ArticlePDF Available

Gender Stereotypes of Leaders: An Analysis of the Contents of Obituaries from 1974 to 1998

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study investigates changes in gender stereotypes by an unobtrusive method. Obituaries of deceased women and men in leading positions as published in four German language newspapers between 1974 and 1998 were analyzed. Following a previous study (E. Kirchler, 1992), terms used in the obituaries to describe former leaders were classified into 58 categories. Frequencies of descriptions were analyzed with regard to leadership success, stability of attributes, and person-orientation versus task-orientation. Results show that gender images were dissimilar in content each of the years. In 1992 and 1998, however, female and male stereotypes became more similar.
Content may be subject to copyright.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Sex Roles, Vol. 45, Nos. 11/12, December 2001 ( C
°2002)
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders: An Analysis
of the Contents of Obituaries From 1974 to 1998
Christa Rodler, Erich Kirchler,1and Erik H¨
olzl
University of Vienna
The present study investigates changes in gender stereotypes by an unobtru-
sive method. Obituaries of deceased women and men in leading positions
as published in four German language newspapers between 1974 and 1998
were analyzed. Following a previous study (E. Kirchler, 1992), terms used
in the obituaries to describe former leaders were classified into 58 categories.
Frequencies of descriptions were analyzed with regard to leadership success,
stability of attributes, and person-orientation versus task-orientation. Results
show that gender images were dissimilar in content each of the years. In 1992
and 1998, however, female and male stereotypes became more similar.
KEY WORDS: gender stereotypes; leadership; management.
INTRODUCTION
Stereotypes provide a base for leader–subordinate interactions
(Martinko & Gardner, 1987). From this perspective, subordinates’ percep-
tions of their leaders’ characteristics are essential factors for acceptance
and followership (Mitchell, 1995; Robbins, 1998). According to leadership
attribution theory, leadership success is not exclusively defined by the com-
petencies of leaders; it is also facilitated by social reality as constructed by
subordinates (Calder, 1977; Pfeffer, 1992). This conceptualization demands
to take the subordinates’ perspective because their assumptions, percep-
tions, attributions, and behavior affect leadership efficiency. With regard to
gender, this approach focuses “...not on how men and women actually dif-
fer, but how people think that they differ” (Deaux, 1984, p. 110). In assigning
1To whom correspondence should be addressed to University of Vienna, Department of Psy-
chology, Universitaetsstrasse 7, A-1010 Vienna, Austria; e-mail: erich.kirchler@univie.ac.at.
827
0360-0025/01/1200-0827/0 C
°2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
828 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
attributes to male and female leadership performance, observers are biased
by subjective social realities and by stereotypes. Biological gender is a key
item of information on which attributions and judgments are based.
According to the female gender role, women are considered to have
more social qualities (e.g., they are perceived to be emotional, sensitive, sup-
porting, gentle, kind, and affectionate), whereas men are assumed to possess
more agentic qualities (e.g., independent, assertive, competitive, daring, and
courageous; Brody, 1997; Carli & Eagly, 1999). “Agency” is the dominant
label in the male stereotype, whereas “communality” is the attribute most
assigned to females. Typical male leaders are said to emphasize goal at-
tainment, whereas their female counterparts emphasize social relationships
(Ashmore, DelBoca, & Wohlers, 1986).
Whether or not leaders are seen as successful depends heavily on fol-
lowers’ implicit theories on leadership (Bass, 1990). There is strong evidence
for the existence of a leadership prototype (Lord, 1985). This prototype of
a “good leader” was found to be cloaked in masculine terms. Extensive re-
search of Schein (2001) confirmed, on an international level, the hypotheses
that managers are perceived to possess characteristics commonly ascribed
to men. Whereas men are perceived as meeting prototypical characteristics,
women are expected to be different (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Butterfield
& Grinell, 1999; Kirchler, 1997; Powell & Butterfield, 1989).
The aspiration to be successful or trying to fit into the image of a leader
prototype puts pressure on women to adopt “a masculine style” (Eagly &
Johnson, 1990; Gardinger & Tiggermann, 1999). But as the role congruity
theory of prejudice toward female leaders (Eagly & Karau, in press) states,
incongruence between the female gender role and the traditional leadership
role accounts for less favorable perceptions of female leaders and their lead-
ership behavior. Female leaders who adopt stereotypical masculine styles
(e.g., autocratic or directive styles) face role conflicts (Heilman, Block, &
Martell, 1995). They also receive more negative evaluations, are called “iron
ladies,” and perceived as aggressive, manipulative, and domineering leaders,
in comparison to men who are not penalized for adopting a feminine style
(Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992; Heller, 1982).
With respect to leadership style concepts (well known in terms of consid-
eration and initiating structure, concern for people vs. concern for production
and profit, or person-orientation vs. task-orientation), person-orientation
seems to be of particular interest because it is traditionally associated with the
female gender role. There is increasing evidence for the “feminization
of management” (Fondas, 1997). Social skills, such as sharing responsibil-
ity, helping and facilitating the development of subordinates, and building
networks of relationships, are characteristics that recently have become im-
portant factors in describing effective leadership (Vinkenburg, Jansen, &
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 829
Koopman, 2000). They are also praised as the crucial characteristics that will
make the difference between effective versus successful leaders in the future
(Weinert, 1998). Rudman and Glick (2001) point out that this might result
in a backlash for female managers. Agentic, competent women are often
seen as dominative and therefore less nice than identical presented men.
When a “feminized” managerial job demands that applicants possess agen-
tic characteristics and at the same time exceptional communal skills, women
again will face discrimination. It is relevant, therefore, to look at changes of
descriptions of female and male leaders with respect to leadership style in
terms of person- versus task-orientation.
Usually people do not only evaluate level of performance but also make
attributions of causality for a performance. Deaux (1984) argues that causal
attributions can be linked to expectations for performances and can be un-
derstood in terms of more general stereotypes held of women and men.
Causal attributions also determine whether a specific performance is seen
occurring accidentally or as likely to be repeatable in the future (attribu-
tions to stable causes; Nieva & Gutek, 1980). Good female performance or
success was often found to be perceived as due to effort and good male per-
formance as due to ability (Burton, 1992; Rimm, 1998). Characteristics used
to describe male leaders should therefore more often refer to stability, than
those used to describe female leaders.
The present study investigates changes in how female and male leaders
are perceived during the last decades. Several reasons foster the general as-
sumption of change: First, the images of female leaders should have changed
due to increasing numbers of females at least in middle management posi-
tions. Second, the female stereotype should be particularly dynamic due to
the greater changes in the roles of women as compared with those of men
(Diekman & Eagly, 1999). In other words, it can be assumed that while fe-
male stereotypes have changed over the past decades, male stereotypes have
remained quite invariant.
Investigations of changes in the social images of female and male lead-
ers should rely on unobtrusive methods rather than using questionnaires or
interview techniques that often lead to results biased by social desirability.
Kirchler, Wagner, and Buchleitner (1996) and Laugheed (2000) strongly rec-
ommend indirect measures because direct questions could hide stereotypes
that might still exist. In this study, we investigated the texts of obituaries of fe-
male and male leaders printed in newspapers and signed by the organization
that the deceased had led. Descriptions of leaders through verbs, adjectives,
nouns, and parts of sentences in obituaries are indicators for stereotypes held
by the persons who describe their deceased leaders. Obituaries in newspa-
pers may also reflect a more general view in what is accepted as “politically
correct” and provide information about what is deemed appropriate to say
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
830 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
about a deceased person who had held a high level position. Because people
tend to idealize deceased persons, the images are, without doubt, positively
biased. Although these biases do not allow drawing evaluative conclusions
about gender stereotypes, it is possible to detect qualitative differences in
content. If at a certain time, consensual beliefs about the characteristics of
female and male leaders differ, the words used to describe these leaders
should also differ.
Kirchler (1992) analyzed the content of obituaries of female and male
leaders, published in daily newspapers in the years 1974, 1980, and 1986. He
found that attributes were highly dissimilar. Although male managers were
continually described over the years using success related attributes such as
intelligent, knowledgeable, experienced, outstanding instructors, unselfish
opinion leaders with an enviable entrepreneurial spirit, etc., in 1974 and
1980 women were described as venerable and likeable superiors. In 1986, the
female leaders’ dominant attributes shifted to courageous and committed.
The present study extends Kirchler’s investigations (Kirchler, 1992) to the
years 1992 and 1998. Moreover, the linkage between stereotypes, success,
stability attributions, and leadership style is addressed.
METHOD
Obituaries written by the organization in which a deceased leader had
been employed and published in newspapers in the years 1974, 1980, 1986,
1992, and 1998 were analyzed. Four newspapers published daily in the
German language were investigated. One newspaper is published in Austria
(Die Presse), two in Germany (Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, S¨
uddeutsche
Zeitung), and one in Switzerland (Neue Z ¨
urcher Zeitung). These newspapers
were selected because they usually contain obituaries placed by private per-
sons but also ones placed by business firms and public organizations. In this
case obituaries are death notices in which the loss of a person is not only
announced but the person is also described by a few sentences.
For the present study, 26 issues of each newspaper were selected per
year according to the following selection principle: for the years 1974, 1980,
1986, 1992, and 1998 the Monday issues of the second week of the year were
chosen, the Tuesday issue of the fourth week, the Wednesday issue of the
sixth week, the Thursday issue of the eight week, etc.
Overall, 894 obituaries reported explicitly on persons who had been
working in leading positions: 39 pertained to deceased females and 757 to
deceased males. A person was defined as leader when he or she had been a
director or head of a business firm, school or other public organization, chair
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 831
Table I. Frequency of Obituaries Pertaining to Female and Male Leaders by Year of
Publication in Four German Language Newspapers
Female leaders
Publication year Male leaders First search Second search Total
1974 169 7 29 205
1980 127 4 18 149
1986 181 9 18 208
1992 142 11 15 168
1998 138 8 18 164
Total 757 39 98 894
Note. Data from 1974 to 1986 were taken from a previous study (Kirchler, 1992).
of a department, etc. Since the number of the obituaries of female managers
was very small, a second search was made for further obituaries of only
female leaders in all issues of the above cited newspapers not considered in
the first round. This second data search was necessary to provide a larger
sample for analysis. The second search yielded 98 additional obituaries on
female leaders (Table I).
The contents of the obituaries were analyzed. All verbs, adjectives,
and nouns were identified. Overall, 4057 semantically relevant words were
counted. In the obituaries of the years 1974–1980 a total of 2365 words were
counted; for the years 1992 and 1998 a total of 1692 words were counted.
The words published in obituaries in 1992 and 1998 were classified into
58 categories by two coders who were trained to use the categorization
scheme established in the previous study (Kirchler, 1992) in which the words
from the years 1974–1986 had already been categorized. The percentage of
interrater-concordance was 93%. In case of dissimilar classification, coders
discussed their categorization until they reached agreement. The descriptive
categories and overall frequencies for men and women are enlisted in the
Appendix in English and German language. Due to small number of female
obituaries it was not possible to explicitly consider type of company.
RESULTS
Content Analysis of Obituaries
First, the frequencies of the 58 descriptive categories for female and
male leaders were computed by year of observation. Male managers were
described as persons with a strong personality (5.6%), as experts (5.2%), as
being companionate (4.5%), committed (4.3%), energetic (4.2%), indefati-
gable (4.2%), outstanding (4.0%), and successful (3.6%). Female managers
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
832 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
were praised most often for being committed (6.2%), for having had a strong
personality (5.8%), for having been a pioneer (4.7%), energetic (4.4%),
loyal (4.4%), indefatigable (4.4%), caring (4.2%), venerable (3.8%), hu-
mane (3.8%), and successful (3.8%).
The cross-classified frequency table for attribute categories and male
and female leaders by year was subjected to a correspondence analysis
(Greenacre, 1993), which is based on chi-square tests and allows for the
exploration of the structure of qualitative data. Correspondence analysis is
an exploratory technique designed to analyze simple two-way and multiway
tables containing some measure of correspondence between the rows and
columns. The results provide information that is similar in nature to those
produced by factor analysis techniques.
The categories included were the 49 categories that appeared in the
obituaries more than 20 times (approximately 0.5% of total frequencies).
The correspondence analysis yielded a two-dimensional solution: the first
dimension explained 39%, the second 18% of variance (the remaining di-
mensions explained less than 10% of variance). Because the contribution
of row points adds up to 1 and a total of 49 row points were used, only
those row points contributing more than 1/49 =0.02 were analyzed further.
Twenty-two descriptive categories meeting these criteria were included
in the second round of correspondence analysis. Table II shows frequencies
for the female and male samples.
The second analysis yielded one dimension explaining 51%, and a sec-
ond dimension explaining 21% of variance. The third and fourth dimensions
explained 10% each. Figure 1 shows only the two-dimensional results be-
cause the third and fourth dimensions did not contribute to the understand-
ing of the attributes assigned to female and male leaders over the time.
The graphical representation shows attributes that describe male and
female managers over the years. Although females were described quite
differently than males in the years 1974 to 1986, the differences decreased
sharply in 1992 and 1998. Figure 1 also shows that female images have shifted
over the years, whereas changes in male images are less apparent. Dimen-
sion 1 ranges from attributes like servant, committed, professional, and hu-
mane to intelligent, efficient, skillful, experienced, and expert. The second
dimension is, at the one pole, described by expert, experienced, professional,
intelligent, and, at the other pole, by venerable. It can be summarized that
men were always described as mainly professional, experienced, experts, and
more often described with attributes reflecting interpersonal orientation in
the 1990s. Women in the 1970s were mainly described as venerable, in the
1980s they became work-oriented and courageous, in the 1990s professional
and committed.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 833
Table II. Frequency of Descriptive Categories (Included in the Correspondence Analysis) by Gender of Leaders and Publication Year
Male leaders Female leaders
Categories 1974 1980 1986 1992 1998 Total % 1974 1980 1986 1992 1998 Total % Total %
Amiable 9 8 10 11 11 49 1.5 4 1 5 2 2 14 3.1 63 1.7
Committed 5 13 23 51 47 139 4.3 0 1 6 11 10 28 6.2 167 4.5
Companionate 34 33 26 26 26 145 4.5 2 3 0 3 1 9 2.0 154 4.2
Courageous 50520120.4114 3092.0210.6
Efficient 117200200.6130 0040.9240.6
Experienced 30 14 25 10 19 98 3.0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0.2 99 2.7
Expert 43 32 41 30 23 169 5.2 0 2 1 2 3 8 1.8 177 4.8
Honest 15 11 14 7 4 51 1.6 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.0 51 1.4
Humane 13 13 20 20 27 93 2.9 1 1 2 4 9 17 3.8 110 3.0
Indefatigable 40 32 36 15 14 137 4.2 5 5 2 3 5 20 4.4 157 4.2
Intelligent 38 19 24 3 8 92 2.8 3 0 1 1 3 8 1.8 100 2.7
Likeable 23266190.6300 0030.7220.6
Loyal 27 18 23 2 9 79 2.4 9 7 4 0 0 20 4.4 99 2.7
Open-minded 5 5 7 15 10 42 1.3 2 0 0 1 6 9 2.0 51 1.4
Pioneer 14 9 8 26 16 73 2.3 5 0 3 8 5 21 4.6 94 2.5
Professional 3 2 5 21 28 59 1.8 0 0 0 5 2 7 1.5 66 1.8
Servant 01199200.6000 1120.4220.6
Skillful 19 13 13 5 3 53 1.6 2 0 3 1 1 7 1.5 60 1.6
Sociable 8 3 10 1 0 22 0.7 2 0 2 0 1 5 1.1 27 0.7
Unselfish 8 11 14 0 1 34 1.0 4 0 3 1 1 9 2.0 43 1.2
Venerable 76537280.91222 10173.8451.2
Work-oriented 8 4 11 10 4 37 1.1 6 0 2 1 0 9 2.0 46 1.2
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
834 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
Fig. 1. Correspondence analysis of the frequencies of 22 descriptive categories
by gender and year of publication.
Success, Stability, and Leadership Style
The next step entailed analyzing leadership success as well as stabil-
ity of attributes in terms of Weiner’s (Weiner, 1986) attribution theory, and
leadership style, namely task-orientation versus person-orientation, inher-
ent in the descriptions. Four psychologists in human resource management
positions at various business firms, who were blind to the study, but trained
to the theoretical concepts, rated each category. Ratings were made with
regard to attributes indicating (a) leadership success as well as (b) stability
of attributes, (c) task-orientation or (d) person-orientation. If three of the
four raters agreed, attribute-categories were judged to indicate leadership
success, stability, and task-orientation or person-orientation.
Categories rated as indicating leadership success were the following:
active, committed, courageous, creative, decision-maker, efficient, entre-
preneurial spirit, esteemed, experienced, expert, farsighted, innovator,
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 835
Fig. 2. Success-related attributes in obituaries over time. Note. Relative frequencies
of attributes related to success; lines show the 95% confidence intervals.
intelligent, open-minded, opinion-leader, pioneer, professional, respected,
responsible, skillful, sociable, strong personality, and successful. Overall, half
of the descriptive categories were related to leadership success: Female lead-
ers were described by attributes indicating success in 47% of the cases, male
leaders in 53% of the cases. Figure 2 illustrates the changes in success-related
attributes over the years. In the year 1974, 35% of the descriptive words for
females were categorized as success-related, whereas in the male sample,
53% of the attributes were related to success. In 1998, the numbers for both
male and female leaders were 57%. The 95% confidence intervals show that
relative frequencies of success-related attributes differed significantly only
in 1974. In the following years, women were described with approximately
equal numbers of success-related words.
Characteristics rated as stable were the following: being committed,
decision-maker, entrepreneurial spirit, farsighted, humane, intelligent, pro-
fessional, reliable, responsible, skillful, sociable, strong personality. Table III
shows that approximately one quarter of the descriptions were rated as be-
ing success-related and stable. The confidence intervals indicate a significant
difference in 1974, where only 12% of descriptions for female leaders, but
22% of descriptions for male leaders were rated in this way. No gender dif-
ferences were found for the following years. Over time, female managers
were more often described as successful in 1998 than in the 1974, and the
stability of success-related attributes increased. For male leaders, the per-
centage of success-related and stable descriptions remained at the same
level.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
836 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
Table III. Relative Frequencies of Descriptions Related to Leadership Success and Stability
Descriptions Leaders’ gender 1974 1980 1986 1992 1998 M
Success-related Male 0.53 (0.49–0.56) 0.49 (0.44–0.53) 0.53 (0.49–0.57) 0.55 (0.51–0.59) 0.57 (0.53–0.61) 0.53 (0.52–0.55)
Female 0.35 (0.26–0.43) 0.37 (0.23–0.50) 0.47 (0.35–0.58) 0.59 (0.49–0.69) 0.57 (0.47–0.66) 0.47 (0.42–0.51)
Success-related Male 0.22 (0.19–0.25) 0.19 (0.16–0.23) 0.22 (0.19–0.25) 0.23 (0.20–0.27) 0.27 (0.24–0.31) 0.23 (0.21–0.24)
and stable Female 0.12 (0.06–0.17) 0.17 (0.07–0.28) 0.24 (0.14–0.34) 0.31 (0.22–0.40) 0.27 (0.18–0.36) 0.22 (0.21–0.24)
Attributes Male 713 483 750 609 606 3161
total Female 121 52 75 97 99 444
Note. Twenty-three categories were judged as success-related, 10 as both success-related and stable. Numbers in parentheses give 95% confidence
intervals.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 837
With regard to leadership style, the overall results show that more de-
scriptive words in the obituaries were rated as indicating task- than person-
orientation. Over the years, a significant increase in the percentages of words
related to person-orientation was observed for the male leaders. Because of
the small numbers of obituaries for female leaders, confidence intervals are
larger and changes in the percentages for female leaders are not signifi-
cant, although the numbers indicate increased task-orientation in the 1990s
(Table IV).
Figure 3 visualizes the results concerning person-orientation and task-
orientation. The graphical presentation follows the managerial grid, which
was used first by Blake and Mouton (1964) to capture the extent to which
a leader is person-oriented and task-oriented, assuming that managers per-
form best if they are high in both dimensions. Over the years, male leaders
were described as task-oriented in 43% of the cases. In 1992 and 1998, a
clear shift towards person-orientation can be observed.
Fig. 3. Person- and task-orientation in obituaries over time. Note. Relative
frequencies of attributes related to person- versus task-orientation; 95% con-
fidence intervals are given in Table IV.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
838 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
Table IV. Relative Frequencies of Descriptions Related to Person- and Task-Orientation
Descriptions Leaders’ gender 1974 1980 1986 1992 1998 M
Person-orientation Male 0.22 (0.19–0.25) 0.23 (0.20–0.27) 0.24 (0.21–0.27) 0.32 (0.28–0.35) 0.33 (0.29–0.36) 0.27 (0.25–0.28)
Female 0.26 (0.18–0.34) 0.25 (0.13–0.37) 0.27 (0.17–0.37) 0.26 (0.17–0.34) 0.34 (0.25–0.44) 0.28 (0.23–0.32)
Task-orientation Male 0.41 (0.37–0.44) 0.42 (0.38–0.46) 0.44 (0.40–0.47) 0.44 (0.40–0.48) 0.42 (0.38–0.46) 0.43 (0.41–0.44)
Female 0.31 (0.23–0.39) 0.38 (0.25–0.52) 0.39 (0.28–0.50) 0.46 (0.36–0.56) 0.42 (0.33–0.52) 0.39 (0.35–0.44)
Attributes Male 713 483 750 609 606 3161
total Female 121 52 75 97 99 444
Note. Fourteen categories were judged as indicating person-orientation, 17 as indicating task-orientation. Numbers in parentheses give 95%
confidence intervals.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 839
DISCUSSION
Top leadership is still essentially male dominated, and the number of
women in top positions remains negligible worldwide (Adler, 2000;
Davidson & Burke, 2000). This imbalance between women and men in
leadership positions is mirrored in the ratio of female obituaries to male
obituaries. The vast majority of the total obituaries pertained to males and
only a small percentage pertained to deceased female leaders, which at first
sight corresponds to the small percentage of women in top management
positions. However, it can also be argued that the mortality rate of female
leaders today reflects the statistics of the past and in the years ahead the
relative figures will change. Konrad, Ritchie, Lieb, and Corrigall (2000) con-
ducted a meta-analysis of 242 samples on sex differences and similarities
in job attribute preference and found that job attributes had become more
important to women and girls, women’s aspirations had risen, and gender
barriers to opportunity had declined. If more women reached higher man-
agement levels in the 1980s and 1990s than in the 1970s, the percentages of
women and men mirrored in the obituaries should be less distorted.
Have gender stereotypes of leaders in the last quarter of a century
really changed and if so, in which direction? It was hypothesized that in the
1970s and 1980s female leaders represented not only a minority, but their
images were quite different than male images. Females were perceived and
expected to be warm and expressive, whereas males were tough experts,
task-oriented, and dominant. Although studies on stereotypes that directly
asked participants for differences between female and male leaders did not
reveal significant differences in the late 1980s, indirect, unobtrusive measures
showed prevailing differences (Kirchler, 1997; Kirchler et al., 1996).
What has clearly been shown by the contents of the analyzed obituar-
ies, is that while content of female images has changed considerably over the
past decades, male stereotypes have remained relatively invariant. First, as
Fig. 1 shows, the female stereotype has changed dramatically since 1974, from
mainly venerable, admirable, and loyal, to courageous and work-oriented,
to finally, 1998, committed and professional. The male stereotype remained
invariant from 1974 to 1986. In the 1990s it changed towards more person-
oriented attributes, such as humane and open-minded. In the years 1992
and 1998, the attributes used to describe deceased female and male leaders
came closer to each other. These findings are consistent with the observation
that roles of women and men have become more similar and that changes in
beliefs about women are highly dynamic (Diekman & Eagly, 1999). People
get more and more used to having a female leader at their workplace. In-
dependent of hierarchy level, stereotypes should fade because of greater
visibility of female leaders.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
840 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
Research on gender and leadership style has focused on gender differ-
ences. Men were and often still are evaluated more positively when scoring
high on the initiation dimension, whereas women are evaluated more
positively when scoring high on the consideration dimension (Bartol &
Butterfield, 1976). Eagly and Karau (1991) report that according to gen-
der stereotypes, men have a stronger advantage on measures of task-related
leadership, whereas female leaders have a stronger advantage on social fac-
tors of leadership. The effect decreased with duration of interaction between
members of a group, or between a leader and subordinates. In the present
study, descriptive words in the obituaries of female and male leaders were an-
alyzed with regard to task-orientation and person-orientation. The changes
in the direction of a more interpersonal style as indicated by the results from
the correspondence analysis were also visible in the further analyses, though
significant only for the male leaders. Men were described consistently task-
oriented over the years, and in the 1990s more person-oriented. Women
were described in the 1970s and 1980s by attributes that indicated neither
much task-orientation nor person-orientation. In the 1990s female leaders’
attributes seem to become more work-related. Overall there is a balance
between task- and person-orientation.
The relation of descriptions in the obituaries to success changed over
the years as well. According to the attributes used, men have always been
perceived as successful. The relative frequency of success-related attributes
in obituaries for females increased from about 0.30 to the same amount
found in obituaries of males (Fig. 2). In their analysis of obituaries in 1990,
Kirchler et al. (1996) still found gender differences in the percentages of
success-related, stable attributes and interpreted their findings as indicating
that both females and males are perceived as potentially successful leaders,
but although females are seen as motivated and struggling for success, men
are seen as solid experts who always knew and always will know how to
manage an enterprise successfully. In the present analysis, these differences
seem to have disappeared in the 1990s.
Because it has been shown by various studies that stereotypes have re-
markable stability (Deaux, 1993), results of the present study have to be
treated with caution. A growing awareness of stereotypical evaluations and
possibly also inappropriate qualitative differentiation between effective fe-
male and male leaders could result in changes of stereotypes. Data resulting
from the analysis of obituaries are not free of these biases and could therefore
also reflect obituary writers’ increasing sophistication about how they should
portray female leaders. Nevertheless the present study shows that changes
have occurred and it illustrates a methodology that allows capturing shifts
in stereotypical “portrays” over time. This is particularly relevant consid-
ering that stereotypes change very slowly and data on beliefs and opinions
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 841
over a longer period were rarely observed. Additional studies are necessary
to underpin the present findings. Also, contextual variables, such as type of
company the deceased male and female leaders had worked for or the occu-
pation should be considered explicitly. If an occupation is female dominated,
feminine personality characteristics and physical attributes are thought to be
more essential for success than masculine attributes (Cejka & Eagly, 1999).
According to the findings of this study, which is based on the assump-
tion that gender stereotypes are inherent in obituaries on female and male
leaders, traditional views seem to have changed. The increasing number
of women particularly in intermediate leadership positions, the decline
of gender barriers to opportunity, and the observable convergence of
gender images give rise to hope that stereotypes of leaders start to change.
APPENDIX
Descriptive Categories in English and German Language
English German original English German original
1 Active Aktiv 30 Instructor Lehrer
2 Admirable Hochachtungsw¨urdig 31 Intelligent Intelligent
3 Amiable Liebensw ¨urdig 32 Kind G ¨utig
4 Balanced character Ausgeglichen 33 Leader F ¨uhrer
5 Calm Ruhig 34 Likeable Beliebt
6 Caring F ¨ursorglich 35 Loyal Treu
7 Consensus-oriented Kompromissbereit 36 Modest Bescheiden
8 Committed Engagiert 37 Natural Tier-/Natursch ¨utzer
9 Companionate Kameradschaftlich 38 Open-minded Aufgeschlossen
10 Conscientious Pflichtbewusst 39 Opinion-leader Meinungsmacher
11 Convincing ¨
Uberzeugend 40 Organizer Organisator
12 Courageous Mutig 41 Outstanding Verdienstvoll
13 Creative Kreativ 42 Patriotic Patriotisch
14 Decision-maker Entscheidungstr¨ager 43 Pioneer Wegbereiter
15 Efficient T ¨uchtig 44 Professional Professionell
16 Energetic Kr ¨aftig 45 Regardful Umsichtig
17 Entrepreneurial Unternehmergeist 46 Reliable Zuverl ¨assig
spirit
18 Esteemed Gesch ¨atzt 47 Religious Religi¨os
19 Ethical Korrekt 48 Respected Angesehen
20 Experienced Erfahren 49 Responsible Verantwortungs-
bewusst
21 Expert Experte 50 Servant Diener
22 Fair Gerecht 51 Skillful F ¨ahig
23 Farsighted Weitblickend 52 Sociable Kontaktfreudig
24 Friendly Freundlich 53 Strong personality Pers ¨onlichkeit
25 Honest Ehrlich 54 Successful Erfolgreich
26 Humane Menschlich 55 Supportive Hilfsbereit
27 Indefatigable Unerm ¨udlich 56 Unselfish Selbstlos
28 Independent Unabh ¨angig 57 Venerable Verehrungsw¨urdig
29 Innovator Initiator 58 Work-oriented Arbeitsorientiert
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
842 Rodler, Kirchler, and H ¨
olzl
REFERENCES
Adler, N. (2000). An international perspective on the barriers to the advancement of women
managers. Applied Psychology: An International Review,42, 289–300.
Ashmore, R. D., DelBoca, F. K., & Wohlers, A. J. (1986). Gender stereotypes. In R. D. Ashmore
& F. K. DelBoca (Eds.), The social psychology of female–male relations: A critical analysis
of central concepts (pp. 69–119). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (1995). Implicit gender stereotyping in judgements of fame.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,68, 181–198.
Bartol, K. M., & Butterfield, D. A. (1976). Sex effects in evaluating leaders. Journal of Applied
Psychology,67, 446–454.
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill’s handbook of leadership: Theory research and applications
(3rd ed.). New York: Free Press.
Blake, R., & Mouton, J. (1964). The managerial grid. Houston, TX: Gulf.
Brody, L. R. (1997). Gender and emotion: Beyond stereotypes. Journal of Social Issues,53,
369–394.
Burton, C. (1992). Merit and gender: Organizations and the mobilization of masculine bias. In
A. J. Mills & P. Tancred (Eds.), Gendering organizational analysis (pp. 185–200). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Butterfield, D. A., & Grinell, J. P. (1999). “Re-viewing” gender, leadership, and managerial
behavior: Do three decades of research tell us anything? In G. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of
gender and work (pp. 223–238). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Calder, B. J. (1977). An attribution theory of leadership. In B. M. Staw & G. R. Salanik (Eds.),
New directions in organizational behavior. Chicago: St. Clair.
Carli, L. L., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender effectson social influence and emergent leadership.
In G. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of gender and work (pp. 203–280). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender stereotypic images of occupations correspond
to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,4, 413–
423.
Davidson, M., & Burke, R. (2000). Women in management: Current research issues. In M. J.
Davidson & R. J. Burke (Eds.), Women in management (Vol. 2, pp. 1–7). London: Sage.
Deaux, K. (1984). From individual differences to social categories. Analysis of a decade’s re-
search on gender. American Psychologist,39, 105–116.
Deaux, K. (1993). Gender stereotypes. In F. L. Denmark & M. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of
women (pp. 126–139). London: Greenwood Press.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men
of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,10, 1171–1188.
Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
logical Bulletin,108, 233–256.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,60, 685–710.
Eagly,A. H., & Karau, S. J. (in press). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders.
Psychological Review.
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders:
A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,111, 3–22.
Fondas, N. (1997). Feminization unveiled: Management qualities in contemporary writings.
Academy of Management Review,22, 257–282.
Gardinger, M., & Tiggermann, M. (1999). Gender differences in leadership style, job stress
and mental health in male- and female-dominated industries. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology,72, 301–315.
Greenacre, M. (1993). Correspondence analysis in practice. London: Academic Press.
Heilman, M. E., Block, C. J., & Martell, R. F. (1995). Sex stereotypes: Do they influence per-
ceptions of managers? Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,10, 237–252.
Heller, T. (1982). Women and men as leaders. New York: Praeger.
P1: GCP/GFU/GCY
Sex Roles [sers] pp489-sers-373450 May 24, 2002 23:29 Style file version Nov. 19th, 1999
Gender Stereotypes of Leaders 843
Kirchler, E. (1992). Adorable woman, expert man: Changing gender images of women and men
in management. European Journal of Social Psychology,22, 363–373.
Kirchler, E. (1997). The unequal equality: Social stereotypes about female and male en-
trepreneurs. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale,2, 63–77.
Kirchler, E., Wagner, J., & Buchleitner, S. (1996). Der langsame Wechsel auf F¨uhrungsetagen—
Meinungen ¨uber Frauen und M ¨anner als F ¨uhrungspersonen. Zeitschrift f ¨
ur Sozialpsycholo-
gie,27, 148–166.
Konrad, A. M., Ritchie, J. E., Jr., Lieb, P., & Corrigall, E. (2000). Sex differences and similarities
in job attribute preferences: A meta analysis. Psychological Bulletin,126, 593–641.
Laugheed, J. (2000). Attitudes toward women leaders analyzed by gender and occupation
(1984–1998). Advancing Women in Leadership Journal,1, 1–6.
Lord, R. G. (1985). An informational processing approach to social perception, leadership
perceptions and behavioral measurement in organizational settings. In L. L. Cummings
& B. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 87–128). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
Martinko, M. J., & Gardner, W. L. (1987). The leader/member attribution process. Academy of
Management Review,12, 235–249.
Mitchell, T. R. (1995). F¨uhrungstheorien-Attributionstheorie. In A. Kieser, G. Reber, & R.
Wunderer (Eds.), Handw ¨
orterbuch der F¨
uhrung 2 (pp. 847–861). Stuttgart: Poeschel.
Nieva, V. F.,& Gutek, B. A. (1980). Sex effects on evaluation. Academy of Management Review,
5, 267–276.
Pfeffer, J. (1992). Managing with power. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Powell, G. N., & Butterfield, D. A. (1989). The “good manager”: Did androgyny fare better in
the 1980s? Group and Organization Studies,14, 395–403.
Rimm, S. (1998). The cultural underachievement of females. In G. Davis & S. Rimm (Eds.),
Education of the gifted (pp. 308–341). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Robbins, S. (1998). Organizational behavior (8th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic
women. Journal of Social Issues,57, 743–762.
Schein, V. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women’s progress in management.
Journal of Social Issues,57, 675–688.
Vinkenburg, C. J., Jansen, P. G. W., & Koopman, P. L. (2000). Differences in managerial be-
haviour and effectiveness. In M. J. Davidson & R. J. Burke (Eds.), Women in management
(pp. 120–137). London: Sage.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer.
Weinert, A. B. (1998). Organisationspsychologie (4th ed.). Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags
Union.
... There seem to be changes in leadership stereotypes over time; however, they are rather small (Eagly et al., 2020). Studies analyzing obituaries over a period of several decades found a change in stereotypes ascribed to male and female leaders, indicating that stereotypes of male leaders come closer to stereotypes of female leaders and stereotypes of female leaders come closer to stereotypes of male leaders (Rodler et al., 2001;Hartl et al., 2013). Leaders are nowadays seen to be more androgynous (Koenig et al., 2011;Kark et al., 2012) and additionally hold stereotypical feminine characteristics (e.g., individualized consideration in transformational leadership suggesting that leaders should be empathic with employees' needs; Sczesny et al., 2004;Eagly and Sczesny, 2009;Vinkenburg et al., 2011). ...
... This is to say that typical leaders and male leaders tend to be ascribed mainly agentic characteristics in the central core of social representations, while female leaders' central core consists mainly of communal characteristics. However, stereotypes of male and female leaders converge over time and masculine as well as feminine characteristics are desirable for leaders (Rodler et al., 2001;Sczesny et al., 2004;Eagly and Sczesny, 2009;Koenig et al., 2011;Vinkenburg et al., 2011;Kark et al., 2012;Hartl et al., 2013;Griffiths et al., 2019). It is assumed that in male leaders' central core also communal characteristics will be found, whereas the central core for female leaders also includes agentic characteristics. ...
... However, a slight change in the leadership stereotype toward having both masculine and feminine characteristics (Sczesny et al., 2004;Eagly and Sczesny, 2009;Koenig et al., 2011;Vinkenburg et al., 2011;Kark et al., 2012) could be argued. Gender stereotypes reflected thru social representations of male leaders did not change over time, contrary to expectations emerging from previous research on gender stereotypes of leaders (Rodler et al., 2001;Hartl et al., 2013). Core social representations of female leaders included agentic and communal characteristics; however, men's primary social representation for female leaders was the communal characteristic "empathic." ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Previous research often examined gender stereotypes in leadership with ratings on predetermined gendered characteristics concerning leaders’ agency and communality (i.e., explicit measures). The aim of the present study was to broaden the understanding of gender stereotypes in leadership by taking more subtle approaches, that focus on what men and women actually ascribe to typical, male, and female leaders and how they implicitly evaluate them. Methods An online survey collected (a) free associations which reflect social representations (e.g., dominant, empathic), (b) evaluations of the given associations as negative, neutral, or positive, and (c) ratings on Peabody’s semantic differential combining non-gendered adjective pairs to an evaluative component of a typical leader, a male leader, and a female leader. Results Using the approach of social representations by analyzing 2,842 free associations from 194 participants shows the predominant gender stereotypes. Ratings of the free associations revealed that women evaluate characteristics associated with female leaders more negatively than those associated with typical leaders and male leaders. By contrast, using the evaluative component of non-gendered adjective pairs shows that typical and female leaders were often rated more positively than male leaders and that women were more likely to devalue male leaders. Discussion Directly asking about leaders (i.e., associations) might retrieve participants’ gender stereotypes, whereas when using non-direct questions (i.e., evaluation component of adjective pairs) gender stereotypes might be less prominent. Thus, when evaluating leaders, practitioners and researchers should consider whether these evaluations were obtained explicitly or implicitly to assess potential influences of gender stereotypes.
... We fill this gap in the literature by examining the role of gender in the creation and persuasiveness of online reviews. According to prior research (Diehl et al., 2004;Gaucher et al., 2011;Madera et al., 2009;Rodler et al., 2001), gender differences exist in written text. In addition, online review research states that review content influences persuasiveness (e.g., Ransbotham et al., 2019). ...
... In the context of natural language use, studies have found communal and agentic differences in texts that relate to women and men (Diehl et al., 2004;Gaucher et al., 2011;Madera et al., 2009;Rodler et al., 2001). Diehl et al. (2004) found that while men include more agencyrelated attributes in their self-descriptions, women use more communion-related attributes. ...
... Letters of recommendation for academic positions describe female candidates with more communal traits and less agentic ones than male candidates (Madera et al., 2009). An analysis of obituaries reveals that female leaders were described as more communal than male leaders (Rodler et al., 2001). Ravula et al. ...
Article
Full-text available
Online reviews play an important role in consumers’ purchase journeys and therefore have received considerable research attention. Yet research is limited as to whether and how a reviewer’s gender affects persuasiveness. In response, we analyze more than one million reviews posted on the website Yelp and find that an author’s gender indeed affects review text and, consequently, persuasiveness. Specifically, we find that (1) reviews posted by women (vs. men) are more authentic, less analytical, more positive-affective, and less negative-affective and (2) authentic, analytical, negative-affective text increases the persuasiveness of reviews while positive-affective text lowers it. We also find that the persuasiveness of reviews from women (vs. men) depends on product category, suggesting that retailers should consider the product category and authors’ gender when ranking reviews.
... The important research available dealing with this gender-sensitive issue belongs to Kastenbaum et al. (1976), Spilka et al. (1979), Kearl (1986), Halbur and Vandagriff (1987), Maybury (1995), Eid (2002), Rodler et al. (2002) and Ogletree et al. (2005). The studies of these researchers mainly intend to gauge the representation and projection of both genders in obituarial discourses under the assumption that gender based partialities perpetuate after death. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study examines the issue of gender discrimination in the post death scenario of obituarial discourse. It aims to identify the way Pakistani newspaper obituaries recognize and project males and females after their deaths. A total of 601 paid obituaries published in a year's time span in Pakistani English newspapers were evaluated for the purpose. 10 qualitative interviews were also conducted to supplement the findings and discussion. Quantification of the data suggests that males not only get more obituaries but also get added projection when compared to females. To understand the reasons behind this varied treatment, the participants' responses were analyzed. The findings reveal that the observed differences in the death reportage of both genders do not purely fall in the line of gender discrimination. Males get situational advantage due to the factors like familial traditions, religious beliefs, cultural traditions and socioeconomic environments.
Chapter
Die Unterrepräsentanz von Frauen in Führungspositionen ist ein weithin bekanntes Thema, welches einen inzwischen hochgradig aktiven Forschungsbereich entfacht hat. Für eine weitere Dynamik sorgen politische Entwicklungen, wie z. B. gesetzliche Regelungen zu Geschlechterquoten. In diesem Kapitel werden zunächst die klassischen und die neueren theoretischen Erklärungsansätze diskutiert, darunter das ökonomische Kapital, die „gekennzeichnete Frau“ (Kanter, 1977), die Theorie der Rolleninkongruenz (Eagly & Karau, 2002) und der Ansatz einer impliziten Geschlechterquote (Smith & Parrotta, 2018). Zudem betrachten wir gesellschaftliche Konstruktionen und Rollenbilder in Bezug auf weibliche Führungskräfte, wie Frauen als Ressource, als „andere Führungspersonen“ oder „weibliche Führung“ (Rosener, 1990). Wir gehen auf die Aussagefähigkeit der „gläsernen Decke“ (Hymowitz & Schellhardt, 1986) und der „gläsernen Klippe“ (Ryan & Haslam, 2005) ein und bieten eine Forschungsübersicht zur Wirkung von gesetzlichen Geschlechterquoten.
Article
This paper reports on study of online COVID-19 memorials posted during 2020 to the Church of England website https://www.rememberme2020.uk/ . The paper employs a Corpus-Assisted Discourse Studies (CADS) approach to analyse networks of co-occurring linguistic items (types and lemmata) and patterns (ngrams) within online memorials, and examines how these frequent items/patterns exist within networked discourses that underpin an overarching bereavement discourse. The analysis finds that bereavement discourse is underpinned by frequent reference to love, relationships and relational identification, time and temporality, loss/absence, and memory, as well as metaphors based on container and journey image schema. An analysis of these metaphorical representations of death and bereavement suggest that online memorials serve as a space for the social practice of bereavement and shows how the language used to grieve attempts to ideologically (re)present the relationships between the bereaved and decedent. All code used in this paper can be found at https://osf.io/khcj2/ .
Chapter
This chapter explores the contributing factors of the underrepresentation of Latina faculty in tenured positions in one higher education institution through a qualitative case study. The narratives from eight tenured Latina faculty in one state public four-year university in the southeast area of the United States were analyzed to identify barriers or supports these minority faculty experienced while working to achieve tenure. Five main themes emerged from the analysis: organizational exclusionary practices, white male-oriented culture where resources are used to benefit white males, demoralizing microaggressions from white faculty, the university leadership's lack of action and accountability to address diversity and inclusion challenges, and the lack of support networks and mentoring. This chapter addresses various reasons higher educational institutions need to remove barriers that negatively affect recruitment and retention of Latina faculty and provides recommendations to academic leaders to implement and hold everyone accountable to an inclusive academic environment.
Chapter
This chapter explores the contributing factors of the underrepresentation of Latina faculty in tenured positions in one higher education institution through a qualitative case study. The narratives from eight tenured Latina faculty in one state public four-year university in the southeast area of the United States were analyzed to identify barriers or supports these minority faculty experienced while working to achieve tenure. Five main themes emerged from the analysis: organizational exclusionary practices, white male-oriented culture where resources are used to benefit white males, demoralizing microaggressions from white faculty, the university leadership's lack of action and accountability to address diversity and inclusion challenges, and the lack of support networks and mentoring. This chapter addresses various reasons higher educational institutions need to remove barriers that negatively affect recruitment and retention of Latina faculty and provides recommendations to academic leaders to implement and hold everyone accountable to an inclusive academic environment.
Thesis
Full-text available
Newspaper paid obituary announcements are contextualized generic structures with religious and socio-cultural communicative functions. This generic structure reflects differences among people from various social strata to project their social status. The analysis in the Pakistani milieu was conducted due to a lack of studies which explore these announcements to determine local socio-cultural and socio-economic factors which influence their communicative intents. A genre analysis was conducted to determine the communicative purposes, stylistic devices, gender representation, content selection and non-linguistic features of this genre. Data were obtained by describing, investigating and analysing the discourse generic modes. The data comprises 601 printed paid obituaries selected from Pakistani English newspapers; The Dawn and The News International. A total of 30 respondents from various background were interviewed. A methodological framework of Al-Ali in 2005 which was an adapted form of Swales‟ in 1990 moves analysis model, is used as a prototypical. Moves analysis and inductive reasoning lead towards the structural and informational understanding of these announcements. Procedural steps involving trilateral phases: identification, comprehension and validation have been adopted. The findings show a blend of similarities and dissimilarities with the similarly produced genre globally. It was also found that the communicational load of the textual moves of this genre is dependent on the contextualized societal and religious practices, and personalized intentions of the discourse community. The findings of this study could serve as an abstract model of guidance for the professional and unprofessional writers of this genre in Pakistan. It also helps to create awareness towards using appropriate linguistic forms among practitioners of English in Pakistan, and provides a theoretical model for future researches dealing with a similar subject matter.
Chapter
This study examines if gender bias affects the recruitment process for top managerial positions in Chile. Using conjoint analysis, we evaluate how recruiters, a sample of 114 business postgraduate students, ranked a set of multi-attribute profiles, showing their preferences among profiles and the trade-off among attributes they are willing to do.
Conference Paper
Stereotypes about gender and emotional expression tend to be imprecise and misleading. They fail to acknowledge situational, individual, and cultural variations in males' and females' emotional expressiveness. They also tend to generalize across emotional intensity and frequency, as well as across different modalities of emotional expression, e.g. verbal vs, behavioral modalities. Moreover they tend to exaggerate the extent of gender differences in emotional expression. I argue that when gender differences in emotional expression do occur they can be traced to social processes such as dissimilar gender roles, status and power imbalances, and differing socialization histories of males and females. These processes may predispose some males and females to express emotions differently in some cultures and in some contexts. To support this argument, I present data from two studies, one showing that the amount of time fathers spend with their children relates to the gender stereotypic nature of their children's emotional expressiveness; and the other showing that gender differences in emotional expressiveness are culturally specific in a sample of Asian international, Asian-American, and European-American college students. Finally I note the potentially destructive limitations imposed by stereotypes on males' and females' interpersonal functioning as well as on their mental and physical health.
Article
This article reports on two studies which investigate gender stereotypes and opinions about leadership success in business organizations. In the first study, 150 participants (male and female entrepreneurs, male and female collaborators and male managers) were asked to indicate typical characteristics of successful male and female entrepreneurs. When asked directly, only a few differences between male and female entrepreneurs were reported. Thus, it would seem that gender stereotypes no longer exist. The second study aimed at avoiding social desirability tendencies, which are likely to bias questionnaire data, and also at investigating changes of stereotypes over time. Obituaries of male and female leaders, formulated by former collaborators of the deceased person, from the years 1960, 1970, 1980 and 1990 were analyzed. It is shown that, independent of the year, male leaders were mainly described as intelligent and experienced. Characteristics attributed to women leaders changed over time. Whereas they were described as adorable persons in 1960 and 1970, during the last decade they were characterized as highly engaged. Leadership success seems to be attributed to both males and females, but while males' success may be based on stable characteristics, females' success seems to depend on rather unstable attributes. This hypothesis was subsequently tested. Five psychologists classified the attributes into categories of stable versus unstable and success-correlated versus success-neutral attributes. Both in the past and at present, success-correlated stable attributes are more likely ascribed to males whereas unstable characteristics are less likely attributed to males than to females in leadership positions.
Article
Within work organisations, perceptions of gender-based competencies and inclinations affect assignment and other practices and thus the distribution of women and men among different opportunity structures. Five illustrations are provided to support the view that ideas about masculinity and femininity are embedded in organisational arrangements. The opportunity to accumulate merit and the attribution of merit appear to be structured along gender lines.
Article
In this article, research is reviewed on the emergence of male and female leaders in initially leaderless groups. In these laboratory and field studies, men emerged as leaders to a greater extent than did women. Male leadership was particularly likely in short-term groups and in groups carrying out tasks that did not require complex social interaction. In contrast, women emerged as social leaders slightly more than did men. These and other findings were interpreted in terms of gender role theory, which maintains that societal gender roles influence group behavior. According to this theory, sex differences in emergent leadership are due primarily to role-induced tendencies for men to specialize more than women in behaviors strictly oriented to their group's task and for women to specialize more than men in socially facilitative behaviors.
Article
Qualities that are culturally associated with females are appearing in descriptions of managerial work in the texts of contemporary writers, and these texts function as carriers of a feminine ethos to practicing managers. The article describes a mechanism-management writings-used to disseminate and legitimate a management idea. It examines the nature of the rhetoric used in management writings and the implication of authors' failure to label the idea by its name: feminization. This "feminization" is discussed from both women's voice and poststructuralist perspectives. Implications for management theorizing and future research are described.
Article
Prejudicial evaluation is often cited as an explanation for the apparent failure of competent women to achieve as much success as men have. In this paper we review research on the evaluation of the qualifications and performance of men and women, and research on attributions about the causes of performance. Although most of the studies reviewed show pro-male evaluation bias, contradictory evidence has also been found. Three factors affecting these research findings are discussed: (1) the level of inference required in the evaluation situation, (2) the effects of sex-role incongruence, and (3) the effect of level of qualifications and performance involved.
Article
Dynamic stereotypes characterize social groups that are thought to have changed from the attributes they manifested in the past and even to continue to change in the future. According to social role theory’s assumption that the role behavior of group members shapes their stereotype, groups should have dynamic stereotypes to the extent that their typical social roles are perceived to change over time. Applied to men and women, this theory makes two predictions about perceived change: (a) perceivers should think that sex differences are eroding because of increasing similarity of the roles of men and women and (b) the female stereotype should be particularly dynamic because of greater change in the roles of women than of men. This theory was tested and confirmed in five experiments that examined perceptions of the roles and the personality, cognitive, and physical attributes of men and women of the past, present, and future.