ArticlePDF Available

Images of Women in Advertisements: Effects on Attitudes Related to Sexual Aggression

Authors:

Abstract

While the power of advertisements has long been known, investigations of sociocultural influences on sexual attitudes have been limited primarily to studies of sexually aggressive media. In this study we examined the effects on sexual attitudes of different portrayals of women in advertisements. Male and female white middle-class university students were exposed to one of three groups of advertisements. In one condition women were depicted as sex objects, in another in progressive or role-reversed roles, and a third condition comprised product oriented advertisements containing no human figures. Sexual attitudes were assessed using four subscales of Burt's Sexual Attitude Survey of 1980, a measure of attitudes believed to be rape-supportive, and conducive to sexual aggression against women. Before completing the Survey, subjects rated a series of advertisements on appeal and aesthetic dimensions. Whereas the product oriented advertisements were rated as more appealing than those featuring female figures, analyses showed that males exposed to the sex-object advertisements significantly more accepting of rape-supportive attitudes, and females exposed to the progressive female images were less accepting of such attitudes than were controls.
Images of women in advertisements: effects on attitudes related to sexual aggression
Date: 1995
Author: Kyra Lanis, Katherine Covell
Publication: Sex Roles: A Journal of Research. Volume: 32. Issue: 9-10
The power of advertising to persuade, manipulate, and shape behavior has long been
recognized. Bretl and Cantor (1988) estimated that the average American is exposed to
over 37,000 advertisements each year through the medium of television alone. Whereas
there has been considerable investigation of gender role portrayals in advertisements,
comparatively little empirical attention has been paid to the portrayal of sexuality in
advertisements. The purpose of this study was to systematically examine the effects of
advertisements in which women are presented in either a sexually provocative or a
non-traditional manner, on sexual attitudes supportive of sexual aggression. There are
both empirical and theoretical bases for supposing a link between images of women in
advertisements and sexual attitudes.
There is some evidence in the existing literature that the stereotypic portrayal in
advertisements of women as housewives and mothers has an impact on attitudes. For
example, findings show an association between high exposure to such advertising and (1)
increased public self-consciousness and social anxiety in women (Gould, 1987), (2) a
decrease in women's achievement aspirations (Geis, Brown, Jennings & Porter, 1984),
and (3) a decrease in women's interest in political participation (Schwartz, Wagner,
Bannert & Mathes, 1987). The power of these stereotyped messages is further
exemplified in the role they play in the socialization of young children (Macklin & Kolbe,
1984; Schwartz & Markham, 1985; Tucker, 1984). In part as a result of such findings,
there has been somewhat of a decrease in stereotypic gender role presentations of women
in advertisements.
Research findings indicate that although some gender role stereotyping still exists
(Livingstone & Green, 1986; Rak & McMullen, 1987), women are being depicted also in
a wider range of social and occupational roles (Bretl & Cantor, 1980; Ferrante, Haynes &
Kingsley, 1988; Sullivan & O'Connor, 1988). However, there has been a parallel increase
in the sexually exploitive use of women in advertisements. In fact Sullivan and O'Connor
(1988) found a 60% increase since 1970 in the portrayal of women in purely "decorative"
roles, whereby the presence of a physically attractive and sexy woman was unrelated to
the advertised product.
While youth, beauty and sexuality are stressed in advertisement depictions of women
(Downs & Harrison, 1985; Ferrante et al., 1988), increasingly the emphasis appears to be
on sexuality and sexual availability. Soly and Kurzbard (1986) described the increased
use of sexually provocative and suggestive content in advertising as approaching
pornography in some cases. In their analysis of magazine advertisements, Soly and
Kurzbard found that illustrations of sexuality were more overt than earlier
advertisements, and that the absolute number of advertisements containing sexual
elements had increased with female models being disproportionately more likely to be
portrayed as sexually clad, partially clad, or nude. Further, in contrast to previous
limitations of such advertisements to certain adult magazines, these were now common in
general interest magazines.
It may be argued that such widespread overt depictions of women as sex objects can be
expected to have an impact on general beliefs and attitudes. There is evidence that
women hold very negative attitudes toward such advertisement portrayals of women (de
Young & Crane, 1992). However, existing empirical evidence of the effects of female
images on sexual attitudes is limited to studies of various media portrayals (e.g., movies
and magazines) of sexual aggression (see Malamuth & Donnerstein, 1984 for a review).
Several authors (e.g., Brownmiller, 1975; Burt, 1980) have suggested that sexually
degrading media depictions of women promote rape-supportive attitudes and beliefs.
Similarly, Malamuth and Briere (1986) proposed an indirect effects model to explain the
contribution of socio-cultural forces to sexual aggression against women.
To assess the influence of sexist advertisement portrayals of women we used four
subscales from Burt's (1980.) Sexual Attitudes Survey that have been used widely to
gauge rape supportive beliefs. Attitudes so measured have been found to predict various
rape-related attitudes, perceptions, and behaviors in a variety of settings (e.g., Check &
Malamuth, 1983; Demare, Lips, & Briere, 1993; Malamuth, 1983, 1986; Malamuth &
Check, 1980; Malamuth, Check & Briere, 1986; Malamuth, Haber & Feshbach, 1980).
Similarly, it has been found that these scales can be used to differentiate sexually
aggressive from non-sexually aggressive males (Briere & Malamuth, 1983; Koss &
Leonard, 1984).
We hypothesized that if advertisement portrayals of women influence beliefs, then scores
on the Sexual Attitude Survey would be higher (i.e., more negative) following exposure
to advertisements in which women are portrayed as sex objects compared to those
showing women in progressive roles. We also expected that males and females would be
differentially affected in their gender role attitudes by the various portrayals of women in
advertisements.
METHOD
Subjects
The participants were 90 (45 males and 45 female) white middle-class undergraduate
students from the University of Toronto. Data from a further two students were discarded
due to their failure to follow experimental instructions. The mean age of the students was
22.8 years (range 17-39 years). The students were recruited from courses in a variety of
disciplines including the social and physical sciences, engineering, and humanities.
Participation was on a voluntary basis in response to a request for help with two brief
pilot studies. Prospective volunteers were told that they would be helping with a study in
consumer psychology in which they would be asked to make a series of judgments about
some advertisements, and a second study in which they would be assisting with the
normalization of a questionnaire being developed by a researcher in the psychology
department. This was done in order to minimize student awareness of the true purpose of
the study and thereby maximize its validity. Students were fully debriefed upon
completion. In order to assess if they thought the two parts of the study were connected,
each student was asked if s/he had any comments or questions and were asked to expand
on any statements made. Based on these post-experimental discussions, it appeared that
none of the participants had realized the connection.
The students were randomly assigned to one of three advertisement female image
conditions (sex object, progressive, and product/control), subject to the constraint that
there be an equal number of males and females in each condition.
Materials
The influence of advertising on sexual attitudes was measured using the four scales from
Burt's (1980) Sexual Attitude Survey. These scales were the 9-item Sex Role
Stereotyping scale (SRS), (example item: "there is something wrong with a woman who
does not want to marry and raise a family"); the 9-item Adversarial Sexual Beliefs scale
(ASB), (example item: "most women are sly and manipulating when they are out to
attract a man"); the 6-item Acceptance of Interpersonal Violence (AIV) scale, 5 items of
which referred specifically to violence against women (example item: "sometimes the
only way to turn a cold woman on is by using force"); and 11 items measuring Rape Myth
Acceptance (RMA) (example item: "any healthy women can resist a rapist if she really
wants to"). The 35 items were scored on a seven point scale ranging from strongly
disagree to strongly agree.
The stimuli used in this study were 30 full page colour advertisements taken from a
variety of current issues of men's, women's and general interest magazines (e.g.,
Cosmopolitan, Vogue, Sports Illustrated, Gentleman's Quarterly, Time, Newsweek). Print
media were selected to allow controlling for extraneous variables such as verbal content,
jingles, and story-line. Twenty-five advertisements in which women were depicted as sex
objects, and 25 in which women were in progressive roles were prerated by five male and
five female undergraduate psychology students who did not participate in the main study.
Sex object images were defined as being depictions of women as sexual beings whose
main function in the advertisement was to be erotically enticing. Progressive image
advertisements were defined as non-traditional role-reversed portrayals of women
performing a variety of competent social functions. In neither of these conditions were
there any other human figures in the advertisement. The judges who performed the
preratings were given the above definitions of sex object and progressive image
advertisements and asked to rate on a 9 point scale the extent to which each
advertisement was representative of both image types. Advertisements used were those
which were rated by every rater as 8 or 9 on the one scale (i.e., highly representative of
one image), and 1 or 2 on the other (i.e., not at all representative of the other). For
example, to be classified as a sex-object image, the advertisement must have been rated
an 8 or 9 as exemplifying the sex object image description, and at 1 or 2 as an exemplar
of the progressive image description. The advertisements in the control condition were
entirely product oriented and devoid of human figures, and were not expected to have any
influence on the attitude measures.
The advertisements, in each of the three conditions equally, were for similar products, for
example, automobiles, hair products, alcohol, cigarettes, and food products. Each
advertisement was mounted on a 5[inches] x 8[inches] card for presentation. The cards
were randomly ordered before each session.
Procedure
Materials, including separate consent and instruction sheets were given to participants
who were then left alone to complete the study. The experimenter remained absent to
avoid possible influence of a female authority figure.
For the first part of the study, the participants were asked to rate the effectiveness of each
of the advertisements they were given (in order) on a 5 point scale on the following
perceptual dimensions: graphical design, use of colour, amount of product information
presented, spatial layout, and general appeal. This general appeal rating was used to
determine if the different types of advertisements were inherently different in their appeal.
The other questions were used to maximize equivalent scanning of the advertisements
across conditions, and as a distractor from the true purpose of the experiment. Upon
completion of this task, the participants were instructed to read and sign a further consent
form and proceed with the second study. In the second part, the participants completed
the four scales of Burt's Sexual Attitude Survey.
RESULTS
To assess the comparability of the three types of advertisements, a one-way ANOVA was
performed on the general appeal ratings. This yielded a main effect of condition (F(2, 84)
= 7.50, p [less than] .01). The control advertisements were rated as more appealing (M =
33.87) than were the sex object (M = 30.33) and the progressive advertisements (M =
29.13). The maximum possible appeal score here was 50.
Separate 2 (gender) x 3 (advertisement) ANOVAs were performed on each of the Sexual
Attitude Survey scales. The means for each condition are presented in Table I.
On the RMA scale there was a main effect of gender F(1, 84) = 11.95, p [less than] .001,
and a two-way interaction between gender and advertisement, F(2, 84) = 7.69, p [less
than] .001. Post-hoc comparisons (all post-hocs were performed with Newman-Keuls
tests) showed that males in the sex object condition were more accepting of the rape myth
than were those in the control condition, and females in both the sex object and
progressive conditions were less accepting of the rape myth than were those in the control
condition.
Similarly on the SRS scale, there was a main effect for gender (F(1, 84) = 9.56, p [less
than] .01), and a gender x advertisement interaction (F(2, 84) = 5.82, p [less than] .01).
Post-hocs showed that males in the sex object condition, and females in the control
condition showed the highest scores.
Table I. Mean Score and Total (TOT) Sexual Attitude Scores as a
Function of Sex and Advertising Condition
Sex-object Progressive Control
Males
SRS 29.80(a) 25.53 23.27
ASB 27.47 24.07 22.33
AIV 18.07(a) 14.73 11.47
RMA 27.40(a) 23.80 21.00
TOT 102.73(a) 88.80 78.07
Females
SRS 20.00 17.27 26.07(a)
ASB 21.20 17.27 22.27
AIV 11.40 10.27 12.80
RMA 17.93 14.93 24.07(a)
TOT 70.53 59.73 85.20
a Significantly different (Newman-Keuls post hoe tests).
For the ASB scale, there was a main effect of gender, F(1, 84) = 11.04, p [less than] .01,
with males being more accepting of adversarial sexual beliefs than were women. For the
AIV scale, there was a main effect of gender, F(1, 84) = 11.48, p [less than] .001, and a
gender x advertisement interaction, F(2, 84) = 6.13, p [less than] .01. Post-hoc tests
showed that males in the sex object condition scored higher on AIV than all other groups.
In order to assess overall sexual attitude, an analysis was performed on the total score.
This yielded a main effect of gender, F(1, 84) = 17.37, p [less than] .001, and a gender x
advertisement interaction, F(2, 84) = 8.50, p [less than] .001. Post-hoc tests showed the
following. Males in the sex object condition scored significantly higher than males in the
control condition, and higher than females in all conditions. Further, males in the sex
object condition and males in the progressive image condition scored higher than did the
females in those conditions. Finally, females in the progressive image condition scored
lower than those in the control condition. These differences are illustrated in Fig. 1.
There were no other main effects or interactions.
DISCUSSION
The findings show support for our hypothesis that media portrayals of women can
influence sexual attitudes and beliefs. The data indicate that males who see print media
advertisements in which women are presented as sex objects are more likely to evidence
increased sex role stereotypic and rape myth beliefs, and are likely to be more accepting
of interpersonal violence (primarily against women), than are males exposed to other
types of advertisements. In contrast, among women, gender role stereotypic beliefs and
rape myth acceptance were actually lowered when they saw females in advertisements. It
appears that the images used in the experimental conditions may have brought to the
attention of the female subjects the issues of sexual exploitation of women in the media,
and perhaps the social alternatives available to women, thus decreasing their tolerance for
rape-supportive beliefs. In fact, females in the progressive conditions consistently
produced the lowest scores, and males in the sex object condition consistently produced
the highest scores, on attitudes supportive of sexual aggression. It is notable that such
effects were obtained among a well educated middle class sample by exposing them to
only 10 advertisements.
Exposing males to progressive images of women in advertisements seemed to have no
impact on attitudes. Of interest here are findings (Baron & Straus, 1984) that rape rates
correlate not only with circulation indices of pornography, but also with women's status:
U.S. states in which women had the most status and social power were those in which
Baron & Straus found the highest rates of sexual assault. Such findings have been
attributed to a real increase in rape incidents arising from a backlash against the feminist
movement, as well as from an increased availability of pornography or sexually exploitive
portrayals of women. The data from the males in our sample are not inconsistent with the
suggestion that attitudes supportive of sexual aggression may be elicited by seeing
women in non-traditional roles. And the findings from the females in our sample indicate
that women exposed to progressive models may be more intolerant of sex-stereotypic
beliefs and sexual aggression. In turn, this may indicate a greater likelihood of reporting
rapes. In today's society, not only are women being sexually exploited more (as reflected
in our sex-object condition), but their status is higher and they are competing with men in
the job market (as reflected in our progressive condition). We assume that for males these
two conditions, and perhaps their social analogues, increase sexually hostile attitudes
toward women. Women in both cases become less tolerant of these attitudes, and
presumably therefore less accepting of sexually aggressive behaviors. Further study is, of
course, necessary to test our assumptions. It would be helpful also in future research to
use a larger sample size than used here, and a sample that comprises the general
population rather than our specialized population of university students.
We remain aware also that our findings are of short-term attitudinal effects only.
However, it seems reasonable to assume that the commonality of advertisements such as
those we used in the sex object condition (in magazines, on television, public
transportation and so forth), provides frequent reinforcement of rape supportive attitudes
among males. There is indication in the literature (Corne, Briere, & Esses, 1992; Davis &
Bauserman, 1993) that repeated exposure to pornography socializes rape supportive
attitudes. Repeated exposure to advertisements in which women are presented as sex
objects also may socialize similar attitudes. Do these attitudes matter? It might be argued
that sexual attitudes alone, especially those engendered by advertisements, are not
predictive of actual sexually aggressive behaviors (c.f., Demare et al., 1993), and are
therefore of little importance. But attitudes do guide behaviors. And even the most subtle
presentation of gender biased information, for example the use of the generic masculine
noun and pronoun, can have a significant impact on social choices and behaviors (Briere
& Lanktree, 1983; see also Gallivan vs. Begg, 1991).
Why the antithetical effect of advertisements portraying women was obtained, and why
the attitudes of males in the progressive condition were not more positive is of interest for
further study, particularly since both male and females rated these advertisements as less
appealing than those of product orientation. We believe the effects of these images of
women may result from the current struggle for sexual equality and the existing power
structure.
By viewing women as exclusively sexual beings whose purpose is to sexually arouse and
gratify men, a power differential is created in which women generally are subordinate.
This power hierarchy may facilitate the perception of women as appropriate targets for
sexually aggressive behaviours. Conversely, being exposed to women in progressive,
traditionally male roles, may create a power differential in favor of women thus
threatening men by challenging their traditional dominance and putting them in direct
competition with women. In this context, attitudes supportive of sexual aggression might
be explained as an attempt to realign the shift in the balance of power. We are currently
examining this hypothesis by assessing reactions to advertisements in which men and
women are depicted as having equal social status.
Our findings underscore the need to be concerned about the narrowing gap between
pornography and the portrayal of females in advertisements. Whereas earlier research
showed a link between exposure to sexually violent media and attitudes supportive of
sexual aggression, this study showed that exposure to common advertisements in which
women are portrayed as sex-objects might be sufficient to increase hostile sexual attitudes
among males. On only one of the four scales used (adversarial sexual beliefs) did
advertisement content have no effect on the sexual attitudes measured. The power of
advertisements to manipulate attitudes of such importance to our interpersonal and
societal well being should not be underestimated. Advertisements are more pervasive
than pornography, and unlike pornography are presented in a socially acceptable
framework.
REFERENCES
Baron, L., & Straus, M. A. (1984). Sexual stratification, pornography and rape in the
United States. In N.M. Malamuth & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Pornography and sexual
aggression, Toronto: Academic Press.
Begg, I. (1991). Essay on man. Canadian Psychology, 32, 16-19.
Bretl, D. J., & Cantor, J. (1988). The portrayal of men and women in U.S. television
commercials: A recent content analysis and trends over 15 years. Sex Roles, 18, 595-609.
Briere, J., & Lanktree, C. (1983). Sex-role related effects of sex bias in language. Sex
Roles, 9, 625-632.
Briere, J., & Malamuth, N.M. (1983). Self-reported likelihood of sexually aggressive
behavior: Attitudinal versus sexual explanations. Journal of Research in Personality, 17,
315-323.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: Men, women and rape. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 38, 217-230.
Check, J. V. P., & Malamuth, N.M. (1983). Sex-role stereotyping and reactions to
depictions of stranger versus acquaintance rape. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 45, 344-356.
Corne, S., Briere, J., & Esses, L. M. (1992). Womens' attitudes and fantasies about rape
as a function of early exposure to pornography. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7,
454-461.
Davis, C. M., & Bauserman, R. (1993). Exposure to sexually explicit materials: An
attitude change perspective. Annual Review of Sex Research, 4, 121-209.
Demare, D., Lips, H. M., & Briere, J. (1993). Sexually violent pornography, anti-women
attitudes, and sexual aggression: A structural equation model. Journal of Research in
Personality, 27, 285-300.
DeYoung, S., & Crane, F. G. (1992). Females' attitudes toward the portrayal of women in
advertising: A Canadian Study. International Journal of Advertising 11, 249-255.
Downs, A. C., & Harrison, S. K. (1985). Embarrassing age spots or just plain ugly?
Physical attractiveness stereotyping as an instrument of sexism on American television
commercials. Sex Roles, 13, 9-19.
Ferrante, C. L., Haynes, A. M., & Kingsley, S. M. (1988). Images of women in television
advertising. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 32, 231-237.
Gallivan, J. (1991). Does language matter? Begging the Question. Canadian Psychology,
32, 20, 21.
Geis, F. L., Brown, V., Jennings, J., & Porter, N. (1984). TV commercials as achievement
scripts for women. Sex Roles, 10, 513-525.
Gould, S. J. (1987). Gender differences in advertising response and self-consciousness
variables. Sex Roles, 16, 215-225.
Koss, M. P., & Leonard, K. E. (1984). Sexually aggressive men: Empirical findings and
theoretical implications. In N.M. Malamuth & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Pornography and
sexual aggression. Toronto: Academic Press.
Livingstone, S., & Green, G. (1986). Television advertisements and the portrayal of
gender. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 149-154.
Macklin, M. C., & Kolbe, R. H. (1984). Sex-role stereotyping in children's advertising:
Current and past trends. Journal of Advertising 13, 34-42.
Malamuth, N. M. (1983). Factors associated with rape as predictors of laboratory
aggression against women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 432-442.
Malamuth, N. M. (1986). Predictors of naturalistic sexual aggression. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 953-962.
Malamuth, N. M., & Briere, J. (1986). Sexual violence in the media: Indirect effects on
aggression against women. Journal of Social Issues, 42, 29-50.
Malamuth, N. M., & Check, J. V. P. (1980). Penile tumescence and perceptual responses
to rape as a function of the victim's perceived reactions. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 10, 528-547.
Malamuth, N. M., & Check, J. V. P. (1981). The effects of mass media exposure on
acceptance of violence against women: A field experiment. Journal of Research in
Personality, 15, 436-446.
Malamuth, N. M., Check, J. V. P., & Briere, J. (1986). Sexual arousal in response to
aggression: Ideological, aggressive and sexual correlates. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 50, 330-340.
Malamuth, N. M., & Donnerstein, E. (Eds.). (1984). Pornography and sexual aggression.
Toronto: Academic Press.
Malamuth, N. M., Haber, S., & Feshbach, S. (1980). Testing hypotheses regarding rape:
Exposure to sexual violence, sex differences, and the "normality" of rapists. Journal of
Research in Personality, 14, 121-137.
Rak, D. S., & McMullen, L. M. (1987). Sex-role stereotyping in television commercials:
A verbal response mode and content analysis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science,
19, 25-39.
Sanday, P. R. (1981). The sociocultural context of rape. Journal of Social Issues, 37,
1-27.
Schwartz, L. A., & Markham, W. T. (1985). Sex-role stereotyping in children's toy
advertisements. Sex Roles, 1, 157-170.
Schwartz, N., Wagner, D., Bannert, M., & Mathes, L. (1987). Cognitive accessibility of
sex role concepts and attitudes toward political participation: The impact of sexist
advertisements. Sex Roles, 17, 593-601.
Schwartz, N., Wagner, D., Bannert, M., & Mathes, L. (1987). Cognitive accessibility of
sex role concepts and attitudes toward political participation: The impact of sexist
advertisements. Sex Roles, 17, 593-601.
Soley, L. C., & Kurzbard, G. (1986). Sex in advertising: A comparison of 1964 and 1984
magazine advertisements. Journal of Advertising 15, 46-64.
Sullivan, G. L., & O'Connor, P. J. (1988). Women's role portrayals in magazine
advertising: 1958-1983. Sex Roles, 18, 181-188.
Tucker, C. M. (1984). Distortions in advertising: The trivialization of American women.
Social Action and the Law, 10, 12-20.
Kyra Lanis University of Toronto
Katherine Covell University College of Cape Breton
... Psychologists have extensively studied the consequences of sexual objectification. Consumption of objectifying images of women is associated with conformity to masculine gender role norms, the acceptance of rape myths, gender-harassing behavior, and increased acceptance of interpersonal violence (Galdi, Maas, and Cadinu 2014;Lanis and Covell 1995). The social psychology literature finds that dehumanization is associated with increased tolerance for violence toward outgroup members as well as other negative intergroup attitudes and behaviors (Ellemers 2017;Viki, Osgood, and Phillips 2013). ...
... However, later work explored how objectification impacts the attitudes and behaviors of all people. As outlined in the introduction, exposure to objectifying portrayals of women can have negative consequences, such as the acceptance of interpersonal violence and harassment (Aubrey, Hopper, and Mbure 2011;Galdi et al. 2014), rape myth acceptance (Lanis and Covell 1995), subscription to masculine gender norms (Galdi et al. 2014), and self-objectification (Quinn et al. 2006). Wright and Tokunaga (2016) find evidence that exposure to objectifying depictions of women is associated with attitudes supportive of violence against women. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectification and dehumanization are topics often discussed within the social psychology and feminist theory literature. Research on objectification has largely focused on the sexual objectification of women’s bodies, whereas the dehumanization literature has focused on dehumanization in the context of racial and ethnic groups. Extant political science research has only recently begun to engage with these concepts. In this manuscript, we build upon these literatures and apply these insights to questions relevant to politics. In particular, we argue that objectifying and dehumanizing portrayals of women impact how voters evaluate women politicians and how much they support gender parity in politics. Through a proposed experimental design, we test our hypothesis that the objectification of women as a group can decrease positive evaluations and likelihood of electoral support for women political candidates.
... This could probably be due to greater exposure to such images, which encourages acceptance (Nowatzki & Morry, 2009). For example, Lanis & Covell (1995) and (Ward, 2016) showed that frequent exposure to sexually objectifying images is related to greater support of sexist beliefs and to greater tolerance of sexual violence. Women in 2000, in fact, agree that objectifying advertisements are frequent. ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to the extensive use of highly visual social media platforms such as Instagram, body image exposure has become a common practice. Specifically, the use of idealized women’s bodies in different sectors of promotion and on social media has been subject to much criticism. The awareness of the negative outcomes of objectifying content triggered movements towards natural body exposure. However, empirical studies on the perceived effectiveness of this use are still needed. In this article, we compare how idealized versus natural women’s bodies are perceived by Instagram users by means of an experimental procedure conducted with 700 users of this platform. We apply the Mann-Whitney U test and PLS-SEM to evaluate how physical attractiveness and idealization are related to the objectifying gazes. Our results suggest that overall publishing potentially objectifying content online is an accepted behavior, especially for more active Instagram users. Besides, both extremes of attractiveness, i.e. idealized beauty and perceived lack of attractiveness are subjected to stronger objectifying, than more natural-looking models. Thus, our paper contributes theoretically and empirically to the research on objectification by extending our knowledge of the factors that drive objectification and its acceptance among social media users. The implications of the emphasis on female bodily appearance on digital presence and the practices of digital influencers are discussed.
... Some researchers have suggested a connection between exposure to sexually-oriented stimuli and aggression against women (Allen, D'Allesio & Brezgel 1995;Emmers, Allen & Gebhardt, 1995;Lanis & Covell, 1995). Inclinations to rape and interpersonal violence against women have also been associated with these contents. ...
Preprint
Purpose: In the current information age, when the attention spans of most people have become very short, marketers are facings serious challenges to grab the attention of their target audience effectively and fruitfully. From street corner to bedroom, virtually every perceivable location of human traces are littered with activity of marketers, whether they are small or large in scale, or the recipients of their information understand the message properly or not. Studying consumers’ acceptance of the main ethical issues in communication, mainly in advertising has recently been receiving much attention from scholars. Therefore, in order to grab the attention of people in the increasing competitive environment, advertisers have resorted to using what they feel can quickly attract the audience. For example, the attachment of attractive women with their physical natural endowments presented in explicit sexually appealing postures to products/services that have no linkage with women. These practices have raised some moral and ethical questions within the society. Therefore, the scope of this study focuses on discussing marketing communication through presenting women as a sexual object from the morality, ethics, and religious perspectives. Design/methodology/approach: This is a descriptive study based on the systematic literature review. Initially, this paper discusses the ethical issues of using women and sexual appeals in the process of marketing communication as well as the current level of practices in the industry. Then, it discusses the consequences and dimensions of the issues from different types of ethical grounds. Finally, it provides recommendations with the objective of finding a common ground from business and social perspectives. It also mentions the scopes of further research, which could lead the secular world to modify their moral values and come closer to the norms of other civilized societies.Findings: The position of the paper takes is that considering the negative effects of the prevalent advertising in society, the practice falls short of human moral values; as a result, it is considered unethical.Originality/value: This review paper examines the ethical implication of using women as marketing tools from the perspectives of morality, business, and Islamic principles that will help business groups as well as the whole religious community, especially Muslims.
... Kadın vücuduna ve vücut kısımlarına odaklanmış reklamcılık anlayışının, toplumsal cinsiyet rolleri üzerine olan etkileri nedeniyle erkekler arasında cinsel saldırganlığı artırabileceğine yönelik kanıtlar mevcuttur. 27 Öte yandan kamu spotları ve bilgilendirme yayınlarının da kadına yönelik şiddetle ilgili toplumsal algıya et-kileri vardır. Yapılan bir çalışmada, şiddetle ilişkili olarak gösterilen ulusal bir kamu spotunun gençlerde fiziksel, psikolojik, ekonomik ve cinsel şiddete karşı farkındalık oluşturduğu gösterilmiştir. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
ÖZET Kadına yönelik şiddet, önemli ve güncel bir halk sağlığı sorunudur. Şiddeti önlemenin en önemli yollarından birisi, şiddete karşı olan tutumların belirlenmesi ve bu tutumların olumlu yönde geliştirilmesinin sağlanmasıdır. Bu çalışmada, kadına şiddet konusunda toplumsal tutuma etki eden faktörlerin ele alınması hedeflenmiştir. Kadına yönelik şiddete toplumsal tutumun en önemli belirleyicisi, toplumun yapısı ve toplumsal cinsiyet algısıdır. Diğer bütün faktörler, bu 2 parametreye doğrudan veya dolaylı etki etmektedir. Kadına yönelik şiddetle mücadelede toplumsal tutumun bilinmesi, mücadeleyi kolaylaştırıcı etki gösterecektir.
... Another early research in Australia showed women take place in advertisement as a housewife or as a sexual appeal object (Wyndham, 1983). Another critical finding tells that men who watch commercials with female nudity tend to approve violence against females and rape myths (Lanis and Covell, 1995). One other finding which can be considered as opposite to that is masculinity has no direct impact on perception of gender stereotypes (Odekerken-Schröder et al., 2002). ...
... Another early research in Australia showed women take place in advertisement as a housewife or as a sexual appeal object (Wyndham, 1983). Another critical finding tells that men who watch commercials with female nudity tend to approve violence against females and rape myths (Lanis and Covell, 1995). One other finding which can be considered as opposite to that is masculinity has no direct impact on perception of gender stereotypes (Odekerken-Schröder et al., 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
The present paper illustrates the preliminary findings of a CORECOM-funded project called "The representation of the image of women in advertising: interlinguistic and multimodal perspectives". Its aim has been to identify, analyze and deconstruct semantic asymmetries which continue to proliferate in the field of information and communication, thus perpetrating discursive constructions of stereotyped and discriminating gender identities. Drawing on the most influential contributions to the study of language and gender (present research has identified possible pragmatic characterizations leading to the identification of subliminal, discriminating or degrading advertising messages which provide the audience with the image of a stereotyped woman or allusively associated, for example, with an aesthetic canon based on beauty as an absolute value. To this end, the comparison between advertising messages disseminated on the regional, national and international territory has brought out quantitative and qualitative data that have allowed us to define the extent to which the presence of gender stereotypes and sexualization of the female body are occurring in regional and national advertising in relation to some European countries. Reference has thus been made to advertisements in English in both English-speaking and non-English-speaking countries. Consequently, the results of interlinguistic and comparative research have also brought out useful data for the definition of pragma-linguistic patterns characterizing ELF (English as a Lingua Franca; Seidlhofer 2001) in advertising.
Article
Full-text available
This paper analyses the sexual overtones in the advertisements of male deodorants. By applying Gunther Kress's theory of multimodality (2010) this paper contends that the female body in the advertisement of male deodorants is objectified to increase the sale of the male specified products. Perfumes and deodorants are considered as a tool to entice women. For this purpose advertisers manipulate the sexual orientation of men consumers and sell their perfumes as sex items.
Article
Full-text available
This article analyses the concept of booty power, objectification and consumption of the female body in popular culture. Booty power is used here to refer to discourses around the Stocko Sama2K, initially a group of five women who were caught on camera dancing the “John Vul’ igate” song and dance challenge, and their sexualized dance routines. I draw on digital ethnography and interviews to argue that objectification presents us with complexities, and conclude that it is a multi-dimensional maze shaped by different socio-cultural agents. The article casts light on how women’s bodies are used as a source of conversations on decency, morality, power and culture. Objectification theory is used to demonstrate the power of objectification by others and also by the self. The research concludes that objectification and self-objectification worked in both directions, as disempowering and empowering, especially to women, given the conflicting socio-cultural elements at play.
Article
Women are significantly less politically engaged than men at both the mass and elite levels. More recent scholarship has found that structural factors and standard predictors of political behavior no longer sufficiently explain this persistent gap in engagement. In the present study, I take a novel approach to exploring the discrepancy in men and women’s political engagement and participation. I ask: Does self-objectification, the internalization of observers’ perspectives of our physical bodies, undermine political engagement and in part, drive the gender gap in engagement? I argue that the cognitive, motivational, and affective correlates of self-objectification work to decrease political engagement and participation. I conduct two separate survey studies on diverse populations. Overall, I find a negative association between trait self-objectification and political engagement. These findings highlight the relevance of objectification and its cognitive and psychological correlates to the study of political engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Using data provided by male university students, several structural equation models were developed and tested to assess the interrelationship of pornography use, anti-women attitudes, and propensity for sexual violence. The model best fitting the data is one in which use of Sexually Violent Pornography and Anti-Women Attitudes are exogenous latent variables predicting self-reported Likelihood of Rape and Likelihood of using Sexual Force, as well as self-reported history of having achieved sexual intercourse by use of Coercion and Force. A variation of this model which includes use of Nonviolent Pornography as an exogenous variable was also tested. Consistent with previous research, use of nonviolent pornography was not uniquely associated with potential or actual sexual aggression. The findings suggest the potential roles of both attitudes and sexually violent pornography in the occurrence of sexual aggression. Further, they support other research findings that suggest it is not merely exposure to sexually explicit materials, per se, but the combination of sex and violence in pornographic materials that encourages or facilitates sexual aggression.
Article
Full-text available
Although much has been learned about how social forces such as pornography can shape or direct men's sexual violence against women, few empirical data exist on how these forces impinge on women's attitudes and behavior. In the present study, 187 female university students responded to a questionnaire regarding childhood exposure to pornography, current sexual fantasies, and endorsement of rape-supportive attitudes. Early exposure to pornography was related to subsequent “rape fantasies” and attitudes supportive of sexual violence against women. Findings were interpreted in the context of women's socialization to accept sexual aggression as a sexual/romantic event.
Chapter
This chapter presents the three hypotheses derived from feminist discussions of rape, using the 50 states and the District of Columbia as the units for analysis. From the perspective of feminist theory, the findings suggest that the combination of a society that is characterized by a struggle to secure equal rights for women, by a high readership of sex magazines that depict women in ways that may legitimize violence, and by a context in which there is a high level of nonsexual violence, constitutes a mix of societal characteristics that precipitates rape. To the extent that this interpretation is correct, it suggests social policies directed toward eliminating or mitigating the conditions that make rape more likely to occur. These steps would include reducing the cultural acceptability of violence, of which rape is a part; and restructuring male gender roles in both sexual and nonsexual relationships to specify equality, warmth, and supportiveness, rather than dominance, aloofness, toughness, and violence. Such a restructuring of relationships between the sexes, while it will not eliminate rape, could reduce the high-incidence rates that currently characterize American society.
Article
This article describes the "rape myth" and tests hypotheses derived from social psychological and feminist theory that acceptance of rape myths can be predicted from attitudes such as sex role stereotyping, adversarial sexual beliefs, sexual conservatism, and acceptance of interpersonal violence. Personality characteristics, background characteristics, and personal exposure to rape, rape victims, and rapists are other factors used in predictions. Results from regression analysis of interview data indicate that the higher the sex role stereotyping, adversarial sexual beliefs, and acceptance of interpersonal violence, the greater a respondent's acceptance of rape myths. In addition, younger and better educated people reveal less stereotypic, adversarial, and proviolence attitudes and less rape myth acceptance. Discussion focuses on the implications of these results for understanding and changing this cultural orientation toward sexual assault.
Article
Our purpose was to determine whether the manner in which women are portrayed in television advertisements has undergone significant change during the past 15 years. Replicating the 1972 study by Dominick and Rauch, a content analysis was performed on 1,480 commercials. Commercials were coded for product advertised, gender of the voice‐over announcer, gender of the on‐camera product representative, setting, age, and occupation of the characters. Women were found to be portrayed in a wider range of occupations and appeared more frequently in settings outside the home than in 1972. Product type, voice‐over announcer, on‐camera product representative, and age categories remained unchanged.
Article
Examines the images of women reflected in current advertising and explores some of the ramifications of those images. Advertising perpetuates sex-role stereotypes. Commercials usually depict boys in active, outdoor settings, accompanied by upbeat music and primary colors. Girls, in contrast, are usually depicted indoors, often in bedrooms, surrounded by soft music and pastel colors. It is argued that advertising tells women that achievement is not feminine and that success will cause men to reject them. This message contributes to a fear of success in women. By relegating women to domestic roles or low-status jobs, advertising supports the status quo, in which women encounter fewer job opportunities, slower rates of promotion, and lower lifetime earnings than their male counterparts. To change advertising, women must compile data that documents how women are portrayed in the advertising media, determine who controls those images, and become aware of the effect media sexism has on their self-images. (6 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Comments on the article written by Begg (see record 2007-09635-001), in which he presents a discussion of Modern English words, focusing on the masculinity and feminity of various words. The current author suggests that we are beginning to create a better language, one that includes everyone, that affirms the equal value of all its users. The vehement response inspired by some of these changes attest to the impact they are having, but these attacks are not going to make language reform go away. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)