Content uploaded by Nicolas Guéguen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Nicolas Guéguen
Content may be subject to copyright.
Social Psychology of Education 13-01: 1–10, 2003.
© 2003 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 1
Nonverbal encouragement of participation
in a course: the effect of touching
NICOLAS GUÉGUEN
Laboratoire GRESICO, IUT de Vannes-Département TC, Université de Bretagne-Sud, 8, rue
Montaigne, BP 561-56017, Vannes, France. e-mail: Nicolas.Gueguen@iu-vannes.fr
Abstract. Previous studies have shown that touching leads to positive behavior, particularly in an
educational context. A new experiment was carried out in which students were encouraged to inter-
vene in a course by demonstrating the solution of a statistical exercise on the blackboard. According
to the experiment, students were or were not briefly touched on the forearm by the teacher during
the corrective exercise. After that, the teacher asked his students to demonstrate the exercise on the
blackboard. The results showed that touching increases the volunteers’ rate. Various explanations
(familiarity, status and mood) were suggested to explain such results.
1. Introduction
Touching the arm or the shoulder of a person for 1–2 s when asking for a request
seems trivial. However, more than 20 years of research has led to the conclusion
that this nonverbal contact definitely affects the subject’s behavior. An experiment
carried out by Kleinke (1977) has shown that the simple fact of touching someone’s
forearm induces him/her to hand back money found in a phone box, or to give
money to a person in the street. Similarly, Hornik (1987) has pointed out that
touching increases the response rates in street surveys. It has also been observed
that people are more persistent when executing a difficult task such as answering a
long questionnaire about personal matters (Nannberg & Hansen, 1994). The simple
touching of a customer by a waiter or waitress in a restaurant increases his or her
tip (Crusco & Wetzel, 1984; Stephen & Zweigenhaft, 1986; Hornik, 1992b; Lynn,
Le, & Sherwyn, 1998). A person demonstrating products in a store will observe
that the willingness to taste or to test his/her products, increases if he/she touches
his/her client while ordering. Moreover, it increases product sales rate in the store
(Smith, Gier, & Willis, 1982; Hornik, 1992a).
Apart from the effects on compliance to a request, touching appears also as a
factor for the encouragement of human behavior. For instance, the fact of a teacher
touching a student twice on the arm during an interview following a first examina-
tion, results in the student improving his/her later performances, more than those
observed in a control group where students were not touched during the interview
(Steward & Lupfer, 1987). A study conducted by Wheldall, Bevan, and Shortall
(1986) has shown that touching leads young children to become more involved in
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.1
Thomson Press (I) Ltd., PIPS No. 5151065 (spoekap:mathfam) v.1.2
2NICOLAS GU ´
EGUEN
a task and to show less disruptive behavior during classes. The conductors of this
experiment asked teachers to touch their pupils whilst they congratulated them on
their results or on their behavior during classes. Teachers were also instructed not
to touch their pupils during other interactions. Observers in the class measured the
number of disruptive behaviors shown by the pupils: getting up without permission
and hitting a classmate; and the behavior concerning the application of the pupils in
their school tasks: use of the appropriate material and concentration. Results have
shown that in two different classes where such observations were made, there was a
60% reduction in disruptive behavior following touching, compared with the aver-
age of such behavior before the adoption of this tactile encouragement. It was also
found that the number of pupils increased positive behaviors in their school tasks:
taking a book in order to read it and checking information in a dictionary increased
by approximately 20%. These results confirm those obtained by Kazdin and Klock
(1973) even with difficult pupils (Clements & Tracy, 1977; Van Houten et al.,
1982). Such positive effects of touching are also found with elderly people within
the framework of manual tasks which were proposed to them (Howard, 1988).
The effects of encouragement induced by tactile contact are also found with
health behavior. Jourard and Friedman (1970) have shown that touching leads
patients in psychotherapy to speak much longer with their therapists about par-
ticularly personal problems. The effect of encouragement aroused by a brief tac-
tile contact is also observed with a measure of self-disclosure behavior (Pattison,
1973). Finally, the simple touching of a patient by a nurse, the day before a surgical
operation, decreases the patient’s real stress (evaluated by physiological measures:
heart rate, blood pressure) and perceived stress (self-evaluated), and increases the
compliance to the preoperative recommendations given to the patient (Whitcher &
Fisher, 1979). However, in this later study, the positive effects of touching were
found only on female patients whereas the reverse effect was found on male pa-
tients. Eaton, Mitchell-Bonair, and Friedmann (1986) have even found that when
the service staff for elderly people push their encouragement to eat with tactile
contact, an increasing number of calories and proteins absorbed by the subjects
was observed. These positive effects on eating behavior lasted 5 days after the
tactile contact.
Thus, all these works tend to show that touching encourages people to produce
the behavior expected by the person who touches. Consequently, a new evaluation
of this effect of encouragement was made with students in order to encourage
them to intervene more frequently as volunteers for correcting an exercise on the
blackboard in front of their classmates.
2. Hypothesis
As far as the positive effects of touching on behavior have been observed in the
literature, we believe that tactile contact between a teacher and a student should
eventually increase the participation of the student in the class.
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.2
TOUCHING AND ENCOURAGEMENT 3
3. Method
3.1. SUBJECTS
One-hundred-and-two undergraduate university students (64 males and 38 females)
between 18 and 20 years were tested during their courses. These students came
from four different kinds of the same degree (social sciences).
3.2. PROCEDURE
The experiment lasted 6 weeks during which a male teacher of statistics was the
conductor of the experiment. The experiment was conducted weekly during each
of the two one-hour classes of statistics lectures given to the students.
The teacher (a 42-year-old man with 11 years of teaching experience in statis-
tics) was trained by the investigator to act similarly in both situations and to touch
a student in the appropriate way. Nevertheless, he was not aware of the goals of
our experiment and had not received any information about previous studies on the
effect of touching on behavior or on evaluation. He was unaware of the hypothesis
of the study.
The experiment was conducted during the period of the course where common
statistical tests were studied (e.g., chi-square independent test, means comparisons
and one-way ANOVA).
Regularly, by the way of a random list of the students’ position in the classroom,
the teacher came to control the solving of an exercise. In order to guarantee an
ethical approach to pedagogy and to maintain the same methodological conditions
between the two experimental groups, the professor was instructed to encourage
positively all the students. In every case, if the exercise was not finished, he helped
the student and encouraged him/her to continue in the following way: “That’s a
good start. It is OK, Go on like that”. If the exercise was finished and the solution
found, the experiment conductor said: “That’s good. You understood everything”.
If there was a mistake, he lavished encouragement all the same and helped the
student find the correct solution. In every case, when leaving the student, he added:
“That’s good. Well done”. The encouragements were given with a low voice in
order to prevent the students around the student-target from hearing the teacher.
At this moment, according to a random selection, the experimentor touched or
did not touch the student for 1 s with his hand. Touching was done by applying a
slight tap on the upper-arm of the target. Then, he left and proceeded in the same
way with another student who was chosen individually at random. During each of
these phases, four students were tested. Once the visit to the fourth student target
was finished, the experiment conductor said to everyone: “So now, we are going
to correct the exercise. Would anyone like to come to the blackboard and show the
solution?”. At French universities, when students want to volunteer to come up
to the blackboard they put up their hand and the teacher has the responsibility of
selecting one student amongst the volunteers. The same procedure was used for
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.3
4NICOLAS GU ´
EGUEN
Table I. Rate of students who volunteered to correct the exercise on the blackboard
Touched No touched Mean
Male-students N=29 N=35
Volunteered 31.0% 11.4% 20.3%
Did not volunteer 69.0 88.6% 79.7%
Female-students N=17 N=21
Volunteered 23.6% 4.8% 13.2%
Did not volunteer 76.4% 95.2% 86.8%
Mean 28.3% 8.9% 17.7%
N: size of the group.
this experiment. The experimentor asked for some volunteers and noted amongst
the four target students who was a volunteer and who was not.
4. Results
The dependent variable was defined as the number of volunteers between the four
target students who accepted the invitation to correct the exercise on the black-
board. The various frequencies obtained according to the subject’s gender and the
independent variable are listed in Table I.
A 2 (touching/no touching) ∗2 (male-students/female-students) ∗2 (volunteer/
no volunteer) log-linear method was used to analyze our data. A main effect of
touching was obtained (χ2(1, N=102) =6.59, p<0.02). As we can see, when
touched, target students were more likely to volunteer than target students who
were not touched. Despite the appearances, between these target students, male
students were not more willing to volunteer than their female counterparts (χ2
(1, N=102) =0.87, ns). There was no interaction effect obtained between the
subject’s gender and touching condition (χ2(4, N=102) =7.68, ns).
5. Discussion
The results obtained show that touching is a factor of encouragement to produce the
behavior expected by the person who touches. We confirm here the different stud-
ies which have pointed out the effect of touching on compliance (Kleinke, 1977;
Willis & Hamm, 1980; Brockner, Pressman, Cabitt, & Moran, 1982; Smith, Gier,
& Willis, 1982; Goldman & Fordyce, 1983; Goldman, Kiyohara, & Pfannensteil,
1985; Hornik, 1987; Hornik & Ellis, 1988). However, in our experiment the re-
quest was impersonal and did not concern a request for the benefit of the solicitor.
These results are congruent with recent studies on nonverbal immediacy. Albers
(2001) found a strong relationship between the use of nonverbal immediacy (e.g.,
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.4
TOUCHING AND ENCOURAGEMENT 5
eye contact, and smiling) and the willingness of audience members to partici-
pate in discussion especially the willingness of students to discuss things with
a teacher who used these nonverbal behaviors in a learning course. In the same
way, Christophel (1990) found that such immediate nonverbal behaviors increased
student motivation and learning.
These results have been replicated in two studies (Allen & Shaw, 1990; Jaasma
& Koper, 1999) and are consistent in many different cultures (McCroskey et al.,
1996). However, in all the studies mentioned above, the nonverbal immediacy
of the teacher was constituted by different behaviors (eye contact, smiling, body
tenseness, trunk and limb movement). Tactile contact was rarely included as a
nonverbal immediate aspect of behavior, and when that was the case (Burgoon,
1991) the effect of touching was not evaluated independently. In addition, most of
this research tested the effect of nonverbal immediacy on perception or attitude,
but the effect on behavior has rarely been tested. Our results seem to show that
touching is sufficient enough to affect students’ behavior. We still have to explain
this effect. Many hypotheses can be considered.
It is possible that touching, coming from a familiar high-status person, is per-
ceived as a sign of distinction which encourages the target to produce the expected
behavior. Jourard and Friedman (1970) found that when a therapist touched his/her
patient, this contact led the patient to produce self-disclosure behavior more easily
and to feel more confident. This feeling brings about a higher probability of further
consultation with the therapist (Bacorn & Dixon, 1984). Thus, we may deduce
that, in our experiment, the teacher’s touching also increased the student’s self-
confidence. So, the effect would have been to overcome the inhibition of correcting
the exercise in front of his/her classmates. The fact of being touched by the teacher
increases the value for the teacher, and consequently his requests are more likely
to be answered.
Many studies have shown that lower status people are more easily the targets
for the initiation of touching than higher status ones. Henley (1973) argued that
individuals of a higher status have a touching privilege that is exercised in order to
express and maintain their status advantage. She found that men, having a higher
status than women, touch women more frequently than women touch men. Further-
more, some later studies have shown that the initiation of touching between male
and female was determined by age: young males tend to initiate touching young fe-
males, whereas older females more frequently tend to initiate touching males (Hall
& Veccia, 1990). Juni and Brannon (1981) found, by manipulating the status of the
appearance of participants who were asking people for directions, that lower status
persons were more frequently touched by people when receiving help, than higher
status ones who only received verbal assistance. Some other researchers found that
different forms of touching are initiated according to the toucher’s or the touchee’s
status. Hall (1996) found that higher status individuals initiate touching more often
as a sign of affection and direct it to the arm or the shoulder. Yet, we also know
that people of a lower status tend to look for a higher status tactile contact (Juni &
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.5
6NICOLAS GU ´
EGUEN
Brannon, 1981). Stier and Hall (1984) considered that people of higher status use
touching in order to demonstrate their status whereas people of a lower status use
it to gain status. So, the fact of touching someone of a higher status brings prestige
to people of a lower status. This could explain the desire of wanting to touch a
politician or someone famous, that turns sometimes into hysterical excess. Thus,
we can talk about the search for gratitude from people of a lower status beyond
the tactile contact. If this search is satisfied, the touchee will have more chances
of accepting the request of the toucher. This compliant behavior would allow the
preservation of an interaction to be considered as a status-enhancing act in itself.
Howard, Gengler, and Jain (1995, 1997) have shown to what extent a sign of
valuation coming from someone of a higher status resulted in eventual compliance
to his/her request. These researchers have shown that, if a teacher were to ask his
students for a donation to a local charity, he was more likely to succeed under the
following circumstances. If the student’s name was remembered correctly by the
teacher after a brief introduction, there was 90% compliance to his request. If a
teacher did not remember correctly a name, there was 60% compliance; and if he
failed to remember, there was 50% compliance. For these authors, the fact that
the teacher did remember the name of the student correctly was considered as a
compliment to him. We know now that a compliment or any positive sign coming
from someone of a higher status is perceived more positively than the same sign
coming from someone similar (Iverson, 1968). In our experiment, the teacher’s
touching may thus have activated this need for valuation that found its fulfillment
in the compliance to come to the blackboard for the corrective exercise.
Another explanation in terms of mood activation can also be considered to
explain our results. We know now that positive mood activation, compared to
a neutral mood, tends to increase compliance to a request made by its solicitor
(Harris & Smith, 1975; Levin & Isen, 1975; Weyant, 1978; Bizman, Yinin, Ronco,
& Schachar, 1980; Forgas, 1997, 1998; Rind, 1997). Now, Fischer, Rytting, and
Heslin (1976) have shown that when a student was touched by an employee of a
library, his/her mood was positively affected by the tactile contact. This effect on
mood could explain why Goldman, Kiyohara, and Pfannensteil (1985) succeeded
in obtaining a positive effect of touching on compliance even when the toucher
was not the solicitor of the request. In their experiment, a male first asked a student
for directions. According to their study, this person touched or did not touch the
subject when formulating his thanks to him. A few minutes later, a second person
approached the subject and asked him/her to give his/her time for a charity. The re-
sults showed that in the absence of touching in the initial contact with the first parti-
cipant, 5% of the subjects agreed to the request without tactile contact against 40%
with tactile contact. One can deduce that touching activates a positive affect which,
in return, facilitates his/her later helpful behavior. In our experiment, we may con-
sider that the same effect of activation had made the student’s participation easier.
Obviously, for the moment, these explanatory hypotheses are extremely spec-
ulative and require new experiments to be conducted. Evaluation of mood, self-
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.6
TOUCHING AND ENCOURAGEMENT 7
confidence, self-esteem and ingratiation would be necessary variables to evaluate
the cognitive effects of tactile contact on targets. The results of our study seem to
reveal that touching is a nonverbal form of encouragement to produce an expected
positive form of behavior. Such results also show the interest in the study of the
effect of tactile contact within the framework of an educational relationship.
Of course such nonverbal behaviors could not be used everywhere. Field (1999)
has pointed out that touching tends to have become taboo in the American school
system. Mazur and Pekor (1985) found that elementary and high school teachers
were warned not to touch children because of potential accusations of sexual ab-
use. In France, there are no recommendations concerning tactile behavior of the
teachers towards the children, especially with university students. However, some
tactile contact could be interpreted as a form of sexual solicitation. Consequently,
precautions are necessary when using this type of nonverbal behavior and it is
important to consider whether touching can be practiced by a teacher whilst still
avoiding misinterpretation.
The results of the study should be interpreted cautiously given the preliminary
nature of the study and the methodological limits of our experiment. In this study,
the experimental teacher was trained to treat all subjects alike except with regard to
the aspects of touching. Furthermore, it was difficult to control all other nonverbal
aspects of behavior. In order to control these effects, comparative studies with other
teachers would be necessary. A positive effect of touching was found in this exper-
iment but this effect might not be extended to every teacher. For example, touching
from a disliked teacher might not have the same result as touching from a popular,
attractive or respected one. Thus, it will be necessary to control this effect on stu-
dent’s response to tactile contact. Such evaluation implies that we need to evaluate
several teachers before testing the effect of touching. It would also be interesting to
evaluate different forms of touching in this pedagogic situation. In this experiment,
a slight tap on the upper-arm of the target was used to create the tactile contact, but
touching can take many different forms that induce different interpretations. In a
recent experiment on the effect of touching on compliance, Guéguen (2002) found
that two different forms of touching led to two different rates of compliance for a
request for help. In Guéguen’s experiment, men and women, taken at random in the
street, were asked for money by participants. In some cases, the request was made
without any contact (control group). In one case, touching consisted in holding the
arm of the subject for 2 s whilst, in the other, it consisted in tapping the arm twice.
The results have shown that compared to the control condition, the two forms of
touching increased compliance. Nevertheless, holding the arm for 2 s led to greater
compliance than just tapping it twice. Different parts of the body can also have
some importance on the reaction to touching. Paulsell and Goldman (1984) found
that touching someone’s upper arm rather than the shoulder or the hand, leads to
a greater helping behavior, for instance, helping an interviewer who accidentally
dropped several survey forms. With these two latter studies, it seems that touching
different parts of the body and different forms of touching are important variables
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.7
8NICOLAS GU ´
EGUEN
that can influence the reaction of the subject towards the toucher. Therefore, we
can deduce that the same effect occurs when touching is practiced by a teacher
with his/her students. Some touching might be interpreted as equivocal or intimate
and lead students to react negatively to tactile contact. Future research will test the
touch of different parts of the body, or the different forms of touching, in order to
isolate which part or form is appropriated to positively affect the student’s behavior
and avoid equivocal reaction.
Future research might assess longer-term tactile contact effect or repeated tactile
contact to determine whether the effect can be maintained over time.
References
Albers, D. (2001). Nonverbal immediacy in the classroom. [On-line manuscript.] Available
http://clearinghouse.mwsc.edu/manuscripts.
Allen, J. & Shaw, D. (1990). Teachers’ communication behaviors and supervisors’ evaluation of
instruction in elementary and secondary classrooms. Communication-Education,39, 308–322.
Bacorn, C. & Dixon, D. (1984). The effects of touch on depressed and vocationally undecided clients.
Journal of Counseling Psychology,31, 488–496.
Bizman, A., Yinin, Y., Ronco, B., & Schachar, T. (1980). Regaining self-esteem through helping
behavior. The Journal of Psychology,105, 203–209.
Brockner, J., Pressman, B., Cabitt, J., & Moran, P. (1982). Nonverbal intimacy, sex, and compliance:
a field study. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,6, 253–258.
Burgoon, J. (1991). Relational message interpretation of touch, conversational distance, and posture.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,15, 223–258.
Christophel, D. (1990). The relationships among teacher immediacy behaviors, student motivation,
and learning. Communication-Education,39, 323–340.
Clements, J. & Tracy, D. (1977). Effects of touch and verbal reinforcement on the classroom
behaviour of emotionally disturbed boys. Exceptional Children,37, 553–554.
Crusco, A. & Wetzel, C. (1984). The midas touch: the effects of interpersonal touch on restaurant
tipping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,10, 512–517.
Eaton, M., Mitchell-Bonair, I., & Friedmann, E. (1986). The effect of touch on nutritional intake of
chronic organic brain syndrome patients. Journal of Gerontology,41, 611–616.
Fisher, J., Rytting, M., & Heslin, R. (1976). Hands touching hands: affective and evaluative effects
on interpersonal touch. Sociometry,39, 416–421.
Forgas, J.P. (1997). Affect and strategic communication: the effects of mood on the production and
interpretation of verbal requests. Polish Psychological Bulletin,28, 145–173.
Forgas, J.P. (1998). Asking nicely? The effects of mood on responding to more or less polite request.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,24, 173–185.
Goldman, M. & Fordyce, J. (1983). Prosocial behavior as affected by eye contact, touch, and voice
expression. The Journal of Social Psychology,121, 125–129.
Goldman, M., Kiyohara, O., & Pfannensteil, D. (1985). Interpersonal touch, social labeling and the
foot-in-the-door effect. The Journal of Social Psychology,152, 143–147.
Guéguen, N. (2002). Kind of touch, gender and compliance to a request: a pilot study. Studia
Psychologica,44, 167–172.
Hall, J. (1996). Touch, status, and gender at professional meetings. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
20, 23–44.
Hall, J. & Veccia, E. (1990). More ‘touching’ observations: new insights on men, women, and
interpersonal touch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,59, 1155–1162.
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.8
TOUCHING AND ENCOURAGEMENT 9
Harris, M. & Smith, R. (1975). Mood and helping. The Journal of Social Psychology,91, 215–221.
Henley, N. (1973). The politics of touch. In P. Brown (Ed.), Radical psychology. New-York: Harper
&Row.
Hornik, J. (1987). The effect of touch and gaze upon compliance and interest of interviewees. The
Journal of Social Psychology,127, 681–683.
Hornik, J. (1992a). Effects of physical contact on customers’ shopping time and behavior. Marketing
Letters,3, 49–55.
Hornik, J. (1992b). Tactile stimulation and consumer response. Journal of Consumer Research,19,
449–458.
Hornik, J. & Ellis, S. (1988). Strategies to secure compliance for a mall intercept interview. Public
Opinion Quarterly,52, 539–551.
Howard, D. (1988). The effects of touch in the geriatric population. Physical and Occupational
Therapy in Geriatrics,6, 35–50.
Howard, D., Gengler, C., & Jain, A. (1995). What’s in a name? A complimentary means of
persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research,22, 200–211.
Howard, D., Gengler, C., & Jain, A. (1997). The name remembrance effect: a test of alternative
explanations. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,12, 801–810.
Iverson, M. (1968). Attraction toward flatterers of different statuses. The Journal of Social Psycho-
logy,74, 181–187.
Jaasma, M. & Koper, R. (1999). The relationship of student–faculty out-of-class communication to
instructor immediacy and trust and to student motivation. Communication-Education,48, 41–47.
Jourard, S. & Friedman, R. (1970). Experimenter-subject ‘distance’ and self-disclosure. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,15, 278–282.
Juni, S. & Brannon, R. (1981). Interpersonal touching as a function of status and sex. The Journal of
Social Psychology,114, 135–136.
Kazdin, A. & Klock, J. (1973). The effect of nonverbal teacher approval on student attentive
behaviour. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,6, 643–654.
Kleinke, C. (1977). Compliance to requests made by gazing and touching experimenters in field
settings. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,13, 218–223.
Levin, P. & Isen, A. (1975). Further studies on the effect of feeling good on helping. Sociometry,38,
141–147.
Lynn, M., Le, J.-M., & Sherwyn, D. (1998). Reach out and touch your customers. Cornell Hotel and
Restaurant Administration Quarterly,39, 60–65.
Mazur, S. & Pekor, C. (1985). Can teacher touch children anymore: physical contact and its value in
child development. Young Children,40, 10–12.
McCroskey, J., Sallinen, A., Fayer, J., Richmond, V., & Barraclough R. (1996). Nonverbal immediacy
and cognitive learning: a cross-cultural investigation. Communication-Education,45, 200–211.
Nannberg, J. & Hansen, C. (1994). Post-compliance touch: an incentive for task performance. The
Journal of Social Psychology,134, 301–307.
Pattison, J. (1973). Effects of touch on self-exploration and the therapeutic relationship. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology,40, 170–175.
Paulsell, S. & Goldman, M. (1984). The effect of touching different body areas on prosocial behavior.
The Journal of Social Psychology,122, 269–273.
Rind, B. (1997). Effect of interest arousal on compliance with a request for help. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology,19, 49–59.
Smith, D., Gier, J., & Willis,F. (1982). Interpersonal touch and compliance with a marketing request.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology,3, 35–38.
Stephen, R. & Zweigenhaft, R. (1986). The effect on tipping of a waitress touching male and female
customers. The Journal of Social Psychology,126, 141–142.
Steward, L. & Lupfer, M. (1987). Touching as teaching: the effect of touch on students’ perceptions
and performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,17, 800–809.
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.9
10 NICOLAS GU ´
EGUEN
Stier, D. & Hall, J. (1984). Gender differences in touch: an empirical and theorical review. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology,47, 440–459.
Van Houten, R., Nau, P., MacKenzie-Keating, S., Sameoto, D., & Colavecchia, B. (1982). An anal-
ysis of some variables influencing the effectiveness of reprimands. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis,15, 65–83.
Weyant, J. (1978). Effects of mood states, costs, and benefits of helping. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,36, 1169–1176.
Wheldall, K., Bevan, K., & Shortall, K. (1986). A touch of reinforcement: the effects of contingent
teacher touch on the classroom behaviour of young children. Educational Review,38, 207–216.
Whitcher, S. & Fisher, J. (1979). Multidimensional reaction to therapeutic touch in a hospital setting.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37, 87–96.
Willis, F. & Hamm, H. (1980). The use of interpersonal touch in securing compliance. Journal of
Nonverbal Behavior,5, 49–55.
Biographical note
Nicolas Guéguen is currently Assistant Professor in Social Psychology at the
University of Bretagne Sud in France. His research interest focuses on the effect
of nonverbal factors on compliance to a request. He also works on the factors that
favor compliance to a request in computer-mediated communication.
13-01.tex; 1/10/2003; 15:24; p.10