Content uploaded by Jörn Theuerkauf
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jörn Theuerkauf on Dec 29, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Content uploaded by Bartosz Pirga
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Bartosz Pirga on Jun 06, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
SHORT COMMUNICATION
How does parental role influence the activity and movements
of breeding wolves?
Hiroshi Tsunoda Æ Roman Gula Æ Jo
¨
rn Theuerkauf Æ
Sophie Rouys Æ Stephan Radler Æ Bartosz Pirga Æ
Julia Eggermann Æ Barbara Brzezowska
Received: 15 January 2008 / Accepted: 3 June 2008 / Published online: 10 July 2008
Ó Japan Ethological Society and Springer 2008
Abstract We studied the activity and movements of a
male and a female wolf during breeding in the Bieszczady
Mountains, Poland. The female was less active and mobile
and on average closer to the den than the male. The male
was less active and mobile when the pack size was seven
compared to a year when the pack consisted only of the
breeding pair. We conclude that the roles played by
breeding males and females rearing pups influence their
activity, and that breeding males in larger packs move less
during the nursing period because of help by other pack
members.
Keywords Bieszczady Mountains Canis lupus
Feeding helpers Poland Reproduction
Introduction
Little is known about the social behavior of free-ranging
wolves (Canis lupus), as these animals are difficult to
observe and most behavioral studies therefore deal with
captive packs (e.g., Ziemen 1982; Asa et al. 1990; Bernal
and Packard 1997). With a few exceptions (e.g., Mech
H. Tsunoda
Wildlife Conservation Laboratory, Graduate School of
Agriculture, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology,
Saiwai-cho 3-5-8, Fuchu-shi, Tokyo 183-8509, Japan
Present Address:
H. Tsunoda
Laboratory of Water Resource Planning,
United Graduate School of Agricultural Sciences,
Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology,
Saiwai-cho 3-5-8, Fuchu-shi, Tokyo 183-8509, Japan
e-mail: dayan_and_jitan@hotmail.com
R. Gula J. Theuerkauf B. Pirga
Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences,
Wilcza 64, 00-679 Warsaw, Poland
e-mail: jtheuer@miiz.waw.pl
B. Pirga
e-mail: wrzosowe_wzgorze@poczta.onet.pl
S. Rouys
Conservation Research New Caledonia, BP 2549,
Noumea 98846, New Caledonia
e-mail: rouys@ifrance.com
S. Radler
University of Applied Forest Sciences Rottenburg,
Schadenweilerhof, 72108 Rottenburg, Germany
e-mail: stephan.radler@fh-rottenburg.de
J. Eggermann
Faculty of Biology and Biotechnology,
Ruhr University Bochum, Universita
¨
tsstr. 150,
44801 Bochum, Germany
e-mail: julia.eggermann@web.de
B. Brzezowska
Department of Animal Ecology,
Institute of Environmental Sciences,
Jagiellonian University, Gronostajowa 7,
30-387 Krakow, Poland
e-mail: bulbulek@wp.pl
R. Gula (&)
45 rue Maurice Herzog, Noumea 98800,
New Caledonia
e-mail: rgula@miiz.waw.pl
123
J Ethol (2009) 27:185–189
DOI 10.1007/s10164-008-0106-z
1999), studies of wild wolves are accordingly based on
indirect methods, such as telemetry. Behavioral information
is therefore often gathered as a byproduct rather than an
answer to a primary question. The social and physiological
status of free-ranging wolves has an impact on the activity
and movements of individuals (Harrington and Mech 1982;
Asa et al. 1990; Ballard et al. 1991), which can vary with
age and sex (Je˛drzejewski et al. 2001; Schmidt et al. 2008),
dominance (Mech 1999) and reproductive status (Harring-
ton and Mech 1982; Ballard et al. 1991; Vila
`
et al. 1995;
Je˛drzejewski et al. 2001; Theuerkauf et al. 2003a). These
intrinsic factors are mitigated by environmental factors
which also shape the activity and movements of wolves.
Documented external factors that influence wolf activity
and movements are prey density (Messier 1985), prey
migration (Walton et al. 2001), prey activity (Theuerkauf
et al. 2003a), the activity of neighboring packs (Mech and
Harper 2002), and human activity (Vila
`
et al. 1995; Ciucci
et al. 1997; Theuerkauf et al. 2003a, b; Kaartinen et al.
2005; Kusak et al. 2005; Theuerkauf et al. 2007).
During breeding, wolves gravitate around den and ren-
dezvous sites (summarized in Mech and Boitani 2003).
Besides the breeding male, nonreproductive adult pack
members also supply the nursing mother and pups with
meat (Harrington et al. 1983; Mech et al. 1999). This al-
loparental care has an impact on the pups’ survival
(Harrington et al. 1983) and should also influence the
activity budget of the breeding male. We expected that in
the absence of help from other nonbreeding adults, the
breeding male, who would then be the sole food supplier,
should increase his activity and movements. On the other
hand, in a larger pack, the breeding male could remain
longer with his mate and pups to protect them. To verify
this hypothesis, we compared the activity and movements
of a breeding female and of a breeding male in a year when
he was alone with his mate with those in a year when there
were seven pack members.
Methods
The study area is situated in the Bieszczady Mountains,
southeastern Poland (49°19
0
–49°50
0
N, 22°15
0
–22°45
0
E) and
covers approximately 1,000 km
2
. Elevations range from
300 to 900 m a.s.l. The mean annual temperature is 5.5 °C
and the annual precipitation ranges from 800 to 1,200 mm.
Forest, mostly beech (Fagus sylvatica), fir (Abies alba),
spruce (Picea abies) and grey alder (Alnus incana), covers
62% of the area. Wolf packs consist of 2–7 individuals and
occupy home ranges of 88–229 km
2
(Gula 2008).
We radiotracked two breeding wolves (one male and
one female) from different packs. Wolves were fitted with
VHF radiocollars without activity sensors (Telonics Inc.,
Mesa, AZ, USA). The female was caught in March 2002.
From the beginning of May 2004 and for the following
40 days she remained mostly in one place, suggesting that
she was rearing pups. In July, we investigated that spot and
found her den. In August and September, howling revealed
that her pack also included pups. We therefore considered
that the female had raised young in 2004.
The second wolf was a three-year-old dispersing male
when caught in spring 2003. He was alone when snow-
tracked in December 2003, but from January 2004 onward
he was accompanied by another wolf, apparently a female,
as we found signs of mating in the snow. This pair must
have bred in 2004, as we heard them howl with pups. The
following summer, based on radio-locations of the male,
we found their den. In the summers of 2005 and 2006 we
also heard pups howling with several adults, so the pack
must have bred in those years too. By snow-tracking, we
estimated that the pack consisted of five individuals in the
winter of 2004/2005 and seven in the winter of 2005/2006.
We estimated the parturition date as the first time that we
located wolves at the spot where we later found a den (in
our study area during the first ten days of May).
In this paper, we used radiotracking data for the female
collected in 2004 and for the male collected in 2004 and
2006. Our data cover the period from mid May to early
September, during which each wolf was followed every
second week over a whole 24-h period. During these 24-h
sessions, we located wolves by ground triangulation and
recorded wolf locations and activity (based on changes in
signal strength) every 15 min. To estimate the proportion
of time active, we assigned a value of 1 when the wolves
were active and their locations changed; a value of 0 when
the wolves were not active and their locations did not
change; and a value of 0.5 when the wolves were either
active or their locations changed (as described in The-
uerkauf and Je˛drzejewski 2002). We estimated the distance
to the den as the straight-line distance from a wolf location
to the den site. The location error was about 250 m (The-
uerkauf et al. 2007). Because we could not find the den of
the male in 2006, we estimated the location of the den site
as the center of the activity of the male in May and June of
2006 (geometric center of the 25% kernel density distri-
bution of locations). Later, we heard the pack howl with
pups at this site. We estimated the daily range of each
radiotracking session as minimum convex polygons
(MCPs) of the 96 locations taken during the 24 h.
Results
As the breeding season progressed, both wolves increased
the distance they traveled from the den (Fig. 1). The time
active, the distance traveled and the daily range of the
186 J Ethol (2009) 27:185–189
123
female increased gradually, whereas den attendance and
the time she spent at the den steadily decreased. In 2004,
when the breeding pair was alone, the male’s activity, the
distance traveled and the daily range were highest just after
the pups’ birth. These values constantly decreased until
about three months after birth, when they reached about the
same levels as those of the female (Fig. 1). In 2004, the
breeding male was mostly away from the den and his
activity and movements were accordingly greater than
those of the female. In 2006, however, when the male’s
Fig. 1 Seasonal changes in mean distance to the den, time spent at
the den, den attendance, distance traveled, time spent active and daily
range of a female (n = 6 days in 2004) and male wolf (n = 5 in 2004
and n = 5 in 2006) during two breeding seasons in the Bieszczady
Mountains, SE Poland
J Ethol (2009) 27:185–189 187
123
pack consisted of seven wolves, his activity pattern and
movements were reduced to levels that were similar to
those of the female (Table 1).
The activity and distance traveled by the female were
higher in the day and at twilight than at night (Table 1),
and most differences between years or individuals occurred
at night. In 2004, the male’s nightly movements were more
than twice as high as they were in 2006. The female, on the
other hand, moved very little at night (Fig. 2). These trends
were also reflected in the time spent at the den. In 2004, the
male spent virtually no time at the den, especially in the
twilight hours. In 2006, however, the male spent compa-
rable amounts of time at the den to the female in 2004.
Accordingly, the male’s daily range was much larger in
2004 (11.3 ± 3.6 km
2
95% confidence interval) than in
2006 (3.2 ± 3.1 km
2
). The female’s range was the smallest
of all (2.5 ± 1.7 km
2
).
Discussion
Although other factors, such as prey availability, might have
influenced the activity of the wolves, we think that the
differences in male and female behavior were a result of their
different roles in the biparental care of pups. Mothers nurse
the pups, keep them warm and keep the den clean and dry
(summarized in Packard 2003). In our study, the female left
the den mostly in the day, which would correlate with her
having to remain with the pups to keep them warm during
cooler periods (twilight and night). Breeding males, on the
other hand, attend the den site less frequently than the adult
females (Harrington and Mech 1982; Ballard et al. 1991;
Potvin et al. 2004), but contribute by feeding the female and
therefore spend most of the time hunting away from the den
(Mech 1999). Wolves in our study area probably hunt mostly
at dawn (Eggermann et al. 2008), which would explain why
this was the time when the breeding male was almost never at
the den. Previous studies report that adult females with pups
were less active during the denning season (Ballard et al.
1991; Mech et al. 1995; Vila
`
et al. 1995;Je˛drzejewski et al.
2001; Theuerkauf et al. 2003a; Schmidt et al. 2008). The
activity of the female we monitored in this study was lower
than the mean activity of wolves in our study area (Egger-
mann et al. 2008), suggesting that she too reduced her
activity while she cared for the pups.
Despite tracking only one breeding male, to our
knowledge this is the only account of a breeding male’s
daily activity and movements during the denning season.
We observed a shift in his behavior after pack size
increased from two to seven individuals. In both years, the
breeding male’s activity and movements seem to have been
strongly linked to the necessity of supplying his mate, and
later pups, with food. His reduction in activity and move-
ments in 2006 support this hypothesis, since he was no
longer the sole food provider for his pack in that year.
About two months after their birth, although they are not
yet able to follow the adults on their extensive movements,
the pups are no longer dependent on their mother for milk
or warmth, and so they can be left alone at rendezvous
sites (summarized in Packard 2003). From this time on,
the breeding pair can resume their normal activity and
Table 1 Mean (with 95% confidence interval) time spent active, distance traveled and daily range of a female (n = 6 days in 2004) and a male
wolf (n = 5 in 2004 and n = 5 in 2006) during two breeding seasons in the Bieszczady Mountains, SE Poland
Sex, year Whole day (24 h) Daylight (13.5 h) Twilight (4 h) Night (6.5 h)
Time spent active (%) Male, 2004 44.2 ± 10.3 33.5 ± 8.3 51.9 ± 22.0 65.0 ± 30.6
Male, 2006 26.5 ± 8.2 20.3 ± 3.1 37.5 ± 11.6 30.4 ± 22.9
Female, 2004 23.1 ± 9.7 30.5 ± 17.8 30.7 ± 11.4 5.7 ± 3.9
Distance traveled (km/h) Male, 2004 0.67 ± 0.14 0.38 ± 0.25 0.77 ± 0.38 1.23 ± 0.79
Male, 2006 0.33 ± 0.14 0.14 ± 0.06 0.44 ± 0.35 0.53 ± 0.37
Female, 2004 0.30 ± 0.17 0.40 ± 0.30 0.49 ± 0.21 0.00 ± 0.00
Time spent at the den (h) Male, 2004 0.3 ± 0.5 0.1 ± 0.1 0.0 ± 0.0 0.3 ± 0.5
Male, 2006 7.4 ± 8.9 4.6 ± 5.5 1.2 ± 1.6 1.6 ± 2.3
Female, 2004 5.8 ± 6.0 2.7 ± 2.9 1.2 ± 1.4 1.9 ± 2.1
Fig. 2 Daily pattern of distance traveled by a female and a male wolf
during the 2004 and 2006 breeding seasons in the Bieszczady
Mountains, SE Poland
188 J Ethol (2009) 27:185–189
123
movements, which probably explains why, in 2004, the
activity and movements of the male and the female reached
the same levels about three months after birth. We there-
fore suggest that the activity patterns of male and female
breeding wolves are shaped by their differing roles during
the denning period and by the availability of feeding
helpers.
Acknowledgments This study was part of the Bieszczady Wolf
Project and was funded by the Polish National Committee for Sci-
entific Research (KBN 6P04F 006), and the Museum and Institute of
Zoology of the Polish Academy of Sciences. Fellowships were pro-
vided by the Japan Student Services Organization (to HT), the
German Donors’ Association for the Promotion of Sciences and
Humanities (to JT), and the German Academic Exchange Service (to
JE). We thank N. Maruyama and K. Perzanowski for their coopera-
tion and L. Aubry, M. Barreteau, M. Carruthers, M. Diemert, S.
Drevet, M. Januszczak, S. Kiener, K. Lahongre, M. Le Peutrec, K.
Mayer, N. Schmidt, M-C. Schultz, M. Skuban and W. Schwimmer for
their help during field work, as well as two anonymous reviewers for
useful comments.
References
Asa CS, Mech LD, Seal US, Plotka ED (1990) The influence of social
and endocrine factors on urine-marking by captive wolves
(Canis lupus). Horm Behav 24:497–509
Ballard WB, Ayres LA, Gardner CL, Foster JW (1991) Den site
activity patterns of gray wolves, Canis lupus, in southcentral
Alaska. Can Field Nat 105:497–504
Bernal JF, Packard JM (1997) Differences in winter activity,
courtship, and social behavior of two captive family groups of
Mexican wolves (Canis lupus baileyi). Zoo Biol 16:435–443
Ciucci P, Boitani L, Francisci F, Andreoli G (1997) Home range,
activity and movements of a wolf pack in central Italy. J Zool
243:803–819
Eggermann J, Gula R, Pirga B, Theuerkauf J, Tsunoda H, Brzezowska
B, Rouys S, Radler S (2008) Daily and seasonal variation in wolf
activity in the Bieszczady Mountains, SE Poland. Mammal Biol.
doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2008.05.010
Gula R (2008) Wolf depredation on domestic animals in the Polish
Carpathian Mountains. J Wildl Manage 72:283–289
Harrington FH, Mech LD (1982) Patterns of homesite attendance in
two Minnesota wolf packs. In: Harrington FH, Paquet PC (eds)
Wolves of the world. Noyes Publications, Park Ridge, NJ, pp
81–105
Harrington FH, Mech LD, Fritts SH (1983) Pack size and wolf pup
survival: their relationship under varying ecological conditions.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 13:19–26
Je˛drzejewski W, Schmidt K, Theuerkauf J, Je˛drzejewska B, Okarma
H (2001) Daily movements and territory use by radio-collared
wolves (Canis lupus) in Białowie
_
za Primeval Forest in Poland.
Can J Zool 79:1993–2004
Kaartinen S, Kojola I, Colpaert A (2005) Finnish wolves avoid roads
and settlements. Ann Zool Fenn 42:523–532
Kusak J, Skrbinsek AM, Hurber D (2005) Home ranges, movements,
and activity of wolves (Canis lupus) in the Dalmatian part of
Dinarids, Croatia. Eur J Wildl Res 51:254–262
Mech LD (1999) Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in
wolf pack. Can J Zool 77:1196–1203
Mech LD, Boitani L (2003) Wolf social ecology. In: Mech D, Boitani
L (eds) Wolves: behavior, ecology and conservation. University
of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 1–34
Mech LD, Harper EK (2002) Differential use of a wolf, Canis lupus,
pack territory edge and core. Can Field Nat 116:315–316
Mech LD, Philips MK, Smith DW, Kreeger TJ (1995) Denning
behavior of non-gravid wolves. Can Field Nat 110:343–345
Mech LD, Wolf PC, Packard JM (1999) Regurgitative food transfer
among wild wolves. Can J Zool 77:1192–1195
Messier F (1985) Solitary living and extraterritorial movements of
wolves in relation to social status and prey abundance. Can
J Zool 63:239–245
Packard J (2003) Wolf behavior: reproductive, social and intelligent. In:
Mech LD, Boitani L (eds) Wolves: behavior, ecology and
conservation. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 35–65
Potvin MJ, Peterson RO, Vucetich JA (2004) Wolf homesite
attendance patterns. Can J Zool 82:1512–1518
Schmidt K, Je˛drzejewski W, Theuerkauf J, Kowalczyk R, Okarma H,
Je˛drzejewska B (2008) Reproductive behaviour of wild-living
wolves in Białowie
_
za Primeval Forest (Poland). J Ethol 26:69–78
Theuerkauf J, Je˛drzejewski W (2002) Accuracy of radiotelemetry to
estimate wolf activity and locations. J Wildl Manage 66:859–864
Theuerkauf J, Je˛drzejewski W, Schmidt K, Okarma H, Ruczyn
´
ski I,
S
´
nie
_
zko S, Gula R (2003a) Daily patterns and duration of wolf
activity in the Białowie
_
za forest, Poland. J Mammal 84:243–253
Theuerkauf J, Je˛drzejewski W, Schmidt K, Gula R (2003b) Spatio-
temporal segregation of wolves from humans in the Białowie
_
za
Forest (Poland). J Wildl Manage 67:706–716
Theuerkauf J, Gula R, Pirga B, Tsunoda H, Eggermann J, Brzezowska
B, Rouys S, Radler S (2007) Human impact on wolf activity in the
Bieszczady Mountains, SE Poland. Ann Zool Fenn 44:225–231
Vila
`
C, Urios V, Castroviejo J (1995) Observations on the daily activity
patterns in the Iberian wolf. In: Carbyn LN, Fritts SH, Seip DR
(eds) Ecology and conservation of wolves in a changing world
(occasional publication series no. 35). Canadian Circumpolar
Institute, University of Alberta, Alberta, Canada, pp 335–340
Walton LR, Cluff HD, Paquet PC, Ramsay MA (2001) Movement
patterns of barren-ground wolves in the central Canadian Arctic.
J Mammal 82:867–876
Ziemen E (1982) A wolf pack sociogram. In: Harrington FH, Paquet
PC (eds) Wolves of the world. Noyes Publishers, Park Ridge, NJ,
pp 282–322
J Ethol (2009) 27:185–189 189
123