ArticlePDF Available

The Impacts of Individualism/Collectivism, Self-Esteem, and Feeling of Mastery on Life Satisfaction among the Turkish University Students and Academicians

Authors:

Abstract

This study had two goals. The first one was todetermine whether the predictors ofindividualism/collectivism, self-esteem, andfeeling of mastery in a specific socio-culturalenvironment in which individuals from differentcultural backgrounds interact with each otherexplain life satisfaction. The second goal wasto illuminate the contradiction between crosscultural and local findings about ongoingTurkish cultural experience within the relatedvariables and the frame of the socio culturalenvironment. Research participants numbered696, 388 students and 308 academicians ofMersin University. Students were enrolled inthe Faculty of Art and Science, the Faculty ofEngineering and the Faculty of AdministrativeSciences and Economics. Fifty percent of themwere females and the fifty percent were males.The academic sample consisted of the academicpersonnel (instructors, assistants, lecturers,and professors) in the above-mentionedfaculties. The sample made up of academicianswas also half female and half male. Theparticipants responded to the Turkish versionof the short form of Individualism/CollectivismScale (INDCOL), the Rosenberg Self-EsteemScale, the Mastery Scale and the Satisfactionwith Life Scale. Among these scales, the shortform of INDCOL showed a high level of constructvalidity and the Mastery Scale showed amoderate level of construct validity in therelated samples. Others have showed high levelsof validities and reliabilities in the previousTurkish studies conducted. The findings of thisstudy revealed that individualism predictedhigh life satisfaction and collectivismpredicted low life satisfaction. Self-esteemand mastery also predicted high lifesatisfaction. It was observed in this studythat university life contributed to maleuniversity students being autonomous andindependent.
UNSAL YETIM
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM/COLLECTIVISM,
SELF-ESTEEM, AND FEELING OF MASTERY ON LIFE
SATISFACTION AMONG THE TURKISH UNIVERSITY
STUDENTS AND ACADEMICIANS
(Accepted 23 July, 2002)
ABSTRACT. This study had two goals. The first one was to determine whether
the predictors of individualism/collectivism, self-esteem, and feeling of mastery
in a specific socio-cultural environment in which individuals from different
cultural backgrounds interact with each other explain life satisfaction. The second
goal was to illuminate the contradiction between cross cultural and local find-
ings about ongoing Turkish cultural experience within the related variables and
the frame of the socio cultural environment. Research participants numbered
696, 388 students and 308 academicians of Mersin University. Students were
enrolled in the Faculty of Art and Science, the Faculty of Engineering and the
Faculty of Administrative Sciences and Economics. Fifty percent of them were
females and the fifty percent were males. The academic sample consisted of
the academic personnel (instructors, assistants, lecturers, and professors) in the
above-mentioned faculties. The sample made up of academicians was also half
female and half male. The participants responded to the Turkish version of the
short form of Individualism/Collectivism Scale (INDCOL), the Rosenberg Self-
Esteem Scale, the Mastery Scale and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. Among
these scales, the short form of INDCOL showed a high level of construct validity
and the Mastery Scale showed a moderate level of construct validity in the
related samples. Others have showed high levels of validities and reliabilities in
the previous Turkish studies conducted. The findings of this study revealed that
individualism predicted high life satisfaction and collectivism predicted low life
satisfaction. Self-esteem and mastery also predicted high life satisfaction. It was
observed in this study that university life contributed to male university students
being autonomous and independent.
INTRODUCTION
In recent years, a striking number of studies have been devoted to
cultural aspects of behavior (Hofstede, 1991; Kim, 1994; Triandis,
1995; Triandis et al., 1985). Culture as a broad concept has been
Social Indicators Research 61: 297–317, 2003.
© 2003 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
298 UNSAL YETIM
considered to be the main variable that shapes individual and group
attitudes, values, trends, and behaviors. One cultural construct that
has received much empirical attention over the past 20 years is
individualism-collectivism (I-C). Despite looseness of the term,
several commonalities in behavior varying across different cultural
contexts are summarized and put forth as the discriminative typolo-
gies about cultures (Kim, 1994; Kagitcibasi, 1994, 1997).
This recent progress has immediately been reflected in the field
of subjective well-being and it has revealed the fact that life satis-
faction could differ depending on cultural context. In fact, there is
enough evidence that scores of happiness are meaningfully related
to I-C levels of societies. Diener et al. (1995) for example, found
a strong correlation between the level of individualism of nations
and their scores of subjective well-being. Similarly, Triandis (1995)
claimed that people consign different values to subjective well-
being depending on their cultural orientation. In short, individualists
pay more attention to their own happiness because the concept
of independence is emphasized in this type of culture. Another
reason the individualistic-collectivistic aspect of societies might
affect subjective well-being is that objective quality of life condi-
tions in individualistic nations are better than those in collectivistic
nations. Moreover, individualistic nations have already developed
their “democratic institutions” and reached a state of stability in
their economic and social life (Inglehart and Rabbier, 1986). In
contrast, many of the collectivistic nations have been suffering from
economic and politic instabilities and poor life conditions. (Inglehart
and Klingemann, 2000).
In addition to the global appearance of I-C and its relation to
subjective well-being, there are also interesting differences among
some of the personality constructs. One focus of cultural compar-
ison has been the concept of self. Generally the self in collec-
tivistic cultures has been described as enmeshed, ensembled, inter-
dependent, and contextualized (Tafarodi and Walters, 1999). In
contrast, the self in individualistic cultures has been described as
self-contained, isolated independent, and clearly bounded (Markus
and Kitayama, 1991; Sampson, 1989).
Kagitcibasi (1990, 1996a) presented an integrative evaluation in
which she emphasized family as the center for self-development.
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 299
In her studies of different cultural and socio-economic contexts,
she argued that antecedents of self-development were mediated
by the family. Kagitcibasi (1990, 1994, 1996b, 1997) differen-
tiated between the relational and separated self in her studies.
According to her, the relational self develops especially in the family
model of emotional and material interdependence-typical in subsist-
ence economics and agrarian life styles with collectivistic cultures
requiring intergenerational dependence. On the other hand, the sepa-
rated self develops in the family model of independence, seen in the
Western urban contexts with individualistic cultures where intergen-
erational dependence is not required for family livelihood. She also
proposed a third category of self that combines a relational orienta-
tion with autonomy in a dialectical synthesis of the above two types
of self. The third category develops in the family model of emotional
interdependence seen particularly in the developed urban areas of
societies with collectivistic cultures where material intergenera-
tional interdependencies weaken but emotional interdependencies
continue. This allows for the introduction of an autonomy orienta-
tion to child rearing as complete intergenerational interdependence
is no longer required, and leaves out the total obedience and depend-
ence of the growing child. The “autonomous-related self emerging
in the family model of emotional interdependence combines within
itself both collectivistic and individualistic elements at individual
level. There are other studies conducted after the introduction of
“autonomous-related self” by Sinha and Tripathi (1994). Kagit-
cibasi, Sunar and Bekman (1988) have provided further evidence
for this proposal in low-income areas of Istanbul. Consistent with
the above findings, Phalet and Claeys (1993) also found combined
preferences among modern urban Turkish youth for both loyalty
and self-realization. This study also included Belgian youth, and in
contrast, self-realization preferences alone were found out among
them. Studying the Turkish university students in Ankara, Aygun-
Karakitapoglu and Imamoglu (2001) suggested that family environ-
ment shapes self-development in terms of both autonomy and
relatedness in the Turkish culture. Similarly, Karadayi’s (1996),
study which included youth from different levels of socio-economic
status and education, found that young Turkish people became
autonomous in their attitudes, values and behaviors while keeping
300 UNSAL YETIM
their relatedness with their families. Therefore, since esteem needs
and motivations of individuals are closely related to the notion of
self, these features could be quite different according to I-C.
Lucas et al. (1996) demonstrated that self-esteem was correlated
with measures of subjective well-being. Myers and Diener (1995)
indicated that most of the people in western societies rate them-
selves as more intelligent and more competent than average people
in their societies. Studying with cross-cultural samples, Diener and
Diener (1995) found a 0.47 correlation between self-esteem and life
satisfaction in the sample of men and women from 31 different
countries. However, they found relatively small correlations for
Turkish university students (0.35 for women, 0.38 for men). In addi-
tion, the results suggested that the more individualistic the nation,
the more powerful the relationship is between self-esteem and life
satisfaction. Thus it seems that the relationship between self-esteem
and life satisfaction may show variations depending on the levels of
I-C.
Another important personality feature is the feeling of mastery.
Feeling of mastery is derived from personal sense of control on
life and from individuals’ perceptions about considering themselves
as initiator or cause of the life events (Gecas and Burke, 1995;
Wells and Marwell, 1978). Pearlin and Radabaugh (1978) defined
the concept of mastery as the extent to which people feel to be in
control of the important circumstances in their life. After studying
adults, they found a significant correlation between mastery and
self-esteem.
If one wants to evaluate the methods of establishing control
in life from other points of view, then there are two different
kinds of control systems: First is the primary control, which is
defined as making a direct individual effect on existing realities
as mentioned by Gecas and Burke (1995), and Wells and Marwell
(1978). The other is the secondary control, which means to establish
control in life by accommodating the existing realities. Weisz et al.
(1984) showed that primary control is important in the USA, which
depends on the individualistic orientation and that secondary control
is important in Japan which supposedly represents collectivistic
culture. It is also stated by Diaz-Guerrero (1979) that secondary
control is important in Mexican culture.
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 301
Campbell (1981) obtained powerful evidence that mastery is
associated with subjective well-being. Perceptions of control can
also vary according to I-C. Sastry and Ross (1998), for example,
found that Asian Americans, Japanese, South Koreans, Chinese,
and Indians have lower levels of perceived control than Anglo-
Saxon Americans. Similar findings were achieved about minorities
in some western countries (Louden, 1978; Verkuyten, 1994). These
findings indicate that there is a strong emphasis on family and
group life in collectivistic cultures and the concept of “self” in these
cultures is regarded as less essential than it is in individualistic ones.
Consequently, a person’s desire for more control on their life is
considered as a norm violation in collectivistic cultures.
Findings of cross-cultural studies and local studies regarding the
status of ongoing Turkish cultural experience have been found to
contradict each other. For example, in a study recently conducted
(Suh et al., 1998), it is found that Turkish university students
scored 3.85 out of 10 on individualism. According to this finding,
it seems that Turkish students are in the same group with Taiwanese
(3.85), Brazilian (3.90), and Singaporean (3.50) students. Similarly,
according to the World Values Survey (1994), Turkish sample had a
score of 3.85 out of 10 on individualism and was in the same group
with Portuguese (3.85), Brazilian (3.90), Mexican (4.00), Latvian
(4.00), Estonian (4.00) and Nigerian (3.00) samples. In conclusion,
various cross-cultural studies (Bierbrauer et al., 1994; Diener and
Diener, 1995; Hofstede, 1991; Schwartz, 1994) show that Turkish
cultural orientation is basically collectivistic.
However, Turkish social psychologists pointed out that ongoing
cultural practices in Turkey are made up of both individualistic and
collectivistic trends and that Turkish cultural structure cannot be
merely classified as collectivistic or individualistic. For instance,
Goregenli (1997, 1995) and Uskul (1998) found that individu-
alism is pervasive especially among the urban, high educated and
young generations in Turkey. Imamoglu (1987) and Imamoglu and
Gultekin (1993) argued that Turkish culture cannot be evaluated as
merely collectivistic and they insisted that Turkish cultural exper-
ience include both individualistic and collectivistic features. Simi-
larly, in a study, which included urban university graduates from the
1970 and 1990 generations, Imamoglu and Aygun-Karakitapoglu
302 UNSAL YETIM
(1999) demonstrated that the two generations were similar in terms
of their terminal values, but the youth of the 1990 generation gave
relatively more importance to individualistic values than the 1970
generation. Kagitcibasi (1996a) whose concept of “autonomous-
related self” was outlined above indicated that changing soci-
eties like Turkey might become individualistic while keeping their
emotional relatedness and some collectivistic values.
This study is the first research conducted on Turkish sample,
which examined the variables of individualism/collectivism,
mastery, and self-esteem as predictors of life satisfaction.
There are two goals of this study. The main aim of the present
study is to investigate the relationship between life satisfaction and
individualism-collectivism, self-esteem, and mastery in a sample of
Turkish university students and academicians. As mentioned, there
are two distinct pieces of evidence about the relationship between
life satisfaction and related variables for Turkish people. On one
hand, Diener and Diener (1995), Diener and Suh (1999), Hofs-
tede (1991), and many others found that Turkish people are mainly
collectivistic and their self-esteem and mastery levels are lower
than those of the people in individualistic cultures. On the other
hand, most of the Turkish social psychologists reported that Turkish
people have mixed orientations. So their self-esteem and mastery
levels may well be close to those of the people in western nations.
The second goal of the present study is to explore these contra-
dicting results in the context of life satisfaction. In accordance with
this second goal, a diverse socio cultural environment is chosen
to conduct this study. On one hand, this environment includes the
university students who are coming from rural, under-developed
regions of Turkey. These students are brought up by agrarian life
styles and collectivistic values. On the other hand, there are also
academicians who had their education either abroad or in Western
cities of Turkey. These academicians were brought up by modern,
urban life styles and individualistic values. It is thought that,
between academicians and students there is an intense interaction,
which shapes the university life. In this context, this study aims
to analyze the effect of such university life on both students’ and
academicians’ life satisfaction levels within the specified variables.
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 303
METHOD
Participants
According to the goals described above, two samples are chosen;
one is among Mersin University students, and the other is among
the academicians of the same university. In year 2001, there were
4400 undergraduate students enrolled at Mersin University. Four out
of five students at Mersin University come from either Eastern or
South-Eastern parts of Turkey where the social structure and rela-
tions are based on traditional and rural values. It was decided to
include the sophomore and junior students in the sample in order
to reflect the cultural change and interaction. At Mersin University,
the total number of sophomore and junior students who were raised
in Eastern, Southeastern, or East Mediterranean part of Turkey was
found to be 1400. Among this population, 420 students who were
enrolled in year 2001 were selected according to their faculties.
Answers from 32 students were found to be invalid. As a result,
388 students who were enrolled in year 2001 were in the first
sample for this study. There are 780 academicians in the teaching
positions of Mersin University. After the initial screening of the
350 of these academicians, 308 academicians were included in this
study according to their faculty. The academicians graduated from
universities in either the largest cities of Turkey such as Istanbul,
Ankara, Izmir, or those from the United States of America or
Europe.
The numbers of female and male student participants were equal
in both samples; in the student sample it was 194, and in the academ-
ician sample it was 154. The average age of the university students
was 19.5. The academicians’ sample had an average age of 30.4.
Students and academicians were chosen from faculty of arts and
science, administrative science, economics and engineering based
on the weighted ratio from the related population of the university.
Materials
The Individualism/Collectivism (INDCOL) Scale developed by
Hui (1988) was adapted to Turkish culture by Goregenli (1995).
INDCOL scale includes 6 subscales and 62 items, which are
evaluated on 6 points Likert type assessment system. The results
304 UNSAL YETIM
revealed high internal consistency and reliability (Hui, 1988; Gore-
genli, 1995, 1997). A short form of Goregenli’s (1995) adaptation
of INDCOL was constructed by selecting the 12 highest collec-
tivistic and 12 highest individualistic items. An example of the
collectivistic items is “I accept my family’s religion”. An example
of the individualistic items is “What happens to me is my own
doing”. The construct validity of this short form was analyzed by
examining a sample of 170 people with mixed professions and
socio economic strata. The result of the factor analysis which was
found by analyzing the chosen 24 items showed that 2 factors
explained the 59% of the variance (χ2= 588.06, df = 229, p<0.00).
This short scale was used to determine participants’ orientations to
I-C.
Self-esteem was measured using Rosenberg’s (1965) Global
Self-Esteem Scale. The scale consists of ten items with four
response categories. Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale is scored by using
the Likert method, with scores ranging from “no, certainly not” (1)
to “yes, certainly” (5). The scale is adapted to Turkish culture by
Cuhadaroglu (1986) and showed a high level of test-retest reliability
(0.89) and criterion-related validity (0.71). The degree to which
participants feel a sense of mastery on their lives was measured
using a 4-item scale. The scale was based on existing scales from
Pearlin and Schooler (1978) and Phinney et al. (1996). The items of
this scale were “what happens to me in the future mostly depends
on me”, “I often feel helpless in dealing with problems of my
life”, “I can achieve anything if I want to”, and “In general, I
have my life under control”. The five response categories ranged
from “no, certainly not” (1) to “yes, certainly” (5). The scale was
adapted to Turkish culture and gained a medium level of relia-
bility (0.69). Using the scale, Werkuyten and Nkuee (1999) found
almost the same level of reliability (0.67). The construct validity
of the scale was analyzed by examining a sample of 170 people
with mixed professions and socio economic strata. The result of
the factor analysis revealed that single factor explained 38% of
the variance significantly (χ2= 326.05, df = 2, p<0.00). The
Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) was applied to
determine the respondents’ life satisfaction judgments. The scale is
combined for five uni-directional attitude expressions. Each expres-
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 305
sion is evaluated based on a 7-points scale (1: strongly disagree
– 7: strongly agree). The scale is adapted to Turkish population
and Turkish form of this scale showed very high levels of alpha
(0.86) and test-retest reliability (0.73) (Yetim, 1993). Larsen et al.
(1983) found that all items of this scale showed high factor load-
ings on a single common factor and Yetim (1991) confirmed these
findings.
Procedures
Data were collected during normal class periods and from individual
sessions. During the administration of the materials, any questions
from the respondents were clarified. The majority of participants
finished responding to the questionnaires in 15 to 20 minutes.
Data Analysis
All statistical procedures were performed using SPSS 9.05 for
Windows. ANOVA, Pearson’s Correlation Analysis and logistic
regression analysis were used for analyzing the data. The reason
for choosing logistic regression was to assess the contribution of
the variables as predictors of life satisfaction. Logistic regression
is able to handle the violations of assumptions of the usual regres-
sion model (Tabaschnick and Fidell, 1996). Because the predictors
showed a linear regression in both samples separately, this linearity
was not viable due to the diverse cultural backgrounds and genera-
tional differences between the two samples of students and academ-
icians. This was the major reasoning behind the decision for using
logistic regression. Logistic regression would enable the researcher
to analyze both samples together without the restrictions of linearity.
Therefore, by identifying 2 levels of life satisfaction (high and low),
the distribution of 7-point life satisfaction scale was analyzed. 48%
of the sample was in the low life satisfaction and the remaining 52%
was in the high life satisfaction group. The mean of this distribution
was 4.23, and the median was 4.2. All respondents who rated their
perceived life satisfaction above 4.2 were considered the high life
satisfaction group; those with ratings below 4.2 were considered the
low life satisfaction group for both samples.
306 UNSAL YETIM
TABL E I
ANOVA Results According to Status Levels and Sex
Variables Female Female Male Male F(3,595)Eta2
students academi- students academi-
cians cians
Life satisfaction 3.81b4.61a4.27ab 4.26b5.78∗∗ 0.070
Individualism 5.89 5.73 5.94 5.78 1.92 0.024
Collectivism 4.60ab 4.91a4.45b4.21b4.09∗∗ 0.051
Mastery 3.56b3.73ab 3.83a3.51b4.10∗∗ 0.051
Self Esteem 5.55 5.71 5.70 5.57 0.81 0.010
∗∗p<0.00.
Note: Differences between means were probed via Newman-Keuls test.
Measures are scored such that higher means represent higher levels of perceived
life satisfaction, individualism, collectivism, mastery and self-esteem. Values
with the same subscript are not significantly different from each other.
RESULTS
Table I shows ANOVA results where means of the variables are
compared according to sex and status levels. It was observed that the
means between the groups were significantly differ for the variables
of life satisfaction, collectivism, and mastery. The difference in life
satisfaction is mainly the result of the differentiation of the means
of the female academicians group and the female students group
according to Newman-Keuls post-hoc test. Among the groups, the
female students’ mean score for life satisfaction was the lowest and
the female academicians’ mean score was the highest.
The variance of the female academicians’ group’s means score
and the male academicians’ group’s mean score caused the signifi-
cant difference for collectivism among the groups. Among the
groups, the female academicians scored the highest and the male
academicians scored the lowest for collectivism.
When examining the mastery variable, it was observed that male
university students scored the highest and male academicians scored
the lowest. This shows that the difference in the mastery vari-
able is the result of the difference of the male university students’
group’s means score and the male academicians’ groups mean score.
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 307
For the variables of individualism and self-esteem, no significant
differences between the groups were observed.
When Eta values, which show the strength of the difference
between the means of the variables within each group, are examined,
it is observed that the variables of life satisfaction, collectivism, and
mastery are moderately strong (see Table I).
When Pearson’s correlation coefficients are examined based on
sex and life satisfaction it is significantly correlated with the vari-
ables. For the female group, the correlation between mastery and life
satisfaction was moderately positive; and between collectivism and
life satisfaction the correlation was negative. For the male group,
the correlations between mastery and life satisfaction, and also
between self-esteem and life satisfaction were moderately positive.
However, the male group’s correlation between life satisfaction and
collectivism was moderately negative. These findings reveal that the
higher the collectivism, the lower the life satisfaction is both in the
male and the female groups.
Some significant correlations were observed in the female group
between the predictors. Among these, self-esteem and individu-
alism showed a moderately positive correlation whereas self-esteem
together with mastery showed moderately negative correlations with
collectivism.
For the male group, individualism showed moderately positive
correlations with both mastery and self-esteem. Consistent with the
above findings, collectivism revealed moderately negative correla-
tions with both mastery and self-esteem since individualism and
collectivism showed negative correlation.
The findings from both the male and female groups show that the
higher the collectivism, the lower the mastery and self-esteem (see
Table II).
When the correlations are examined according to the status
levels, the findings show that life satisfaction is significantly corre-
lated with all predictors for both academicians and university
students. In the university student group, life satisfaction is moder-
ately correlated with the predictors. University student group find-
ings reveal that individualism is moderately correlated with both
mastery and self-esteem. For academicians, there is a moderately
strong positive correlation between life satisfaction and mastery
308 UNSAL YETIM
TABL E I I
Correlations between the Variables According to Sex
Variables 12345
1. Life Satisfaction 0.23–0.27∗∗ 0.45∗∗ 0.23
2. Individualism 0.30∗∗ –0.24∗∗ 0.29∗∗ 0.38∗∗
3. Collectivism –0.47∗∗ –0.33∗∗ –0.33 –0.44∗∗
4. Mastery 0.36∗∗ 0.38∗∗ –0.32∗∗ 0.11
5. Self-esteem 0.33∗∗ 0.41∗∗ –0.41∗∗ 0.03 —
p<0.01; ∗∗p<0.00.
Significance for two tailed.
Note: Bold values represent the correlations for females.
TABLE III
Correlations between the Variables According to Status Levels
Variables 12345
1. Life satisfaction 0.40∗∗ –0.210.58∗∗ 0.61∗∗
2. Individualism 0.30∗∗ –0.15 0.46∗∗ 0.40∗∗
3. Collectivism –0.47∗∗ –0.26∗∗ –0.26–0.41∗∗
4. Mastery 0.36∗∗ 0.38∗∗ –0.32∗∗ 0.47∗∗
5. Self-esteem 0.33∗∗ 0.41∗∗ –0.41∗∗ 0.03 —
p<0.01; ∗∗p<0.00.
Significance for two tailed.
Note: Bold values represent the correlations for academicians.
and also self-esteem. In both groups, collectivism is negatively
correlated with life satisfaction. Similar to the findings based on
sex, it is also found that individualism had positive correlations
with self-esteem and mastery. Since collectivism and individualism
are negatively correlated, collectivism is found to have negative
correlations with self-esteem and mastery (see Table III).
A direct logistic regression analysis was performed on life satis-
faction as being the outcome and the five predictors of individu-
alism, collectivism, mastery, self-esteem and sex/status level. –2LL
for only constant model was 325.57 and for the full model with all
five predictors it was 220.59. The difference between these models
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 309
TABL E I V
Logistic Regression Analysis of Life Satisfaction
Variables B Wald Significance R Ex. β
(odd ratio)
Individualism 1.383 13.07 0.001 0.1622 4.33
Collectivism –0.964 11.67 0.004 –0.1365 0.382
Mastery 0.942 10.48 0.001 0.1132 2.56
Self-esteem 1.39 49.09 0.000 0.2668 4.02
Female student –0.73 7.78 0.01 –0.1124 0.421
Female academician –1.59 2.08 0.556 –0.0000 1.147
Male student 0.21 0.09 0.77 0.000 1.23
Constant –18.22 48.25 0.000
was 104.98 (df = 7, p<0.0000). It was determined that predictors
classified subjects with low life satisfaction with an accuracy of
85.1% and with high life satisfaction with an accuracy of 72.8%.
Generally these predictors classified the whole sample with an
accuracy of 79.1%. These results show that predictors contribute
meaningfully to life satisfaction.
When Wald values are studied, except than the groups of the
female academicians and the male students, all predictors explain
life satisfaction significantly. In addition to these, when odd ratios
are examined, a person with a high score of individualism is
approximately three times more likely to have a high life satisfac-
tion. Similarly, a person with a high self-esteem score is approxi-
mately three times more likely to be in the group of people with
high life satisfaction. An individual with a high score of mastery is
approximately one and a half times more likely to be in the group
of people with high life satisfaction. However, a person with a high
score of collectivism is more likely to be in the group of people
with low life satisfaction with a probability of approximately 60%.
Female students compared with the other groups are more likely to
have low life satisfaction with a probability of approximately 68%
(see Table IV).
310 UNSAL YETIM
DISCUSSION
This study, which aims to predict life satisfaction of individuals
from two different cultural backgrounds by using the predictors of
individualism, collectivism, self-esteem, and mastery, has reached
multidirectional meaningful results. First of all, it is found that high
life satisfaction is interrelated with high levels of individualism,
self-esteem and mastery for the sample in this study. Collectivism
on the other hand, predicts low levels of life satisfaction for the
subjects in this research. These findings point out that especially
male students become more autonomous and more individualistic
at Mersin University. Male university students having the highest
mastery level and relatively lower levels of collectivism compared
with the other groups support this analysis. Male students are the
ones that are mostly affected by the university environment, which
is liberating and autonomous. In this process, male students take
the advantage of the privilege that is granted by the traditional
gender roles in the eastern part of Turkey. These traditional gender
roles state that males should dominate and be the master of their
own lives. During their university educations, by observing the
academicians and using them as role models, male students learn
to support a political or social view, to be goal-directed and to
determine individualistic preferences for themselves. These find-
ings reveal that young male students become more independent and
more individualistic. At this point, it is observed that traditional
gender roles and the modern university environment interact with
each other in a positive direction where both of them tell the male
students to become more self-oriented. The results of the studies
conducted by Goregenli (1995), Uskul (1998), and Imamoglu and
Aygun-Karakitapoglu (1999) where they state that individualism
is pervasive among the young, urban and the educated people in
Turkey are consistent with this study’s findings about the male
university students.
On the other hand, there is a different situation for the female
university students. The traditional gender roles and the socializ-
ation values of the females are highly collectivistic especially in
the eastern parts of Turkey. For example, females are expected
to care for their parents, never to break off their close relation-
ships with their families even if they are married, to manage
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 311
cultural practices and ceremonies (wedding, circumcision, funerals),
to protect the dignity of their family (not to engage in sexual activity
before marriage), and to be well-mannered (Yetim, 2001). The only
possible way for the females to become autonomous is to be enrolled
in a university. The entrance process to the universities in Turkey
is highly competitive and harsh. Due to the poor economic condi-
tions and inequalities between the regions, it is relatively difficult
for the youth from the eastern and southeastern parts of Turkey
to become enrolled in a university. Conservative Islamic values,
which are held in higher esteem in those regions of Turkey, restrict
the girls from getting involved in interpersonal relationships and
from behaving freely. Under these circumstances, females regard
the university education as their only way to escape from those
restraints. Apparently, getting enrolled in the university did not bring
the expected result for the female students. According to the find-
ings of this study, female students had high scores of collectivism
and the lowest levels of life satisfaction. This might be the result
of female students internalizing the restrictions and the external
controls acting on them. Since they never had similar experiences
before, female students cannot behave as freely as the male students
and they find it difficult to make decisions independently during
their university lives. This situation may raise a conflict for the
female students. It can be stated that this conflict is the product
of two life styles one of which is based on relatively autonomous,
independent behavior and tolerance of sexual freedom, and the other
of which is based on restrictions and limitations on behaviors and on
intolerance of sexual activities. In other words, traditional gender
roles and socialization values contradict the university’s modern
values. All these factors make it difficult for the female students
to adapt to the new environment of the university and as a result this
causes the low level of life satisfaction among the female students.
Among the academicians, it was observed that males have the
lowest level of collectivism, and they had slightly higher levels
of individualism. In addition male academicians had low levels of
mastery. On the other hand, female academicians had the highest
levels of life satisfaction. Although they had high levels of collec-
tivism, according to the logistic regression analysis, the probability
of the female academicians having high individualism, mastery
312 UNSAL YETIM
and self-esteem is expected to be high. As can be seen, logistic
regression and ANOVA results reveal conflicting findings about
the female academicians. There is the question of how come the
female academicians have the highest life satisfaction and a high
collectivism level at the same time. There is not any answer to this
question with the available findings. It might be that female academ-
icians have liberated the traditional women roles by becoming
competent in their area of study together with their mastery exper-
iences. Female academicians might have succeeded to blend this
liberation with the functional collectivistic values. For example, they
might have integrated the functional collectivistic features such as
emotional support from the family and cooperation with family and
group – with those of individualism like self-esteem and mastery. It
is possible to answer this question by introducing another question:
Why do male academicians have the lowest level of collectivism and
the lowest level mastery at the same time? It seems that traditional
sex roles encourage males to be dominant, to take initiative, and to
control their lives. This result is confirmed in the male university
students’ sample. However, it is obvious that male academicians
lose their mastery for some reason. It should be noted that the find-
ings might be the result of the characteristics of the sample and of
other factors that cannot be controlled.
In order to generalize the findings of this study, other research in
Turkey must be conducted that will confirm the results of this study.
Their explanations would be available about the male academicians’
mastery level, which is related to management. In Turkey, males
mainly dominate university management positions. Management is
an important source for males to gain prestige. In Turkey, in order
to come to a management position, it requires to be compatible with
the central system. It is not possible to overcome the central hier-
archical bureaucracy, which determines the policy in public arena
by individual effort and mastery. Thus, male academicians who
want to gain power achieve this goal by accommodating the central
authority. However, since secondary control is used, this might
result in loss of mastery and a decrease in life satisfaction. It should
be noted again that this comment is just a suggestion and there is no
direct finding that tests this suggestion. As a result, it is necessary
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 313
to conduct new studies that will investigate the relationship between
individualism, collectivism and life satisfaction.
Results of the correlation analysis reveal that in all groups of
male/female and university students/academicians, life satisfaction
is positively correlated with individualism, self-esteem and mastery
and negatively correlated with collectivism. Individualism is posi-
tively and collectivism is negatively correlated with self-esteem and
mastery. These findings are consistent with the logistic regression
results.
In conclusion, individualism, self-esteem and mastery are strong
predictors of life satisfaction. All these results do not support
the findings of cross cultural studies. This study did not confirm
the findings of Suh et al. (1998), Hofstede (1991), World Values
Survey (1994) and varying others, which state that Turkish culture
is basically collectivistic. On the contrary, this study states that
collectivism predicts low life satisfaction and individualism predicts
high life satisfaction. If collectivism were really a trend that is
accepted and encouraged by the society, the individual and the
culture, then one would expect it to increase life satisfaction. Again,
the result of Diener and Diener (1995), which states that self-esteem
is high in individualistic cultures, is not supported by this study.
Strong evidence is obtained through studies about the fact that both
individualism and high self-esteem is affective on life satisfaction
in cultures like Turkey where “emotional relatedness” of “related
autonomous self” is highly pervasive.
This study emphasizes the need to reevaluate the extent to which
cross-cultural studies can be generalized. It is clarified by this
study that cross cultural studies’ results which represents a specific
culture should be reexamined, especially in terms of the variables
mentioned in this study.
REFERENCES
Aygun-Karakitapoglu, Z. and E.O. Imamoglu: 2001, Perceived parenting,
Constructs of Self and Well-being: The Case of Turkish Adolescents. Paper
presented at annual meeting of Society for Cross-cultural Research, San Diego,
CA.
Bierbrauer, G., H. Meyer and U. Wolfradt: 1994, ‘Measurement of normative
and evaluative aspects in individualistic and collectivistic orientations: The
314 UNSAL YETIM
Cultural Orientation Scale (COS)’, in U. Kim, H. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi et
al. (eds.), Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications
(Sage, Beverly Hills, CA), pp. 175–188.
Campbell, A.: 1981, The Sense of Well-Being in America: Recent Patterns and
Trends (McGraw-Hill, New York).
Cuhadaroglu, F.: 1986, Youth Self-esteem. Paper presented at the XXIth National
Psychiatry and Neurological Science Congress, Adana, Turkey, October.
Diaz-Guerrero, R.: 1979, ‘The development of coping style’, Human development
22, pp. 320–331.
Diener, E., R.A. Emmons, R.J. Larsen and S. Griffin: 1985, ‘The satisfaction with
life Scale’, Journal of Personality Assessment 49, pp. 71–75.
Diener, E. and M. Diener: 1995, ‘Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and
self- esteem’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68, pp. 653–663.
Diener, E., M. Diener and C. Diener: 1995, ‘Factors predicting the subjective
well-being of nations’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69,
pp. 851–864.
Diener, E. and E. Suh: 1999, ‘National differences in subjective well-being’, in
D. Kahneman, E. Deiner and N. Schwartz (eds.), Well-being: The Foundations
of Hedonic Psychology (Russell-Sage, New York), pp. 434–450.
Gecas, V. and P.J. Burke: 1995, ‘Self and identity’, in K.S. Cook, G.A. Fine and
J.S. House (eds.), Sociological Perspectives in Social Psychology (Allyn and
Bacon, Boston, MA), pp. 41–67.
Goregenli, M.: 1995, ‘Individualism-collectivism orientations in the Turkish
culture: A preliminary study’ (in Turkish), Turkish Journal of Psychology 10,
pp. 1–14.
Goregenli, M.: 1997, ‘Individualist-collectivist tendencies in a Turkish sample’,
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 28, pp. 787–794.
Hofstede, G.: 1991, Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind (McGraw-
Hill, London).
Hui, H.C.: 1988, ‘Measurement of individualism-collectivism’, Journal of
Research in Personality 22, pp. 17–36.
Imamoglu, E.O.: 1987, ‘An interdependence model of human development’, in C.
Kagitcibasi (ed.), Growth and Progress in Cross-cultural Psychology (Swets &
Zeitlinger, Lisse, Holland), pp. 138–145.
Imamoglu, E.O. and Z. Aygun-Karakitapoglu: 1999, ‘Values from 1970s to 1990s:
Time, generation and gender differences on university level’ (in Turkish),
Turkish Journal of Psychology 14, pp. 1–18.
Imamoglu, E.O. and Y.Y. Gultekin: 1993, ‘Problems of university youth in the
light of recommended balanced society-individualmodel: Evaluation, comment
and recommendation to the studies conducted in the period between 1982–
1992’ (in Turkish), Turkish Journal of Psychology 8, pp. 27–41.
Inglehart, R. and H.D. Klingemann: 2000, ‘Genes, culture, democracy and happi-
ness’, in E. Diener and M.E. Suh (eds.), Subjective Well-being Across Cultures
(MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp. 165–183.
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 315
Inglehart, R. and J.R. Rabbier: 1986, ‘Aspirations adapt to situations-But why
are the Belgians so much happier than the French?’, in F.M. Andrews (ed.),
Research on the Quality of Life (Survey Research Center, University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor), pp. 1–56.
Kagitcibasi, C.: 1990, ‘Family and socialization in cross-cultural perspective: A
model of change’, in J. Berman (ed.), Cross-cultural Perspectives: Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation, 1989 (University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln),
pp. 135–200.
Kagitcibasi, C.: 1994, ‘A critical appraisal of individualism and collectivism:
Toward a new formulation’, in U. Kim, H. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi et al. (eds.),
Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications (Sage,
Beverly Hills, CA), pp. 52–66.
Kagitcibasi, C.: 1996a, ‘The related autonomous self: A new synthesis’ (in
Turkish), Turkish Journal of Psychology 11, pp. 36–43.
Kagitcibasi, C.: 1996b, Family and Human Development Across Cultures: A
View from the Other Side (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillside).
Kagitcibasi, C.: 1997, ‘Individualism and collectivism’, in J.W. Berry, M.H.
Segall and C. Kagitcibasi (eds.), Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology
(Vol. 3) (Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA), pp. 1–49.
Karadayi, F.: 1996, Differentiations in Related Autonomy. Paper presented at IXth
National Psychology Congress, Bosphorus University, Istanbul, October.
Kim, U.: 1994, ‘Individualism and collectivism: Conceptual clarification and
elaboration’, in U. Kim, H. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi et al. (eds.), Individualism
and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Application (Sage, Beverly Hills, CA),
pp. 19–40.
Larsen, R.J., E. Diener and R.A. Emmons: 1983, Validity and meaning of
Measures of Subjective Well-being. Paper presented at the Midwestern Psycho-
logical Association Convention, Chicago.
Louden, D.M.: 1978, ‘Self-esteem and locus of control: Some findings on
immigrant adolescents in Britain’, New Community 4, pp. 218–234.
Lucas, R.E., E. Diener and E. Suh: 1996, ‘Discriminant validity of well-being
measures’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71, pp. 616–628.
Markus, H.R. and S. Kitayama: 1991, ‘Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion and motivation’, Psychological Review 98, pp. 224–253.
Myers, D.G. and E. Diener: 1995, ‘Who is happy?’, Psychological Science 6,
pp. 10–19.
Pearlin, L.I. and C. Radabaugh: 1976, ‘Economicstrains and the coping functions
of alcohol’, American Journal of Sociology 82, pp. 652–663.
Pearlin, L.I. and C. Schooler: 1978, ‘The structure of coping’, Journal of Health
and Social Behavior 19, pp. 2–22.
Phalet, K. and W. Claeys: 1993, ‘A comparative study of Turkish and Belgian
youth’, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 24, pp. 319–343.
Phinney, J.S., L.J. Santos and T. Madden: 1996, Individual Differences in
Perceived Discrimination among Minority and Immigrant Adolescents. Paper
316 UNSAL YETIM
presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral
Development, Quebec City, Canada.
Rosenberg, M.: 1965, Society and the Adolescent Self-image (Princeton Univer-
sity Press, Princeton, NJ).
Sampson, E.E.: 1989, ‘The challenge of social change for psychology: Glob-
alization and psychology’s theory of person’, American Psychologist 44,
pp. 914–921.
Sastry, J. and C.E. Ross: 1998, ‘Asian ethnicity and sense of personal control’,
Social Psychology Quarterly 61, pp. 101–121.
Schwartz, S.H.: 1994, ‘Beyond individualism/collectivism’, in U. Kim, H.
Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi et al. (eds.), Individualism and Collectivism: Theory,
Method, and Application (Sage, Beverly Hills, CA), pp. 85–119.
Sinha, D. and R.C. Tripathi: 1994, ‘Individualism in a collectivist culture: A case
of coexistence of opposites’, in U. Kim, H.C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S-C.
Choi and G. Yoon (eds.), Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method and
Applications (Sage, Thousand oaks, CA), pp. 123–136.
Suh, E., E. Diener, S. Oishi and H.C. Triandis: 1998, ‘The shifting basis of
life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms’, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 74, pp. 482–493.
Tabachnick, B.G. and L.S. Fidell: 1996, Using Multivariate Statistics (3rd edition)
(Harper Collins, Northridge, CA).
Tafarodi, R. and P. Walters: 1999, ‘Individualism-collectivism, life events, and
self-esteem: A test of two trade-offs’, European Journal of Social Psychology
29, pp. 797–814.
Triandis, H.C.: 1994, Culture and Social Behavior (McGraw-Hill, New York).
Triandis, H.C., K. Leung, M.V. Villareal and F.L. Clark: 1985, ‘Allocentric versus
idiocentric tendencies: Convergent and discriminant validation’, Journal of
Research in Personality 19, pp. 395–415.
Uskul, A.K.: 1998, How to Insult Effectively Across Cultures? Insults as Mirrors
of the Self. Unpublished master thesis, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The
Netherlands.
Verkuyten, M.: 1994, ‘Self-esteem among ethnic minority youth in Western
countries’, Social Indicators Research 32, pp. 21–47.
Weisz, J.R., F.M. Rothbaum and T.C. Blackburn: 1998, ‘Standing out and
standing in’, American Psychologists 39, pp. 955–969.
Wells, L.E. and G. Marwell: 1976, Self-esteem: Its Conceptualization and
Measurement (Sage, London).
Werkuyten, M. and S. Nekuee: 1999, ‘Subjective well-being, discrimination and
cultural conflict: Iranian living in the Netherlands’, Social Indicators Research
47, pp. 281–306.
World Values Study Group: 1994, World Values Survey, 1981–1984 and 1990–
1993. Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR)
version (computer file) (Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor).
THE IMPACTS OF INDIVIDUALISM 317
Yetim, U.: 1991, Reliability and Validity of Satisfaction with Life Scale in Turkish
form (in Turkish). Paper presented to the 6th National Psychology Confer-
ence, Istanbul, Turkey. In the state of Art Lectures of 6th National Psychology
Conference Book, 200-206, Istanbul University Publication.
Yetim, U.: 1993, ‘Life satisfaction: A study based on the organization of personal
projects’, Social Indicators Research 29, pp. 277–289.
Yetim, U.: 2001, The Happiness Scenes: From Society to Individual (in Turkish)
(Baglam, Istanbul).
Mersin University
Mersin
Tur key
... His study results revealed six dimensions (hexagon model) of well-being, which are physical, spiritual, emotional, occupational, intellectual and social well-being (Hettler, 1980). style, parenting style to be effective factors on students' QoL (Bailey & Miller,1998;Cha, 2003;Cheung, 2000;Pilcher, 1998;Yetim, 2003). Personality traits were also found to in uence one's subjective well-being (Cha, 2003). ...
... Personality traits were also found to in uence one's subjective well-being (Cha, 2003). Similarly, having high self-esteem was identi ed to predict high life satisfaction (Yetim, 2003). Additionally, the socio-economic status of the student is also considered to be an important predictor of QoL. ...
Preprint
Various factors contributing to students' quality of life in higher education have been researched in existing literature. Yet, the role of university culture on students' quality of life in higher education is an underexplored area. In the present study, we aimed to investigate the relationships between students' quality of life and university culture in Turkish higher education. The sample involved 617 undergraduate students studying in three privately funded foundation universities in Turkey. We used WHOQOL-BREF(TR) as an assessment tool to measure the four domains of quality of life (physical health, psychological, social relationships and environment). We used the Turkish version of OCAI instrument to measure university culture types (hierarchical, market and innovative team). Ordinal regression analysis was performed to determine the signi cant effects of university culture types on the domains of quality of life. This current study reveals novel ndings about Turkish higher education. Results demonstrate that innovative team culture affects the psychological and social relations domains of students' quality of life positively. One interesting nding is that positive correlation was identi ed between market culture and the physical health domain of QoL. In addition, we found economic status and gender to be signi cant predictors of students' quality of life. This current research provides university authorities and policy makers with an in-depth analysis for informed decision making and constructive strategy planning. It also presents an analysis of the quality of life of university students to inform future studies in higher education policy and management.
... Cronbach alpha internal consistency coefficient was reported as .86 and test re-test reliability was .73 (Yetim, 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
The transition to face-to-face education after COVID-19 has brought along new changes that need to be adapted for students. This study aims to reveal how the state anxiety levels of individuals predict life satisfaction through the adjustment to university life after post-COVID face-to-face education. Besides, the differences in anxiety, life satisfaction, and university adjustment in 2nd and 3rd-graders were tested. The study group consists of 263 university students. Data were collected through The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, The Adjustment to University Life Scale, and The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Independent groups t-test was used to test whether the scores differed at the grade level, and Structural Equation Modelling was used to test the hypothesized model. Research findings indicate that there is no difference between the levels of state anxiety, life satisfaction, and adjustment to university life of second and third-year students. In other words, there is no significant difference between students who started university face-to-face and online in terms of relevant variables after the pandemic. According to the structural equation modeling findings, state anxiety is a significant predictor of life satisfaction through adaptation to university life. The model-data fit was found to be good and the indirect effects were statistically significant. The findings were discussed in line with the relevant literature. Based on the findings of the present study, a focus on university adaptation programs and interventions can be recommended after possible crises in the future.
... Öznel iyi olmanın üst seviyelerde olduğu durumlarda yaşam doyumlarının da yüksek olması beklenir. Sportif faaliyetlerin bireylerin kişiliklerine olumlu etki ettiği, kendine güven ve kendine saygı duymada etkisi olduğu (Yıldız ve Ekici, 2017), beden sağlığı kazandırdığı, psikolojik ve fizyolojik yönden olumlu etkiler verdiği, stresi azalttığı, mutlu olma ve eğlenme gibi duygulara etki ettiği bilinmektedir (Yetim, 2001). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Paradigma, bilim insanlarının dünyaya bakış açısını şekillendirmek amacıyla kullanılmakta olan görüş demektir. Çok yönlü bir nitelik barındıran paradigma, yaklaşım ve modelleri çevreleyen ve araştırmacı bireyin olay ve olgularla alakalı durumdaki bakış açısını yahut dünya görüşünü ifade etmesine paradigma denmektedir. Bundan dolayı bilimsel konular üzerinde gerçekleştirilen her türlü araştırmada paradigmalar etken niteliktedir. Ayrıca araştırmacı bireyin kendi felsefi düşüncesine göre nicel, nitel yahut karma paradigmalardan birinden yararlanılabilir. Sporda Yeni Paradigmalar isimli kitabımız, spor bilimlerindeki akademik çalışmaları bir araya getirerek multidisipliner bir yaklaşımla hazırlanmıştır. Spor bilimleri alanındaki yenilikçi çalışmalara yer verilerek hazırlanan kitapta altı bölüm bulunmaktadır. Bu bölümlerde teknolojik yeniliklerin ve yeni yaklaşımların oluşturulması hedeflenmiştir. Ayrıca Spor eğitiminden ve spor yönetimine kadar farklı bölümler mevcuttur. Alandaki yenilikçi çalışmaların literatüre katkı sağlaması amaçlanmaktadır.
... Psychological resources are assumed to contribute not only to adaptation to stressful life events but also to subjective well-being and positive adjustment in everyday life (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Indeed, it was shown that mastery and optimism were positively related to life satisfaction, an indicator of well-being, among both adolescents and adults (Ben-Zur, 2003), among Turkish students and academic staff (Yetim, 2003), and among Israeli Jewish and Arab students (Zeidner & Ben-Zur, 2013). These resources, together with self-esteem and social support, were also shown to mediate the effects of health on students' general well-being (Ben-Zur, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to assess the interdependent relations among psychological resources, marital support, and marital satisfaction in married couples from the perspective of both dyad members, using the Actor-Partner-Interdependence-Model (APIM) approach. One hundred and fifty-one heterosexual married couples ( N = 302) completed questionnaires assessing psychological resources (dispositional optimism and sense of mastery), marital support, marital satisfaction, social desirability, and demographic variables. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test a proposed mediation model adopting the dyadic approach. It was found that husbands’ and wives’ own psychological resources contributed positively to their own perceptions of marital support and that marital support mediated the effects of these resources on their own (actor–actor effect) and on their spouses’ marital satisfaction (actor–partner effect). The results highlight the important contribution of each dyad member’s own psychological resources, as well as the valuable role of perceived support as a mediator, regardless of gender. Hence, clinical practice should encourage individuals to invest in maintaining their personal assets and abilities because of their positive effect on expanding intra- and inter- processes of well-being within the marriage.
Article
Full-text available
Happiness is an important element in human’s life. In the era of globalization and economic acceleration, humans are exposed to the pressures of work, self-status achievement and relationship problems with others. Happiness becomes a medium that can improve the well-being of human psychological elements. In achieving this desire, cultural elements become an important factor. Therefore, this study aims to explore the influence of culture through the construct of individualism and collectivism on individual happiness. This is a qualitative study using a scoping review method. Study revealed that constructs of individualism and collectivism that exist in a person's culture will affect their self-achievement and further determine aspects of the individual's happiness. Thus, understanding this cultural concept is significant in understanding human needs and desires to achieve happiness. Furthermore, it can be used as a guideline and basis in helping relationships and the development of policies related to increasing social mobility and the quality of human life. Kegembiraan merupakan elemen penting yang menjadi tumpuan dan matlamat manusia pada hari ini. Dalam era globalisasi dan kepesatan ekonomi, manusia tidak terkecuali menerima tempias tekanan dalam pekerjaan, pencapaian status diri dan masalah perhubungan dengan orang lain. Oleh yang demikian, kegembiraan menjadi medium yang dapat meningkatkan kesejahteraan elemen psikologi manusia. Dalam mencapai hasrat ini, elemen budaya menjadi faktor penting. Justeru, kajian ini bertujuan untuk melihat pengaruh budaya melalui konstruk individualisme dan kolektivisme terhadap kegembiraan individu. Kajian ini merupakan kajian kualitatif dengan menggunakan reka bentuk kajian skop. Dapatan kajian mendapati bahawa konstruk individualisme dan kolektivisme yang wujud dalam budaya seseorang itu akan mempengaruhi pencapaian kendiri mereka dan seterusnya menentukan aspek kegembiraan individu tersebut. Justeru, pemahaman terhadap konsep budaya ini adalah signifikan dalam memahami keperluan dan hasrat manusia bagi mencapai kegembiraan. Seterusnya, ia dapat dijadikan sebagai garis panduan dan asas dalam perhubungan menolong dan pembentukan dasar yang berkaitan dengan peningkatan mobiliti sosial dan kualiti hidup manusia.
Chapter
Life satisfaction among university students is a global concern that continues to draw attention. It defines how human beings assess the worth of their lives as well as how they discern the choices they make, the judgment of well-being, which can be estimated in terms of self-perceived capability to manage one’s mood and daily life occurrences. This chapter aims to analyze life satisfaction among first-year students. The chapter reviews the meaning of life satisfaction, measurement of life satisfaction, bottom-up and top-down theories, the obstacles to better life satisfaction and ways of enhancing it. The study uses secondary data by utilizing Google Scholar, Google, and Science Direct, among others. Study findings indicate that personal and educational factors influence the life satisfaction of students. Additional findings indicate the existence of a close relationship between life satisfaction and self-esteem. Finally, the study recommends that institutions need to create awareness and to provide a conducive environment which fosters positive life satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
Quality of life among students is considered an important contributor to academic success. Quantitative assessments of student well-being have identified it as a risk factor for academic procrastination and higher drop-out rates. Nevertheless, a subjective investigation into university students’ well-being is underexplored. In the present meta-synthesis, we identified 20 qualitative studies and implemented Critical Appraisal Skills Programme as an assessment tool. Three main themes emerged from our synthesis: pressures on students, social psychological barriers, and hierarchy. Our findings support the consensus that certain academic tracks such as medical and health sciences can act as a risk factor for student well-being. We propose an integration of both quantitative and qualitative assessment to reach a more holistic perspective into quality of life.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing on the literature on person-culture fit, we investigated how culture (assessed as national-level familism), personality (tapped by attachment styles) and their interactions predicted social network characteristics in 21 nations/areas (N = 2977). Multilevel mixed modeling showed that familism predicted smaller network size but greater density, tie strength, and multiplexity. Attachment avoidance predicted smaller network size, and lower density, tie strength, and multiplexity. Attachment anxiety was related to lower density and tie strength. Familism enhanced avoidance’s association with network size and reduced its association with density, tie strength, and multiplexity. Familism also enhanced anxiety’s association with network size, tie strength, and multiplexity. These findings contribute to theory building on attachment and culture, highlight the significance of culture by personality interaction for the understanding of social networks, and call attention to the importance of sampling multiple countries.
Article
Purpose This study aims to investigate the role of self-esteem in mediating the relationship between passive social networking usage (PSNU) and life satisfaction, as well as whether the relationships between PSNU and self-esteem/life satisfaction as well as self-esteem and life satisfaction vary by gender. Design/methodology/approach This research is conducted in compliance with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association’s study on a subject of 304 students. Study participants are invited to participate in the survey by completing the anonymous questionnaire regarding passive social network usage, self-esteem and life satisfaction. Findings The findings indicate that self-esteem mediated the relationship between PSNU and life satisfaction. Furthermore, gender moderated the self-esteem and life satisfaction relationship, and such an effect was stronger for females than males. Research limitations/implications This study, adopting a cross-sectional design and self-report scale, examined the relationship between PSNU and life satisfaction. However, the short study duration hindered establishing a cause-and-effect relationship. Credibility concerns arose from participant-induced noise variables in the self-report scale. Future research should use diverse methods to validate underlying mechanisms. Despite limitations, the study revealed self-esteem as a mediating factor, alleviating the negative impact of PSNU on life satisfaction. Both male and female users are encouraged to engage in self-education, valuing their self-esteem for heightened life satisfaction. Originality/value These findings contribute to the understanding of how passive social network usage predicts life satisfaction (mediating effect of self-esteem) based on social comparison theory and when self-esteem increases life satisfaction (moderating effect of gender) based on social role theory of gender differences.
Article
Full-text available
Life satisfaction in general, is considered as our evaluation of feeling, emotions, and sentiments about our own life and how pleased we are with it. Life satisfaction is a very critical parameter, therefore researchers are keen to investigate the various parameters which are going to affect it directly or indirectly. The present study was focused on to analyze the role of Self-esteem, Self-efficacy and Self-compassion which show direct or indirect association with the individuals Life satisfaction. The study use correlational research design. A sample of 102 individuals has been selected for this study from Delhi region of India.The findings obtained indicates a significant positive association between Self-esteem and Life satisfaction (r= 0.478) and regression analysis reveal that Self-esteem is considered as a vital predictor of Life satisfaction. The obtained values of correlation has also indicate a significant positive association between Self-efficacy and Life satisfaction (r= 0.337) and a significant positive association between Self-compassion and Life satisfaction (r= 0.379).The results clearly represents that Self-esteem, Self-efficacy and Self-compassion play crucial role for the development of Life Satisfaction among individuals.
Article
Full-text available
Changes in value priorities of university students between 1970s-1990s, gender differences in each time period, generation differences (between adolescents' and their parents') in the 1970s and basic dimensions of the university students' value orientations in the 1990s were examined Rokeach Value Survey was administered to 150 (32 females, 118 males;) students from Nacettepe University and some of their parents (27 mothers, 30 fathers) during the late 1970s; and to 114 students (22 females, 91 males, 1 unidentified) from the same university at the beginning of 1990s. In addition to the ranking procedure used in the 1970s, in the 1990s, respondents were also asked to rate values in terms of their importance using a 7-point scale of "not important - important". Findings demonstrated that the university students of the 1970s and 1990s were more similar in their values (especially in terms of terminal values) than different. However, a trend to attribute relatively more importance to individualistic values was observed in the 1990s. Similarly, gender-related findings indicated similarities to be move important than differences. On the other hand, generation differences in the 1970s were found to be relatively more important particularly in terms of terminal values; the students of the 1970s attributed more importance to individualistic values, whereas their parents considered socio-cultural-normative values to be more important. Thus, results indicated generation differences in value orientations to be relatively more apparent than cohort and gender differences. In the 1990s, value orientations of Autonomy, Self-Development- Maturation and Adjustment-Recognition-Love were identified as second-order value orientations. The implications of these value orientations and other findings were discussed from a cross-cultural perspective and with reference to self-developmental tendencies in the Turkish society.
Chapter
The question of what constitutes the good life has been pondered for millennia. Yet only in the last decades has the study of well-being become a scientific endeavor. This book is based on the idea that we can empirically study quality of life and make cross-society comparisons of subjective well-being (SWB). A potential problem in studying SWB across societies is that of cultural relativism: if societies have different values, the members of those societies will use different criteria in evaluating the success of their society. By examining, however, such aspects of SWB as whether people believe they are living correctly, whether they enjoy their lives, and whether others important to them believe they are living well, SWB can represent the degree to which people in a society are achieving the values they hold dear. The contributors analyze SWB in relation to money, age, gender, democracy, and other factors. Among the interesting findings is that although wealthy nations are on average happier than poor ones, people do not get happier as a wealthy nation grows wealthier. Bradford Books imprint
Article
• There are at least 2 general paths to a feeling of control. In primary control, individuals enhance their rewards by influencing existing realities (e.g., other people, circumstances, symptoms, or behavior problems). In secondary control, individuals enhance their rewards by accommodating to existing realities and maximizing satisfaction or goodness of fit with things as they are. It is argued that American psychologists' exclusive focus on primary control reflects a cultural context in which primary control is heavily emphasized and highly valued. In Japan, by contrast, primary control has traditionally been less highly valued and less often anticipated, and secondary control has assumed a more central role in everyday life. Japanese and American perspectives and practices are contrasted in childrearing, socialization, religion and philosophy, work, and psychotherapy. These comparisons reveal some key benefits, and some costs, of both primary and secondary approaches to control. The comparisons suggest that an important goal, both for individuals and for cultures, is an optimally adaptive blend of primary and secondary control. (116 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved) • There are at least 2 general paths to a feeling of control. In primary control, individuals enhance their rewards by influencing existing realities (e.g., other people, circumstances, symptoms, or behavior problems). In secondary control, individuals enhance their rewards by accommodating to existing realities and maximizing satisfaction or goodness of fit with things as they are. It is argued that American psychologists' exclusive focus on primary control reflects a cultural context in which primary control is heavily emphasized and highly valued. In Japan, by contrast, primary control has traditionally been less highly valued and less often anticipated, and secondary control has assumed a more central role in everyday life. Japanese and American perspectives and practices are contrasted in childrearing, socialization, religion and philosophy, work, and psychotherapy. These comparisons reveal some key benefits, and some costs, of both primary and secondary approaches to control. The comparisons suggest that an important goal, both for individuals and for cultures, is an optimally adaptive blend of primary and secondary control. (116 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This article reports the development and validation of a scale to measure global life satisfaction, the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). Among the various components of subjective well-being, the SWLS is narrowly focused to assess global life satisfaction and does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness. The SWLS is shown to have favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability. Scores on the SWLS correlate moderately to highly with other measures of subjective well-being, and correlate predictably with specific personality characteristics. It is noted that the SWLS is suited for use with different age groups, and other potential uses of the scale are discussed.