ChapterPDF Available

Participation and the Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action

Authors:

Abstract

This chapter takes up two questions essential to participatory environmental education: What experiences prepare children to be aware of their environment and to take action on its behalf? And, how can communities support children’s environmental learning and action? I suggest answers to these questions based on an ecological approach to psychology and show how research on the significant life experiences of people committed to environmental education and action can be understood within this framework. I also argue that environmental education can most productively encourage children to know, value, and protect the diversity of life on this planet if it builds on a theoretical foundation that embeds human development in an ecological context. Keywords community development, children’s agency, ecological psychology, environmental learning, environmental citizenship
Chapter 6
Participation and the Ecology of Environmental
Awareness and Action
Louise Chawla
Keywords community development, children’s agency, ecological psychology,
environmental learning, environmental citizenship
6.1 Introduction
This chapter takes up two questions essential to participatory environmental
education: What experiences prepare children to be aware of their environment and
to take action on its behalf? And, how can communities support children’s environ-
mental learning and action? I suggest answers to these questions based on an
ecological approach to psychology and show how research on the significant life
experiences of people committed to environmental education and action can be
understood within this framework. I also argue that environmental education can
most productively encourage children to know, value, and protect the diversity of
life on this planet if it builds on a theoretical foundation that embeds human development
in an ecological context.
6.2 Learning to see and Learning to Take Action
Growing up to know and value the diversity of life is partly a matter of learning to
see: learning to see communities of plants and animals, details of their individual
existence and interactions, and patterns of their ever-changing habitats. In cities and
towns it includes learning to see the diversity of human communities and the ways
in which people interact with their place. As the wildlife biologist Aldo Leopold
observed many years ago, environmental education faces a great challenge, ‘how to
bring about a striving for harmony with land among people, many of whom have
forgotten there is any such thing as land, among whom education and culture have
98
A. Reid et al. (eds.), Participation and Learning, 98–110.
© Springer 2008
College of Architecutre & Planning, University of Colorado, Campus Box 314, Boulder,
Co 80309-0314, USA, louise.chawla@colorado.edu
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 99
become almost synonymous with landlessness’ (Leopold 1949, 1966:210).
Therefore this chapter begins with a discussion of theories of perception.
Yet, learning to see brings risks as well as fascination and delight. What if a
person sees painful things? As Leopold (1949, 1966:197) also observed, ‘one of the
penalties of an ecological education is that one lives alone in a world of wounds.’ He
compared attentiveness to the land, in the midst of a culture that is largely oblivious
to the effects of its actions on other forms of life, to the condition of ‘the doctor who
sees the marks of death in a community that believes itself well and does not want
to be told otherwise’ (ibid.). Therefore, learning to see needs to be balanced by
learning to take action on the environment’s behalf – learning that one is not, in fact,
alone, learning how to organise and collaborate with others in order to investigate
the sources of problems, and learning how to build a common vision for a better
world and take steps to bring it to reality. This goal is comparable to the concept of
action competence that has been defined by Jensen and Schnack (1997) and Breiting
and Mogensen (1999). It is the theme of the second half of this chapter.
For both learning to see and learning to take action, participation in the life of
one’s community is essential – where ‘community’ is meant in an expansive sense
of the plants and animals as well as the people and cultures of one’s locality. To
maintain this broad meaning, this chapter is equally concerned with formal and
informal participation. Informal participation involves freedom to move about and
explore natural and built environments, to gather with others and to observe and try
out roles in public places. Children cannot see the diversity of life unless they have
a chance to venture out into it. By getting out, they come to find out about issues
taken up in formal channels for participation, like the loss of habitat or homeless-
ness. Thus formal and informal participation are two interdependent sides of the
inclusion of children in their communities. Through freedom to move about in their
localities and become part of diverse settings, children gain authority to speak
about the conditions that they find. They also gain reasons to become involved. If
they have benefited from open and accessible communities with a variety of
resources, they are likely to defend these advantages. If they live in places that are
boring or dangerous, but they have had opportunities to see alternatives, they are
likely to advocate a more generous plan (see, for example, Chapter 18 by Barratt
and Barratt Hacking, this volume).
One advantage of ecological psychology for environmental education is that it
directs attention to the importance of informal as well as formal learning. In doing
so it accommodates the learning of very young children, from infancy up, who
primarily know their communities through the informal contexts of everyday life.
Typically, when formal mechanisms for participatory planning involve young peo-
ple they focus on pre-adolescents and adolescents. Younger children, however,
often have thoughtful views as well. Even before they learn to talk, their disposition
to engage with the world and to make their mark – a disposition essential for com-
munity participation – is rooted in infancy. Ecological psychology provides a
foundation for a conception of environmental education that extends beyond the
school to all areas of a child’s life and furthermore, to all periods of the life span.
It lays responsibility for the quality of environmental learning not only on the
100 L. Chawla
shoulders of school administrators and teachers, but also on parents, city planners,
public officials, and other people whose decisions shape the places where children
live and the opportunities that these places afford.
6.3 An Ecological Theory of Perception and Action
Ecological psychology offers a strong foundation for conceptualising children’s
environmental learning and participation, in both the formal and informal sense, for
at least three reasons. It focuses on children’s agency, it describes the environmen-
tal context of action and development, and not least, it places children and the
environment together in a common realm.
Although people caught up in the natural attitude of everyday life generally assume
that they perceive their surroundings directly, this is not the dominant epistemology of
psychology and the social sciences. The view that has gained wide currency in these
fields is that people never know the world as such but only mental representations of it.
Social constructivist theory – a version of this representationalist view that has gained
particular prominence – posits that individuals inhabit separate subjective realms that
are socialised by gender, class, ethnicity, and all of the other contingencies of social
existence. This view places the world and people’s awareness of it in separate physical
and mental realms and emphasises divisions in human experience.
Ecological psychology does not deny the variety of human cultures and individ-
ual differences, but it does maintain that as humans we share a single and singular
planet where we have evolved fundamentally common characteristics. Our co-
evolution with other elements of the environment, as well as our cultural evolution,
points to a relational theory of perception and action. According to a central princi-
ple of ecological psychology developed by James and Eleanor Gibson (Gibson and
Pick 2000; Gibson 1966, 1979), to be a living organism – to be animate – means to
have agency that functions in the service of maintaining an awareness of the envi-
ronment. Action is a means of staying in touch with the environment’s significant
properties: whether this be an infant following its mother’s face with its gaze or an
adult striding through the landscape. The evolutionary history of each species
includes selection for systems of perception and action that detect aspects of the
environment with functional significance. Awareness of the world always occurs
within this animal–environment relationship, and thus the mind and the world are
not two separate realms, mediated somehow by a mental representation, but recip-
rocal facets of a shared functional system. Rather than being mediated, this rela-
tionship is direct.
To take the case of vision, which James Gibson (1979) studied intensively, the
properties of objects are carried to the eye as structures in reflected light. These
structures serve as potential information about the objects and their qualities, avail-
able for all perceivers who are present. Because reflected light carries a multitude
of information from all sides and as different perceivers may be attuned to different
features of it, they may not see the same thing. What they see, however, are features
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 101
of the world, not mental fabrications. The stimulus information around them con-
stitutes a common ground. Therefore, with experience, people can learn to see the
same features of their shared world.
This view of perception leads to another contribution of the Gibsons’ ecological
approach: the concept of affordances. Affordances are relational, functionally sig-
nificant properties of the environment. By this view the meaning of environmental
features is inherent in the relationship between the environment and an individual,
rather than being a mental construction imposed on the environment. For example,
children can build a tree house only if the specific properties of the construction
material and the tree match the competencies of the children and the goals of the
task. They can climb into the tree only if its branches begin low enough relative to
each child’s height and reach. They can build a platform to stand on only if the
boards are light enough for these children to lift but sturdy enough to bear the chil-
dren’s weight. From the perspective of these children, these environmental features
are functionally meaningful and their meaning is specified relative to the properties
of the objects, the task and the children considered together.
This example also serves to illustrate the relationship between perceiving and
acting discussed previously. Perceiving and acting proceed together in a system
that is simultaneously defined by the environment’s properties and by a creature’s
body, capabilities, and goals. In addition, this example demonstrates Edward
Reed’s (1996a) distinction between exploratory and performatory activities. In
exploratory activities, an animal scans and tests the properties of the environment
without significantly altering what it finds. In performatory activities, it alters the
environment according to its goals. Yet successful performance depends on expe-
rience gained from exploration. The children had doubtless tested the properties
of trees, boards, and their bodies before they carried out their work. In the
process, they discovered values and meanings inherent in the environment that
they could put to use. This example also shows that the world of culture and the
natural world are inseparable. People typically learn performatory actions in
social groups but the cultural world that they construct depends on the properties
of the natural world.
This view of agency and perception avoids a matter–mind dichotomy. It places
humans in a world in which they have co-evolved with other living things, depend-
ent on the intrinsic qualities of the world, its resources and its limits, but equipped
to discover what these qualities and limits are. It recognises that humans, like other
animals, alter their environment, but in the process they need to preserve the
resources that are the conditions for their well-being. Because ecological psychol-
ogy emphasises these common dimensions of human life it forms a basis for
collective action for the environment.
This emphasis on agency is in harmony with the goals of participatory environ-
mental citizenship, which seeks to engage people in actively learning about, moni-
toring and managing their surroundings. According to both ecological psychology
and this view of participation, people flourish more fully when they have a rich
range of opportunities to realise their capabilities, and their capabilities include
seeing the environment accurately and knowing how to take effective action in
102 L. Chawla
response. For all of these reasons, ecological psychology forms a sound theoretical
foundation for environmental education.
Although it gives due attention to the intrinsic qualities of the physical world,
ecological psychology does not deny the social and cultural dimensions of action.
As Reed (1996a) has noted, humans have evolved very special modes of action and
awareness that are highly interactive and imbued with symbolism. At around six
months of age, children begin to pay attention to features of the environment to
which another person is attending. This achievement of joint attention forms a basis
for all subsequent instruction and learning. From this time on, whether others are
teaching deliberately or by chance example, most of children’s discoveries build on
involvement with others who direct their attention to particular features of their
surroundings. At first, these features are primarily concrete, but as children grow,
they increasingly include abstract relationships and ideas. Barbara Rogoff (1990)
describes this way of learning through joint attention as a series of apprenticeships
with people who are more experienced in different domains. This point, too, is vital
for environmental education.
These principles of ecological psychology suggest that the following conditions
support the development of children’s environmental awareness and competence:
Affordances that promote discovery and responsive person/environment
relationships
Access and mobility to engage with affordances
Perceptual learning to notice and value the environment
Opportunities to take responsible roles in community settings
The following sections of this chapter will take up each of these conditions in turn
and suggest their implications for environmental education.
6.4 Affordances that Promote Discovery
People are most likely to continue to engage with the environment when they
receive immediate information in response to their actions. Even infants, who are
so limited in their ability to act on the world, seek to catch their mother’s eye and
if she smiles back, a coordinated exchange begins in which the infant’s wiggles and
kicks activate its mother’s encouragement (Stern 1985). This relationship is equally
true in interactions with the physical environment. Rovee-Collier (1986), for exam-
ple, compared infants as young as two months under two conditions. One set of
infants had a cord from a mobile that hung over their crib tied to their ankle, so that
they could make the mobile move by kicking. For another set of infants, the mobile
was moved by the experimenter. When the infants caused the mobile’s movement,
their rate of kicking increased. When their kicking had no effect, it rapidly declined.
The infants were motivated to continue to engage with the mobile when they could
see themselves producing effects by their own actions.
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 103
This reciprocity between self-produced action and environmental events lies at the
heart of the development of a sense of competence (Bandura 1997). For this reason,
Fuglesang and Chandler (1997) argue that responsive early childhood programmes
and parent training to increase interactive experiences of this kind are important
precursors to children’s readiness for more formal types of participation. In research
with adolescents and adults, this experience of self-efficacy promotes learned opti-
mism (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi 2000) and life satisfaction (Ryan and Deci
2001). As Bandura (1997) has noted, to inspire people to continue working towards
goals, nothing is as motivating as ‘mastery experiences’ of their own capability.
Environmental features vary, however, in the degree to which they provide these
effects (Heft 1988; Kytta 2002). For ball play, rollerblading, or cycling, nothing is
better than hard, flat surfaces. For most other actions, children need a more mallea-
ble world. Natural elements are particularly rich in the effects they offer, and what
is more, although they are predictable to a point, they also offer intriguing surprises.
Water flows and splashes in fluid forms. Soft earth can be dug and moulded in infi-
nite iterations. Every rock has a different heft and shape for building. Every rotting
log reveals a different universe of insect life inside. Convivial city streets and public
spaces offer similarly responsive settings for social interactions, which – like natu-
ral elements – never do exactly the same thing twice.
In environmental autobiographies, childhood places that afford experiences of
this kind resonate in memory. Adults’ accounts of special childhood places show
that they tend to be on the margins of the adult world, where children can find free-
dom to take risks and prove themselves, get dirty, make and unmake play worlds
with abandon and interact intensively both with the physical world and with friends
(Cooper Marcus 1978; Goodenough 2003). These sites include tree houses, play
forts, creek banks, and overgrown lots, colonised by children on the edge of the
adult world in both cities and rural areas. They also include public spaces where
children feel welcomed by adults. Ethnographic work with contemporary children
shows that these opportunities for interactive engagement still figure prominently
in children’s choices of favourite places (Chawla 1992, 2002).
When environmental activists and environmental educators are asked about their
sources of commitment to their work, one of the most frequent reasons that they
give is the experience of natural areas of this kind in childhood or adolescence
(Tanner 1998). Not only is the natural world responsive, but it yields alluring sen-
sory information. This quality is evoked by the words of an Australian teacher in a
study that asked environmental educators in nine countries to write about signifi-
cant experiences that led to their environmental concern:
Sensory flashes of childhood: being swooped by magpies; listening to the rain on our tin
roof; smelling pine needles under shadowy trees; building cubbies from bracken fern; car-
ing for and sharing life with pets; awe-inspiring thunderstorms; the taste, smell and chill of
the sea. (Quoted in Palmer et al. 1999:184.)
Similarly, a study of citizens in England revealed that memories of nature play in
childhood emerged as a major reason for people’s concern to preserve parkland and
wild areas (Harrison et al. 1987).
104 L. Chawla
When environmental educators and activists describe their formative experiences,
they mention natural areas outside school more often than schools. Yet schools can
provide these experiences too if they turn their yards into natural habitats. An evalu-
ation of these changes at a California elementary school showed that children value
what they gain. The following statements are representative of 50 students who
were interviewed about their reactions:
When I see another school I think, too bad, they’ve just got a cement yard and we’ve got
trees and a river and ponds with fishes, frogs, tadpoles, snakes and a turtle.
You would never say, “Let’s go outside and learn about a cement yard.” Now there is
always something new to find out.
I know how it feels to have ponds. I know what lives there. I’ve seen the way ponds
change. (Quoted in Moore 1989:203, 205.)
This emphasis on affordances that promote discovery owing to their responsive
nature also applies to the design of participatory projects with children. The
wheels of community change often grind slowly and the realisation of children’s
recommendations often depends on many other stakeholders. It is therefore
important to build in a sequence of goals, from those that are securely within a
group’s own power to more ambitious distant goals. For example, while children
are working on the clean-up of a local stream, they can restore a wetland in a
corner of their school grounds.
6.5 Access and Mobility
No matter how rich the array of affordances may be in children’s surroundings, they
are of little consequence unless children can reach them. As Kytta (2004) has
observed in her study of affordances for children in a variety of communities in
Finland and Belarus, the best places for children provide ‘positive interactive
cycles’. In this case, children enjoy independent mobility to explore their surroundings
and when they get out, they discover responsive affordances. As a consequence,
they feel motivated to explore further and in doing so, they discover more. In the
process, they build a growing repertoire of environmental competencies.
Children themselves consider freedom to move about safely an important meas-
ure of a good place in which to grow up (Chawla 2002). In contrast, studies from
industrialised nations around the world indicate that children’s independent mobil-
ity and access to natural areas is eroding. In a world of rapid urbanisation and rising
populations, a number of barriers impede access – the hazards of automobile traffic,
crime and pollution, parents’ fears of strangers and children’s fears of bullies (see,
for example, Chawla 2002; Rissotto and Tonucci 2002; Kytta 2004). In one of the
most systematic studies, Gaster (1991) examined children’s free play in northern
Manhattan from 1910 to 1980. He found vanishing natural play areas and, since
the 1940s, a decrease in the number of places that children visited, combined with
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 105
a steady increase in the age at which they were first allowed out alone and in the
amount of adult-supervised play. Recent studies of children’s free range show that
it is not unusual for urban children to stay within their block or immediate housing
site, even in adolescence (Chawla 2002).
Considering how often people cite childhood play in nature as a reason for envi-
ronmental concern and commitment, provision for environmental learning needs to
include a broad vision of urban planning. In addition to naturalising schoolyards,
much can be done to increase children’s free access to nature even in densely built
cities. Nature can be brought near through landscaping, site design, and affordable
and secure public transportation to resources like parks and ecological reserves
(Chawla and Salvadori 2003; Louv 2005).
6.6 Perceptual Learning
Children are rarely alone as they detect information present in their surroundings.
Even if no other person is nearby, children exist in worlds that are structured by
others. Often it is a structure that is deliberately designed to guide children’s behav-
iour such as a toy placed near at hand or the bars of a crib to contain movement. In
other words, even when children enjoy fields of free action where they explore
autonomously, they usually move within fields of promoted action or constrained
action, where a distinction between the physical and social world is artificial (Reed
1996a; Kytta 2004).
When adults and other children are nearby, they often collaborate to help a nov-
ice child learn a new activity or joint task. By placing resources within a child’s
reach, by direct aid or by example, they ‘scaffold’ learning (Vygotsky 1978). In a
variety of ways, the principle function played by more experienced people is to
direct the learner’s attention to relevant features of the task. They may do this by
verbal instruction, but what the guide knows is often tacit and not easily articulated
in words. In this case she may point, position an object so that the critical feature
will be noticed, or simply say ‘look!’ or ‘listen!’. These are the conditions of
apprenticeship in the broadest sense and the contexts for social learning that is so
characteristic of human beings (Rogoff 1990, 2003). They are also the conditions
for perceptual learning, which involves becoming selectively attuned to particular
information out of a larger field of potential information.
Representations of the environment in books and on television and computer
screens can contribute to this process of learning to see, but Reed (1996b) argues
that they can never replace the role of direct experience. Primary or first-hand
experience of the world exposes a person to inexhaustible possibilities for learning,
and the richer the environment, the richer the possibilities. A person outdoors
encounters a dynamic, dense, multisensory flow of diversely structured informa-
tion. In secondary experience, when people learn about the world second-hand
106 L. Chawla
through texts, images or someone else’s story, this information is radically reduced.
The realm of full-bodied primary experience is also where people form personal
relationships and place attachments – where they find people and places to care for
and others to join with them in action. Despite the growing importance of the inter-
net for political and environmental organising, people who work to defend wild
places report that they draw strength from their bonds with real places and from
face-to-face networks of supportive people (Zavestoski 2003).
The power of simply drawing attention to elements of the natural world in an
appreciative way is suggested by the fact that another reason given by environ-
mental educators and activists for their concern and commitment, often men-
tioned as frequently as a special childhood place, is an influential role model such
as a parent or other family member (Tanner 1998). What people recall are rarely
didactic messages such as ‘you ought to protect wild areas’. Instead, they remem-
ber people who drew their attention to the natural world as something worth valu-
ing. As a Norwegian biologist who fought against the damming of wild rivers
recalled, everyone in Norway in the 1950s went hiking, berry picking, and fish-
ing. What distinguished her family was that, ‘My mother knew the names of the
plants more than other mothers did. So we talked more deeply about things. We
didn’t only fetch berries and fish, but talked about it’ (quoted in Chawla 1999:20).
Sometimes this instruction was wordless. Another activist, who fought against
the damming of a wild river in Kentucky, tried to understand why he was differ-
ent than others of his age who also grew up hunting and fishing. He described a
father who, ‘could teach you how to make a willow whistle or a pop gun out of
certain things or how to find the fishing bait under the rocks and appreciate
what’s there. Or who takes you out on the porch when a thunderstorm comes in
so you could enjoy it’ (quoted in Chawla 1999:20). This combination of special
childhood places and people is exactly what the naturalist Rachel Carson advised
for an undying sense of wonder. In addition to abundant time in the natural world,
she wrote that each child, ‘needs the companionship of at least one adult who can
share it, rediscovering with him the joy, excitement and mystery of the world we
live in’ (Carson 1956:45).
Certainly, teachers too can direct children’s attention to the natural world.
Although time in nature in childhood and influential family members or friends
are the most frequent reasons that environmental activists and educators give for
their concern and commitment; education is another reason mentioned, often
third or fourth in frequency (Tanner 1998). At their best, schools use the processes
of scaffolding, directed attention, and apprenticeship to encourage children to
notice the environment in new ways and value what they see. However these
concepts point to the importance of fieldwork beyond the classroom, where
children can be surrounded by the world that they are learning to observe with
closer attention. Because teachers can rarely equal the influence of parents or
other family members in a child’s life, schools should also reach out to families,
communicating the importance of sharing appreciation for the natural world and
seeking to include family members in environmental education activities as often
as possible.
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 107
6.7 Participation in Community Settings
Aldo Leopold (1949/1966) remarked that as people learn to notice places around
them with a keener eye, they begin to see wounds on the landscape. As young peo-
ple discover problems, if they are to believe that they can help solve them, they need
to feel like a part of their community and find channels open to them to participate
in environmental decisions. Another tool from ecological psychology – the concept
of behaviour settings developed by Roger Barker – provides a means to assess
opportunities for individuals to participate in the social life of their community and
to play a role in charting its direction (Barker 1968; Schoggen 1989).
Behaviour settings are places where individuals gather to engage in coordi-
nated activities. They are constituted by individuals’ actions together, as well
as the affordances of the place that make these actions possible (Heft 1989,
2001). Like affordances, behaviour settings are real physical entities with per-
ceptually meaningful features, for example, a gathering of an environmental
organisation at a particular time and place. They are also similar to affordances
because some behaviour settings are more responsive than others to an individ-
ual’s actions and, in this way, offer better support for the development of
competence.
Barker (1968) distinguished different ‘zones of penetration’ that an individual
can occupy in a setting, which correspond to varying levels of involvement and
responsibility in contributing to a group’s goals:
1. Onlooker, who takes no active role in the setting
2. Audience, who has a recognised role, but very little power
3. Member, who has potential rather than immediate power, such as an organisation
member who is eligible to vote for officers
4. Active functionary, with power over part of the setting, such as someone engaged
in group activities like conducting a bird count or monitoring a stream
5. Joint leader, with immediate but shared power over the entire setting
6. Single leader, with immediate and sole authority over the operations of the
setting
Because the concentrated power of a single leader violates the spirit of participa-
tory decision-making, its presence in a programme is a sign of dysfunction.
Otherwise, the deeper that young people penetrate into a setting, from peripheral
levels that primarily involve observation to central levels with considerable influ-
ence over activities, the more engagement and responsibility they take on and the
correspondingly greater opportunities they enjoy to develop a range of
competencies.
Research in large and small schools (Barker and Gump 1964) and different com-
munities (Barker and Schoggen 1973) has shown that the availability of responsible
positions affects the likelihood that children will take on multiple roles and experi-
ence leadership. This research provides a model for how the idea of zones of pene-
tration could be applied in planning and evaluating participatory programmes with
108 L. Chawla
children. It shows how to identify existing behaviour settings where children may
already be playing active roles in their community and where action for the envi-
ronment might be introduced. It also indicates the importance of planning
programmes that give positions of influence to as many children as possible. For
example, a weekly child-run radio programme on community issues requires a
large number of ongoing roles such as researchers, reporters, informants, editors,
and technicians. Child-to-child teaching, such as high school students preparing
and presenting environmental lessons to elementary school students or older chil-
dren leading younger children on field trips, provides other examples. In addition
to creating many responsible roles for young people to fill, activities of this kind
create many occasions for apprenticeships, not only between novice children and
more experienced children, but also between child leaders and adult facilitators.
Thus programmes of this kind provide key ingredients for the development of com-
petence: role models who include peers and encouraging adults, guided participation
in fields of promoted action, opportunities to influence the setting, and experiences
of achievement.
In two comprehensive reviews of what motivates people to take action for the
environment, some similar variables emerge. In the analysis of Hungerford and
Volk (1990), ‘environmental sensitivity’ is the major entry-level variable that
predicts responsible environmental behaviour and it is derived from significant
life experiences such as positive experiences of the natural world in childhood,
and early role models who communicate nature’s value. Other critical variables
include a personal investment in issues, knowledge and skill in environmental
action strategies, and a belief that one’s actions can have an effect. In a review
by Stern (2000), important variables include biospheric values (care for plants,
animals, and communities of living things) and the self-perceived ability to
reduce threats to the environment. In both analyses, valuing the natural world and
confidence in one’s own ability to make a difference on its behalf are critical
predictors of responsible action.
These conclusions correspond to the ecological view of human develop-
ment presented in this chapter. Ecological psychology presents a theoretical
rationale for the importance of enabling children to know the natural world
through their own exploration of it and for the company of people who direct
their attention to other living things in a way respectful of their inherent
value. It also indicates the importance of providing children with opportunities
to take increasing levels of responsibility for environmental decision- making
and action, and thus develop their competences. In this way, children find
occasions to develop confidence in their capabilities, including their ability to
join with others for collective action, as they follow issues of their own interest
and concern.
Acknowledgment This chapter owes a great debt to collaboration with the ecological psycholo-
gist Harry Heft. It draws on ideas that we have developed together in Chawla and Heft (2001) and
Heft and Chawla (2005), and on Heft’s clear articulation of the work of James and Eleanor Gibson
and Roger Barker. It has also benefited from his careful reading and suggestions.
6 Ecology of Environmental Awareness and Action 109
References
Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Barker, R. (1968) Ecological Psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Barker, R. and Gump, P. (1964) Big School, Small School. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Barker, R. and Schoggen, P. (1973) Qualities of Community Life. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Breiting, S. and Mogensen, F. (1999) Action competence and environmental education.
Cambridge Journal of Education, 29, 3: 349–353.
Carson, R. (1956) The Sense of Wonder. New York: Harper & Row.
Chawla, L. (1992) Childhood place attachments. In: Altman, I. and Low, S. (eds), Place
Attachment. New York: Plenum, pp. 63–86.
Chawla, L. (1999) Life paths into effective environmental action. Journal of Environmental
Education, 31, 1: 15–26.
Chawla, L. (ed) (2002) Growing Up in an Urbanising World. London/Paris: Earthscan
Publications/UNESCO.
Chawla, L. and Heft, H. (2001) Children’s competence and the ecology of communities: a func-
tional approach to the evaluation of participation. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
22: 201–216.
Chawla, L. and Salvadori, I. (2003) Children for cities and cities for children: learning to know
and care about urban ecosystems. In: Berkowitz, A., Nilon, C., and Hollweg, K. (eds),
Understanding Urban Ecosystems: A New Frontier for Science and Education. New York:
Springer, pp. 294–314.
Cooper Marcus, C. (1978) Remembrance of landscapes past. Landscape, 22, 3: 34–43.
Fuglesang, A. and Chandler, D. (1997) Children’s Participation: A Case for a Strategy of
Empowerment in Early Childhood. Oslo: Save the Children Norway.
Gaster, S. (1991) Urban children’s access to their neighbourhoods. Environment and Behavior,
23, 1: 70–85.
Gibson, E. and Pick, A.D. (2000) An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Leaning and
Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gibson, J.J. (1966) The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA:
Houghton-Mifflin.
Gibson, J. J (1979) The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Goodenough, E. (ed) (2003) Secret Spaces of Childhood. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
Press.
Harrison, C., Limb, M., and Burgess, J. (1987) Nature in the city: popular values for a living
world. Journal of Environmental Management, 25: 347–362.
Heft, H. (1988) Affordances of children’s environments: a functional approach to environmental
description, Children’s Environments Quarterly, 5: 29–37.
Heft, H. (1989) Affordances and the body: an intentional analysis of Gibson’s ecological approach
to visual perception. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 19: 1–30.
Heft, H. (2001) Ecological Psychology in Context. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Heft, H. and Chawla, L. (2005) Children as agents in sustainable development: the ecology of
competence. In: Spencer, C. and Blades, M. (eds), Children and Their Environments.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 199–216.
Hungerford, H. and Volk, T. (1990) Changing learner behavior through environmental education.
Journal of Environmental Education, 21, 3: 8–21.
Jensen, B.B. and Schnack, K. (1997) The action competence approach in environmental educa-
tion. Environmental Education Research, 3, 2: 163–178.
Kytta, M. (2002) Affordances of children’s environments in the context of cities, small towns,
suburbs and rural villages in Finland and Belarus. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
22: 109–123.
110 L. Chawla
Kytta, M. (2004) The extent of children’s independent mobility and the number of actualized
affordances as criteria for child-friendly environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
24: 179–198.
Leopold, A. (1949/1966) A Sand County Almanac. New York: Oxford University Press.
(Originally published in 1949.)
Louv, R. (2005) Last Child in the Woods. Chapel Hill, NC: Algonquin Books.
Moore, R. (1989) Before and after asphalt: diversity as an ecological measure of quality in chil-
dren’s outdoor environments. In: Bloch, M. and Pelligrini, T. (eds), The Ecological Context of
Children’s Play. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 191–213.
Palmer, J., Suggate, J., Robottom, I., and Hart, P. (1999) Significant life experiences and forma-
tive influences on the development of adults’ environmental awareness in the UK, Australia
and Canada. Environmental Education Research, 5, 2: 181–200.
Reed, E.S. (1996a) Encountering the World. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Reed, E.S. (1996b) The Necessity of Experience. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Rissotto, A. and Tonucci, F. (2002) Freedom of movement and environmental knowledge in ele-
mentary school children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22: 65–77.
Rovee-Collier, C.K. (1986) The rise and fall of infant classical conditioning research. In: Lipsett, L.P.
and Rovee-Collier, C.K. (eds), Advances in Infancy Research, 4: 139–159. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rogoff, B. (1990) Apprenticeship in Thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, B. (2003) The Cultural Nature of Human Development. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2001) On happiness and human potentials. In: Fiske, S.T., Schachter,
D.L., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (eds), Annual Review of Psychology, 52: 141–166. Palo Alto, CA:
Annual Reviews.
Seligman, M. and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000) Positive psychology: an introduction. American
Psychologist, 55: 5–14.
Schoggen, P. (1989) Behavior Settings. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Stern, D. (1985) The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books.
Stern, P.C. (2000) Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. Journal of
Social Issues, 56, 3: 407–424.
Tanner, T. (ed) (1998) Special issue on significant life experiences. Environmental Education
Research, 4, 4: pp. 365–463.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978) Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Zavestoski, S. (2003) Constructing and maintaining ecological identities. In: Clayton, S. and
Opotow, S. (eds), Identity and the Natural Environment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
pp. 297–315.
... Çevre eğitimi, çevreye duyarlı bireylerin yetişmesine hizmet etme, çevreyle ilgili konulara aktif katılımı sağlama, koruma, sürdürülebilir kalkınma, sorumluluk alma ve çevresel farkındalık geliştirme gibi konuları içinde barındırır (Taylor ve ark. 1998, Chawla 2007, Ağyar 2014. Çevre eğitiminin amacı bireylerin hem çevreyle ilgili bilgileri almasını hem de bireylerde çevreye yönelik tutumlarının gelişmesini ve bu tutumların da davranışa dönüşmesini sağlamaktır (Erten 2003). ...
... Yapılan çalışmalar, yetişkinlik döneminde çevreye olan duyarlılık, bireylerin çocukluk dönemlerinde doğa ile kurdukları bağla ilgili olduğunu göstermiştir (Taylor ve ark. 1998, Chawla 2007, Ağyar 2014. Çocuklarda çevre farkındalığı yaratmak için en etkili yol, onları çevre ile erken yaşta tanıştırmaktır. ...
Article
Full-text available
Çevre eğitimi, doğal çevrenin korunması ve çocuğun gelişimi için oldukça önemlidir. Çevre ve dış mekanlar, çocuklar için yaratıcılığının gelişiminin yanında eğlence dolu bir ortam oluşturması nedeniyle ona sahip çıkmanın heyecanını ve sorumluluğunu çocuklara yaşatır. Bu araştırmanın amacı gelecek nesillere çevre bilincini taşımada, olumsuz tutumların ve duyguların önlenmesinde, koruyucu alışkanlıkların geliştirilmesinin sağlanmasında çevre ve eğitimin önemini araştırmaktır. Çalışmada, araştırma alanı olarak Emirgan Parkı’nda yer alan çocuk oyun alanları ve çevresi seçilmiş ve alanda çevre eğitiminde çocuk oyun alanlarının ve çevre okuryazarlığının önemini analiz etmek için anket yöntemi kullanılmıştır. Çevre okuryazarlığı kriterleri olan bilgi-tutum-davranış arasındaki ilişkiler, cinsiyet, yaş, sosyo-ekonomik yapı ile birlikte çalışma alanında bulunan eksiklikler vb. bilgiler değerlendirilmiş ve çevre bilincinin aşılanması için öneriler getirilmiştir.
... More than half of the topics presented showed that island schoolers are more knowledgeable as compared to the counterparts in upland and urban areas. They are specifically aware on corals reefs, crabs, whale shark (butanding) and aquatic invertebrates and this can be attributed to the information being transferred from parents, friends and communities or in most cases these are the resources that the children in the island usually observed when they are in coastal areas or are playing along the beach (Chawla, 2008). On the other hand, scallops, seaweeds and shrimps are the remarkable topics where upland and urban pupils are most aware of as compared to island children and may be attributed media outlets-as these species are usually seen or are contents of advertisements. ...
Article
Full-text available
Knowledge and awareness on the natural resources among elementary and high school students are important attributes for future resource conservation and management. However, the enslaving features of gadgets and technologies has shifted the focus of the students away from nature-based knowledge and has motivated the government to integrate environmental education (EE) to the basic education programs. However, its implementation has not been successful due to lack of teaching materials and limits on teachers’ capabilities. As such, it was found out that knowledge of elementary, junior high school (JHS) and senior high school (SHS) students on the native and unique natural resources of Bicol Region are almost the same, with an average score of 40%. But intervention through the use of EE teaching materials developed from the research publications and reports of Bicol University Tabaco Campus faculty-researchers showed that an increase of 31%, 21%, and 28% have been observed for elementary, JHS, and SHS students, respectively. However, a significant difference (p<0.05) was observed between high schoolers from urban setting as compared to the upland and island students with regards to the prior knowledge on natural resources, on the other hand, significant change was also observed in the increase of knowledge in upland (30% increase for JHS, and 42% for SHS) and island students (22% and 27% for JHS and SHS, respectively) as compared to the urban students (12% and 17% for JHS and SHS, respectively). No differences were observed for the elementary students across sites. Several factors were identified to influence knowledge and awareness of students such as, access to internet and mass media, exposure to local situations and prior knowledge, and influence of family and community. Overall, knowledge and awareness of elementary and high school students on the local natural resources are relatively low but existence of local and relevant EE learning materials can help increase them. It is therefore, recommended that EE through the KADUNUNG program should be institutionalized to improve knowledge and awareness of students in Tabaco City.
... The stated Objective of the programs also influences the participants' perspectives about the objective (Fenwick 2006). We found both considerations affect the diversity and intensity of young people's participation, and thereby opportunities to build the skills needed to engage with their learning environment (Fazey et al. 2007;Chawla 2008;Roth, Lee, and Hsu 2009;Krasny and Roth 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated youth participation in three Community and Citizen Science (CCS) programs led by natural history museums in out-of-school settings. Using second generation Activity Theory, we looked at repeated participation over time, collecting and then qualitatively analyzing ethnographic fieldnote observations on focal youth participation and components of the activity systems. We found each program provided multiple and unique access points for youth to participate in environmental science. Further, when facilitators emphasized the scientific goals of the programs clearly and repeatedly, youth participation in the scientific processes of the CCS programs deepened. Access to scientific tools, facilitation in using them, and repeatedly applying them in authentic research, enabled youth to participate in different aspects of CCS, from exploring to submitting biological data. Repeated participation in CCS activities provided the opportunities for youth to try the same type of participation multiple times (intensification), as well as provided the opportunity for youth to try different types of participation (diversification). Our findings suggest that repeated participation in authentic scientific research in CCS contexts fosters youth development of new roles and possible development of environmental science identities.
... Its main task was to create and disseminate models of individual and group behaviour in society, capable of introducing and maintaining harmony between society and the environment (Papuziński, 1998). Therefore, most attention in scientific research is focussed to the role of environmental education in shaping ecological awareness (Ardoin and Bowers, 2020;Chawla, 2008;Corraliza and Collado, 2019;Şahin et al., 2018;Schleicher, 1989). ...
Article
Dynamic and unrestrained socio-economic development is upsetting the balance of nature’s mechanisms, causing a climate stalemate, or even climate destabilisation. After the Second World War a new political system – real socialism – was enforced on Poland. It brought about changes of a social, cultural, economic and environmental nature. Its immanent feature was the application of top-down decisions that did not take into account environmental components. There was also little ecological awareness within Polish society at that time. The transformations of the 1990s resulted not only in the liberalisation of the Polish economy, but also in the permeation of new trends oriented towards pro-environmental activities. The aim of the article is to find an answer to the question: How is ecological awareness currently shaped in the context of Anthropocene in Poland during the transition from a socialist economy to a capitalist economic system?
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: The main goal of architectural design is to create high-quality, comfortable spaces for people, ensuring that the area has a favourable effect on them. Studies on learning environments have produced broad guidelines for school design that should accommodate a variety of learning modalities (LM) in order to attain high-quality learning and teaching. The primary objective of the research is to expand Post-Occupancy Evaluation (POE) studies for school building assessments by using the notion of Learning Modalities. Methods: One crucial component for environmental comfort for educational institutions is the space's teaching role. Encouraging education by combining environmental sciences with artistic endeavours is a fun and inventive approach to get young people interested in both science and art. The results of a learning endeavour that students in Nanjing completed served as the basis for this investigation. The initiative included carrying out a number of activities that explored ideas pertaining to science, art, and solidarity. Results: The project's efficacy was evaluated based on the level of student participation, the calibre of the exchanges, and the final outputs. As per the students' perspectives, their involvement in this project was thrilling and enhanced their comprehension and enthusiasm towards environmental matters. Conclusion: This research emphasises how crucial it is to use artistic endeavours as a means of teaching and learning environmental science. This work attempts to examine educational research articles that have been published in the field of environmental artistic instruction with the goal of identifying the most influential element and shedding light on how it might enhance the policies put in place.
Article
Full-text available
In research, the implementation of participatory environmentalism based on human ecology can overcome the four problems that will be analyzed and solved through the TS (tourism sector) and ES (economic sector) programs. the implementation of participatory environmentalism emphasizes how important it is to be an independent society in the ES (economic sector) by utilizing its natural wealth primarily in the maritime sector, and the importance of participation from various parties to share space with nature, caring for sea and air ecosystems. What is expected from the program is the creation of an ecological 'balance' between humans and nature, as well as the establishment of an 'independent society' in terms of economy and health. In developing the TS (tourism sector) the program is carried out through several important steps, such as making a grand plan, seeking outsourcing mediation, implementing knowledge management, capacity building. Community empowerment program with the development of TS and ES based on the tourism sector and the economic sector. The tourism sector is able to revive the economy of the surrounding community. The tourism sector is also positioned as an important means of introducing the culture and natural beauty of the area concerned. The tourism sector is a source of income that can be continuously renewed and rejuvenated, this form of rejuvenation of tourist areas can be in the form of renovations and regular maintenance. The economic sector in the tourism sector is a long economic chain (Multiplier effect), starting from travel agencies, transportation services, hotels, restaurants, scouting activities, folk crafts, maintenance of tourist objects and so on. Furthermore, the economic sector will also require agricultural, livestock, fishery products and a number of workers can also be absorbed in it as a support for the success of this chain.
Article
Individuals increasingly rely on digital media technologies to learn about and access new information. Yet, despite the dramatic rise in electronic media use among youth and adults, our understanding of its impact, opportunities, and challenges within environmental education remains limited, especially in home contexts. Using a learning ecology framework, we conducted a two-week diary study with 58 diverse adult caretakers, asking them to engage in environmental learning moments with their children. Drawing on qualitative reports of those moments, we found that families relied heavily on digital technologies to support their child’s environmental learning, frequently to supplement other activities or spark rich discussions. Findings illustrate how the digitalization of environmental education can extend beyond passive screen time. Overall, this study reveals the critical role of family conversations and shared digital technologies in supporting environmental learning.
Article
Full-text available
Environmental art education is gaining importance in schools as arts education begins to acquire a more significant role in environmental education. This emerging field of study is an interdisciplinary endeavor that is centered on the different fields of environmental education and visual art education and provides a means of making students aware of environmental issues through environmental art education. It has been suggested that students get into a relationship with nature prior to the request to conserve the environment in order to be nature connected. This abstract focuses on teaching and learning through the arts, a pedagogical way in which students discuss the challenging aspects of environmental issues. The aim of this study is to make students act like protectors of their environment through an eco-art place-based curriculum. This paper’s pedagogies will provide educators with a framework for developing environmental art education lessons and curricula. This experimental study has been planned to gather data from interviews and observation of students and by making the students participate in nature-related activities. The findings show that students prefer to let go of their fascination with formulating better ecological perspectives. On the positive side, a few students went through some frustration during the program and the activities. Students have given positive feedback on the program in positive terms, such as “fun”, “interesting”, and “cool”, to express their experience gained through the class activities.
Article
Full-text available
The great challenge of the twenty-first century may well be achieving sustainable development – which is ‘development that meets the needs of the present without compromising the ability of future generations to meet their needs’ (WCED, 1987: 8). Children stand at the heart of this definition in two respects. First, concern for future generations, which takes form in each new cohort of children, motivates development of this kind. Second, if practices consistent with sustainable development are to be carried forward through time, then children must be the bridge conveying their value and ways. For these reasons, many municipal governments and agencies that work with children are currently experimenting with approaches to integrate children into environmental planning. What is lacking in these efforts, however, is a coherent theoretical framework for investigating the question that these practical initiatives raise: what experiences prepare children to value and care for their local environment and join in community decision-making? Although there have been many surveys of young people's environmental attitudes and knowledge, much less is known about environmental learning as children engage with their localities, or about how children learn to take collaborative action on behalf of the places where they live (Rickinson, 2001). Drawing on ideas in ecological psychology, we propose a framework for research on this topic. We submit that one impediment to advances on this front resides in dominant assumptions about the nature of perceiving and cognition.
Chapter
The often impassioned nature of environmental conflicts can be attributed to the fact that they are bound up with our sense of personal and social identity. Environmental identity—how we orient ourselves to the natural world—leads us to personalize abstract global issues and take action (or not) according to our sense of who we are. We may know about the greenhouse effect—but can we give up our SUV for a more fuel-efficient car? Understanding this psychological connection can lead to more effective pro-environmental policymaking. Identity and the Natural Environment examines the ways in which our sense of who we are affects our relationship with nature, and vice versa. This book brings together cutting-edge work on the topic of identity and the environment, sampling the variety and energy of this emerging field but also placing it within a descriptive framework. These theory-based, empirical studies locate environmental identity on a continuum of social influence, and the book is divided into three sections reflecting minimal, moderate, or strong social influence. Throughout, the contributors focus on the interplay between social and environmental forces; as one local activist says, "We don't know if we're organizing communities to plant trees, or planting trees to organize communities."
Article
A study of the environmental biographies of architecture and landscape architecture students, and descriptions of the emotions evoked by favourite childhood place leads to a criticism of suburban life. Very few students remembered suburban environments with any pleasure. Suburban children opted either for the city or the real country. The Dutch are praised for their incorporation of wild landscape into their housing projects. Careful thought is needed to incorporate opportunities for environmental experience. -J.Johnson