ArticlePDF Available

Does Gender Matter? Relationship of Gender, Spousal Support, Spirituality, and Dispositional Forgiveness to Pastoral Restoration

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We examined the relationship of gender, forgiveness, and spirituality to restoration attitudes expressed toward pastors who committed a transgression. In Study 1, participants’ restoration responses favored the opposite gender. In Study 2, men and women responded differently depending on the offense. Dispositional forgiveness was significantly associated with pastoral restoration following offenses of infidelity, anger, child abuse, and substance abuse. Measures of spirituality were primarily associated with forgiveness but explained little of the variance in restoration attitudes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
DOI 10.1007/s11089-007-0072-3
ORIGINAL PAPER
Does Gender Matter? Relationship of Gender, Spousal
Support, Spirituality, and Dispositional Forgiveness
to Pastoral Restoration
Geoffrey W. Sutton ·Kelly C. McLeland ·
Katherine L. Weaks ·Patricia E. Cogswell ·
Renee N. Miphouvieng
Published online: 9 March 2007
C
Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007
Abstract We examined the relationship of gender, forgiveness, and spirituality to restoration
attitudes expressed toward pastors who committed a transgression. In Study 1, participants’
restoration responses favored the opposite gender. In Study 2, men and women responded
differently depending on the offense. Dispositional forgiveness was significantly associated
with pastoral restoration following offenses of infidelity, anger, child abuse, and substance
abuse. Measures of spirituality were primarily associated with forgiveness but explained
little of the variance in restoration attitudes.
Keywords Clergy .Restoration .Forgiveness .Gender .Spirituality .Sexual offense
Introduction
History is replete with examples of leaders who have committed offenses against the peo-
ple they purport to govern. Heads of State, corporate executives, and religious leaders
have typically been men. However, in some societies, women have recently been enter-
ing leadership roles and becoming more involved in decisions that affect the restoration of
an errant leader. Clergy sexual transgressions are particularly damaging to individual vic-
tims and the faith communities they serve. Although the American Association of Pastoral
Counselors (1994) and other professional associations (see Sutton & Thomas, 2005a,fora
review of ethics codes) prohibit sexual contact with clients, problems persist. Following a
review of the relevant literature, we present two studies that explore gender, spirituality, for-
giveness, and restoration following various types of pastor transgressions. In one study, we
included spousal support for the offending pastor as a variable. In both studies, we considered
G. W. Sutton ()
Associate Professor of Psychology, Evangel University, 1111 N. Glenstone Ave.,
Springfield, MO 65802, USA
e-mail: suttong@evangel.edu
K. C. McLeland ·K. L. Weaks ·P. E. Cogswell ·R. N. Miphouvieng
Graduate Students in Clinical Psychology, Evangel University, Springfield, MO, USA
Springer
646 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
participant characteristics that could affect their attitudes toward restoring a pastor following
a moral transgression.
Transgressions, forgiveness, and restoration
Despite the farrago of forgiveness research, researchers conceptualize forgiveness in different
ways (Sutton & Thomas, 2005b; Thompson et al., 2005; Worthington & Scherer, 2004).
Several researchers view forgiveness as one method that can alleviate the distress condition
labeled unforgiveness (Worthington, Berry, & Parrott, 2001; Worthington & Wade, 1999),
which occurs following an interpersonal offense. In the forgiveness process, victims can
attain a peaceful intrapersonal state in which they let go of anger and negative feelings and
cease to avoid the offender. Some researchers hold that a benevolent attitude is a necessary
component of forgiveness (e.g., Enright & Gassin, 1992; McCullough, Fincham, & Tsang,
2003) whereas others do not (Thompson et al., 2005). Furthermore, most researchers have
identified what forgiveness is not and in so doing, have separated the concept of forgiveness
from that of reconciliation (Fincham, Beach, & Davila, 2004; Sutton & Thomas, 2005b)and
restoration (Sutton & Thomas, 2005b). However, Kanz (2000) found that a majority of his
student sample viewed reconciliation as a part of forgiveness. Similarly, Aquino, Tripp, and
Bies (2001) considered reconciliation to be a behavioral manifestation of forgiveness.
Conceptually, we agree with those who view forgiveness as an intrapersonal process that
is distinct from reconciliation (Sutton & Thomas, 2005b). That is, a victim can experience
forgiveness toward an offender without engaging in any prosocial behavior toward the
offender. In contrast, we view reconciliation as an interpersonal experience that involves the
development of trust stimulated by prosocial behaviors. We agree with Sutton and Thomas
(2005b) that forgiveness enhances the reconciliation process and attenuates the extent of
reconciliation.
Restoration is a concept that can encompass forgiveness and reconciliation. When people
forgive and experience an inner release of negative emotions, they have taken steps toward
a restoration to wholeness. When an offender and a victim display prosocial behaviors
following a transgression that has damaged the relationship, they engage in a process of
restoring a relationship. However, when people have lost their leadership position in a
community along with their sociopsychological status, restoration describes the process of
the tasks required to regain a position and status within a community. In this article, we refer
to pastoral restoration in the sense of a return to a ministry position within a faith community
following a transgression that offended those in the community who have both the power to
remove and restore the pastor.
If a married pastor had sexual relations with a congregant, the research suggests that the
congregant could suffer various problems of distress. Luepker (1999) found that women who
had been sexually abused by a pastor or psychotherapist, experienced such psychological
conditions as Post-traumatic Stress Disorder, depression, and suicidality. In addition, Disch
and Avery (2001) found that people who had experienced sexual boundary violations by
mental health or medical professionals or clergy experienced such symptoms as emotional
turmoil, isolation, shame, fear, rage, self-blame, and problems with trust. Moreover, the
spouses of married pastors are additional victims, that would likely experience considerable
anger and negative emotions. Both victims, the spouse and the congregant, would likely ex-
perience unforgiveness. In time, they might learn to forgive the offender. One or both victims
might attempt some degree of reconciliation. Beyond the intrapersonal and interpersonal
sequelae, the church leadership would likely remove the offending pastor from leadership
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 647
status. If the pastor completed a rehabilitation plan, the pastor might experience some degree
of restoration within the faith community. Theoretically, the community restoration could be
independent of any forgiveness and reconciliation process that might occur for the victimized
spouse and congregant. That is, if the victims (congregant, spouse) did not forgive the pastor
and if they did not attempt reconciliation, community representatives could still restore the
pastor to ministry. Nevertheless, we expect that if one or both victims expressed forgiveness
and responded to reconciliation, then these actions would facilitate community restoration
for the pastor.
The preceding paragraph is primarily theoretical because we wished to explain the dis-
tinctions we are making among the concepts of forgiveness, reconciliation, and restoration.
Few studies have examined the restoration responses of congregants to the sexual transgres-
sions of pastors. Thoburn and Balswick (1998) found that 15.6% of seminarians engaged
in extramarital sexual activity. The Hartford Institute for Religion Research (2002) found
that 23% of 532 Christian congregations experienced a pastor-congregation disruption due
to a pastor’s sexual activity. These data, coupled with the evidence cited above regarding the
impact of pastoral sexual transgressions on victims, demonstrate the importance of the sexual
transgression problem. Opinions vary as to whether pastors should be restored. Hughes and
Armstrong (1995) opined that clergy should not be restored to pastoral leadership. Wells
(2003) found that 44% of his sample believed an errant pastor should not return to an active
pastoral ministry but noted that 74% believed clergy should be allowed to return to ministry
positions where they do not have contact with victims. In an experimental study, Sutton and
Thomas (2005a) presented experienced pastors with case scenarios that varied a pastor’s age
and type of sexual offense. They found that the participants were more willing to restore a
younger pastor who had a sexual relationship with a congregant than a middle-aged pastor,
but age was not a factor for pastors involved in an inappropriate romantic relationship with a
congregant. Pop, Sutton, and Jones (2005) found that undergraduates at a Christian university
who considered self-interest and participated in a group discussion about a pastor’s sexual
transgression were more willing to restore a pastor than those who considered self-interest
and acted alone.
Are some people more forgiving than others are? Researchers have begun to examine
forgiveness in relationship to personality variables. For example, students’ dispositional
forgiveness, measured by the Transgression Narrative Test of Forgiveness was negatively
correlated with anger and neuroticism and positively correlated with agreeableness (Berry,
Worthington, Parrott, O’Connor, & Wade, 2001). DeShea (2003) reported a positive cor-
relation between the Willingness to Forgive Scale and agreeableness in an undergraduate
sample. Brown (2003) reported that the Tendency to Forgive Scale predicted depression,
life-satisfaction, and situational forgiveness in an undergraduate sample. In this article, we
refer to this willingness, or tendency to forgive, as dispositional forgiveness, which appears
to be consistent with the few extant empirical studies.
In the present studies, we examined the relationship between forgiveness and restoration
in two ways. In the first study, we measured forgiveness responses and restoration responses
toward married pastors who became sexually involved with a married congregant. In both
studies, we examined the relationship between level of dispositional forgiveness of the
participants and their responses to pastor transgressions.
Gender, forgiveness, and restoration
Researchers have found that women are more empathic than men are (e.g., Gault & Sabini,
2000; Macaskill, Maltby, & Day, 2002). Other researchers have found empathy to be
Springer
648 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
positively correlated with forgiveness (Fincham, Paleari, & Regalia, 2002;Toussaint&
Webb, 2005). However, researchers have not found gender differences on measures of for-
giveness (e.g., Toussaint & Webb, 2005).
In studies of interpersonal relationships, Finkel, Rusbult, Kumashiro, and Hannon (2002)
found a tendency for men and women to report differences in prosocial behaviors. Restora-
tion includes prosocial behaviors, and expressed forgiveness may serve as a catalyst for
prosocial behaviors. Some researchers have found evidence that suggested a link between
pastor or participant gender and restoration responses. Sutton and Thomas (2005a) examined
restoration attitudes toward a male pastor who became sexually involved with a congregant.
A few female pastors were among the respondents. Although there were too few women to
assess gender differences, the authors reported a tendency for women to respond differently
to questions about restoring a male pastor and recommended further study of gender differ-
ences in restoration. Pop, Sutton, and Jones (2005) found that group discussion affected the
outcome of pastoral restoration. They used gender as a covariate in the analysis because of
the correlation between participant gender and the restoration measure and suggested that
researchers include gender as a variable in future studies. Sutton, Washburn, Comtois, and
Moeckel (2006) examined referral attitudes toward community agencies where supervisors
had an affair with a client. They found that type of offense negatively affected level of com-
fort with a referral and trust in the agency regardless of supervisor gender; however, social
work students were significantly more likely to increase referral follow-up to a male rather
than a female community supervisor.
Infidelity is one of the most troublesome problems a couple can face. Wiederman (1997)
analyzed national survey data and found men reported more extramarital sexual relation-
ships (22.5%) than did women (11.6%). Harris (2000) reviewed research on physiological
responses to infidelity and reported findings that purported to identify different responses for
men and women. According to the specific innate modular theory of jealousy, men are more
upset by sexual infidelity and women are more upset by emotional infidelity. Although her
findings supported greater physiological responses for men than for women, the responses
could not be attributed to jealousy as distinct from general physiological responses to sexual
imagery. In addition, women with sexual experience had stronger physiological responses
to sexual infidelity than to emotional infidelity. That is, women with sexual experience re-
sponded in a similar fashion to men. Given these limited findings about gender differences,
we included both pastor gender and participant gender in our studies.
Spousal support, forgiveness, and restoration
How does the marital status of a pastor affect attitudes of congregants following a pastor’s
sexual transgression? In a study of 1,865 senior pastors, Barna Research (2001), found that
more than nine of ten pastors were married men and that 13% had been divorced at least one
time. In general, marital stress negatively affects marital satisfaction as well as physical and
mental health (Hoyt, Fincham, McCullough, Maio, & Davila, 2005; McLeland & Sutton,
2005). In addition, level of marital satisfaction was a factor in pastoral infidelity (Thoburn
& Whitman, 2004). Moreover, Whisman, Dixon, and Johnson (1997) found that therapists
considered infidelity as one of the most difficult problems to treat. Clearly, an act of pastor
infidelity is a stressor that has the potential to destroy the pastor’s marriage and to affect many
members within a community of faith. Sutton and Thomas (2005a) found that experienced
pastors believed that the successful restoration of a male pastor’s marriage following sexual
activity with a congregant would be greater for a younger rather than a middle-aged pastor.
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 649
There can be many reasons for a spouse to remain in a marriage following an act of infidelity.
What is the effect on congregants when a pastor’s spouse remains in a relationship that has
been damaged by an extramarital relationship? To answer this question, we included the
response of the pastor’s spouse (remain in the marriage or seek a divorce) as a variable in
the first study.
Spirituality, forgiveness, and restoration
More than 100 years ago, James (1902) wrote about the relationship between psychology and
religion. Recently, there has been a resurgence of interest in psychology and religion with an
emphasis on the importance of spirituality to human well-being. Richards and Bergin (1997)
provided broad guidance, including a model, for the role of spirituality in counseling and
psychotherapy. Worthington and Sandage (2001) reviewed nine existing studies on religion
and psychotherapy and urged clinicians to be sensitive to the importance of spirituality for
highly religious clients. Other researchers (Hathaway, Scott, & Garver, 2004) found that
clinicians minimally considered client spirituality in clinical practice. Further, Miller and
Thoresen (2003) provided an overview of research on spirituality and health and described
many problems including a lack of a clear distinction between the concepts of spirituality and
religion in the psychology literature. In fact, they noted a common finding that researchers
reduced spirituality or religiosity to a measure of church attendance. In this article, we follow
the lead of Miller and Thoresen and consider spirituality as a multidimensional concept that
involves more than the practices of religion, but spirituality may include observable evidence
such as church attendance and participation in prayer and other religious activities.
Although there is a long history of interest in religion and psychology, the focus on
discovering empirical relationships between spirituality and psychological variables is recent
(e.g., Hathaway, Scott, & Garver, 2004; Worthington & Sandage, 2001). Therefore, it is
not surprising that we found few studies describing the relationships among spirituality,
forgiveness, and restoration. From a theoretical perspective, forgiveness has been examined
in the context of Christian traditions (Marty, 1998) and Jewish traditions (Dorff, 1998)as
well as in relation to religious coping (Pargament & Rye, 1998). In an empirical study,
Edwards et al. (2002) found a positive relationship between religious faith and forgiveness.
They operationally defined religious faith using the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith
Questionnaire (Plante & Boccaccini, 1997) and measured forgiveness based on the Heartland
Forgiveness Scale (Thompson et al., 2005). Rye (2005) reviewed the results of religious and
secular forgiveness interventions and found no consistent differences. However, he noted that
participants’ remarks suggested that many drew upon their religious traditions regardless of
the type of intervention they received. Given the rather obvious role of forgiveness in the
history of many religions (Rye, 2005), the advice of Finkel et al. (2002) to include measures
of spirituality in forgiveness and relationship research is timely.
One area of growing interest in the psychology of religion has been the application of
attachment theory (e.g., Bowlby, 1982) to the study of the attachment relationship between
people and God (e.g., Rowatt & Kirkpatrick, 2002; Kirkpatrick, 1997). Extending the con-
ceptual work of Ainsworth (1989) and Kirkpatrick (1998), Beck and McDonald (2004)and
McDonald, Beck, Allison, and Norsworthy (2005) explored attachment to God and attach-
ment to parents using the Attachment to God Inventory (AGI; Beck & McDonald, 2004).
The AGI operationally defined the attachment relationship in terms of avoidance of God
and anxiety toward God. Researchers would be unreasonable to limit the study of person-
God relationships to the attachment model operationally defined by the AGI; however, the
Springer
650 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
attachment studies illustrated one aspect of potential multidimensional methods that re-
searchers can employ to explore the relationship between spiritual and psychological vari-
ables. Given this context for spirituality, we elected to examine two direct measures of spir-
ituality in addition to collecting background information. Specifically, we assessed strength
of religious faith and attachment to God along with measures of forgiveness and restoration.
Research overview and hypotheses
Our purpose was to explore factors related to the restoration of clergy who have committed
a transgression within a faith community that resulted in a loss of position. Specifically,
we wanted to build on previous research (Sutton & Thomas, 2005a) that identified type of
offense and age as factors that could affect restoration attitudes. In these studies, we explored
gender, spousal support, forgiveness, spirituality, and various types of pastor transgression.
We also considered several participant characteristics that could affect their attitudes toward
restoring a pastor.
In Study 1, we manipulated youth-pastor gender (man, woman) and spousal support
(remained married, divorced) following a sexual relationship between the pastor and a
congregant. All relationships were heterosexual. We included participant gender as a quasi-
independent variable and measured forgiveness and restoration responses. We hypothesized
that gender (pastor, participant, or both in interaction) would have an effect on restoration,
but we did not predict a direction. In addition, we hypothesized that the participants would
more likely favor restoration if the spouse were supportive (i.e., did not seek a divorce).
Finally, we included measures of spirituality and dispositional forgiveness to explore the
relationship of these variables to restoration. We hypothesized that spirituality would be
significantly related to dispositional forgiveness and that both spirituality and forgiveness
would be significantly associated with restoration. Lastly, we hypothesized that men and
women would be similar on measures of spirituality and forgiveness because we had no
research to support differences.
In Study 2, we used a correlational strategy to examine gender differences in association
with dispositional forgiveness on restoration. Specifically we hypothesized that women and
men would have different views regarding support for restoration depending on the type
of transgression and their level of dispositional forgiveness (high or low). In addition, we
hypothesized that people who were generally more disposed toward forgiveness would be
more likely to favor restoration than those who were less disposed toward forgiveness.
As in Study 1, we measured spirituality to explore the relationship between spirituality,
forgiveness, and restoration. We hypothesized that spirituality would be significantly related
to both forgiveness and restoration. We continued to expect similar scores for men and
women on the measures of spirituality and forgiveness.
Study 1
The major purpose of Study 1 was to explore the relationship of gender to restoration
attitudes. Specifically, we were interested in whether pastor gender or participant gender
would be associated with the restoration attitudes expressed by the participants. Because
the participants were young adults, we prepared hypothetical cases in which the pastor
was a youth pastor. The participants were congregants in a protestant denomination that
includes women as pastors and would have seen both men and women speak in student
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 651
chapels. Thus, we were able to study gender from two perspectives: A pastor’s gender and
the participant’s gender. In addition, we varied the reaction of the spouse within the narrative
as supportive (remained married following the transgression) or nonsupportive (pursued
divorce). Moreover, as noted above, we included measures of dispositional forgiveness,
spirituality, and background variables that may be related to restoration attitudes.
Method
Participants and design
Sixty-seven volunteers (23 men, 44 women; mean age=18.84 years; SD =.91) participated
in the experiment. They were mostly of European descent (73%) with 22% reporting other
and 2% not reporting. Most (91%) were undergraduates in their first two years of college.
Less than half (37%) reported knowing a pastor who had a problem that affected ministry
and 26.9% reported knowing a pastor who had a sexual problem while in the ministry. Two
participants were ministers, and 15 were children of ministers.
We used a between-groups design and presented four narratives that varied pastor gender
(man, woman) and spousal support following transgression (marriage continued or ended in
divorce). The third variable was the gender of the participant.
Materials and procedure
In Midwestern Christian university classrooms, participants completed consent forms, read
the hypothetical narratives, and completed the dependent measures. They were debriefed
and thanked by a research assistant.
We manipulated pastor gender by referring to the pastor as a man or woman in the first
few words and by using appropriate pronouns throughout the one-paragraph narratives. In
addition, we manipulated spousal support of the pastor by explicitly stating whether the
marriage continued or ended in a divorce. We identified variables that could affect attitudes
toward restoration and held them constant in the narratives (e.g., pastor age [26], length of
extramarital relationship [three months], apologetic response of the pastor). Following is the
narrative for the male youth pastor whose wife agreed to remain in the relationship:
A 26-year-old man worked as a youth pastor. He worked closely with a young woman
on several special projects. On one occasion, he took her home and left her residence
just as her husband came home unexpectedly during a workday. After speaking to
his wife about the unusual visit, she confessed to feeling terribly upset about how
things had changed between her and the youth pastor in the last three months. They
had often worked together on special projects and discussed how much they had in
common. Their visits had become distinctly romantic and sexual in the past month. The
husband was both angry and devastated when he learned of the relationship. Fearing
what he might do if he confronted the youth pastor, he called a church board member
who subsequently met with the full board and the pastor. The youth pastor appeared
overwhelmed by guilt and confessed to having an affair with the woman. He affirmed
the woman’s story in essential details and apologized for his behavior. He had no prior
history of unfaithfulness. Initially the youth pastor’s wife of three years was distraught
and was advised to seek individual counseling. She spent a month away at a relative’s
home before returning to her husband. After two months, she agreed to participate with
her husband in a rehabilitation program.
Springer
652 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
Dependent measures
Each participant read only one narrative and completed a global rating of overall restoration
and Likert-type items that addressed attitudes of forgiveness and restoration toward the youth
pastor. The items were similar to those used by Sutton and Thomas (2005a). The global item
read: “Based on the situation presented, please give us your overall rating of the most
reasonable outcome for the youth pastor.” The ratings for the global item ranged from a
rating of one (no restoration to any public or nonpublic ministry) to seven (full restoration
to the same public ministry). Following administration, we organized the Likert-type items
into two groups based on content and correlational analysis. All items were rated on a seven-
point scale from one (very strongly agree) to seven (very strongly disagree). Three items
included pastoral forgiveness and restoration (PFR; coefficient alpha =.92) and three items
referred to pastor restoration (PR) without including the concept of forgiveness (coefficient
alpha =.60). An example of a PFR item is: “The victim or victims offended by the person
need to forgive the person before the person can be restored to the same public ministry
position.” An example of a PR item is: “The person should be restored to a similar ministry
position with supervision.” High scores (e.g., 6, 7) indicate a highly favorable attitude, and
group scores were averaged for ease of comparison.
Other measures
Following the recommendation of Finkel et al. (2002), we measured spirituality because it
may be a factor affecting forgiveness ratings. We used the abbreviated edition of the Santa
Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire (ASCSRFQ; Plante, Vallaeys, Sherman, &
Wallston, 2002). The ASCSRFQ is a five–item scale by which participants rate their level
of agreement with statements such as “My religious faith is extremely important to me” or
“I look to my faith as a source of comfort.” In previous studies, coefficient alpha had been
reasonable. For example, Hoover and Sutton (2005) reported the same alpha value that we
obtained (.83). In the four studies reported by Plante et al. (2002), the means ranged from
13.56 to 19.2. The 5-item measure correlated with the longer 10-item measure in the range
of r=.95 to .99 among four samples. For the 67 participants in our sample, the scores were
negatively skewed (M=18.06, SD =2.37, skew =−1.89) and leptokurtic (4.71).
In addition, we assessed forgiveness as a dispositional trait using the DeShea Willingness
to Forgive Scale (DWTFS; DeShea, 2003). The DWTFS requires participants to read and
respond to 12 scenarios based on their willingness to forgive the offender in a given scenario.
The scenarios included a range of situations that would be familiar to young adults (e.g.,
unpaid loan, rejection by dating partner) and thus was suitable for our participants. The scale
response options range from zero (not at all willing to forgive) to six (completely willing to
forgive). DeShea (2003) found that the results of factor analysis supported one factor. She
reported coefficient alphas of .91 (M=26.96, SD =13.12) and .89 in two studies. In our
study alpha was .92 (M=38.60, SD =12.86).
Results and discussion
Preliminary analyses
Measures of skew and kurtosis were acceptable for the dependent measures. Table 1con-
tains the means and standard deviations by gender for the dependent measures. In addi-
tion, we examined the relationship between several demographic variables (participant age,
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 653
Tab l e 1 Study 1: Means and standard deviations of dependent variables by gender
Female participant Male participant
Pastor Male (n=24) Female (n=19) Male (n=9) Female (n=14)
variables Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
PR 3.15 .82 2.83 1.03 2.22 1.01 3.43 .99
PFR 4.01 1.18 3.39 1.53 4.11 1.86 4.19 1.92
Global 3.12 1.39 2.95 1.35 2.78 1.64 3.22 1.63
Note. PR is a 7-point measure of pastor restoration. PFR is a 7-point measure of pastor forgiveness and
restoration. Global is a single 7-point rating of overall favorability toward restoration.
relationship status, experience with a pastor who had a problem [including type of problem]
in the ministry, and whether the participant was related to a minister) that we thought might
affect the outcomes and the dependent measures and found no significant relationships
(ps>.05).
We examined the measures of spirituality (ASCSRFQ) and dispositional forgiveness
(DWTFS) for gender differences. For the ASCSRFQ, men (n=23, M=17.48, SD =2.52)
were similar to women (n=44, M=18.36, SD =2.26), t(65) =−1.46, p=.15. In addition,
for the DWTFS, men (n=23, M=37.24, SD =15.44) were also similar to women (n=44,
M=39.30, SD =11.42), t(65) =−.62, p=.54.
Main analyses
We conducted a three-way (pastor gender ×supportive spouse ×participant gender)
MANOVA and found significance for the pastor gender×participant gender interaction,
Wilks’s =.871, F(2, 57) =4.23, p=.02, η2=.13. The main effects and other interactions
were not significant (ps>.05). Because the supportive spouse variable contributed so little
to the model (η2=.02) and reduced the cell sizes, we reanalyzed the data to focus on the two
gender conditions. The two-factor (gender ×gender) MANOVA revealed little change from
the three-factor MANOVA for the significant gender ×gender interaction, Wilks’s =.866,
F(2, 61) =4.72, p=.01, η2=.13. Follow-up ANOVAs indicated the interaction effect was
not evident for items that measured both forgiveness and restoration (p>.05), but was
evident for those items that measured solely restoration, F(1, 62) =9.39, p=.00, η2=.13.
Women were significantly less willing to restore a female pastor (M=2.83, SD =1.03) than
amalepastor(M=3.43, SD =.99) and men were significantly less willing to restore a male
pastor (M=2.22, SD =1.01) than a female pastor (M=3.15, SD =.82). An ANOVA for the
global restoration item did not yield significant results (p>.05).
Additional analyses
In addition to the experimental manipulation, we wished to explore the relationship of
two participant characteristics (spirituality, forgiving disposition) to restoration. Thus, we
began by examining the correlations between the measures of spirituality (ASCSRFQ),
dispositional forgiveness (DWTFS) and restoration (PR: See Table 2). For our sample,
spirituality was strongly related to a general willingness to forgive (r=.37, p=.002) and
remained so (r=−.38, p=.002) after data transformation (square root and reflection) to
attenuate skew and kurtosis. One ASCSRFQ item (item 3) may be more desirable than others
because the skew and kurtosis did not require an adjustment and it was strongly associated
Springer
654 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
Tab l e 2 Study 1: Correlations, means, and standard deviations
Measure 1 2 3 MSDn
1. Abbreviated Santa Clara Strength
of Religious Faith Questionnaire
(ASCSRFQ)
– .366∗∗ .174 18.06 2.37 67
2. Willingness to Forgive Scale
(DWTFS)
– .305∗∗ 38.60 12.86 67
3. Restoration (PR) 2.99 1.00 66
Note. ∗∗Significant at .01 (one-tailed). Measures: 1. Abbreviated Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith
Questionnaire, 2. deShea Willingness to Forgive Scale, 3. pastor restoration.
with willingness to forgive (r=.35, p=.004) even when we used a more stringent pvalue
(.01) to reduce the risk of a Type I error. Neither the 5-item ASCSRFQ nor item 3 was
significantly associated with restoration (p>.05), but willingness to forgive was significantly
associated with restoration (r=.31, p=.01). At this point, we computed regression equations
with restoration as the criterion variable and the gender ×gender interactions entered in the
first block followed by willingness to forgive and spirituality in the second block. As expected,
based on the MANOVA, the first model with gender ×gender interactions explained a
significant portion of the variance in restoration (R=.38, R2=.14, R2
adj =.10) although the
female-pastor to male-participant group interaction was dropped from the model. Only the
male-pastor to male-participant group term was significant (B=−.321, t=−2.5, p=.02).
In the second model, willingness to forgive was retained (R=.46, R2=.21,R2
adj =.16) and
the change in R2was significant (R2=.07, Fchange (1, 61) =5.45, p=.02). In model 2,
both the male-pastor to male-participant group term (B=−.28, t=−2.27, p=.03) and
willingness to forgive (B=−.27, t=2.33, p=.02) were significant. As expected from the
intercorrelations, spirituality did not significantly contribute to the model.
Discussion
The data indicate that gender contributes to an understanding of restoration attitudes toward
pastors who had a sexual relationship with a congregant. However, the relationship is not
simple because congregants are generally less favorable toward pastors of their same gender.
We used the phrase less favorable because the ratings were on a 7-point scale and all means
were below the scale midpoint of 4.0. In addition, the congregants appeared to discern
between forgiveness and restoration in that when they rated items that included both the
concept of forgiveness and the concept of restoration, there were no differences among the
groups. In contrast, differences were evident when the items only contained the concept of
restoration to ministry. In terms of the hypotheses, we were able to find support for a gender
effect, but this was as an interaction effect and not a simple effect. We did not find support for
our hypothesis about spousal support. The failure to find an effect for spousal support could
be related to the limited marital experience of the predominantly single-student sample.
The results from our additional exploration indicate support for our hypotheses regarding
a significant relationship between spirituality and dispositional forgiveness and between
dispositional forgiveness and pastoral restoration. We did not find support for our hypothesis
regarding a significant relationship between spirituality and restoration. The data supported
our hypotheses regarding gender similarity on measures of spirituality and forgiveness.
Finally, we found that both gender and dispositional forgiveness contribute to an explanation
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 655
of restoration. The gender emphasis seems to be weighted toward the attitudes of men toward
men rather than other gender combinations.
Study 2
In Study 2, we used a correlational strategy to examine the relationship among gender,
dispositional forgiveness, and restoration attitudes. We assessed the disposition to forgive
using the DeShea Willingness to Forgive Scale (DeShea, 2003) and divided the women and
men into high- and low-forgiveness groups using a median split. Each participant read and
rated ten scenarios containing common transgressions likely to affect a pastor’s leadership
within a faith community. We analyzed the data using a between-groups analysis of variance
procedure where participant gender (man or woman) and level of forgiveness (high or
low) formed a 2 ×2 design and the restoration ratings following the various scenarios
were the dependent variables. As in Study 1, we gathered additional information about
the participants’ spirituality and background to explore relationships among spirituality,
forgiveness, and pastoral restoration.
Method
Participants and design
The participants were 78 undergraduate students in introduction to psychology classes. Two
students did not complete the demographic information and were removed from the study
leaving 76 volunteers (36 men, 40 women). Four of the participants were clergy and 24 were
children of clergy. The mean age of the participants was 18.78 years (SD =.97). The mean
education level of participants was 13.08 years (SD =1.58). Of the 76 participants, 11 were
from minority ethnic groups, including African American (n=3), Asian (n=1), Hispanic
(n=5), and Native American (n=2). Less than half (29.5%) reported knowing a pastor who
had a relationship problem that affected ministry and 20.5% reported knowing a pastor who
had a sexual problem while in the ministry.
Materials and procedure
Participants, who signed a consent form, completed the packet of measures and a de-
mographic form at the end of a class period. They were debriefed and thanked for their
participation.
Measures
As in Study 1, we measured dispositional forgiveness using the DeShea Willingness to
Forgive Scale (DWTFS; DeShea, 2003). In this study, coefficient alpha was .88. We used the
median DWTFS score to divide the participants into two groups (high and low willingness
to forgive). We also included the ASCSRFQ (Plante et al., 2002) to measure the strength of
religious faith. In this study, coefficient alpha was .83.
We added the Attachment to God Inventory (AGI: Beck & McDonald, 2004)asanad-
ditional measure of the spiritual relationship between a participant and God. The measure
has two subscales with acceptable alpha values (Avoidance .85 and Anxiety .83). Each
subscale has 14 items rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1-Disagree Strongly to
Springer
656 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
7-Agree Strongly. In their student sample (Beck & McDonald, 2004), the AGI means (Anx-
iety M=47.03, SD =13.11; Avoidance M=41.06, SD =11.42) were higher than for their
community sample (Anxiety M=36.74, SD =15.03; Avoidance M=36.91, SD =13.83).
We measured restoration by scoring participant responses to ten transgression scenarios
in which a pastor violated a common social norm (Sutton & Thomas, 2004). There were
no indications of pastor gender in the scenarios. The Likert-type ratings ranged from one
(no restoration to ministry) to seven (full restoration to the position previously held). The
transgression scenarios included violations of personal character norms (anger management
problem, pornography), substance abuse (alcohol abuse, pain medication abuse, marijuana
abuse), interpersonal issues of infidelity (infidelity lasting a year, a second offense of in-
fidelity, an inappropriate romantic relationship), child abuse, and financial irresponsibility
(one item about embezzlement).
Results and discussion
Preliminary analyses
Measures of skew and kurtosis were acceptable for the dependent measures. Table 3contains
the means and standard deviations by gender and level of forgiveness for the restoration
measures. In addition, we examined the relationship between several demographic variables
(participant age, experience with a pastor who had a problem in the ministry, and whether
the participant was related to a pastor) and the dependent measures and found no significant
results (ps>.05).
We explored the relationships between the measures of spirituality, a forgiving disposi-
tion, and restoration (see Table 4). First, we computed the intercorrelations between the mea-
sures of spirituality (ASCSRFQ, AGI), dispositional forgiveness (DWTFS) and restoration.
Strength of religious faith (ASCSRFQ) was not significantly related to an anxious attachment
to God (AGI Anxiety subscale) but it was inversely related to an avoidant attachment to God
(AGI Avoidance subscale; r=−.309, p=.003). Strength of religious faith (ASCSRFQ)
was not significantly related to dispositional forgiveness (DWTFS; r=.04, p=.35) or any
of the restoration measures (all ps>.05). As expected, the AGI subscales of avoidance and
Tab l e 3 Study 2: Group means and standard deviations of dependent variables
High forgivers Low forgivers
Men (n=19) Women (n=19) Men (n=17) Women (n=21)
Offense Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Adultery 1 year 3.68 1.70 3.37 1.77 2.65 1.80 2.52 1.50
Adultery 2nd offense 3.16 1.77 2.95 2.09 2.12 1.45 2.05 1.50
Romantic affair 4.95 1.51 4.63 1.42 3.94 1.64 3.52 1.66
Alcohol 4.58 1.46 4.63 1.54 3.65 1.77 4.52 1.86
Pain Meds 5.16 1.61 5.26 1.28 3.65 1.77 4.52 1.78
Marijuana 4.79 1.71 3.79 1.81 2.65 1.66 3.81 1.91
Embezzlement 3.84 1.74 4.42 1.77 3.24 1.44 4.19 1.69
Anger 4.68 1.60 4.00 1.76 3.29 1.76 3.95 1.80
Pornography 4.53 1.87 3.74 1.97 3.18 2.10 3.52 1.86
Child Abuse 4.11 2.11 2.68 1.86 2.29 2.08 2.16 1.50
Note. All 10 offense scenarios were rated on a 7-point Likert type scale where high scores reflected
favorable restoration ratings toward a pastor whose gender was not reported.
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 657
Tab l e 4 Study 2: Intercorrelations for the Measures
Measure12 345678 910
1. AGI Anx .428∗∗ .077 .117 .160 .085 .061 .198.183 .035
2. AGI Avoid .292∗∗ .309∗∗ .026 .021 .157 .074 .032 .063
3. DWTFS .044 .446∗∗ .348∗∗ .270∗∗ .272∗∗ .327∗∗ .346∗∗
4. ASCSRFQ .017 .084 .099 .072 .059 .090
5. Infidelity .489∗∗ .279∗∗ .456∗∗ .882∗∗ .692∗∗
6. Substance – – – .489∗∗ .590∗∗ .635∗∗ .544∗∗
7. Embezzle ––––.279
∗∗ .418∗∗ .266∗∗
8.Anger– –––– .419∗∗ .518∗∗
9.Porn– ––––– – .558∗∗
Note. Significant at .05, ∗∗Significant at .01 (one-tailed). Measures: 1. AGI Anxiety, 2. AGI Avoidance, 3. DeShea Willingness to Forgive, 4. Abbreviated Santa Clara Strength
of Religious Faith Questionnaire. Items 5–10 are restorations ratings from the restoration scenarios: 5. Infidelity, 6. Substance abuse, 7. Embezzlement, 8. Anger, 9. Pornography,
10. Child abuse.
Springer
658 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
anxious styles of attachment were positively correlated with each other (r=.428, p=.00).
Avoidance was negatively associated with dispositional forgiveness (r=−.292, p=.005),
but there was no significant relationship between anxious attachment and dispositional
forgiveness (r=−.077, p=.25). In addition, avoidance was not significantly associated
with any of the restoration measures (all ps>.05). In contrast, anxious attachment was sig-
nificantly associated with responses to pastoral problems of anger (p<.05) and pornography
(marginal, p=.057).
As in Study 1, we examined the measures of spirituality and dispositional forgiveness
for gender differences. On strength of religious faith (ASCSRFQ), men (n=36, M=17.46,
SD =3.45) were similar to women (n=40, M=17.63, SD =3.27), t(74) =−.22, p=.83.
Likewise, on willingness to forgive (DWTFS), men (n=36, M=47.39, SD =11.80) were
similar to women (n=40, M=45.13, SD =12.00), t(74) =.83, p=.41. Moreover, there
were no gender differences for the scores on the two subscales of the Attachment to God
Inventory. The results for the AGI Avoidance subscale were: men (n=36, M=41.67,
SD =13.04) and women (n=38, M=38.16, SD =10.80), t(72) =1.26, p=.21. Finally,
the results for the AGI Anxiety subscale were: men (n=36, M=42.78, SD =13.57) and
women (n=40, M=45.08, SD =12.92), t(74) =−.76, p=.45.
Main analyses
We conducted the main analyses using analysis of variance models to extend our exploration
of gender in a between-groups design. We included two levels of dispositional forgiveness
(high and low based on a median split of the DWTFS scores). We examined the role of
spirituality in the model using two measures as covariates (ASCSRFQ, AGI Anxiety). We
did not include the avoidance subscale because it was not significantly associated with
any restoration measure and because we wanted to avoid problems of multicollinearity and
confounding caused by the shared variance with the Anxiety subscale.
The MANCOVA for the three measures of infidelity (affair one-year, second affair, ro-
mantic relationship) revealed a significant overall association between level of forgiveness
and restoration (Wilks’s =.871, F(3, 68) =3.37, p=.02, η2=.13) but no association for
gender and no significant interaction (ps>.05). Follow-up ANCOVAs indicated significant
differences between high and low forgivers on their restoration ratings for the three infidelity
scenarios (all ps<.05). The covariates did not significantly contribute to the model.
The MANCOVA for substance abuse revealed significant differences for level of forgive-
ness (Wilks’s =.850, F(3, 68) =3.99, p=.01, η2=.15) but not gender (p>.05) or the
interaction. The covariate measure of religious faith was significant (Wilks’s =.891, F
(3, 68) =2.77, p=.05, η2=.10). Follow-up ANCOVAs indicated that the type of substance
abuse made a difference. The participants were similar in their responses toward alcohol
abuse (p>.05). Level of forgiveness made a significant difference for abuse of pain medica-
tion (F(1, 70) =6.73, p=.01, η2=.09) and abuse of marijuana (F(1, 70) =8.69, p=.004,
η2=.11), but there were no significant differences for gender. Although forgiveness level
accounted for differences in responding to marijuana abuse (p<.01), the gender-forgiveness
interaction was significant (F(1, 70) =5.39, p=.02, η2=.07). As can be seen in Table 3,
women classified as high or low forgivers were not very different in their restoration scores;
however, men were significantly divergent. Men in the high forgiveness group were more
favorable toward restoration than women were, and men in the low forgiveness group were
less favorable toward restoration than women were.
We used four 2 ×2 ANCOVAs to examine restoration responses toward pastors in-
volved in other behaviors that may affect ministerial effectiveness (embezzlement, anger,
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 659
pornography, and child abuse). There was a marginal gender association for embezzlement
(F(1, 70) =3.61, p=.06, η2=.05), but no significant differences for level of forgiveness
or the interaction and neither covariate contributed to this model. Level of forgiveness was
marginally related to restoring pastors following a report of pornography (p=.058), but there
were no significant differences for gender or the interaction (ps>.05). Anxious attachment
to God was marginally significant (p=.09). Level of forgiveness was significantly related
to restoring pastors with an anger problem (p=.05), but there were no significant differ-
ences for gender or the interaction (ps>.05) and neither covariate contributed significantly
to the model. Both level of forgiveness (F(1, 70) =8.91, p=.00, η2=.11) and gender
(F(1, 70) =3.99, p=.05, η2=.05) were significantly associated with restoration responses
toward a pastor involved in child abuse but, there was no interaction effect (p>.05). Neither
covariate achieved significance.
Discussion
The results support our hypothesis regarding a general positive relationship between partic-
ipants’ dispositional forgiveness and their level of support for restoring a pastor to ministry
following common transgressions. In contrast, we obtained mixed results for hypotheses
about significant gender relationships, which were limited to certain situations and in some
cases interacted with level of forgiveness to qualify the restoration response. Women were
more favorable than men were when considering restoration of a pastor involved in embezzle-
ment. In contrast, women were less favorable than men were when considering restoration of
a pastor following child abuse, but level of dispositional forgiveness was the primary source
of influence. The gender response toward substance abuse issues is complex. Gender does
not make a difference regarding alcohol abuse or abuse of pain medication. However, men
were polarized in their reactions toward a pastor who abused marijuana. The men were more
favorable toward restoration than were women in the high-forgiveness group and less favor-
able than were women toward restoration in the low-forgiveness group. Men and women
were similar in their attitudes toward restoration for pastors involved in infidelity, pornogra-
phy, and for those with an anger problem. Overall, the measures of spirituality contributed
little to understanding the relationship among gender, forgiveness, and restoration attitudes.
We suspect this limited contribution is due to at least two factors. First, the sample is from a
conservative evangelical population that may have a more restricted range of values on the
spirituality measures than would a more diverse sample. Second, the dominant role of a for-
giving disposition may have incorporated an aspect of spirituality. Although the forgiveness
scale is not explicitly spiritual or religious, the concept of forgiveness is clearly a common
religious value (Rye, 2005). The data supported our hypotheses that men and women would
score similarly on measures of spirituality and forgiveness.
General discussion
Our general purpose was to extend previous work on attitudes toward restoring pastors
following acts of transgression within faith communities. One specific focus of these studies
was to explore the relationship of gender to the restoration outcome. We examined this in
two ways. First, we examined pastor gender and participant gender in an experiment that
allowed us to manipulate pastor gender through case narratives, and second, we examined
participant gender related to several types of offense in a correlational design. A second
Springer
660 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
focus was to examine the relationship of the participants’ spirituality, forgiving disposition,
and their restoration attitudes.
Does gender matter? The short answer is, yes. Of course, a more reasoned answer requires
us to consider the conditions under which gender makes a difference in pastoral restoration.
In the first study, we found that given the same transgression, young men and women respond
differently depending on the gender of the pastor who committed the transgression. There was
a cross-gender effect. Men were less favorable toward men than toward women, and women
were less favorable toward women than toward men. However, in the second study, the gender
differences were limited. Although we found differences between men and women, the strong
association of a forgiving disposition with restoration was the primary factor accounting for
the score differences. That is, a forgiving disposition was significantly related to restoration
ratings and accounted for more of a difference than did gender. Furthermore, we found that
gender is not associated with a difference for spirituality as assessed by religious faith or
attachment to God, nor is gender associated with a difference for a forgiving disposition.
What is the role of spirituality in forgiveness and restoration? The data for spirituality are
mixed. We obtained different findings for the relationship between the measure of religious
faith (ASCSRFQ) and dispositional forgiveness (DWTFS) depending on the study. The
samples were both drawn from students at a conservative Christian university and may
be expected to report high levels of strength of religious faith that could have attenuated
the correlations. One possible reason for the difference between Study 1 and Study 2 was the
exposure in Study 1 to different experimental conditions before completing any measures.
It could be that the differential exposure to the case narratives served as a priming stimulus
that encouraged students to draw on their spiritual resources when responding to the pastor’s
transgression, and thus the experimental manipulation may be the third variable in accounting
for the significant relationship between spirituality and forgiveness. This would be similar
to Rye’s (2005) finding that people report drawing on their religious resources when in
forgiveness interventions. Another explanation for the lack of a significant relationship
between religious faith and dispositional forgiveness in Study 2 could be due to the collection
of measures. In Study 2, the participants completed the AGI in addition to the religious faith
measure (ASCSRFQ). The completion of multiple spirituality measures could have affected
the results in an unknown way. Finally, we note that the means in our samples were similar
to those reported by Plante et al. (2002) for their religious faith measure, but the means in
our sample were considerably higher on the Willingness to Forgive Scale than in DeShea’s
(2003) original sample. This difference in means may suggest that for our samples, the
Willingness to Forgive Scale reflected a dimension of spirituality. At least the difference
suggests that the samples are dissimilar in this respect and suggests the importance of
routinely examining psychometric data of the measures used in different studies. Study 2
did reveal significant relationships between another measure of spirituality and dispositional
forgiveness. Participants who were more avoidant toward God in their attachment style
(AGI attachment subscale) were less willing to forgive (DWTFS). Thus, there remains
an association between spirituality, albeit based on a different dimension of spirituality,
and dispositional forgiveness. Clearly, the constructs of spirituality and forgiveness are
multidimensional constructs and the available measures are relatively new. On the one
hand, the data from our samples support the strong internal consistency findings reported
by the scale developers. On the other hand, both of these constructs require considerable
research before we can build adequate models of the relationship between spirituality and
forgiveness.
The positive relationship between dispositional forgiveness and restoration attitudes was
consistent for both studies. This makes intuitive sense and lends support to the construct
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 661
validity of the measures. In addition, the findings support those researchers who have
attempted to distinguish among the concepts of forgiveness, reconciliation, and restora-
tion (e.g., Fincham, Beach, & Davila, 2004; Sutton & Thomas, 2005b). That is, our studies
support the existence of a positive relationship between dispositional forgiveness and restora-
tion, which suggests that dispositional forgiveness may lead to the development of an attitude
that favors restoration, which could eventually lead to restorative action. However, the mod-
erate correlation suggests that the concepts of forgiveness and restoration remain distinctive
enough that other variables, such as gender (this article), type of offense and age of the pastor
(Sutton & Thomas, 2005a), and social influence (Pop, Sutton, & Jones, 2005) account for
additional variance. Other studies are needed to explore the relationship of forgiveness to
restoration in other samples, with other participant and situational variables, and with other
measures.
Our studies are limited by the usual concerns of using undergraduate participants and
self-report measures. Nevertheless, this age sample has intrinsic value because they are part
of various faith communities. Some had experience in churches where pastors had a problem.
In addition, some were children of pastors. Moreover, the situations in Study 1 described a
youth pastor to increase the relevance of the issues for the sample. We do not speculate that
these findings would generalize to other age groups or to populations who do not share the
conservative values of our sample.
Acknowledgements The authors presented the findings of these studies at the International meeting of the
Christian Association for Psychological Studies (March, 2006), Cincinnati, OH. The authors wish to thank
students from the 2004 Research Methods class for their assistance in data collection and data entry.
References
American Association of Pastoral Counselors. (1994, April 28). Codes of ethics: Principle III—Client Rela-
tionships. Retrieved January 6, 2007, from http://www.aapc.org/ethics.cfm#III.
Aquino, K., Tripp, T. M., & Bies, R. J. (2001). How employees respond to personal offense: The effects
of blame attribution, victim status, and offender status on revenge and reconciliation in the workplace.
Journal of Applied Psychology,86, 52–59.
Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist,44, 709–716.
Barna Research Group. (2001, September 25). A profile of protestant pastors in anticipation of “Pas-
tor Appreciation Month.” Retrieved August 1, 2005, from http://www.barna.org/FlexPage.aspx?Page =
BarnaUpdate&BarnaUpdateID =98.
Berry, J. W., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Parrott, L., III, O’Connor, L. E., & Wade, N. G. (2001). Dispositional
forgivingness: Development and construct validity of the Transgression Narrative Test of Forgivingness.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,27, 1277–1290.
Beck, R., & McDonald, A. (2004). Attachment to God: The attachment to God inventory, tests of working
model correspondence, and an exploration of faith group differences. Journalof Psychology and Theology,
32, 92–103.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.
Brown, R. P. (2003). Measuring individual differences in the tendency to forgive: Construct validity and links
with depression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,29, 759–771.
DeShea, L. (2003). A scenario-based scale of willingness to forgive. Individual Differences Research,1,
201–217.
Disch, E., & Avery, N. (2001). Sex in the consulting room, the examining room, and the sacristy: Survivors
of sexual abuse by professionals. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,71, 204–217.
Dorff, E. N. (1998). The elements of forgiveness: A Jewish approach. In E. L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Di-
mensions of forgiveness: Psychological research and theological perspectives (pp. 29–55). Philadelphia:
Templeton Foundation Press.
Edwards, L. M., Lapp-Rincker, R. H., Magyar-Moe, J. L., Rehfeldt, J. D., Ryder, J. A., Brown, J. C. et al.
(2002). A positive relationship between religious faith and forgiveness: Faith in the absence of data?
Pastoral Psychology,50, 147–152.
Springer
662 Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663
Enright R. D., & Gassin, E. A. (1992). Forgiveness: A developmental view. Journal of Moral Education,21,
99–114.
Fincham, F. D. Beach, S. R. H., & Davila, J. (2004). Forgiveness and conflict resolution in marriage. Journal
of Family Psychology,18, 72–81.
Fincham, F. D., Paleari, F. G., & Regalia, C. (2002). Forgiveness in marriage: The role of relationship quality,
attributions, and empathy. Personal Relationships,9, 27–37.
Finkel, E. J., Rusbult, C. E., Kumashiro, M., & Hannon, P. A. (2002). Dealing with betrayal in close
relationships: Does commitment promote forgiveness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82,
956–974.
Gault, B. A., & Sabini, J. (2000). The roles of empathy, anger, and gender in predicting attitudes toward
punitive, reparative, and preventative public policies. Cognition and Emotion,14, 495–520.
Hartford Institute for Religious Research. (2002). A quick question: How common is clergy sexual misconduct?
Retrieved May 28, 2004, from Hartford Seminary, Hartford Institute for Religious Research Web site:
http://hirr.hartsem.edu/research/quick question18.html.
Harris, C. R. (2000). Psychophysiological responses to imagined infidelity: The specific innate modular view
of jealousy reconsidered. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,78, 1082–1091.
Hathaway, W. L., Scott, S. Y., & Garver, S. A. (2004). Assessing religious/spiritual functioning: A neglected
domain in clinical practice? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,35(1), 97–104.
Hoover,K. L., & Sutton, G. W. (2005). ProrelationshipBehavior Following Betrayal: The Role of Commitment,
Empathy, and Relationship Status. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Hoyt, W. T., Fincham, F., McCullough, M. E., Maio, G., & Davila, J. (2005). Responses to interpersonal trans-
gressions in families: Forgiveness, forgivability, and relationship-specific effects. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,89, 375–394.
Hughes, R. K., & Armstrong, J. H. (1995). Why adulterous pastors should not be restored. Christianity Today,
39(4), 33–36.
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A study in human nature. Retrieved July 28, 2005,
from http://www.gutenberg.org/dirs/etext96/varre10.txt.
Kanz, J. E. (2000). How do people conceptualize and use forgiveness? The forgiveness attitudes questionnaire.
Counseling & Values,44, 174–189.
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1997). A longitudinal study of changes in religious belief and behavior as a function of
individual differences in adult attachment style. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,36, 207–217.
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1998). God as a substitute attachment figure: A longitudinal study of adult attachment
style and religious change in college students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,24, 961–973.
Luepker, E. T. (1999). Effects of practitioners’ sexual misconduct: A follow-up study. Journal of the American
Academy of Psychiatry and the Law,27, 51–63.
Macaskill, A., Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Forgiveness of self and others and emotional empathy. The
Journal of Social Psychology,142, 663–665.
Marty, M. E. (1998). The ethos of Christian forgiveness. In E. L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Dimensions of
forgiveness: Psychological research and theological perspectives (pp. 9–28). Philadelphia: Templeton
Foundation Press.
McCullough, M. E., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. (2003). Forgiveness, forbearance, and time: The temporal un-
folding of transgression-related interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84, 540–557.
McDonald, A., Beck, R., Allison, S., & Norsworthy, L. (2005). Attachment to God and parents: Testing the
correspondence vs. compensation hypotheses. Journal of Psychology and Christianity,24, 21–28.
McLeland, K. C., & Sutton, G. W. (2005). Military service, marital status, and men’s relationship satisfaction.
Individual Differences Research,3, 177–182.
Miller, W. R., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). Spirituality, religion, and health: An emerging research field.
American Psychologist,58, 24–35.
Pargament, K. I., & Rye, M. S. (1998). Forgiveness as a method of religious coping. In E. L. Worthington,
Jr. (Ed.), Dimensions of forgiveness: Psychological research and theological perspectives (pp. 59–78).
Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press.
Plante, T. G., & Boccaccini, M. (1997). The Santa Clara strength of religious faith questionnaire. Pastoral
Psychology,45, 375–387.
Plante, T., Vallaeys, C. L., Sherman, A. C., & Wallston, K. A. (2002). The development of a brief version of
the Santa Clara strength of religious faith questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology,50, 359–368.
Pop, J. L., Sutton, G. W., & Jones, E. G. (2005). Restoring pastors following a moral failure: The effects of
self-interest and group influence. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Richards, P. S., & Bergin, A. E. (1997). A spiritual strategy for counseling and psychotherapy. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Springer
Pastoral Psychol (2007) 55:645–663 663
Rowatt, W. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2002). Two dimensions of attachment to God and their relation to affect,
religiosity, and personality constructs. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,41, 637–651.
Rye, M. S. (2005). The religious path toward forgiveness. Mental Health, Religion, & Culture,8, 205–215.
Sutton, G. W., & Thomas, E. K. (2004). Measures of restoration: What do people believe about restoring lead-
ers to wholeness? Poster session presented at the meeting of the Christian Association for Psychological
Studies, Cleveland, OH: Midwest Regional Conference.
Sutton, G. W., & Thomas, E. K. (2005a). Can derailed pastors be restored? Effects of offense and age on
restoration. Pastoral Psychology,53, 583–599.
Sutton, G. W., & Thomas, E. K. (2005b). Restoring Christian leaders: How conceptualizations of forgiveness
and restoration can influence practice and research. American Journal of Pastoral Counseling,8, 29–44.
Sutton, G. W., Washburn, D. M., Comtois, L. L., & Moeckel, A. R. (2006). Professional ethics violations,
gender, forgiveness, and the attitudes of social work students. Journal of College and Character,7, 1–7.
Thoburn, J. W., & Balswick, J. O. (1998). Demographic data on extra-marital sexual behavior in the ministry.
Pastoral Psychology,46, 447–457.
Thoburn, J. W., & Whitman, D. M. (2004). Clergy affairs: Emotional investment, longevity of relationship
and affair partners. Pastoral Psychology,52, 491–506.
Thompson, L. Y., Snyder, C. R., Hoffman, L., Michael, S. T., Rasmussen, H. N., Billings, L. S. et al. (2005).
Dispositional forgiveness of self, others, and situations. Journal of Personality,73, 313–360.
Toussaint, L., & Webb, J. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between empathy and forgiveness.
The Journal of Social Psychology,145, 673–685.
Wells, K. (2003). A needs assessment regarding the nature and impact of clergy sexual abuse conducted by
the Interfaith Sexual Trauma Institute. Sexual Addiction and Compulsivity,10, 201–217.
Whisman, M. A., Dixon, A. E., & Johnson, B. (1997). Therapists’ perspectives of couple problems and
treatment issues in couple therapy. Journal of Family Psychology,11, 361–366.
Wiederman, M. W. (1997). Extramarital sex: Prevalence and correlates in a national survey. Journal of Sex
Research,34, 167–174.
Worthington, E. L. Jr., Berry, J. W., & Parrott, L., III (2001). Unforgiveness, forgiveness, religion, and health.
In T. G. Plante, & A. C. Sherman (Eds.), Faith and health: Psychological perspectives (pp. 107–138).
New York: Guilford.
Worthington, E. L. Jr., & Sandage, S. J. (2001). Religion and spirituality. Psychotherapy,38, 473–478.
Worthington,E. L. Jr., & Scherer, M. (2004). Forgivenessis an emotion-focused coping strategy that can reduce
health risks and promote health resilience: Theory, review, and hypotheses. Psychology and Health,19,
385–406.
Worthington, E. L. Jr., & Wade, N. G. (1999). The psychology of unforgiveness and forgiveness and implica-
tions for clinical practice. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,18, 385–418.
Springer
... Theorizing within philosophy has su^ested foe importance of humility in religious leaders (Roberts & Wood, 2003;2007). In the current paper, we sought to elaborate a model of relational humility (Davis et al., 2013) to incorporate the construct o flH and then to examine initial evidence for this model in relationships between subordinates and religious leaders. ...
... Previous work has examined factors that promote forgiveness of a religious leader after a transgression, although victim rather than offender characteristics have tended to be foe focus of study (Sutton 8c Thomas, 2006). Factors such as gender, dispositional forgiveness, and religious faith have been examined (Sutton, McLeland, Weaks, Cogswell, & Miphouvieng, 2007;Thomas, White, & Sutton, 2008). Interestingly, one study found that apology was associated with lower forgiveness, and it was theorized that an apology may be seen as weak (Thomas et al., 2008). ...
... Awareness of one's limitations is often a prerequisite for offering a sincere apology. O ur findings are generally consistent with Sutton et al., (2007) which found that that the engagement of spiritual resources when considering a pastor's transgression promoted forgiveness, and that an avoidant attachment Style towards God was associated with less willingness to forgive. The current research makes an important step towards examining more closely offender characteristics that maybe associated with forgiveness. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article focused on how perceptions of intellectual humility (IH)-humility regarding one's knowledge or influence over ideas affect relationships with religious leaders. We developed an informant report measure of IH perceptions using exploratory (Study 1; N = 213) and confirmatory (Study 2; N = 213) factor analyses, as well as a basic manipulation of IH (Study 3; N = 139). Then in Study 4 (N = 105), we examined IH in the context of a major betrayal by a religious leader (i.e., aligning with several factors theorized to strain the practice of IH). The results provide preliminary evidence for the psychometric soundness of the scale, including reliability and content validity of the scores. The scale was able to distinguish between IH and other constructs. Furthermore, the results provide initial evidence that IH is related to social bonds, as perceptions of IH were related to trust and higher forgiveness toward the religious leader, and positive attitudes towards the Sacred.
... Attachment to God (AG; Kirkpatrick & Shaver, 1990), based on the original attachment-to-human-caretaker models developed by Bowlby and Ainsworth has functioned as a predictive factor of Christian spirituality. For example, recent studies found positive relationships between AG and such variables as forgiveness (e.g., Davis, Hook, Van Tongeren, Gartner, & Worthington, 2012;Sutton, McLeland, Weaks, Cogswell, & Miphouvieng, 2007) and compassion (e.g., Sutton, et al., 2014). Although research has examined the relationship between AG and many variables, we found no published studies linking AG to satisfaction with Christian psychotherapy or patient well-being thus we included a measure of attachment to God in these studies. ...
Article
Christian psychotherapy is in high demand but in the few existing studies, outcomes from spiritually accommodated treatments typically do not outperform secular treatments on mental health outcomes. Likewise, it is unclear whether spiritual patient factors account for variance in satisfaction with treatment or patient well-being beyond what is explained by other patient factors. We conducted two studies on adults who attended Christian psychotherapy within the last six months to understand the relative contributions of patient factors to satisfaction with Christian psychotherapy and current well-being. We drew on hope theory as a primary general patient factor but considered personality traits given prior research. Second, we drew upon attachment theory framed as attachment to God (AG) as the primary patient spiritual factor but considered spiritual practices. In study 1 (two Christian universities; N = 75). hope accounted for most variance but extraversion was also predictive. Spiritual factors, primarily AG, added incremental value. In Study 2, we sampled adults (Amazon mTurk) who saw different providers (clergy, 46; mental health 57). Dispositional hope accounted for most of the variance in satisfaction with, and a willingness to return, to treatment as well as general and spiritual well-being. Spiritual factors (AG, practices) predicted additional variance for all criteria in the mental health sample but were only related to general well-being in the clergy sample. We concluded that when patients’ perspectives are considered, most of the variance in treatment satisfaction can be accounted for by hope but spiritual factors, primarily attachment to God, add nuanced incremental value. (prepublication version) Key words: Christian counseling and psychotherapy, Christian spirituality, Measuring counseling outcomes, Christianity and the psychology of hope, psychotherapy and personality,
... Research on the role of gender in forgiveness has produced mixed results. Although most studies have reported no gender differences in the tendency to forgive (e.g., Azar, Mullet, & Vinsonneau, 1999;Sutton, McLeland, Weaks, Cogswell, & Miphouvieng, 2007), some research indicates forgiveness may vary by gender depending on type of transgression. Shackelford and colleagues (2002) found that men were less forgiving than women of a partner's sexual infidelity while women were less forgiving than men of a partner's emotional infidelity (Shackelford, Buss, & Bennett, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Orientations towards apology, forgiveness, and reconciliation were investigated from an ecological world-view approach in 543 men and women. Participants completed the Apology, Forgiveness, and Reconciliation Scale, the Personal and Institutional Rights to Aggression Scale, the Revised Conflict Tactic Scales, an attachment measure and several demographic items. Scores on most childhood aggression scales were positively associated with beliefs interfering with apology, forgiveness, and reconciliation. Regression analyses revealed that the best predictive equation for beliefs facilitating apology and forgiveness included age, secure attachment, religiosity, (in)tolerance for governmental aggression, and advocacy of nonviolence. The best predictive equation for beliefs interfering with an orientation towards apology and forgiveness included fearful attachment style, religiosity, tolerance for governmental aggression, and (non)advocacy of non-violence. Future research may benefit from considering both orientations in support of and orientations against beliefs about the utility of apology, forgiveness and reconciliation within the context of conflict resolution.
... Previous empirical research found that PTG involves alterations in self (such as new directions or enhanced spirituality and religiosity) and changes in relationships with others, forgiveness as the release of negative feelings toward another and the adoption of positive attitudes might form a causal link between the offense and later growth, in fact, a prominent model of forgiveness suggests that individuals find an altered sense of purpose, one component of posttraumatic growth, through engaging in forgiveness [9]. Beside, several research has shown that religiosity is related to trait forgivingness [14]- [16], [30]. Tsang et al suggested that religion might be used to either encourage forgiveness or justify holding grudges, depending on the nature of one's religion [16]. ...
Article
People in Aceh are still suffering from a highly traumatic experience of Aceh conflict. As a religious community, Acehnese people believe that forgiveness is righteous. The purpose of the present study was to examine the mediational role of forgiveness on the relationship between spirituality and posttraumatic growth (PTG). Two hundred sixty-eight participants were recruited to complete the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI), Heartland Forgiveness Scale (HFS), and the Spiritual Transcendence Scale (STS). Results based on Sobel analyses revealed that forgiveness mediated the relationship between spirituality and PTG. Implications of these findings for future research on PTG and forgiveness are discussed.
... Emerging research in this area suggests that forgiveness is linked to a willingness to restore an errant pastor to ministry (e.g., Sutton, McLeland, Weaks, Cogswell, & Miphouvieng, 2007;Sutton & Thomas, 2005). Some research suggests that congregants discern between more serious and less serious offenses when considering restoration (Sutton & Jordan, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
This article will review the concept of forgiveness, studies about forgiveness and health, and studies related to psychological interventions. In addition, it will also review studies that have a direct or indirect link to Christian spirituality.
... Several studies have documented the willingness of Pentecostals to forgive others and restore errant clergy to leadership under certain conditions. Those studies also included a positive correlation between measures of spirituality and forgiveness (e.g., Althouse & Wilkinson, in press;Sutton, McLeland, Weaks, Cogswell, & Miphouvieng, 2007;Sutton & Thomas, 2005a, 2005bThomas, White, & Sutton, 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we explored an empirical basis for several dimensions of interpersonal love. From a psychological perspective, we considered multidimensional components of love using a holistic rubric that includes spiritual, cognitive, behavioral, affective, biological, and social space dimensions. From a theological perspective, we considered the traditional basis for understanding love of God and love of others in Christian communities with a special focus on early and contemporary Pentecostal beliefs and practices. Finally, we suggest ideas for further research and clinical practice.
Article
Although world religions extol the virtue of forgiveness, psychological research has uncovered a religion-forgiveness discrepancy in which individual differences in religiousness are positively related to valuing forgiveness, but less strongly related to forgiveness on transgression-specific, state forgiveness measures. The current study examines the relationship between trait religiousness and trait versus state forgiveness, and compares these with the effects of a religious prime on state forgiveness. 365 undergraduate participants were randomly assigned to a religious or neutral prime, and wrote about a transgression. Consistent with the religion-forgiveness discrepancy, we found that individual differences in religiousness were related more strongly to dispositional forgiveness than to transgression-specific forgiveness, although there were significant relationships to both. The religious prime had no noticeable effect on state forgiveness. Results are consistent with the religion-forgiveness discrepancy.
Article
The aims of this article are to explore the experience of depression among palliative care clients and caregivers, describe the strategies they use in coping with depression, and clarify the role of spirituality in preventing and/or overcoming depression. This article discusses an aspect of the findings of a larger doctoral study that explored the nature of spirituality and spiritual engagement from the viewpoint of individuals with life-limiting conditions and their caregivers. van Manen’s phenomenology was used in the study. The data generated from the doctoral study were subjected to secondary analysis to uncover the experience of depression. The methodology underpinning the secondary analysis was phenomenology also by van Manen. Fourteen clients and caregivers from across regional and rural South Australia informed the study. Data collection involved in-depth nonstructured home-based interviews that were audiotaped and transcribed verbatim. The findings highlighted relate to participants succumbing to depression, but having spiritual beliefs and practices helped them cope. One of the most insightful understanding was the role spirituality played in protecting individuals from depression, encapsulated in the theme “finding paradise within.” Spirituality, understood from a religious or secular perspective, must be embedded in palliative care as it assisted in preventing and overcoming depression.
Article
Blola Unir/ersity, 0091-6 4 7 1 /4 1 0-7 3 0 Hope College preliminary evidence for the psychometric s o u n d n e s s ofthe scale, including reliability and content validity ofthe scores. The scale was able to distinguish between IH and other constructs. Furthermore, the results provide initial evidence that IH is related to social bonds, as perceptions o‫؛‬ IH were related to trust and higher forgiveness toward foe religious leader, and positive attitudes towards foe Sacred.
Article
Full-text available
In the present article, we extend a model of relational spirituality and forgiveness to the context of major offenses by clergy. In Study 1, undergraduate students (N = 208) described a major offense that they had experienced by a religious leader and then completed several questionnaires in relation to the offense and the religious leader. Appraisals of relational spirituality were significantly associated with forgiveness, after controlling for known predictors of forgiveness (i.e., hurtfulness, time since the offense, and commitment to God or another source of spirituality). In Study 2, we replicated and extended the findings from Study 1 using an independent sample of undergraduates (N = 365). In a series of structural equation models, we found that relational engagement of God (or another source of spirituality) partially mediated the relationship between anger towards God (or another source of spirituality) and unforgiving motivations. We conclude by discussing implications for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Four studies examine the construct validity of the Tendency to Forgive Scale (TTF), a brief measure of dispositional forgiveness. Study 1 showed that romantic partners' ratings of targets converged with targets' self-ratings, and Study 2 demonstrated that higher scores on the TTF were associated with lower offense accessibility. Study 3 examined the TTF`s relation to self-reported depression symptoms, both independent of and interacting with attitudes toward forgiveness and dispositional vengeance. Lower TTF scores were associated with higher degrees of depression, especially for individuals with positive attitudes toward, forgiveness or those low in dispositional vengeance, although neither of these latter variables displayed significant zero-order relations with depression. Finally, Study 4 examined relations between the TTF, dispositional empathy, another recent measure of dispositional forgiveness, and the dimensions of the Big Five, providing both convergent and discriminant validity evidence for the TTF.
Article
Full-text available
The authors examined the relationship between militazy status, marital status, and relationship satisfaction in a sample of 46 men. Modifications of the Kansas Marital Satisfaction Scale (KMSS) and the ENRICH Marital Satisfaction Scale (EMS) were used as measures of relationship satisfaction. A two-way MANCOVA (military status x marital status) with age as the covariate revealed lower satisfaction scores for military men. Follow-up ANCOVAs indicated a significant association between military status and relationship satisfaction when measured on the EMS. Additional analyses indicated that deployment alert status explained part of the variance for lowered relationship satisfaction. The results were discussed in terms of a contextual model of marriage (Bradbury & Fincham, 1988).
Article
Full-text available
Forgivingness is the disposition to forgive interpersonal transgressions over time and across situations. There is currently no acceptable measure of forgivingness for use in testing theoretical propositions. The authors describe a five-item scenario-based scale, the Transgression Narrative Test of Forgivingness (TNTF). In five studies examining 518 university students from three disparate universities, the authors assess the item and full-scale functioning of the TNTF and its concurrent and 8-week predictive validity relative to trait anger, rumination, neuroticism, agreeableness, and hostility. Test-retest reliability and stability of item locations were both good. Norms are presented by gender, ethnicity, and religious activity. The TNTF is a brief measure of forgivingness that is not theory dependent and is therefore useful in basic and intervention research from a variety of theoretical perspectives.
Book
Full-text available
A Spiritual Strategy for Counseling and Psychotherapy, Second Edition shows mental health professionals how to deal sensitively with clients whose spirituality or religion is an important part of their lives. This book highlights the therapeutic possibilities religion and spirituality can offer. Building on the success of the first edition, the new edition provides timely updates and additional theoretical grounding for integrating a theistic, spiritual strategy into mainstream psychology. Also ideal for students and scholars, this book provides helpful background and insight into the history and philosophy of science and psychology, the world religions, the practice of psychotherapy, and the process of research and scientific discovery.
Article
The investigators proposed that transgression-related interpersonal motivations result from 3 psychological parameters: forbearance (abstinence from avoidance and revenge motivations, and maintenance of benevolence), trend forgiveness (reductions in avoidance and revenge, and increases in benevolence), and temporary forgiveness (transient reductions in avoidance and revenge, and transient increases in benevolence). In 2 studies, the investigators examined this 3-parameter model. Initial ratings of transgression severity and empathy were directly related to forbearance but not trend forgiveness. Initial responsibility attributions were inversely related to forbearance but directly related to trend forgiveness. When people experienced high empathy and low responsibility attributions, they also tended to experience temporary forgiveness. The distinctiveness of each of these 3 parameters underscores the importance of studying forgiveness temporally.
Article
This study investigated the relationships between blame, victim and offender status, and the pursuit of revenge or reconciliation after a personal offense. Results from a sample of 141 government agency employees showed that blame is positively related to revenge and negatively related to reconciliation. In addition, victim-offender relative status moderated the relation between blame and revenge such that victims who blamed sought revenge more often when the offender's status was lower than their own. The victims' own absolute hierarchical status also moderated this relation such that lower, not higher, status employees who blamed sought revenge more often.
Article
Randomly selected samples of practicing couple therapists who were members of the American Psychological Association's Division 43 or the Association for Marriage and Family Therapy completed a survey of couple problem areas and therapeutic issues encountered in couple therapy. Therapists rated problem areas in terms of occurrence, treatment difficulty, and damaging impact. A composite of these 3 dimensions suggested that the most important problems were lack of loving feelings, power struggles, communication, extramarital affairs, and unrealistic: expectations. Comparison of the findings with therapist ratings obtained by S. K. Geiss and K. D. O'Leary (1981) suggests considerable stability in presenting problems in couple therapy over the past 15 years. Therapist-generated characteris tics associated with negative outcome were also identified, the most common being partners' inability or unwillingness to change and lack of commitment.
Article
Recent theoretical and empirical work by Lee Kirkpatrick and others has suggested that relationship with God can be fruitfully described as an attachment bond. However, this literature has been limited by the lack of a sound theoretical and psychometric scale that operationalizes the attachment to God construct. Toward that end, the paper presents data from three samples, two college and one community sample, describing the psychometric properties of the Attachment to God Inventory (AGI) as well as providing tests of the correspondence and compensation hypotheses. In general, the AGI subscales of Avoidance of Intimacy and Anxiety about Abandonment display good factor structure, internal consistency, and construct validity. Comparisons of the AGI with adulthood attachment measures appear to support, albeit weakly, a correspondence between working models of romantic others and God.