opportunities that are not worthy further pursuit are
in line with this finding of previous research.
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 697
123
6 Limitations
Using PSED data limited our research design in a few
ways. First, due to the large amount of questions
included in the survey, survey designers elected to
shorten many of the scales used to measure certain
cognitive variables (in some cases) to single
responses (Shaver 2004). We are confident that the
measures we use are consistent with the core theory
utilized here; however, we do recommend that future
research seek to re-confirm our results with more
complete scales to further strengthen the evidence we
present in this paper. We also recommend that the
PSED data should be used to model changes in self-
efficacy and goal commitment instead of stock
measures, as has been analyzed here. It may be that
over subsequent waves of data, the changes in these
measures are more influential than the actual level at
the onset of the processes itself.
Also, we do recognize that there is a long-running
debate in the entrepreneurship literature regarding the
differences between small business owners and high
growth entrepreneurial business ventures (Carland
et al. 1984; Shane and Venkataraman 2001; Mahoney
and Michael 2005). The majority of respondents in
the PSED sample are actually classified as reproducer
small business owners, and not as innovating entre-
preneurs (Aldrich and Ruef 2006). This, although a
true reflection of the kinds of new businesses that the
American population is starting, may disappoint
those who are more interested in understanding the
dynamics of high-growth innovator firms.
Finally, it should be noted that our analysis does
not causally link planning and self-efficacy to
outcome status. Our findings highlight how the odds
of transitioning from start-up status to either new firm
or quitting the process are influenced by goal
formality and self-efficacy. Therefore, we acknowl-
edge this may represent a study limitation.
7 Conclusion
Entrepreneurship involves human agency. People start
businesses, they are not started by macro-economic
conditions, presence of opportunities, availability of
finance, social networks, positive entrepreneurial cli-
mate, regional/geographic attributes, or market char-
acteristics. Although such factors are influential, the
entrepreneurial process occurs because people are
motivated to act and pursue perceived opportunities.
All action is the result of motivational factors. There-
fore, it is imperative for scholars to incorporate theories
of motivation into entrepreneurial research to better
comprehend the entrepreneur and how he/she operates.
Accordingly, we have developed and tested a frame-
work that looks at interconnections between goals,
self-efficacy, and start-up process outcomes to under-
stand how individuals navigate the nascent entrepre-
neurial process. Goal-setting theory is not limited to
but focuses primarily on motivation in work settings.
Social cognitive theory and the research that underlies
it are primarily focused on self-efficacy, its measure-
ment, its causes, and its consequences. The nascent
entrepreneurship hub framework presented here has
integrated the key constructs from both theories. Our
empirical results have shown that significant relation-
ships between self-efficacy, goal specificity, and start-
up process outcomes exist. As a result, we believe that
the nascent entrepreneurship hub truly provides a
starting point to understand how motivation impacts
outcome status among would-be entrepreneurs.
Acknowledgements The authors would like to acknowledge
the College of Business at the University of Cincinnati, which
awarded our project funds from Title VI Learning Grant to
facilitate presentation of this research at the 2008 International
Council for Small Business World Conference in Halifax,
Nova Scotia Canada. We would also like to thank participants
at the 2008 Symposium on the Panel Study of Entrepreneurial
Dynamics: ‘‘Research on Business Creation’’ in Greenville,
South Carolina for their valuable feedback on a prior version of
this work.
References
Agresti, A. (1996). Introduction to categorical data analysis. In
Agresti discusses Mantel-Haenszel chi-square stratified
analysis (pp. 231–236). New York: Wiley.
Aldrich, H., & Ruef, M. (2006). Organizations evolving, 2nd
edn. London: Sage
Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of
human motivation in terms of motives, expectancies, and
incentives. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy,
action and society (pp. 288–305). New York: Van
Nostrand.
Autio, E., Keeley, R. H., Klofsten, M., Parker, G., & Hay, M.
(1997). Entrepreneurial intent among students: Testing an
intent model in Asia, Scandinavia, and USA. 17th Annual
Babson-College-Kauffman-foundation entrepreneurship
research conference. Babson College Center Entrepre-
neurial Studies.
698 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory.
American Psychologist, 44(9), 1175–1184.
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of human behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 71–81).
New York: Academic. (Reprinted in H. Friedman (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of mental health. San Diego: Academic
(1998).
Bandura, A. (1997). Self efficacy: The exercise of control. New
York: Freeman.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic per-
spective. Annual Review of Psychology 52(1), 1–26.
Bandura, A., & Jourden, F. J. (1991). Self-regulatory mecha-
nisms governing the impact of social comparison on
complex decision making. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 60, 941–951.
Barbosa, S., Gerhardt, M., & Kickul, J. (2007). The role of
cognitive style and risk preference on entrepreneurial self-
efficacy and entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of Lead-
ership and Organizational Studies, 13, 86–104.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond
expectations. New York: The Free Press.
Baum, J. R., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The relationship of
entrepreneurial traits, skill, and motivation to subsequent
venture growth. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(4),
587–598.
Baum, J. R., Locke, E. A., & Smith, K. G. (2001). A multi-
dimensional model of venture growth. Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 44, 292–303.
Bird, B. (1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: The case
for intention. Academy of Management Review, 13(3),
442–453.
Borland, C. M. (1974). Locus of control, need for Achievement
and entrepreneurship. Doctoral Dissertation, University
of Texas, Austin.
Boyd, N., & Vozikis, G. (1994). The influence of self-efficacy
on the development of entrepreneurial intentions and
actions. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 18(4),
63–77.
Brockhaus, S. R. H. (1980). Risk taking propensity of entre-
preneurs. Academy of Management Journal, 23(3),
509–520.
Brockhaus, R. H., & Horwitz, P. S. (1986). The psychology of
the entrepreneur. In D. L. Sexton & R. W. Smilor (Eds.),
The art and science of entrepreneurship. Cambridge:
Ballinger.
Brush, C. G., Manolova, T. S., & Edelman, L. F. (2008).
Properties of emerging organizations: An empirical test.
Journal of Business Venturing, 23(5), 547.
Campbell, D. (1988). Task complexity: A review and analysis.
Academy of Management Review, 13(1), 40–52.
Carland, J. W., Hoy, F., Boulton, W. R., & Carland, J. A. (1984).
Differentiating entrepreneurs from small business owners.
The Academy of Management Review, 9(2), 354–359.
Cassar, G., & Friedman, H. (2009). Does self-efficacy affect
entrepreneurial investment? Strategic Entrepreneurship
Journal, 3, 241–260.
Cervone, D., & Peake, P. K. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and
action: The influence of judgmental heuristics on self-
efficacy judgments and behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 50, 492–501.
Chen, C. C., Greene, P. G., & Crick, A. (1988). Does entre-
preneurial self-efficacy distinguish entrepreneurs from
managers? Journal of Business Venturing 13(4), 295–316.
Corman, J., Perles, B., & Vancini, P. (1988). Motivational
factors influencing high-technology entrepreneurship.
Journal of Small Business Management, 26, 36–42.
Cyert, R., & March, J. (1963). Behavioral Theory of the Firm.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Delmar, F., & Shane, S. (2003). Does business planning
facilitate the development of new ventures? Strategic
Management Journal, 24(12), 1165–1185.
DeTienne, D. R., Shepherd, D. A., & De Castro, J. O. (2008).
The fallacy of ‘‘only the strong survive’’: The effects of
extrinsic motivation on the persistence decisions for
under-performing firms. Journal of Business Venturing,
23(5), 528.
Dimov, D. (2007). From opportunity insight to opportunity
intention: The importance of person-situation learning
match. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 31(4), 561.
Gartner, W. B. (1988). ‘‘Who is an entrepreneur?’’ is the wrong
question. American Journal of Small Business, 12(4),
11–32.
Gatewood, E. J. (2004). Entrepreneurial expectancies. In
W. B. Gartner, K. G. Shaver, N. M. Carter, & P. D. Reynolds
(Eds.), The handbook of entrepreneurial dynamics: The
process of business creation. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Gatewood, E. J., Shaver, K. G., Powers, J. B., & Gartner, W. B.
(2002). Entrepreneurial expectancy, task effort, and per-
formance. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice 27(2),
187–206.
Gelderen, M. V., Thurik, R., & Bosma, N. (2006). Success and
risk factors in the pre-startup phase. Small Business
Economics, 26(4), 319.
Gimeno, J., Folta, T., Cooper, A., & Woo, C. (1997). Survival
of the fittest? Entrepreneurial human capital and the per-
sistence of underperforming firms. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 42, 750–783.
Gruber, M. (2007). Uncovering the value of planning in new
venture creation: A process and contingency perspective.
Journal of Business Venturing, 22(6), 782.
Hansemark, O. (1998). The effects of an entrepreneurship
programme on need forachievement and locus of control
of reinforcement. International Journal of Entrepreneur-
ship Behaviour and Research, 4(1), 28–50.
Hansemark, O. C. (2003). Need for achievement, locus of
control and the prediction of business start-ups: A longi-
tudinal study. Journal of Economic Study,24(3), 301–319.
Harper, D. A. (2008). Towards a theory of entrepreneurial
teams. Journal of Business Venturing, 23(6), 613.
Herron, L., & Sapienza, H. J. (1992). The entrepreneur and the
initiation of new venture launch activities. Entrepre-
neurship Theory and Practice, 17, 49–55.
Hmieleski, K. M., & Baron, R. A. (2008). When does entre-
preneurial self-efficacy enhance versus reduce firm per-
formance? Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal, 2, 57–72.
Hmieleski, K. M., & Corbett, A. C. (2008). The contrasting
interaction effects of improvisational behavior with
entrepreneurial self-efficacy on new venture performance
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 699
123
and entrepreneur work satisfaction. Journal of Business
Venturing 23(4), 482–496.
Honig, B., & Karlsson, T. (2004). Institutional forces and the
written business plan. Journal of Management, 30(1),
29–48.
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-
evaluations traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy,
locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satis-
faction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86, 80–92.
Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial and orga-
nizational psychology. In M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of
industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 75–170).
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Kaufman, B., Lewin, D., & Adams, R. (1995). Workforce
governance. In G. Ferris, S. Rosen, & D. Barnum (Eds.),
Handbook of human resource management (pp. 404–424).
Cambridge: Blackwell.
Krueger, N. F. (1993). The impact of prior entrepreneurial
exposure on perceptions. Entrepreneurship Theory and
Practice, 18(1), 5–22.
Krueger, N., Reilly, M. D., & Carsrud, A. L. (2000). Com-
peting models of entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of
Business Venturing, 15, 411–432.
LaPorte, R. E., & Nath, R. (1976). Role of performance goals
in prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68,
260–264.
Latham, P. G., & Pinder, C. C. (2005a). Work motivation
theory and research at the dawn of the Twenty-first cen-
tury. Annual Review of Psychology,56, 485–516.
Latham, G. P., & Pinder, C. C. (2005b). Work motivation
theory and research at the dawn of the twenty-first cen-
tury. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 485–516.
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. (1975). Assigned versus par-
ticipative goal setting with educated and uneducated wood
workers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 299–302.
Liao, J., & Gartner, W. (2006). The effects of pre-venture plan
timing and perceived environmental uncertainty on the
persistence of emerging firms. Small Business Economics,
27(1), 23–40.
Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and
incentives. Organizational behavior and human perfor-
mance, 3(2), 157–189.
Locke, E. A. (1991). The motivation sequence, the motivation
hub and the motivation core. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 50, 288–299.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. (1990). A theory of goal setting
and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey:
Prentice Hall.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically
useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 57(9), 705–717.
Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. N., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P.
(1981). Goal setting and task performance: 1969–1980.
Psychological Bulletin (American Psychological Associ-
ation), 90(1), 125–152.
Mahoney, J. T., & Michael, S. C. (2005). A subjective theory
of entrepreneurship. In S. Alvarez, R. Agarwal, &
O. Sorensen (Eds.), Handbook of entrepreneurship
(pp. 33–55). Boston, MA: Kluwer.
Margolis, H., McCabe, P. (2006). Improving self-efficacy and
motivation: What to do, what to say. Intervention in
School and Clinic, 41(4), 218–227.
Markman, G. D., & Baron, R. A. (2003). Person-entrepre-
neurship fit: Why some people are more successful
entrepreneurs than others. Human Resource Management
Review, 13(2). 281–301.
Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T., & Baldwin, G. C. (2005).
The implications of advances in research on motivation
for cognitive models. Journal of Experimental and The-
oretical Artificial Intelligence, 17(4), 371–384.
McClelland, D. C. (1965). Need for achievement and entre-
preneurship: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personal
Social Psychology, 95, 389–392.
McGee, J. E., Peterson, M., Mueller, S. L., & Sequeira, J. M.
(2009). Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy: Refining the Mea-
sure. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 33(4), 965.
Naffziger, D. W., Hornsby, J. S., & Kuratko, D. F. (1994). A
proposed research model of entrepreneurial motivation.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 18(3), 29.
Phillips, J. M., & Gully, S. M. (1997). Role of goal orientation,
ability, need for achievement, and locus of control in the
self-efficacy and goal-setting process. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 82(5), 792–802.
Reynolds, P. D. (1994). Autonomous firm dynamics and eco-
nomic growth in the United States, 1986–1990. Regional
Studies 28(4), 429–442.
Reynolds, P. (2007). Entrepreneurship in the United States.
New York: Springer.
Reynolds, P. D., & Curtin, R. T. (2004). Appendix A: Data
collection. In W. B. Gartner, K. G. Shaver, N. M. Carter,
& P. D. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of entrepreneurial
dynamics: The process of business creation. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Robb, A. M., & Coleman, S. (2009). Kauffman firm survey 3:
Characteristics of new firms: A comparison by gender.
Kansas City: Kauffman Foundation.
Rotter, J. (1966). Generalized experiences for internal versus
external control of reinforcement. Psychological Mono-
graphs 80(1) (whole No. 609).
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2001). The effect of distal
learning, outcome, and proximal goals on a moderately
complex task. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22,
291–302.
Shane, S., & Delmar, F. (2004). Planning for the market:
Business planning before marketing and the continuation
of organizing efforts. Journal of Business Venturing, 19,
767–785.
Shane, S., & Venkataraman, S. (2001). Entrepreneurship as a
field of research: A response to Zahra and Dess, Singh, and
Erikson. Academy of Management Review, 26(1), 13–16.
Shane, S., Locke, E. A., & Collins, C. J. (2003). Entrepre-
neurial motivation. Human Resource Management
Review, 13, 257–279.
Shaver, K. G. (2004). Overview: The cognitive characteristics
of the entrepreneur. In Handbook of entrepreneurial
dynamics: The process of business creation (pp.
131–141). Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA.
Simon, H. (1964). On the concept of organizational goal.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 9, 1–22.
700 D. M. Hechavarria et al.
123
Smith, K., Locke, E., & Barry, D. (1990). Goal setting, plan-
ning and organizational performance: An experimental
simulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
sion Processes, 46, 118–134.
Stajkovic, A., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-
related performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 124, 240–261.
Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: A study of
escalating commitment to a chosen course of action.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(1),
27–44.
Stone, D. N. (1994). Overconfidence in initial self-efficacy
judgments: Effects on decision processes and perfor-
mance. Organizaitonal Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 59, 452–474.
Steel, P., & Konig, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of moti-
vation. Academy of Management Review, 31(4), 889–913.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate
statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
Teece, D. J. (2007). Explicating dynamic capabilities: The
nature and microfoundations of (sustainable) enterprise
performance. Strategic Management Journal, 28(13),
1319.
Timmons, J. A. (2000). New venture creation: Entrepreneur-
ship 2000 (5th ed.). Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Townsend, D. M., Busenitz, L. W., & Arthurs, J. D. (2010). To
start or not to start: Outcome and ability expectations in
the decision to start a new venture. Journal of Business
Venturing, 25, 192–202.
van Gelder, J.-L., de Vries, R. E., Frese, M., & Goutbeek, J.-P.
(2007). Differences in psychological strategies of failed
and operational business owners in the Fiji Islands.
Journal of Small Business Management, 45(3), 388.
Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., Tischner, E., & Putka, D.
J. (2002). Two studies examining the negative effect of
self-efficacy on performance. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 87(3), 506–516.
White, S. S., & Locke, E. A. (2000). Problems with the Pyg-
malion effect and some proposed solutions. Leadership
Quarterly, 11, 389–415.
Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Subjective
career success and emotional well-being: Longitudinal
predictive power of selection, optimization, and com-
pensation. Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 60, 321–335.
Wilson, F., Kickul, J., & Marlino, D. (2007). Gender, entre-
preneurial self-efficacy, and entrepreneurial career inten-
tions: Implications for entrepreneurship education.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 31(3), 387–406.
Wood, R. E., Mento, A. J., & Locke, E. A. (1987). Task
complexity as a moderator of goal effects: A meta-anal-
ysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 416–425.
Zacharakis, A. L. (1999). Storage Networks: Orchestrating the
explosion. Babson Entrepreneurial Review, 14(1), 5–6.
Zhao, H., Seibert, S. E., & Hills, G. E. (2005). The mediating role
of self-efficacy in the development of entrepreneurial
intentions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6),
1265–1272.
The nascent entrepreneurship hub 701
123