ArticlePDF Available

Territory defence in tropical birds: Are females as aggressive as males?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Much of our knowledge concerning the functions of territorial behaviour and how territories are defended by individuals comes from research on birds. The vast majority of this work has focused on temperate zone breeding territoriality in which territories are defended most obviously by males. Our understanding of the female role in territory defence is limited because they are less conspicuous and much harder to observe. We studied sex roles in territory maintenance and defence in a duetting, resident neotropical passerine, the white-bellied antbird (Myrmeciza longipes). This species maintains territories and pair bonds year round and both sexes sing and actively participate in territory defence. We performed a series of playback experiments throughout the dry (non-breeding) and wet (breeding) seasons. We exposed territorial pairs to three types of stimuli including: (1) single sex, male only songs, (2) single sex, female only songs, and (3) both sex songs/duets. Contrary to findings for most other tropical species, individuals defended their territories with equal levels of aggression regardless of stimuli. Furthermore, sex roles were very different, with males responding more aggressively than females to all stimuli throughout both seasons. Both males and females consistently responded more aggressively to territorial intrusions during the dry season than during the wet season, likely because food abundance is low in the dry season and territory value is high. Our analysis of duetting behaviour suggests that duets do not serve a significant role in mate guarding, or territory defence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2005) 58: 414–422
DOI 10.1007/s00265-005-0928-4
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Bradley C. Fedy ·Bridget J. M. Stutchbury
Territory defence in tropical birds: are females as aggressive
as males?
Received: 30 January 2004 / Revised: 28 January 2005 / Accepted: 8 February 2005 / Published online: 23 April 2005
C
Springer-Verlag 2005
Abstract Much of our knowledge concerning the func-
tions of territorial behaviour and how territories are de-
fended by individuals comes from research on birds. The
vast majority of this work has focused on temperate zone
breeding territoriality in which territories are defended
most obviously by males. Our understanding of the female
role in territory defence is limited because they are less
conspicuous and much harder to observe. We studied sex
roles in territory maintenance and defence in a duetting,
resident neotropical passerine, the white-bellied antbird
(Myrmeciza longipes). This species maintains territories
and pair bonds year round and both sexes sing and actively
participate in territory defence. We performed a series of
playback experiments throughout the dry (non-breeding)
and wet (breeding) seasons. We exposed territorial pairs to
three types of stimuli including: (1) single sex, male only
songs, (2) single sex, female only songs, and (3) both sex
songs/duets. Contrary to findings for most other tropical
species, individuals defended their territories with equal
levels of aggression regardless of stimuli. Furthermore, sex
roles were very different, with males responding more ag-
gressively than females to all stimuli throughout both sea-
sons. Both males and females consistently responded more
aggressively to territorial intrusions during the dry sea-
son than during the wet season, likely because food abun-
dance is low in the dry season and territory value is high.
Our analysis of duetting behaviour suggests that duets do
not serve a significant role in mate guarding, or territory
defence.
Communicated by A. Cockburn
B. C. Fedy ()
Department of Biology, York University,
4700 Keele St.,
Toronto, Ontario, M3J 1P3, Canada
e-mail: bfedy@interchange.ubc.ca
Present address:
B. J. M. Stutchbury
Centre for Applied Conservation Research, University of
British Columbia,
3rd Floor Forest Sciences Centre,
Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 1Z4, Canada
Key words Sex roles .Territory defence .Tropics .
White-bellied antbirds
Introduction
Territory defence in birds has been extensively studied,
but primarily in temperate zone species where breeding
territories are defended for a short period in spring and
summer, and male defence via song and border disputes is
conspicuous (Catchpole and Slater 1995). Female territory
defence against other females is relatively under-studied
in birds because it is more subtle and therefore difficult
to observe (Amundsen 2000). Tropical passerines offer a
unique opportunity to ask new questions about territorial
behaviour because in many species both sexes sing and
defend the territory and territory defence takes place all
year round (Stutchbury and Morton 2001). Are males and
females equally aggressive in territory defence? Do they
direct their aggression primarily at same-sexed intruders,
or does the pair co-operate and repel all intruders regardless
of sex?
The evolution of sex roles is primarily driven by the op-
erational sex ratio (OSR) (Emlen and Oring 1977), which
in turn affects the intensity of sexual selection within each
sex. Factors that influence OSR include: (1) the relative
abundance of each sex (Emlen and Oring 1977), (2) the po-
tential for mating (Clutton-Brock and Vincent 1991), and
(3) the stringency of mate choice (Owens and Thompson
1994). These factors appear to be equal for male and fe-
male tropical passerines that have a monogamous mating
system and yearlong territoriality (Stutchbury and Morton
2001). Furthermore, both males and females require territo-
ries and are capable of defending them alone (Levin 1996b;
Morton and Derrickson 1996; Fedy and Stutchbury, unpub-
lished data). Greenberg and Gradwohl (1983) concluded
that both sexes of dot-winged antwrens (Microrhopia quix-
ensis) participate equally in territory defence behaviour.
However, this is not the case for all species. In other species
of antbirds, males are more aggressive than females (Willis
1973; Morton and Derrickson 1996; Bard et al. 2002).
415
In several tropical bird species, individuals of a pair de-
fend their territories primarily against intruders of the same
sex (Freed 1987; Levin 1996b; Morton and Derrickson
1996; Appleby et al. 1999; Morton et al. 2000). In
playback experiments, both Levin (1996b) and Morton and
Derrickson (1996) found that females responded more
aggressively to same sex songs than to duets or male
songs. Same-sex territory defence by females may deter
trespassing, and eviction, by other females (e.g. McElroy
and Ritchison 1996). This type of same-sex aggression
in females has also evolved in several other taxa with
monogamous mating patterns, including several species of
monogamous antelope (the dikdik Madoqua kirkii, Broth-
erton and Rhodes 1996; and the klipspringer Oreotragus
oreotragus,, Roberts and Dunbar 2000). Alternatively,
year round territory defence by males and females may
lead to co-operative territory defence within a pair. Hall
(2000) documented apparent co-operative behaviour in a
series of playback experiments investigating the function
of duetting in magpie-larks (Grallina cyanoleuca). In that
study, females maintained both territory and pair bond by
attempting to exclude intruders of either sex in apparent
co-operation with their mates.
We studied sex roles in territory maintenance and defence
in the white-bellied antbird (Myrmeciza longipes), a resi-
dent neotropical passerine which maintains territories and
pair bonds year round. Both males and females sing and
exhibit aggressive territory defence and pairs routinely duet
throughout the year. Like other species of antbirds (Formi-
cariidae), both sexes contribute equally to nest building,
incubation and care of the young. We performed a series
of playback experiments throughout the dry and wet sea-
sons. We exposed territorial pairs to three types of stimuli
including: (1) single sex, male only songs, (2) single sex,
female only songs, and (3) both sex songs/duets. Because
the threat of eviction from a territory applies only to a same
sex competitor we predicted that individuals would defend
their territories more aggressively from same sex intruders
than opposite sex (Levin 1996b; Morton and Derrickson
1996; Appleby et al. 1999). Territorial intrusions are ex-
pected to pose a greater threat to resident birds in the wet
season than in the dry season because the majority of breed-
ing occurs during the wet season, mates are likely fertile
and thus intruders present an immediate risk of cuckoldry.
Furthermore, if an intruder evicts a resident during this
period, the evicted bird likely will have difficulty finding
a new territory in time to breed that season. Thus, given
the importance of territory ownership for breeding, we also
predicted that individuals would respond more strongly to
playbacks during the wet season than the dry season.
Duets generally have the two key characteristics of
a constant lag time between the contributions of the
two individuals and a repeated and predictable structure
(Langmore 2002). Several non-mutually exclusive hy-
potheses exist for the function of avian duetting (Hall 2004).
One key hypotheses is that duetting may represent a form
of co-operative territorial defence between pair members
(Wickler 1976, Wickler and Seibt 1980; Hall 2000; Grafe
and Bitz 2004; but see Farabaugh 1982). If this hypoth-
Fig. 1 Tracing of a white-bellied antbird (Myrmeciza longipes) duet.
Black male song, white female song. Females join the male song
which results in song overlap
esis is correct, then duets should be perceived as more
threatening territorial signals than solo songs. If duetting
is a more threatening signal, then the proportion of duets
sung by the defenders should increase during playbacks
compared with natural background rates. Furthermore, duet
playbacks should elicit a more aggressive response from the
territory owners than single sex playbacks. White-bellied
antbirds are unusual because their duetting appears to be al-
most entirely under female control. A duet is formed when
the female begins singing during a male song (Fig. 1). In
most other species, either sex can join the song of their
mate to create a duet (Farabaugh 1982), and evidence
seems to suggest that it may be more common among
species for the male to join the female to create the duet
(see Langmore 1998). Therefore, we can also predict that
duets, as a form of female defence, will be more common
in response to female song playbacks than to male song
playbacks.
Methods
Study site and study species
White-bellied antbirds inhabit second growth and edge
habitat in lowland tropical areas from northern Brazil to
central Panama (de Schauensee 1966; Wetmore 1972).
They are a medium-sized (Xbar +−SE =29 g ±0.3,
range =25–32.5 g), sexually dimorphic, passerine of
the Formicariidae family and males are slightly heav-
ier than females (males: Xbar +−SE =28.7±0.3g,
n=42; females: Xbar +−SE =27.5±0.3g,n=29; t-test
P=0.005). They are insectivorous birds that associate in
pairs throughout the year, forage almost exclusively in the
ground leaf litter, and do not follow ant swarms.
Research was conducted from 15 January to 30 July 2000
in Parque Nacional Soberania, Panama (97N, 7940W)
at the junction of the Panama Canal and the Chagres River.
This area is a tropical lowland forest receiving approxi-
mately 2.6 m of rain per year (Karr 1971), almost all of
which falls during the wet season. Central Panama has a
pronounced dry season from mid-December to late April
(Windsor 1990). In general, the non-breeding season for
ground foraging, insectivorous birds in this area corre-
sponds with the dry season and lasts approximately from
January to April. Breeding can begin in late April, when
the wet season begins and litter arthropod populations in-
crease (Levings and Windsor 1982). However, breeding is
416
highly asynchronous and evidence of breeding can still be
found into December for species with similar ecology to
white-bellied antbirds (Robinson et al. 2000).
Birds were caught in mist nets by broadcasting a con-
specific song on their territory and every individual was
banded with a unique colour combination (n=48 males
and 34 females). Some birds were flushed into the nets
when necessary. Territory boundaries were determined by
mapping bird locations during song bouts, border disputes
and by assessing responses to playback of recorded songs
(Falls 1981).
Natural song rates
Data on natural song rates were collected between 0620
and 1230 hours throughout the non-breeding and breeding
seasons. Sessions lasted1hwithaminimum of 24 h be-
tween sampling sessions on any one territory (total 140 h,
Xbar +−SE =8.8h±0.52 per territory, n=16 territo-
ries). Researchers sat quietly on the edge of the territory
and recorded all vocalisations of the focal pair. Distance
between pairs was recorded every time a pair duetted. Total
song rate refers to the number of songs/h for a focal pair and
includes solo and duet songs. Natural song rate data were
not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk P=0.01), therefore
non-parametric tests were used in all analyses of these
data.
Playback experiments
Playback experiments were conducted in the dry season
(10 March to 12 April) and the wet season (21 May to
18 June) between 0600 and 1230 hours. Each pair of ter-
ritorial birds (n=15 pairs) was subject to three playback
experiments in both seasons. Each experiment involved
broadcasting a song stimulus to the focal birds. The three
different stimuli were: (1) male song only, (2) female song
only, and (3) duets. The order of stimuli presentation was
randomised for each pair within each season. Experiments
were spaced between 20 and 48 h apart and neighbouring
territories were not tested on the same day.
Playback tapes were made from high-quality recordings
of territorial individuals located a minimum of 3 km away
from the focal pair’s territory. This distance is well out-
side the dispersal range of white-bellied antbirds (Fedy
and Stutchbury 2004) and is sufficient to ensure that all
individuals were exposed to unfamiliar songs. All record-
ings were made within 5 m of the subject using a Sony
TC-D5 PRO II recorder with a Sennheiser MKH 816 di-
rectional microphone. Playback tapes mimicked the typical
singing pattern and amplitude of aggressive white-bellied
antbirds as determined by observing natural boundary dis-
putes and response to simulated intrusions. Throughout
the study we observed 32 boundary disputes (13 territo-
ries). During these disputes average male song rate was
2.0 songs/min (SD 0.5), average female song rate was
1.1 songs/min (SD 0.4) and the average number of songs
sung as a duet was 0.7/min (SD 0.3). Each playback tape
was made by looping a 1-min segment of a natural recording
for 10 min (male song only: 2 songs/min; female song only:
1 song/min; duets: 2 male only songs and one duet/min).
Four tapes of different individuals were made for each of
the three stimuli (male, female, duets n=12 tapes) to avoid
the potentially confounding affects of psuedoreplication
(Kroodsma 1989). One of the four tapes was chosen ran-
domly for each trial within an experiment.
The playback speaker was concealed within a pair’s ter-
ritory a minimum of 15 m from the nearest boundary and
observers retired to cover a minimum of 10 m away. Birds
showed no response to our presence during the playback
experiments. To determine the focal birds’ distance to the
playback speaker, 5-m intervals around the speaker were
marked off with flagging tape. Between each trial the play-
back speaker was moved 5–10 m within the territory to
avoid habituation to any particular location. Focal birds
were silent for a minimum of 10 min before each exper-
iment began. This species has low natural song rates and
therefore the situation of 10 min of no singing was not un-
common. Songs were broadcast for the first 10 min of the
experiments and were followed by a 10-min post-playback
silent period. We recorded the behaviour of the resident
male and female for the entire duration (20 min). Six vari-
ables were collected for all birds during playback experi-
ments: total time responding—time from the first song or
movement towards the speaker until the bird moved >15 m
away and stopped singing (TTR; s), the latency to first vo-
calization of any kind (LFV; s), number of songs (NS),
closest approach to speaker (CA; m), time spent between
20 and 5 m to the speaker (T20; s), and time spent <5mto
the speaker (T5; s). A strong response by territory owners
would show high TTR, NS, T20, T5 and low values for
LFV and CA. To quantify the intensity of response to the
playback experiments a principal components analysis was
performed on the variables by the use of the correlation ma-
trix (James and McCullough 1990; McGregor 1992). Prin-
cipal components analysis does not accommodate missing
response values, so it was necessary to insert values for
LFV and CA for birds that did not respond. In these cases
the individual was assigned an LFV of 1200 s (20 min, the
entire duration of the experiment) and a CA of 30 m (the
largest recorded in responding birds). All response vari-
ables measuring the intensity of an individual’s response
were entered without transformation, except for CA which
was log transformed to achieve a closer approximation of a
normal distribution. Means are presented ±SE throughout
and all data were analysed using SPSS v. 10.
Results
Natural song rates
White-bellied antbirds have low natural song rate and al-
though average male song rate was only 12–17 songs/h,
in two watches we recorded up to and over 200 songs/h,
whereas for 43% of watches no songs were recorded from
417
Fig. 2 Natural song rates
throughout the day. Each circle
represents a sampling session.
Closed circles dry sessions,
open circles wet season
sessions. Unlike many other
tropical passerines,
white-bellied antbirds do not
sing a dawn chorus in either the
dry or wet season
either the male or female of a pair. The average number of
songs/hour for males and females combined did not vary by
time of day (0630–1230 hours) throughout the sampling pe-
riod (Fig. 2, ANOVA: F1,146=2.4, P=0.12), and there was
no dawn chorus. Average total song rate/h in the dry sea-
son (12.0±2.9) was not significantly different than in the
wet season (17.6±4.1 Wilcoxon signed ranks test: n=16,
z=−0.83, P=0.41). When males and females were con-
sidered separately the song rate/h of either sex (including
solo songs and duets) did not differ between the dry and
wet seasons (males: dry 8.3±1.9, wet 13.5±3.1, n=16,
z=−1.32, P=0.19 females: dry 3.7±1.1, wet 4.2±1.2,
z=−0.06, n=16, P=0.96). Average male song rate per hour
(10.9±1.6) was significantly higher than female song rate
per hour (3.9±0.8, z=−3.16, n=16, P<0.001) across both
seasons (Fig. 3). The number of duets given per hour did not
differ between the dry (2.7±1.0) and wet seasons (2.6±0.9,
z=−0.17, n=16, P=0.87).
Playback experiments
Song rate for both males and females increased signifi-
cantly from natural levels during playback experiments.
During playback trials individuals averaged 18.6±1.9
songs/20 min. Natural song rates of the same birds were sig-
Fig. 3 Box plots showing natural song rates between seasons
(n=16). Grey boxes dry season, white boxes wet season
nificantly lower 2.6±0.4 songs/20 min (z=−4.78, n=30,
P<0.001). In a few cases, territorial individuals did not re-
spond to the playback experiments. In the dry season, males
(n=15) responded to 100% of the experiments (45/45 tri-
als) and females (n=15) responded to 91% of experiments
(41/45). Both sexes decreased the number of experiments
they responded to in the wet season. Males responded to
88% of the experiments (40/45) and females to 71% of
experiments (32/45). Over both seasons combined males
responded to 94% of experiments (85/90) and females to
81% (73/90).
The principal components analysis (PCA) showed that
PC1 varied positively with TTR, NS, T20 and T5. The
range of loadings varied little from T5 (0.78) to T20 (0.95).
PC1 varied negatively with the other two variables (LFV,
0.87; CA, 0.74). PC1 accounted for 74% of the total
variation in the original data set. Data were then analysed
using a repeated measures ANOVA (RMA).
There was a significant difference in the intensity of
response to playbacks between the dry and wet seasons
(MANOVA: F1,15=6.4, P=0.02) and in the intensity of re-
sponses from the different sexes (MANOVA: F1,15=19.7,
P<0.001). Thus, on average, both sexes respond more ag-
gressively to playbacks in the dry season than in the wet
season and males consistently respond more aggressively
than females throughout both seasons. The different stimuli
(male song, female song, duet) did not elicit significantly
different responses (MANOVA: F2,15=0.13, P=0.88). Fur-
thermore, analysis of interaction terms revealed no sig-
nificant interactions between season, stimuli or sex (sea-
son*sex, MANOVA: F2,15=1.4, P=0.27; stimuli ×sex,
MANOVA: F2,15=1.3, P=0.31, season ×stimuli ×sex,
MANOVA: F2,15=0.8, P=0.44). An RMA was also con-
ducted to see if the presentation order of the tapes had any
effect on the intensity of the birds’ responses and no signifi-
cant difference was found within the tape order or the inter-
action terms (tape order, MANOVA: F2,15=0.0, P=0.99;
season ×tape order, MANOVA: F2,15=1.2, P=0.32).
Figure 4shows the actual values for three of the key vari-
ables (TTR, NS, LFV) which contributed to the calculation
of PC1 and reflects the findings of the principal components
analysis.
418
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
duets male
only
female
only
duets male
only
female
only
duets male
only
female
only
duets male
only
female
only
duets male
only
female
only
duets male
only
female
only
Total time responding
(seconds ± se, n=15)
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
Number of songs (± se, n=15)
Latency to first vocalization
(seconds ± se, n=15)
NON-BREEDING BREEDING
Total time responding
(seconds ± se, n=15)
Number of songs (± se, n=15)Latency to first vocalization
(seconds ± se, n=15)
Fig. 4 Male and female responses to the three different playback stimuli. Black bars male response, white bars female response. Variables
reported are: total time responding (s), number of songs, and latency to first vocalisation (s)
Since duetting results from females singing after the male
initiates his song, there are two ways to gauge the frequency
with which females duet and hence use duetting in terri-
torial aggression. First, what proportion of female songs
are given as duets with the male, and second, what propor-
tion of male songs are joined by female song to create a
duet? We averaged the proportion of female songs in duets
across both seasons in response to male song only and fe-
male song only playbacks. Duets occurred in 54%±8%
of female songs in response to female song playback and
56%±8% in response to male song playbacks. This was
not significantly different (z=−0.47, n=15, P=0.64). We
conducted a paired analysis of natural duetting behaviour
and duets sung during playbacks to determine if a sim-
ulated intrusion increases the frequency of duetting by
females. An increase in duetting during intrusions may
indicate that duets are a more aggressive form of territorial
signal than solo songs. We averaged the proportion of songs
sung in duets throughout all playback experiments (n=6
experiments per individual) and compared this against the
natural duet rate for the same individuals. During the play-
back experiments females joined 24% of male songs to
419
create a duet, compared with 27% for natural song obser-
vations (z=−0.97, n=14, P=0.33). Similarly, duets com-
prised 60% of all female songs given during playbacks,
and 56% of all female songs during natural observations
(z=−1.16, n=14, P=0.25). Summarising, both predictions
regarding the possible role of female duetting in territory
defence were rejected.
Discussion
The territorial behaviour of white-bellied antbirds is not
typical compared with other similar species (Levin 1996b;
Morton and Derrickson 1996), demonstrating that our un-
derstanding of the prevalent tropical territorial system of
year round defence is still quite limited. Sex roles in terri-
tory defence were very different, and birds responded with
equal aggression to same sex and opposite sex playbacks.
Song rates are typically low and there is no conspicuous
dawn chorus. This is likely due to low intruder pressure,
compared with temperate zone species, and stable terri-
tory boundaries with known neighbours (Greenberg and
Gradwohl 1986).
Seasons
White-bellied antbirds responded more strongly to play-
backs during the dry season. They feed on leaf-litter arthro-
pods which increase dramatically in abundance during the
wet season (Levings and Windsor 1982). The stronger re-
sponses during the dry season may be the result of a higher
territory value, due to a greater competition for rare food
resources. This suggests that territory defence may be re-
lated to food abundance. Territory defence may be more
important when food is limited during the dry season to
ensure access to food and avoid competition with intrud-
ers over food resources. These findings agree with other
theoretical and empirical studies on the role of food re-
sources in territorial behaviour in that an increase in food
resources led to a decrease in territorial aggression (Brown
1964; Gill and Wolf 1975; Dubois and Giraldeau 2004). Of
course, maintaining territories in the dry season would also
secure a breeding position in the wet season, when nesting
actually occurs.
Constraints during the breeding (wet) season may ac-
count for some of the disparity observed in aggression
levels between the two seasons. Attendance at the nest,
or young, could constrain participation in territory defence
during the breeding season due to time or energy restric-
tions. If birds were attending to eggs or young it may be
too costly in terms of time lost foraging and feeding young
to engage in active territory defence. Territory defence may
also expose young to predators as territory defence is very
conspicuous and young seem to stay close to their parents.
We were unable to determine the exact stage of breeding
during playbacks in the breeding season, so are unable to
directly compare the defence behaviour at different nest
stages.
Both sexes also responded to a greater proportion of the
playback experiments during the dry season. It is unlikely
that focal birds did not hear the playbacks as white-bellied
antbird songs are loud and can often be heard across two
territories (B. Fedy, personal observation). Territory bound-
aries were well established and stable and we knew the
extent of every marked bird’s territory. Furthermore, radio-
telemetry has shown it is very rare for a white-bellied
antbird to engage in forays that are further than two ter-
ritories away (Fedy and Stutchbury 2004). Therefore, we
can conclude that birds chose to ignore the intrusion when
they did not respond to the playback.
Natural song rates were low (males: 11/h, females: 4/h)
and similar between both seasons. Although birds are more
aggressive during the dry season in response to intrusions,
they do not show a higher natural song rate in the absence
of intruders. It is unlikely that low song rates were the
result of limited food abundance because natural song rates
did not increase with higher food abundance in the wet
season.
Sex roles
Males are more aggressive in territory defence than fe-
males. An inequality in sex roles has been documented in
this type of territorial system before, but not to the same
degree (Morton and Derrickson 1996). Male white-bellied
antbirds consistently responded to more of the playback ex-
periments and were more aggressive than females regard-
less of stimuli or season. This sex difference in territorial
aggression may be connected to the benefits of retaining
versus switching mates and territories. Levin (1996a) and
Morton et al. (2000) conducted removal experiments on
their duetting study species. In both species, it was found
that territorial vacancies (both experimentally created and
natural) were filled very rapidly (less than 1 day). This rapid
replacement rate was observed in another duetting species,
the buff-breasted wren (S. Gill and B. J. M. Stutchbury
unpubl. data). Furthermore, Levin (1996a) found that bay
wrens (Thryothorus nigricapillus) would quickly increase
their song rate after the loss of a mate and this was inter-
preted as a means of attracting a new mate. In an even more
obvious effort at mate attraction, widowed dusky antbirds
(Cercomacra tyrannina) begin singing a unique song
within minutes of their mate’s disappearance (Morton and
Derrickson 1996). Such long distance mate attraction ef-
forts do not exist in white-bellied antbirds (Fedy and
Stutchbury 2004). Temporary removals of white-bellied
antbirds showed no evidence of long-distance song ad-
vertisement in widowed males or females (Fedy and
Stutchbury 2004). Removed birds were not replaced
rapidly, with replacements occurring after 3 days in sev-
eral cases. Therefore, white-bellied antbirds do not face
the same intense/short term threat of replacement which
exists in other species. Furthermore, this suggests that in
white-bellied antbirds retaining a familiar mate is ben-
eficial as they do not advertise or switch readily. For
white-bellied antbirds, sex roles in territory defence may
420
therefore be tightly linked to the traditional notion of pair
bonds. When both sexes readily switch mates and break
long-held bonds, territory defence is focussed primarily on
same sex individuals (Levin 1996b; Morton and Derrickson
1996). When mate switching is less common, as in white-
bellied antbirds, territory defence is equal against all intrud-
ers regardless of sex. This implies the pair is co-operating in
territory defence, in the sense that each individual benefits
by preventing intruders from displacing either themselves
or their mate.
White-bellied antbirds are ground foraging birds which
likely leads to high predation risk for adults. Therefore,
mates may be important for predator vigilance as well as
for breeding partners. In a species where life is dangerous,
familiar mates may become an important resource for sur-
vival. Morton and Stutchbury (2000) found that adult dusky
antbirds, which forage in dense vine tangles, had a survival
rate of 82%. In our study of white-bellied antbird only
55% of individuals banded in one year survived to the next
(Fedy and Stutchbury, unpublished data). Furthermore, us-
ing radio telemetry we have found that pair members spend
over 60% of their time within 5 m of each other (Fedy and
Stutchbury, unpublished data). This reliance on mates for
vigilance could partially explain why white-bellied antbirds
do not readily switch or advertise for new mates. Each in-
dividual within a pair receives benefits from their mates
in the form of the non-depreciable ‘resource’ of predator
vigilance which would moderate the conflict between the
sexes and allow them to act in a more co-ordinated man-
ner in territory defence. Therefore, predation risk could be
playing a significant role in the expression of sex roles in
white-bellied antbirds.
The higher levels of aggression shown by males may be
the result of a greater threat to their territory position than
to females. If males are more likely to be ousted from a
territory, males should respond more strongly to male in-
truders than to female intruders; however, males responded
equally to all stimuli. Males would face a greater competi-
tion for their territorial position if there were a male-biased
sex ratio. However, our data from removal experiments
suggests this is not the case. We confirmed the presence
of similar numbers of male and female floaters, and male
and female vacancies were equally likely to get filled in
response to natural and artificially created vacancies (Fedy
and Stutchbury 2004).
Breeding activities may also influence sex roles and ag-
gression. Similar male and female mating effort could pro-
duce similar levels of aggression by the sexes in both
seasons. We observed that males were more aggressive
than females during the breeding season. Although males
and females likely contribute equally to nest building and
care of the young, females still need to invest energy
(Walsberg 1983) and/or dietary protein (Ramsay and Hous-
ton 1998) into egg production. This may partially influence
the unequal sex roles observed during the breeding period.
However, this does not explain the differences in aggression
levels between the sexes observed during the non-breeding
season and is not likely to be a significant factor influencing
the disparity in aggression between the sexes.
The ultimate mechanism influencing the difference in
aggression levels between males and females is not imme-
diately obvious. However, sexual selection in white-bellied
antbirds is likely stronger on males than females, which
may explain the sex role differences in territory defence.
Males are bigger than females and the species is sexually di-
morphic. Furthermore, males sing more than females and
are consistently more aggressive to territorial intrusions.
Despite the similar threat of replacement between the sexes,
competition among males may be high due to control of
high quality territories and/or mates. If males are primar-
ily responsible for territory defence then female choice of
mates may in turn depend on the male’s aggression levels
and ability to maintain a high quality territory throughout
the entire year.
Duets
Duets represent a more aggressive form of territory defence
than solo songs in some species (Hall 2000). Thus, we pre-
dicted that males and females would respond more strongly
to duet playbacks than to single sex playbacks. This was
not the case as individuals responded with equal levels of
aggression to single sex and duet playbacks. We also pre-
dicted an increase in the proportion of duets in response
to the playbacks than observed in natural vocalizations.
However, the intense threat posed by the playback experi-
ments did not result in a greater proportion of duets. Female
white-bellied antbirds create duets by beginning to sing af-
ter the male song begins, but before it ends. Consequently,
if birds were more aggressive to intruders of the same sex
(Levin 1996b; Morton and Derrickson 1996), more duets
should ensue in response to female only playbacks than
to male only playbacks. Because white-bellied antbirds do
not differentiate their responses between same and oppo-
site sex playbacks, it is not surprising, in retrospect, that
they did not differ in the proportion of duets sung in re-
sponse to female only and male only playbacks. Duets may
also function as a form of mate guarding (Hall 2004). If
this were the case in white-bellied antbirds we would have
observed an increase in duets in response to female only
playbacks. No such increase was observed and thus we can
conclude that duetting in white-bellied antbirds likely does
not function as a form of mate guarding.
Farabaugh (1982) explored the idea that individuals may
join in duets to avoid the possibility of mis-directed ag-
gression from their mates. This could be the case in white-
bellied antbirds as males respond with equal levels of ag-
gression to male and female songs. It would therefore be
beneficial for the female to let her mate know where she
is to avoid mis-directed aggression from her mate. This
would also allow the male to better focus his efforts on
the intruding birds. The use of interactive playback ex-
periments (e.g. Mennill et al. 2002) could be an excellent
tool for investigating this possible function of duetting.
Temporary removal of a female would allow investigators
to playback her song to her mate with two treatments:
(1) a trial in which the playback does not duet with the
421
resident male, and (2) an alternate experiment in which the
playback results in duetting with the male. The avoidance
of mis-directed aggression hypothesis would predict lower
aggression in the male when the female playback overlaps
with his songs to form duets.
Acknowledgements Earlier versions of the manuscript were im-
proved by valuable comments from Eugene Morton, Laurene Rat-
cliffe and two anonymous reviewers. We thank the Smithsonian Trop-
ical Research Institute for logistical support in Panama. Rachel Houli-
han provided field assistance and Michael Friendly assisted with
statistical analysis. We also thank Stan and Pat Rand for providing
transportation throughout much of the project. B.C.F. was supported
by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC)
Scholarship and York University Graduate Studies Scholarships. Re-
search was supported by York University Graduate Studies and Sigma
Xi grants to B.C.F., NSERC grant to B.J.M.S., and a Smithsonian
Scholarly Studies Grant to E.S. Morton. Research was conducted
under permits issued by the Autoridad Nacional del Ambiente
References
Amundsen T (2000) Why are female birds ornamented? Trends Ecol
Evol 15:149–155
Appleby BM, Yamaguchi N, Johnson PJ, MacDonald DW (1999)
Sex-specific territorial responses in tawny owls Strix aluco.Ibis
141:91–99
Balthazart J (1983) Hormonal correlates of behavior. In: Farner DS,
King JR, Parkes KC (eds) Avian Biology, vol 7. Academic
Press, New York, pp 221–365
Bard SC, Hau M, Wikelski M, Wingfield JC (2002) Vocal distinc-
tiveness and response to conspecific playback in the spotted
antbird, a neotropical suboscine. Condor 104:387–394
Brotherton PNM, Rhodes A (1996) Monogamy without biparental
care in a dwarf antelope. Proc R Soc Lond B 263:23–29
Brown JL (1964) The evolution of diversity in avian territorial
systems. Wilson Bull 76:160–169
Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (1995) Bird song. Cambridge University
Press, UK
Clutton-Brock TH, Vincent ACJ (1991) Sexual selection and the
potential reproductive rates of males and females. Nature
351:58–60
de Schauensee RM (1966) The species of birds of South America and
their distribution. Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia,
Philadelphia
Dubois F, Giraldeau L-A (2004) Reduced resource defence in an
uncertain world: an experimental test using captive nutmeg
manikins. Anim Behav 68:21–25
Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology, sexual selection, and the
evolution of mating systems. Science 197:215–223
Falls JB (1981) Mapping territories with playback: an accurate
census method for songbirds. Stud Avian Biol 6:86–91
Farabaugh SM (1982) The ecological and social significance of
duetting. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic commu-
nication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 85–124
Fedy BC, Stutchbury BJM (2004) Territory switching and floating in
white-bellied antbird (Myrmeciza longipes) a resident tropical
passerine in Panama. Auk 121:486–496
Freed LA (1987) The long-term pair bond of tropical house wrens:
advantage or constraint? Am Nat 130:507–525
Gill FB (1990) Ornithology. Freeman, New York.
Gill FB, Wolf LL (1975) Economics of feeding territoriality in the
golden-winged sunbird. Ecology 56:333–345
Grafe TU, Bitz JH (2004) Functions of duetting in the tropical
boubou, Laniarius aethiopicus: territorial defence and mutual
mate guarding. Anim Behav 68:193–201
Greenberg R, Gradwohl J (1983) Sexual roles in the dot-winged
antwren (Microrphopias quixensis), a tropical forest passerine.
Auk 100:919–925
Hall ML (2000) The function of duetting in magpie-larks: conflict,
cooperation or commitment? Anim Behav 60:667–677
Hall ML (2004) A review of hypotheses for the functions of avian
duetting. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:415–430
Hau M, Wikelski M, Soma KK, Wingfield JC (2000) Testosterone
and year-round territorial aggression in a tropical bird. Gen
Comp Endocr 117:20–33
James FC, McCullough CE (1990) Multivariate analysis in ecology
and systematics: panacea or Pandora’s box? Annu Rev Ecol
Syst 21:126–166
Karr JR (1971) Structure of avian communities in selected Panama
and Illinois habitats. Ecol Monogr 41:207–233
Ketterson ED, Nolan V Jr, Wolf L, Ziegenfus C (1992) Testos-
terone and avian life histories: effects of experimentally
elevated testosterone on behavior and correlates of fitness
in the dark-eyed junco (Junco hyemalis). Am Nat 140:980–
999
Kroodsma DE (1989) Suggested experimental designs for song
playbacks. Anim Behav 37:600–609
Langmore NE (1998) Functions of duet and solo songs of female
birds. Trends Ecol Evol 3:136–140
Langmore NE (2002) Vocal duetting: definitions, discoveries and
directions. Trends Ecol Evol 17:451–452
Levin RN (1996a) Song behavior and reproductive strategies
in a duetting wren, Thryothorus nigricapillus: I. Removal
experiments. Anim Behav 52:1093–1106
Levin RN (1996b) Song behavior and reproductive strategies
in a duetting wren, Thryothorus nigricapillus: II. Playback
experiments. Anim Behav 52:1107–1117
Levings SC, Windsor DM (1982) Seasonal and annual variation in
litter arthropod populations. In: Leigh EG, Rand AS, Windsor
DM (eds) The ecology of a tropical forest: seasonal rhythms and
long-term changes. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington
DC, pp 355–387
McElroy DB, Ritchison G (1996) Effect of mate removal on singing
behavior and movement patterns of female northern cardinals.
Wilson Bull 108:550–555
McGregor PK (1992) Quantifying responses to playback: one,
many, or composite multivariate measures? In: McGregor PK
(ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum,
London, pp. 79–96
Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM, Boag PT (2002) Female eavesdropping
on male song contests in songbirds. Science 296:873
Morton ES, Derrickson KC (1996) Song ranging by the dusky
antbird, Cercomacra tyrannina: ranging without song learning.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39:195–201
Morton ES, Stutchbury BJM (2000) Demography and reproductive
success in the dusky antbird, a sedentary tropical passerine. J
Field Ornith 71:493–500
Morton ES, Derrickson KC, Stutchbury BJM (2000) Territory
switching behavior in a sedentary tropical passerine, the
dusky antbird Cercomacra tyrannina. Behav Ecol 11:648–
653
Owens IPF, Thompson DBA (1994) Sex differences, sex ratios and
sex roles. Proc R Soc Lond B 258:93–99
Ramsay SL, Houston DC (1998) The effect of dietary amino acid
composition on egg production in blue tits. Proc R Soc Lond B
265:1401–1405
Roberts SC, Dunbar RIM (2000) Female territoriality and the func-
tion of scent-marking in a monogamous antelope (Oreotragus
oreotragus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 47:417–423
Robinson TR, Robinson WD, Edwards EC (2000) Breeding ecology
and nest-site selection of song wrens in central Panama. Auk
117:345–354
Stutchbury BJM, Morton ES (2001) Behavioral ecology of tropical
birds. Academic Press, San Diego
Walsberg GE (1983) Avian ecological energetics. In: Rarner DS,
King JR, Parkes KC (eds) Avian biology vol. 7. Academic
Press, London, pp 161–220
Wetmore A (1972) The birds of the Republic of Panama part 3 –
Passeriformes: Dendrocolaptidae (Woodcreepers) to Oxyrunci-
dae (Sharpbills). Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC
422
Wickler W (1976) Duetting songs in birds: biological significance
of stationary and non-stationary processes. J Theor Biol
61:493–497
Wickler W, Seibt U (1980) Vocal duetting and the pair bond II.
Unisono duetting in the African forest weaver, Symplectes
bicolour. Z Tierpsychol 59, 217–226.
Wikelski M, Hau M, Robinson WD, Wingfield JC (1999) Seasonal
endocrinology of tropical passerines – a comparative approach.
In: Adams NJ, Slotow RH (ed) Proceedings of the 22nd
International Ornithology Congress, Durban, Johannesberg:
Birdlife South Africa, pp 1224–1241
Willis EO (1973) The behavior of oscellated antbirds. Smithson
Contrib Zool 144:1–57
Windsor DM (1990) Climate and moisture variability in a tropical
forest: Long-term records from Barro Colorado Island, Panama.
Smithsonian Contributions to Earth Sciences No. 29
Wingfield JC (1994) Regulation of territorial behavior in the
sedentary song sparrow, Melospiza melodia morphna.Horm
Behav 28:1–15
Wingfield JC, Hegner RE, Dufty AM Jr, Ball GF (1990) The
“challenge hypothesis”: theoretical implications for patterns of
testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies.
Am Nat 136:829–846
... It has long been suggested that female birds primarily engage in mate selection, while males compete for mates and territory defence (Darwin, 1859). However, evidence suggests that females also actively participate in territory defence (Fedy & Stutchbury, 2005;Greenberg & Gradwohl, 1983;Quinard & C ezilly, 2012). In our study, we observed aggressive behaviours from females in response to various combinations of conspecific and heterospecific songs and taxidermic mounts. ...
... This behaviour is common among tropical passerines, where both sexes often engage in year-round territory defence (Stutchbury & Morton, 2022). Fedy and Stutchbury (2005) proposed that long-term monogamy and pair bonding may underlie territorial defence by both sexes, regardless of the intruder's sex. While we did not conduct detailed studies on these aspects, our observations of individuals, both male and female, captured in 2022 and remaining in the same territory until 2023, as confirmed by their colour bands, may lend support for this Figure 4. Aggressive behaviours towards the taxidermic mount (PC2) of female ( ) and male ( ) wrens to various combinations of conspecific (Con) and heterospecific (Hetero) song and mount stimuli. ...
... Distinct sex-specific roles may also emerge, such as in territory and nest defence, where males are typically more aggressive and vocal than females. however, engaging in such behavior may come at the cost of increased mortality among adult males (Clutton-Brock, 1991;Fedy & Stutchbury, 2005;Brumm & Goymann, 2018;Diniz et al., 2020). In addition, parental investment theory suggests that the aggression in nest defence appears to intensify as offspring development progresses, possibly as nestling value increases (Trivers, 1972;Andersson et al., 1980;Montgomerie & Weatherhead, 1988;Wetzel & Westneat, 2014;Dadwal & Bhatt, 2017). ...
... Our findings also reveal that male Mato Grosso Antbirds invest more in nest defence than females. It is well-established for many species that males take on a primary role in defence, engaging in more extended vocalisations, possessing a broader vocal repertoire and exhibiting greater aggression (Fedy & Stutchbury, 2005;hall et al., 2015;Brumm & Goymann, 2018;Diniz et al., 2020). ...
Article
Parental care enhances reproductive success. Yet these advantages come with associated costs for the parents, including those affecting their own survival. Environmental conditions and species life history can influence which parental care strategy emerges. In birds, parental care is prevalent and often biparental, in which both males and females contribute to the same activities, although the extent varies between species. This study aimed to compare parental investment between male and female Mato Grosso Antbirds Cercomacra melanaria in the Pantanal wetlands, Brazil. Analysed behaviours included carrying material and nest shaping during nest-building; total incubation duration and incubation related to time of day (morning and afternoon) during the incubation period; and brooding, feeding offspring and faecal sac removal during the nestling stage. Nest defence events were recorded for all three periods, investigating whether this behaviour intensified with nestlings’ development. Whereas no differences between males and females were found in most analysed behaviours, males were observed to incubate more throughout day-time, specifically during the morning. This heightened provision by males may serve as compensation for the substantial costs associated with egg production and/or as a response to the elevated predation risk that females incur during nocturnal incubation. Males defended the nest more than females, but the outcomes do not suggest that Mato Grosso Antbird parents tend to show increased aggressiveness when the value of their offspring is higher, which diverges from parental investment theory. Further studies incorporating manipulative methods, such as vocalisation playbacks, are necessary to establish whether this similarity in behaviour between developmental periods holds true. If so, it raises questions regarding what motivates the pair to defend the nesting stages to a similar extent.
... As relações de custo e benefício desempenham um papel importante na regulação dos comportamentos de territorialidade, favorecendo variantes comportamentais que apresentam um equilíbrio favorável entre benefício e custo, acarretando alterações demográficas, regulação populacional e mudanças na ecologia espacial (Adams, 2001;Carpenter, 1987). Nas aves, uma variedade de comportamentos relacionados à defesa de recursos é observada, como a produção de vocalizações territoriais e a exibição de comportamentos ameaçadores, como sinais de advertência, perseguição e até mesmo ataques físicos (Fedy;Stutchbury, 2005 (Tobias, 2011;Kattan;Beltran, 2002;Kajiki, 2023). Essa estratégia de uso de áreas territoriais e não territoriais pode ser vantajosa para algumas espécies de aves, especialmente aquelas que vivem em habitats onde os recursos são dispersos ou reduzidos (Estades, 2001 (Adams, 2001;Khoury, 2012). ...
... Por exemplo, um estudo realizado com aves do gênero Fringilla, pertencentes à ordem Passeriformes, observou que as fêmeas defendem o território durante todo o ano(Cooper;Garcia-Del-Rey; Lachlan, 2023). Por outro lado, um estudo com o tiranídeo Myrmeciza longipes demonstrou que, apesar da defesa conjunta do território ocorrer ao longo de todo o ano, a escassez sazonal de recursos favorece uma maior agressividade por parte de ambos os sexos, especialmente quando ocorre a invasão do território(Fedy; Stutchbury, 2005). Outros estudos indicam também que fêmeas apresentam variação sazonal na intensidade da defesa do território, ...
Article
Full-text available
A defesa territorial é uma estratégia adotada em muitas espécies de Passeriformes para proteger recursos vitais para a sobrevivência e reprodução contra membros da mesma espécie. O tamanho do território defendido varia de acordo com a espécie, podendo ter um tamanho similar ou menor do que a área de vida. Em algumas espécies, tanto os machos quanto as fêmeas atuam na defesa ativa do território. O choca-de-asa-vermelha (Thamnophilus torquatus), uma ave da Subordem Tyranni encontrada em extensas áreas da América do Sul, se caracteriza como um excelente modelo de estudo para compreender a territorialidade por apresentar uma boa responsividade a playbacks, com vocalizações específicas para a defesa territorial e, dicromatismo sexual que permite a fácil diferenciação entre machos e fêmeas. O objetivo do estudo foi caracterizar o papel de machos e fêmeas da choca-de-asa-vermelha (Thamnophilus torquatus) na defesa territorial, analisar a diferença de tamanho da área defendida por cada sexo e investigar possíveis diferenças de tamanho entre território e área de vida. A pesquisa foi realizada na Estação Ecológica de Águas Emendadas (ESECAE) em Brasília/DF. O estudo foi realizado entre 2022 e 2023. Os indivíduos foram capturados, marcados e monitorados durante todo o período. Foram registradas as coordenadas que representam pontos da área de vida e do território dos indivíduos monitorados. Os resultados sugerem que existem diferenças significativas entre a área de vida e o território, observando-se que a área de vida é estatisticamente maior em comparação com o território. Ao realizar a comparação territorial entre machos e fêmeas, os resultados obtidos indicam que ambos os sexos defendem a área de forma igualitária. Também foi possível observar que o tamanho do território defendido pelos machos é maior do que o defendido pelas fêmeas.
... However, several factors mitigate this concern. While territorial aggression in birds can vary considerably across the year in the Northern Hemisphere due to climatedriven resource shifts (Landys et al. 2007, Merritt et al. 2018), year-round territorial behavior is more prevalent among tropical birds (Hau et al. 2000, Fedy andStutchbury 2005). This distinction is crucial for understanding avian behavior across different ecozones. ...
Article
Full-text available
Interspecific territoriality is a prevalent form of interference competition among animals across environments. However, the connections between interspecific territorial aggression and other related aspects such as hybridization and climate remain unexplored. We investigated territorial aggression in two Neotropical wren species, Campylorhynchus zonatus brevirostris and C. fasciatus pallescens, along a precipitation gradient in western Ecuador using playback experiments. Campylorhynchus f. pallescens exhibits geographic variation: northern populations (C. f. pallescens north) are hybrids of C. z. brevirostris and C. f. fasciatus, while southern populations (C. f. pallescens south) show primary genetic admixture with C. f. fasciatus (from northeastern Peru). We pursued three objectives: 1) to compare intra‐ and inter‐territorial aggression of C. z. brevirostris and the admixed C. f. pallescens north and south; 2) to assess territorial aggression across these three genetic clusters; and 3) to examine direct and indirect (via primary productivity) associations between climate and territorial aggression. We simulated territory intrusion using playback experiments and quantified aggressive responses using principal component analysis (PCA) to integrate three behavioral measurements: minimum approach distance to the stimulus, latency to reach this distance, and the number of aggressive displays (fixed action patterns). Admixed C. f. pallescens north displayed no significant differences in aggression across treatments, supporting the established trend of high interspecific territoriality in hybrids. Campylorhynchus f. pallescens south was the only genetic cluster that showed significant differences among treatments, exhibiting more pronounced aggressive responses to intraspecific stimuli and higher aggression than C. z. brevirostris and C. f. pallescens north. This pattern of dominance in territorial aggression contrasts with the previously reported direction of genetic introgression from C. z. brevirostris towards C. f. pallescens. Precipitation is related to aggression, potentially through resource availability. We emphasize the importance of understanding the interactions among hybridization, territoriality, and environmental stressors in tropical birds.
... Most research has focused on territorial behaviour of males during the breeding season in temperate zones and northern latitudes (Barnett et al., 2014;Billerman & Carling, 2016;Byers, 2017). Studies in year-round territorial aggression (both during and outside the breeding season in both sexes) have been conducted mostly in tropical birds (Greenberg & Gradwohl, 1983;Stutchbury & Morton, 2001;Fedy & Stutchbury, 2005;Dowling & Webster, 2016;Odom et al., 2017), but also in the temperate zones of the southern hemisphere (Fraga, 1980;Ippi et al., 2017;Mentesana et al., 2020). Interestingly, a growing body of evidence indicates that some species exhibit year-round territoriality in northern latitudes (Wingfield, 1994;Goymann & Landys, 2011;Landys et al., 2013;Randler & Randler, 2020). ...
Article
Many bird species defend their territories against conspecifics during the breeding season. Although some temperate species have been observed to defend territories outside of the breeding season, such cases are rarely noted in the literature, and it remains largely unknown which environmental factors influence such behaviour. In this study, we investigated non-breeding territorial behaviour in the Eurasian nuthatch (Sitta europaea). Territorial responses were elicited with a simulated territory intrusion (playbacks of calls combined with a wooden model of nuthatch) in autumn, winter and spring. Territorial responses did not differ between autumn and winter but differed between the autumn/winter and spring; they declined until about mid-December (winter) and increased after that into the spring. We found an increase in territorial responses with increasing photoperiod (day length) in autumn and with decreasing ambient temperature during spring. Our study provides novel evidence about non-breeding territorial behaviour in temperate birds and indicates that this behaviour is influenced by environmental factors.
Article
Preferences for specific habitat attributes during territory establishment are primarily shaped by natural selection. The active defense of these territories, despite the high associated costs, promotes territoriality due to the assumed benefits of enhanced survival and reproduction. This dynamic is particularly pronounced in Tropical species that maintain territories year-round. In this study, we examine territory defense and habitat selection in the rufous-winged antshrike (Thamnophilus torquatus), a Neotropical passerine with limited ecological and behavioral information available. We monitored banded individuals to estimate territory sizes and used vegetation type and canopy height databases to investigate habitat preferences for territory settlement. Our results show that rufous-winged antshrikes defend small territories year-round, typically in pairs, with males being more active in defense. Compared to other members of the Thamnophilidae family, the territories of this species are generally smaller. We found that rufous-winged antshrikes prefer more open habitats with lower canopy height; however, vegetation characteristics were not significantly related to territory size. The preference for open habitats may be associated with greater availability and accessibility of invertebrate prey, or it may reflect interspecific competition and niche differentiation. Future research should explore the sexual roles in territory defense and the fitness benefits of territoriality and habitat selection.
Article
Full-text available
Research on avian bioacoustics in the Neotropics has surged over the last several decades due to increased interest in the large diversity of vocal behaviors and vocalization and the broader accessibility of recording equipment and software. Here, we present a synthesis of the current and past knowledge of Neotropical bird bioacoustics. This synthesis is the result of the symposium "Bioacoustics in the Neotropics", organized for the XI Neotropical Ornithological Congress in San Jose, Costa Rica, in July 2019. We covered what we consider the main topics in avian bioacoustics that have been studied in this region over the last 30 years. Our review includes repertoire descriptions, geographic variation, diversity in vocal behaviors, seasonality, duetting, genetic association, and playback experiments. Additionally, we present information for what we believe may be the main veins of investigation for the coming future in the Neotropics, considering the large diversity of species that are found in the region and the new investigations developed in other geographic areas. We expect this review to work as a summary of the current literature and a guide to stimulate future research in important areas within the field of avian bioacoustics in the Neotropics.
Article
Birds defend territories by engaging in potentially costly behaviours, such as vocalizations and aggressive interactions. Songs are usually good proxies of individual quality, thus could help guarantee access and exclusivity to food, sexual partners and nesting sites. We described territorial and vocal behaviours of the Riverbank Warbler Myiothlypis rivularis, a year-round territorial wood-warbler (family Parulidae), and tested for relationships between territory size and individuals' vocal attributes. From October 2018 to April 2019, we banded 14 males and 9 females in the Atlantic Forest of south Brazil. We estimated territory sizes of nine pairs and recorded 10 and 5.4 h of 9 males and 6 females vocalizations, respectively. In male songs, a tradeoff between trill rate and frequency bandwidth indicates mechanical vocal constraints in the production of both parameters. Territory size did not relate to male song parameters, which could signal individual, rather than territorial, quality. However, females with broader bandwidth songs occupied smaller territories. Although the reduced sample size, which precludes generalization, this result may be a first indication of female song acting on territoriality in this species. Overall, this work presents new data on Riverbank Warbler and contributes to the knowledge on vocal tradeoffs in male songs and both male and female territorial behavior for wood-warblers. In addition, we highlight the need for further studies focused on female song, which like male song could be associated with territory defense.
Article
Full-text available
Experimental approaches to measure territory defence can be used to interpret the relative magnitude of intraspecific aggression in different ecological contexts. The aim of this study is to examine the response of Fijian forest bird species to conspecific intruders using playback of previously recorded songs along an elevational gradient. We address two questions: Do different foraging guilds in Fiji’s forest birds show different patterns of territorial defence behaviour towards conspecific intruders? Secondly, do Fiji’s forest birds show stronger territorial defence at higher elevations? We explore these questions across two foraging guilds (insectivore and omnivore) on Viti Levu Island, Fiji. All four focal species had a stronger response at the highest elevation, and the endemic insectivore had the strongest response. Perhaps high elevation insects are less abundant and hence increased aggressiveness is favoured to defend the territory, and/or perhaps in general more aggressive birds colonise higher elevation areas, both ideas that require further testing. This study provides first insights into response to conspecifics in Fiji’s forest birds that can be explored in relation to avian behaviour also in the context of human activity, human disturbance and threats to the persistence of birds across elevational gradients.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the yearlong territorial behavior of White-bellied Antbirds (Myrmeciza longipes) in Panama by conducting 17 experimental removals during the nonbreeding season on both sexes. We also monitored the territorial behavior and occupancy of 48 males and 34 females throughout the nonbreeding and breeding seasons. We tested the importance of territory switching, mate advertisement, foray behavior, and role of floaters. In seven of the removal experiments, both members of a pair were radiotagged and tracked throughout the duration of the experiments. It was predicted that widowed birds would attempt to attract a new mate through increased song rate or unique vocalizations; however, that behavior was not observed in White-bellied Antbirds. We documented a weak response in floaters to territorial vacancies and found that territory switching occurs regularly in response to experimental removals and naturally within populations. We found density of birds in an area influences the probability of replacement and is likely a reflection of territory quality given that birds in high-density areas were in better physical condition, spent less time off-territory, and competed more for those territories. Telemetry revealed that individuals made silent extraterritorial forays during the nonbreeding season. Birds could use those forays to assess the quality and status of neighboring territories and to find food during this period when food abundance is low. Several birds were observed to temporarily abandon territorial behavior for periods from 2 to 13 months, a previously undocumented behavior in this type of territorial system.
Article
Dot-winged Antwrens maintain long-term pair bonds on year-long territories in tropical forests and woodland of Central and South America. Pairs partition breeding-related activities nearly equally. Like other antbirds (Formicariidae), both sexes incubate and brood. Both sexes participate equally in territorial maintenance behavior, which primarily involves active border displays. Initial investment in offspring is possibly equalized by long-term nuptial feeding of females by males. Overall, both sexes contribute equally to nestling care, although considerable variation is observed between nests, much of which probably results from variation in the foraging ability of parents. Females feed young consistently less often than do males during the early part of the nestling period and may be recovering from depletion of reserves resulting from egg production. In most pairs females bring more spiders than do males. This difference in prey type is unexpected, because the pair forages close together in the same microhabitat and their bill size is similar. Pairs are distinct in their interactions: some pairs have a rigid feeding order, and others do not.
Article
We describe nest-site selection and report measures of breeding productivity from a population of Song Wrens (Cyphorhinus phaeocephalus) in central Panama. We studied 31 pairs from 1995 to 1998 and collected data on nest predation from 1996 to 1998. Song Wrens are year-round residents that build breeding nests for reproduction and dormitory nests for roosting. Twenty-five of 50 Song Wren breeding nests produced fledglings (daily nest survival rate = 0.968). Song Wrens usually (78%) placed their breeding nests in ant-defended acacias (Acacia melanoceras). However, nests placed in acacias did not experience lower rates of nest predation than nests placed in sites not defended by ants. The population-wide breeding season was seven months long, but individual pairs often bred within a span of only 2.5 months. Modal clutch size was two (n = 42), but the frequency of three-egg clutches was high (≥27%). Pairs that lost eggs or chicks renested more quickly than pairs that fledged young and attempted a second brood. Observed annual production of young was 1.4 fledglings per pair. Compared with other wrens, Song Wrens had a much longer breeding season and longer intervals between broods. The annual productivity of Song Wrens was the lowest recorded for any wren, which may have been due to smaller clutch sizes, long intervals between broods, and the correspondingly low number of nesting attempts per pair per year.
Chapter
Acoustic communication is one of the better studied areas of animal behaviour (Halliday and Slater 1973). Part of the reason for the large number of studies in this area is the relative ease with which sounds can be recorded and broadcast. This has permitted experiments in the laboratory and in the field in which sound is played to animals and their responses noted. Such playback experiments have been conducted on a large number of species, notably songbirds and anuran amphibians (see chapters by Falls and by Gerhardt respectively in this volume), but including a wide diversity of taxonomic groups; from whales (e.g. Clark and Clark 1980) to wolves (e.g. Harrington 1989), from reef fish (e.g. Myrberg and Riggio 1985) to rodents (e.g. Cherry 1989), and from deer (e.g. Clutton-Brock and Albon 1979, McComb chapter in this volume) to Drosophila (e.g. Bennet-Clark and Ewing 1969). As argued by Catchpole (this volume), playback is now regarded as an important part of any integrated investigation of a communication system.
Article
We investigated the yearlong territorial behavior of White-bellied Antbirds (Myrmeciza longipes) in Panama by conducting 17 experimental removals during the nonbreeding season on both sexes. We also monitored the territorial behavior and occupancy of 48 males and 34 females throughout the nonbreeding and breeding seasons. We tested the importance of territory switching, mate advertisement, foray behavior, and role of floaters. In seven of the removal experiments, both members of a pair were radiotagged and tracked throughout the duration of the experiments. It was predicted that widowed birds would attempt to attract a new mate through increased song rate or unique vocalizations; however, that behavior was not observed in White-bellied Antbirds. We documented a weak response in floaters to territorial vacancies and found that territory switching occurs regularly in response to experimental removals and naturally within populations. We found density of birds in an area influences the probability of replacement and is likely a reflection of territory quality given that birds in high-density areas were in better physical condition, spent less time off-territory, and competed more for those territories. Telemetry revealed that individuals made silent extraterritorial forays during the nonbreeding season. Birds could use those forays to assess the quality and status of neighboring territories and to find food during this period when food abundance is low. Several birds were observed to temporarily abandon territorial behavior for periods from 2 to 13 months, a previously undocumented behavior in this type of territorial system.
Article
Demographic data are presented from an 8-yr study of a marked population of the Dusky Antbird (Cercomacra tyrannina), a sedentary neotropical passerine with year-long territories and pairbonds. Dusky Antbirds had an annual survival rate of 82%, and the survivorship curve was linear. Sexes did not differ in survival. The probability of reproducing successfully, based on the number of pairs accompanied by independent young the following dry season, was only 8% and no territory produced more than one successful nest, if any. In one year (1997) reproduction was significantly better (25%) than the other years, perhaps associated with lowered predation during an extremely dry wet season. We suggest that for Dusky Antbirds, a species with no extra-pair mating behavior, a long adult lifespan appears to be the primary means to increase lifetime reproductive success.
Article
PRONOUNCED sex differences in mating competition are a prominent feature of many animal breeding systems. These differences are widely attributed to sex differences in parental investment 1,2 which bias the ratio of sexually receptive females to males 3 (the operational sex ratio), generating more intense competition between members of one sex, usually males 3-5. Unfortunately, relative parental investment 1 is usually impossible to measure in species where both sexes invest in their offspring 6,7 and there is currently no empirical basis for predicting the pattern of mating competition in these species. In contrast, the potential rate of reproduction by males and females (measured as the maximum number of independent offspring that parents can produce per unit time) is both more directly related to the operational sex ratio and more easily estimated in natural populations 7. Here we show that among species where males care for the young, the sex with the higher potential reproductive rate competes more intensely for mates than the sex with the lower potential rate of reproduction.