ChapterPDF Available

States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping: Perspectives from Research on Meditation Experiences

Authors:

Abstract

Three categories of meditation practices have been proposed: focused attention meditations, which involve voluntary and sustained attention on a chosen object; open monitoring meditations, which involve non-reactive monitoring of moment-to-moment content of experience; and automatic self-transcending meditations, which are designed to transcend their own activity. While focused attention and open monitoring meditations explore the nature of individual cognitive, affective, and perceptual processes and experiences, automatic self-transcending meditations explore the state when conscious processing and experiences are transcended, a state called pure consciousness. This paper reports unique phenomenological and physiological patterns during the state of pure consciousness, as experienced during Transcendental Meditation (TM) practice, a meditation in the automatic self-transcending category. These data support the description of pure consciousness as a fourth state of consciousness with unique phenomenological and physiological correlates. This paper also discusses the Junction Point Model that integrates meditation experiences with the three ordinary states of waking, sleeping, and dreaming. The Junction Point Model is supported by EEG data and provides a structure to integrate ordinary experience during waking, sleeping, and dreaming with meditation experiences and so can serve as a foundation for investigating the full range of human consciousness.
Dean Cvetkovic
l
Irena Cosic
Editors
States of Consciousness
Experimental Insights into Meditation,
Waking, Sleep and Dreams
Editors
Dean Cvetkovic
RMIT University
School of Electrical
and Computer Engineering
PO Box 2476V
3001 Melbourne Victoria
Australia
dean.cvetkovic@rmit.edu.au
Irena Cosic
RMIT University
College of Science, Engineering
and Health
PO Box 2476V
3001 Melbourne Victoria
Australia
irena.cosic@rmit.edu.au
Series Editors:
Avshalom C. Elitzur
Bar-Ilan University, Unit of Interdisciplinary Studies, 52900 Ramat-Gan, Israel
email: avshalom.elitzur@weizmann.ac.il
Laura Mersini-Houghton
Dept. Physics, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3255, USA
email: mersini@physics.unc.edu
Maximilian A. Schlosshauer
Niels Bohr Institute, Blegdamsvej 17, 2100 Copenhagen, Denmark
email: schlosshauer@nbi.dk
Mark P. Silverman
Trinity College, Dept. Physics, Hartford CT 06106, USA
email: mark.silverman@trincoll.edu
Ru
¨
diger Vaas
University of Giessen, Center for Philosophy and Foundations of Science, 35394 Giessen,
Germany
email: ruediger.vaas@t-online.deH.
Dieter Zeh
Gaiberger Straße 38, 69151 Waldhilsbach, Germany
email: zeh@uni-heidelberg.de
ISSN 1612-3018
ISBN 978-3-642-18046-0 e-ISBN 978-3-642-18047-7
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-18047-7
Springer Heidelberg Dordrecht London New York
Library of Congress Control Number: 2011931470
# Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is
concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting,
reproduction on microfilm or in any other way, and storage in data banks. Duplication of this publication
or parts thereof is permitted only under the provisions of the German Copyright Law of September 9,
1965, in its current version, and permission for use must always be obtained from Springer. Violations
are liable to prosecution under the German Copyright Law.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not imply,
even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective
laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
Cover design: eStudio Calamar S.L.
Printed on acid-free paper
Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)
Chapter 10
States of Consciousness Beyond Waking,
Dreaming and Sleeping: Perspectives from
Research on Meditation Experiences
Frederick Travis
Abstract Three categories of meditation practices have been proposed: focused
attention meditations, which involve voluntary and sustained attention on a chosen
object; open monitoring meditations, which involve non-reactive monitoring of
moment-to-moment content of experience; and automatic self-transcending
meditations, which are designed to transcend their own activity. While focused
attention and open monitoring meditations explore the nature of individual
cognitive, affective, and perceptual processes and experiences, automatic sel f-
transcending meditations explore the state when conscious processing and
experiences are transcended, a state called pure consciousness. This paper reports
unique phenomenological and physiological patterns during the state of pure con-
sciousness, as experienced during Transcendental Meditation (TM) practice, a
meditation in the automatic self-transcending category. These data support the
description of pure consciousness as a fourth state of consciousness with unique
phenomenological and physiologica l correlates. This p aper also discusses the Junc-
tion Point Model that integrates meditation experiences with the three ordinary
states of waking, sleeping, and dreaming. Th e Junction Point Model is supported
by EEG data and provides a structure to integrate ordinary experience during
waking, sleeping, and dreaming with meditation experiences and so can serve as a
foundation for investigating the full range of human consciousness.
10.1 Introduction
Traditional meditation tec hniques are part of a subjective approach to gaining
knowledge that parallels and complements the objective approach of gaining
knowledge in the natural sciences. The objective approach in western science has
F. Travis (*)
Center for Brain, Consciousness, and Cognition, Maharishi University of Management, Fairfield,
IA 52557, USA
e-mail: ftravis@mum.edu
D. Cvetkovic and I. Cosic (eds.), States of Consciousness, The Frontiers Collection,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-18047-7_10,
#
Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011
223
used instruments to objectively measure phenomena to identify the principles and
laws that explain material and social interactions. Similarly, the subjective
approaches in eastern traditions have used med itation techniques to directly expe-
rience and so understand the full range of human experience. Some meditation
techniques are designed to explore the range of waking processes and experiences;
others are designed to explore the nature of consciousness at the source of thought
when mental processes and content are transcended. In this time when East meets
West, scientists can objectively evaluate growth of subjectivity through meditation
practice. Thus, meditation techniques can serve as scientific probes to fathom the
range of human experience.
Lutz has divided meditat ion practices into two categories: focused attention
meditations, which involve voluntary and sustained attention on a chosen object,
and open monitoring meditations, which involve non-reactive monitoring of the
moment-to-moment content of experience (Lutz et al. 2008). In focused attention
meditations, attention is focused on a given object and regulative skills are devel-
oped to monitor the movement of attention detecting distraction, disengaging
attention from the source of distraction, and redirecting and refocusing on the object
(Lutz et al. 2008). Open monitoring or mindfulness-based meditations refer to an
alert and open mode of perceiving and monitoring mental content from moment to
moment, including perception, sensation, cognition, and affect (Kabat-Zinn 2003).
These meditation practices involve the non-reactive, dispassionate monitoring of
the content of ongoing experience to become reflectively aware of the nature of
emotional and cognitive processes.
Meditation techniques in these two categories explore the nature of waking
processes and experiences. Waking experiences are characterized by subject/object
duality. A subject, agent or experiencer observes and reflects on affective, cogni-
tive, or sensory objects of perception that are separate from himself or herself I am
here observing the experience out there. Focusing attention on a speci fic object of
experience or maintaining an orientation to monitor changing objects of experience
uses and maintains the subject/object duality. One keeps the attent ion involved with
the procedures of the technique.
A third meditation category has been proposed, automatic self-transcending,
which includes meditation techniques designed to transcend their own activity
(Travis and Shear 2010a). Meditation techniques in this category do not attempt
to control the movement of attention or to monitor ongoing experience; rather they
are designed to transcend their own activity to allow a state of consciousness to
emerge when mental activity and cognitive control has been transcended.
Techniques in this third category necessary must be automatic, because any inten-
tion to control the attention would keep the mind active and not allow mental
activity to settle to silence.
Meditation techniques in the automatic self-transcending category provide
insight into the state of consciousness when thoughts have ceased, revealing a
ground state of human consciousness. This paper explores the nature of this state,
and then presents a model that integrates this state during meditation with those
during waking, dreaming, and sleeping.
224 F. Travis
Note that these three categories are not necessarily mutually exclusive within a
single session or over the course of a life-time of meditation practice. Focused
attention and open monitoring are combined in Zen, Vipassana, and Tibetan
Buddhism meditation traditions (Austin 2006; Gyatso and Jinpa 1995; Lutz et al.
2008). Also, with diligent practice over many years, focused attention meditations
may lead to reduced cognitive control and could result in effortless concentration
(Lutz et al. 2008; Wallace 1999).
A meditation technique within the automatic self-transcending category is the
Transcendental Meditation
®
(TM
®
) technique. During TM practice, one appre -
ciates a mantra at finer levels in which the mantra becomes secondary in experience
and self-awareness becomes primary (Maharishi 1969; Travis and Pearson 2000).
Ultimately, the mantra disappears and the subject-object relation that defines
customary experiences is transcended. The subject, or the experiencer, finds him/
herself awake to his/her own existence called pure consciousness or a ground state
of consciousness (Maharishi 1997). Pure consciousness is pure in the sense that it is
free from the processes and contents of knowing. It is a state of consciousness in
that self-awareness is maintained. Pure consciousness is a non-dual state of aware-
ness the self is both the subject and object of awareness. This would contrast with
the end state of some Buddhist meditations that seek to lose the self in the object,
such as during the practice of loving, kindness, and compassion (Lutz et al. 2008).
While this is also a non-dual state, it is a state of object referral the object alone is
(see Travis and Shear 2010b).
The non-dual state of pure consciousness differs from the duality of conscious
awareness or conscious experience. Conscious experience has a three-part structure
the experiencer, the object of experience, and the process of experience. These three
components exist as separate even at the same time as they are unified in the
conscious experience. In pure consciousness, the three-part structure of experiencer,
object of experience, and process linking the two has been transcended. Now, the
experiencer or subject is the both the subject and object of experience it is described
as a purely self-referral experience.
Table 10.1 presents a schematic of the qualitative shift of inner experience from
sleeping to pure consciousness. This table presents a 2 2 grid with the presence
or absence of affective, cognitive, or perceptual content as one axis, and presence or
absence of sense of self as the other. As presented in this table, the waking state is
characterized by the inner experience of a sense of self, the experiencer or doer and
the experience of outer objects in the mind or in the environment. There is a clear
separation between my inner reality and my outer experience.
Table 10.1 Phenomenological characteristics that differentiate waking, dreaming, sleeping and
pure consciousness
Sense of self is present
Yes No
Inner and/or outer perception is present
Yes Waking Dreaming
No Pure consciousness Sleeping
10 States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping 225
The sleep state is characterized by no sense of self and no awareness of any
content. “Sleeping like a log” is a saying for having a good night’s sleep. During
deep sleep, there is no awareness of self or ongoing cognitive or perceptual
experiences for large blocks of time.
Dreaming is arguably characterized by no sense of self and vivid dream images .
This describes most dream experiences. Lucid dreaming, we argue, is meta-cogni-
tion within the dream state. A careful analyze of lucid dream content reveals that
the dream ego and dream intellect make decisions that the waking ego and waking
intellect would not make (see Travis 1994).
The fourth box in this 2 2 table is a sense of self without mental content.
Before reading this paper, you along with the vast majority of today’s scientists
might say that state does not exist. The empty cell is simply an artifact of setting up
a2 2 grid. How can there be a sense of self without an object; without a sense of
the body or the thinker or the thinking? William James, in his Principles of
Psychology, observed:
...it is difficult for me to detect in the activity any purely spiritual element at all. Whenever
my introspection glance succeeds in turning round quickly enough to catch one of those
manifestations of spontaneity in the act, all it can ever feel distinctly is some bodily process,
for the most part taking place within the head (James 1950/1890, p. 300).
This conclusion is a valid conclusion if the experience of consciousness has been
limited to waking experience, which includes sense-of-self (inner) and outer
experiences. In waking consciousness, the self is never found without an object.
However, the proposal put forth in this paper, is that meditation techniques uncover
that state of pure consciousness and so make this seemingly anamolous state
available for discussion and experimentation.
10.2 Phenomenological and Physiological Investigations
of Pure Consciousness
Fifty-two college students who practiced the TM technique for a few months to
over 8 years were asked to describe their deepest experiences during TM practice.
They were asked to use their own words to describe their experiences, as though
they were describing it to someone who did not meditate. A content analysis of
these descriptions yielded three themes that were common to all reports absence
of time, absence of space, and absence of body sense (Travis and Pearson 2000).
Time, space, and body sense are the framework that give meaning to waking
experience. During deepest TM experiences, both the fundamental framework
and the content of waking experience were reported to be absent. This suggests
that the experience of pure consciousness may not be an “alte red” state of waking.
It is not described in terms of distorted content strong emotions, strong visual,
auditory or tactile sensations, or distorted sense of self. Rather, pure consciousness
was described by the absence of the customary framework and characteristics that
226 F. Travis
define waking experience. Phenomenologically, pure consciousness is distinct from
experiences that characterize waking, dreaming, and sleeping.
Physiological, pure consciousness is also distinct from waking, dreaming, and
sleeping. During pure consciousness, research reports higher EEG alpha coherence,
and apneustic breathing slow, extended inhalation from 10 to 20 s with skin
conductance orienting and a heart rate preparatory response at the onset of breath
changes (Badawi et al. 1984; Travis and Pearson 2000; Travis and Walla ce 1997).
Apneustic breathing is not repor ted in normal populations (outside of meditation
practices), and has never been reported in the literature with durations longer
than 4–6 s (Plum and Posner 1980). The respiratory drive centers responsible
for apneustic breathing (the parabrachialis medialis nuclei) are quiet during waking,
dreaming, and sleeping, but become active during pure consciousness periods
(Kesterson and Clinch 1989). Changes in the brainstem nuclei driving breathing,
in autonomic functioning and brain state, with distinct phenomenological reports,
supports the description of pure consciousness as a fourth major state of conscious-
ness funda mentally different from waking, dreaming, or sleeping (Maharishi 1997).
10.3 Self-Referral Default Mode Network: Pure
Consciousness Experiences Activate the Intrinsic
Default State of the Brain
During TM practice, brain activity is reported to increase in the default mode
network (DMN) (Travis et al. 2010). This network was first noted when comparing
data from nine different neural imaging studies. Since neural imaging involves
subtracting control from experimental images, higher activation in a control condi-
tion could lead to perc eived “decreases” in the experimental condition. In these nine
studies of unrelated and independent tasks, decreases in midline frontal and pariet al
cortices were consistently reported (Raichle et al. 2001); eyes-closed rest or simple
fixation on a point were used as the control conditions. The researchers concluded
that a default mode network exists that is an intrinsic, default property of the brain
(Fox and Raichle 2007). Activation in this default mode is higher during low
cognitive load periods, such as eyes-closed resting control periods, and is lower
during goal directed behaviors requiring executive control (Gusnard et al. 2001;
Raichle and Snyder 2007).
Further research into DMN activation reported higher activation during (1) self-
referential mental activity (Gusnard et al. 2001; Kelley et al. 2002; Vogele y et al.
2001); (2) self-projection tasks; and (3) taking the viewpoint of others (Buckner and
Carroll 2007). Activity in the default state is higher during eyes-closed experiences.
When one closes the eyes, objects are reduc ed but sense-of-self remains. The
person knows that they are sitting in space; they are there waiting for the next
instruction. This is a predominately self-referral experience and DMN activity is
reported to be high. When opens the eyes and attention streams through the senses
and falls on an object an object referral experience DMN activity is reduced.
10 States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping 227
Relative to eyes-closed rest, DMN activation was higher during TM. This
supports the description of pure consciousness during TM practice as being a fuller
or higher sense of self-referral than just eyes-closed rest. Self can be written with a
small and a capital “S”. When “self” is written with a small “s” it denotes the self
that thinks, feels, decides, and experiences the self in a waking state; when “Self”
is written with a big “S” it denotes that part of the individual that does not change
and is the source of all streams of individual activity (Maharishi 1969). Thus, DMN
activation during eyes-closed rest small self-referral would rise during the
experience of pure consciousness large self-referral.
10.4 Junction Point Model of Pure Consciousness, Waking,
Sleeping, and Dreaming
A proposed Junction Point Model integrates meditation experience with waking,
dreaming, and sleeping. This model helps to locate meditation experiences relative
to those three states. It also provides a model for discussing higher states of
consciousness. The Junction Point Model posits that waking, sleeping, and dream-
ing are not isolated states that interact, but are sequential expressions of an
undifferentiated field pure consciousness that underlies them (Maharishi
1972; Travis 1994). This model starts with the observation that waking, sleeping,
and dreaming are discrete states. This assumption is supported by unique brain stem
activity (Siegel 1987), neurotransmitter balance (Hobson 1988), and EEG, EMG,
and eye movement patterns (Niedermeyer 1997) during each state. The model
suggests that one state must completely fade away before the next begins, and
that between any two, a junction point can be located that will mark the end of one
state and the beginning of the next. These junction points are windows into the field
of consciousness posited to underlie waking, sleeping, and dreaming.
10.5 Research Testing the Existence of Pure Consciousness
Between States of Consciousness
The prediction that pure consciousness can be located between states of conscious-
ness is supported by two lines of research. First, similar EEG patterns have been
reported during TM practice, which leads to pure consciousness between thoughts,
as during the waking/sleepi ng transition. For instance, frontal alpha and slowing of
peak EEG frequency by 1–2 Hz, reporte d during TM practice (Wallace 1970), were
later independently reported (Santamaria and Chiappa 1987) during the waking/
sleeping transition. This relation between EEG patterns during TM program and the
waking/sleeping transition has also been experimentally investigated. EEG in 15
experienced TM subjects during TM practice was compared to EEG during the
waking/sleeping transition in 15 non-meditating subjects matched for age, gender,
and handedness. The raw EEG and the resulting power and coherence spectra were
228 F. Travis
not significantly different between the TM sessions and the waking/sleeping transi-
tion (Travis 1990). However, the duration of thes e EEG patterns were different.
During TM practice, they lasted for the entire 10-min TM session; during the
waking/sleeping transition, they lasted for 3–5 min.
Other researchers have reporte d this similarity of EEG patterns during TM and
during the waking/sleeping transition, and the fact that they last longer during TM.
They concluded that TM practice balanced awareness betwee n waking and sleeping
(Fenwick et al. 1977; Stigsby et al. 1981; Wachsmuth and Dolce 1980), or that TM
practice freezes the hypnagogic process (Pagano and Warrenberg 1983; Schuman
1980). The junction point model gives a more comprehensive interpretation of these
findings. According to this model, EEG patterns would be similar during the
waking/sleeping transition and during TM practice because both states involve a
gradual minimizing of mental activity followed by pure consciousness periods
between states in the first case, and between thoughts in the second. Also, this
model would predict a longer duration of this pattern during TM practice because
one continues to give an inward direction to awareness during TM, thereby cycling
through pure consciousness many times in each session, in contrast to the natural
transition between states of consciousness.
A second line of research directly compared EEG patterns during TM prac tice to
those during the junction points between waking/sleeping, sleeping/dreaming, and
dreaming/sleeping. In the subjects’ power spectra, activity in each band, except
7–10 Hz, could be explained by known sleep mechanisms (Travis 1994). For
instance, the rise and fall of 1–4 Hz power occurred during periods of slow wave
sleep marked by high delta density and power; 13–16 Hz power was highest during
Stage 2 sleep, reflecting sleep-spindle activity. In contrast, the rise and fall of
7–10 Hz activity (alpha1) occurred at the transitions between waking, sleeping,
and dreaming in all subjects. Activity in this same band was seen in these subjects
during their Transcendental Meditation program. In terms of the Junction Point
Model, significant peaks in EEG power during the transitions between waking,
sleeping, and dreaming and during TM practice suggests that a similar state might
be available between states of consciousness and between thoughts.
10.6 Research Testing the Integration of Pure Consciousness
with Waking, Sleeping and Dreaming
If pure consciousness underlies waking, dreaming and sleeping, can it be integrated
with the three customary states of consciousness? If pure consciousness represents a
fourth state of consciousness, then the integration of pure consciousness with
waking, dreaming and sleeping will be a fifth state. This is the first stabilized
state of enlightenment described in the Vedic tradition, called turiyatit chetana
(Maharishi 1997). Since subj ective experiences and states of consciousness have
defining physiological characteristics, this proposed fifth state of consciousness
should also have distinct physiological markers.
10 States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping 229
10.7 Research Testing the Integration of Pure Consciousness
with Sleeping
Two research papers report EEG data that support the description of the experience
of pure consciousness along with the body sleeping. Banquet and Sailhan (1974)
recorded EEG during sleep in advanced TM subjects, and reported that alpha1
activity, seen during the TM practice, was superimposed over delta activity, seen
during deep sleep. Although they used experienced TM subjects, they did not
correlate this EEG pattern with self-reports of the integration of pure consciousness
with sleep.
Mason tested this hypothesis more directly (Mason et al. 1997). She compared
sleep EEG in 11 subjects reporting the integration of pure consciousness with sleep,
to sleep EEG in 11 short-term TM subjects, who did not report this experience, and
11 non-meditating controls. Subjects reporting the integration of pure conscious-
ness with sleep had simultaneous alpha1 and delta in their sleep records, which
supports their subjective experience of self-awareness while the body rested deeply.
Simultaneous alpha and delta during sleep, called alpha/delta sleep, has also been
reported in clinical cases of subjects in pain (Moldofsky et al. 1983). However,
these clinical subjects only reached Stages 2 and 3 during sleep. In contrast, the TM
subjects did not complain of pain, discomfort, or problems during sleeping, and
they had the same amount of Stage 4 sleep as normal subjects.
10.8 Research Testing the Integration of Pure Consciousness
with Waking
A second line of research has investigated the integration of pure consciousness
with waking tasks. EEG was recorded during simple and choice paired reaction
time tasks in 17 long-term TM subjects, reporting the integration of pure conscious-
ness with waking and sleeping, and compared to EEG patterns in 17 short-term TM
subjects who did not repor t this experience, and 17 non-meditating controls. In
individuals reporting the integration of pure consciousness with waking and sleep-
ing, brain preparatory responses during the paired reaction time tasks were highe r in
simple but lower in choice trials, and alpha relative power and broadband frontal
EEG coherence were higher during the challenging tasks (Travis et al. 2002).
Increased alpha amplitude and coherence, characteristic of TM practice, appeared
to become a stable EEG trait during challenging tasks in these subjects.
These individuals were also given a battery of personality and psychological
tests including inner/outer orientation, moral reasoning, anxiety, and personality.
Scores on these tests were factor analyzed. The first unrotated PCA component of
the test scores yielded a “consciousness factor,” analogous to the intelligence
g factor. The individuals reporting the integration of pure consciousness with
waking and sleeping had significantly higher consciousness factor scores more
230 F. Travis
inner directed, higher levels of moral reasoning, higher emotional stability, and
lower anxiety . These same individuals had higher scores on the Brain Integration
Scale (BIS) (Travis et al. 2004).
We can use a movie metaphor to give a sense of the growth of consciousness.
Watching a movie, most individuals are “lost” in the movie. The movie is real.
Emotions and thoughts are dictated by the ever-changing sequence of the film. This
is a predominantly object-referral state that characterizes the waking state. The
meditative experience of transcending the repeated experience of pure, self-
referral consciousness alters this common movie-going experience. Subje ctively,
the individual begins to “wake up” to his/her own inner status. Although continuing
to enjoy the movie, he/she gradually becomes aware that they exist independently
of the movie. They experience a value of witnessing the activity around them. To
these individuals, the ever-changing movie frames are a secondary part of experi-
ence because these frames are always changing. The most salient part of their every
experience is pure self-awareness. What is “real” shifts with time from the movie to
self-awareness, from the thoughts, feelings, and actions to the Self, from object-
referral to self-referral awareness (Travis et al. 2004).
10.9 Other Research on the Brain Integration Scale
The Brain Integration Scale (BIS) was constructed from cross-sectional data of
individuals reporting more frequent experiences of pure consciousness. A 3-month
random assignment longitudinal study with college students supports the finding
that TM practice leads to higher scores on this scale. After 3 months of TM practice,
college students increased on brain integration scores and decreased in sympathetic
reactivity (Travis et al. 2009). They also decreased in negative personality traits,
such as total mood disturbance, anxiety, and depression, and increased in positive
personality traits such as vigor, emotional intelligence, and behavioral and emo-
tional coping (Nidich et al. 2009). Thus, the experience of pure consciousness
during TM could to be a causal mechanism for increasing levels of brain integration
over time.
In addition, BIS scores were explored in two groups of athletes: professional
athletes who placed in the top ten in the Olympics, world games, or national games
for 3 consecutive years, or control athletes who did not consistently place. The
professional athletes who excelled had higher BIS scores, faster skin conductance
habituation to loud tones, and higher moral reasoning and ego development than the
controls (Harung et al. in press). The athletes were not practicing a meditation
technique. Their level of brain integration reflects the sum of their lifestyle and life
experiences to that point. However, this finding suggests greater success in life with
those markers that could index higher consciousness.
10 States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping 231
10.10 Conclusion
Meditation techniques can serve as probes to investigate states of consciousness.
Investigating the Transcendental Medita tion technique, a technique designed to
transcend its own activity, has led to phenomenological and physiological
descriptions of a state called pure consciousness, a proposed fourth state of con-
sciousness, and has generated a model, the Junction Point Model, which integr ates
waking, sleeping, and dreaming with meditation experiences. This model is
supported by similar EEG patterns during the transitions betwee n waking, sleeping,
and dreaming and during TM practice. This model also sugges ts that the underlying
field of pure consciousness can coexist with ordinary waking, sleeping and dream-
ing. This would be a fifth state of consciousness. Individuals reporting this experi-
ence were distinguished during slow wave sleep by the coexistence of alpha EEG,
observed during TM, and delta EEG, observed during sleep, and during waking
tasks by higher scores on the Brain Integration Scale and higher consciousness
factor scores. The Junction Point Model could provide a structure to integrate
ordinary experience with meditation experiences to help model and research the
full range of human consciousness.
References
Austin JH (2006) Zen-brain reflections. MIT, Cambridge
Badawi K, Wallace RK, Orme-Johnson D, Rouzere AM (1984) Electrophysiologic characteristics
of respiratory suspension periods occurring during the practice of the Transcendental Medita-
tion program. Psychosom Med 46(3):267–276
Banquet JP, Sailhan M (1974) Quantified EEG spectral analysis of sleep and Transcendental
Meditation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 42:445–453
Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11(2):49–57
Fenwick PBC, Donaldson S, Gillis L, Bushman J, Fenton GW, Perry I et al (1977) Metabolic and
EEG changes during Transcendental Meditation: an explanation. Biol Psychol 51:101–118
Fox MD, Raichle ME (2007) Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional
magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8(9):700–711
Gusnard DA, Raichle ME, Raichle ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and
the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(10):685–694
Gyatso T, Jinpa T (1995) The world of Tibetan Buddhism: an overview of its philosophy and
practice. Wisdom Publications, Somerville
Harung HS, Travis F, Pensgaard AM, Boes R, Cook-Greuter S, Daley K, (2011) High Levels of
Brain Integration in World-class Norwegian Athletes: Towards a Brain Measure of Perfor-
mance Capacity in Sports. Scandinavian Journal of Exercise and Sport, 1:32–41
Hobson J (1988) The dreaming brain. Basic Books, New York
James W (1950/1890) The principles of psychology. Dover Books, New York
Kabat-Zinn J (2003) Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin
Psychol Sci Pract 10:144–156
Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton TF (2002) Finding the self?
An event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 14(5):785–794
232 F. Travis
Kesterson J, Clinch NF (1989) Metabolic rate, respiratory exchange ratio, and apneas during
meditation. Am J Physiol 256(3 (Pt 2)):R632–R638
Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ (2008) Attention regulation and monitoring in
meditation. Trends Cogn Sci 12(4):163–169
Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1969) Maharishi Mahesh Yogi on the Bhagavad Gita. Penguin,
New York
Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1972) The science of creative intelligence. MIU, New York
Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1997) Celebrating perfection in education, 2nd edn. Maharishi Vedic
University Press, Noida
Mason LI, Alexander CN, Travis FT, Marsh G, Orme-Johnson DW, Gackenbach J et al (1997)
Electrophysiological correlates of higher states of consciousness during sleep in long-term
practitioners of the Transcendental Meditation program. Sleep 20(2):102–110
Moldofsky H, Lue FA, Smythe HA (1983) Alpha EEG sleep and morning symptoms in rheuma-
toid arthritis. J Rheumatol 10(3):373–379
Nidich SI, Rainforth MV, Haaga DA et al (2009) A randomized controlled trial on effects of the
Transcendental Meditation program on blood pressure, psychological distress, and coping in
young adults. Am J Hypertens 22:1326–1331
Niedermeyer E (1997) The normal EEG of the waking adult. In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva R
(eds) Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications and related fields. Urban
Schwarzenberg, Baltimore, pp 301–308
Pagano RR, Warrenberg S (1983) Meditation: in search of a unique effect. In: Davidson JM,
Schwartz GE, Shapiro D (eds) Consciousness and self-regulation: advances in research and
theory. Plenum, New York
Plum F, Posner JB (1980) The diagnosis of stupor and coma. F.A. Davis, Philadelphia
Raichle ME, Snyder AZ (2007) A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving
idea. Neuroimage 37(4):1083–1090, discussion 1097–1089
Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001) A default
mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98(2):676–682
Santamaria J, Chiappa I (1987) The EEG of drowsiness. Demos Publishing, New York
Schuman M (1980) A psychophysiological model of meditation and altered states of conscious-
ness: a critical review. In: Davidson JM, Davidson RC (eds) The psychobiology of conscious-
ness. Plenum, New York
Siegel J (1987) Brain stem mechanisms generating REM sleep. In: Krye HH (ed) Principles and
practice of sleep medicine. Raven, New York
Stigsby B, Rodenberg JC, Moth HB (1981) EEG findings during mantra meditation (TM): a
controlled quantitative study of experienced meditators. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
81:434–442
Travis F (1990) EEG patterns during TM practice and hypnagogic sleep. Soc Neurosci Abstr
15(1):244
Travis F (1994) The junction point model: a field model of waking, sleeping, and dreaming
relating dream witnessing, the waking/sleeping transition, and Transcendental Meditation in
terms of a common psychophysiologic state. Dreaming 4(2):91–104
Travis F, Pearson C (2000) Pure consciousness: distinct phenomenological and physiological
correlates of “consciousness itself”. Int J Neurosci 100:77–89
Travis F, Shear J (2010a) Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending:
categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Conscious
Cogn 19(4):1110–1118
Travis F, Shear J (2010b) Reply to Josipovic: duality and non-duality in meditation research.
Conscious Cogn 19(4):1122–1123
Travis F, Wallace RK (1997) Autonomic patterns during respiratory suspensions: possible markers
of transcendental consciousness. Psychophysiology 34(1):39–46
10 States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping 233
Travis FT, Tecce J, Arenander A, Wallace RK (2002) Patterns of EEG coherence, power, and
contingent negative variation characterize the integration of transcendental and waking states.
Biol Psychol 61:293–319
Travis FT, Arenander A, DuBois D (2004) Psychological and physiological characteristics of a
proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum of self-awareness. Conscious Cogn 13
(2):401–420
Travis F, Haaga DA, Hagelin J, Tanner M, Nidich S, Gaylord-King C et al (2009) Effects of
Transcendental Meditation practice on brain functioning and stress reactivity in college
students. Int J Psychophysiol 71(2):170–176
Travis F, Haaga D, Hagelin J, Arenander A, Tanner M, Schneider R (2010) Self-referential
awareness: coherence, power, and eLORETA patterns during eyes-closed rest, Transcendental
Meditation and TM-Sidhi practice. Cogn Process 11(1):21–30
Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A, Herrmann S, Happe F, Falkai P et al (2001) Mind reading:
neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage 14(1 (Pt 1)):170–181
Wachsmuth D, Dolce G (1980) Rechnerunterst
utzte Analyse des EEG wahrend Transzendentaler
Meditation und Schlaf. ZEEG-EMG 11:183–188
Wallace RK (1970) Physiological effects of Transcendental Meditation. Science 167(926):
1751–1754
Wallace A (1999) The Buddhist tradition of Samatha: methods for refining and examining
consciousness. J Conscious Stud 6:175–187
234 F. Travis
... Another interesting point to note is that, although the ancients devised various techniques like meditation to get a glimpse of this experience and stabilize oneself in that state, they also claim that this is not a new state to be attained. This is because, according to them, one is already functioning in this state of awareness and it underlies all other normal states of consciousness like waking, dreaming (REM), and deep (non-REM) sleep states (Shear and Jevning, 1999;Sharma, 2004;Nikhilananda, 2006;Cvetkovic and Cosic, 2011;Travis, 2011;Parker, 2019). They also indicate that this state acts as a substratum on which other states of consciousness happen and are recognized. ...
... Grounded on this view, this state is also referred to as a natural meditative state (Reddy and Roy, 2018a,b). Travis, in his studies, while exploring the neural correlates of pure consciousness, adopted this perspective (Travis and Pearson, 2000;Travis, 2011). Here, the authors looked for the presence of the state of awareness that underlies all three states of consciousness (waking, dreaming, and deep sleep states). ...
... As we mentioned, studying the neural oscillations associated with this state has been attempted previously by Travis (Travis and Pearson, 2000;Travis, 2011) and also by others (Josipovic, 2013;Berman and Stevens, 2015;Turjman, 2018). Recently, this has also been explored as a state of mental silence in the context of Sahaja Yoga meditation (Hernandez et al., 2015;Hernández et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Owing to its benefits on various cognitive aspects, one’s emotions, and well-being, meditation has drawn interest from several researchers and common public alike. We have different meditation practices associated with many cultures and traditions across the globe. Current literature suggests significant changes in the neural activity among the different practices of meditation, as each of these practices contributes to distinct physiological and psychological effects. Although this is the case, we want to find out if there is an underlying commonality among all these different practices. Thus, we ask the following questions related to different practices of meditation, the traditional goal of meditation and its significance—what is the central purpose of meditation? Do traditions define the final goal of all the practices of meditation? Are the purpose and goal of these practices different or is there a common goal to be attained through all these distinct practices? Embracing the traditional perspective, through this paper, we want to emphasize that, although these techniques and practices may appear different on the periphery, eventually, they seem to subject one to the same experience at the end, a natural meditative state (discussed in various spiritual traditions as the goal of meditation). In view of future studies on different meditation practices and also those exploring this subjective state, we offer some interesting ideas based on the traditional insights into meditation. In this context, we would also like to make a few comments on the way contemporary researchers view different practices of meditation.
... In context with the theme of the article we are discussing; Lee et al (2018), studying the neural oscillations associated with this state has been attempted previously by Travis (Travis and Pearson, 2000;Travis, 2011) and others (Josipovic, 2013;Berman and Stevens, 2015). Recently, this has also been explored as a state of mental silence in the context of Sahaja Yoga meditation (Hernandez et al., 2015;Hernandez et al., 2018). ...
... Traditions across the globe discussed this state as a baseline underlying other states of consciousness that forms the core of our natural experiences of living (Ramamurthi, 1995;Travis, 2011). Thus, considering this state as a baseline (for the purposes of evaluation), we recently proposed a noise-free model of meditation (Reddy and Roy, 2018a). ...
... Though current research on meditation has helped us in developing various meditation-interventions or target specific techniques, to tackle certain health conditions, one should not forget the essential purpose as to why ancients developed these techniques. It will be very interesting to study this state as previously attempted by few researchers (Ramamurthi, 1995;Travis and Pearson, 2000;Rao, 2011;Travis, 2011;Josipovic, 2013;Baars, 2013;Berman and Stevens, 2015) in a rigorous way. In the light of extensive interest in understanding consciousness and in developing and devising various clinical interventions, it is crucial to look for the possible existence of such a state, which is known to be underlying the other three states of consciousness. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Owing to its benefits on various cognitive aspects, emotions and one’s wellbeing meditation have drawn much interest from several researchers and common public alike. We have different meditation practices associated with numerous cultures and traditions across the globe. In addition, current literature also suggests distinct differences in neural oscillatory activity among different practices of meditation. Since these diverse practices influence in a different way, they contribute to distinct physiological and psychological effects. Though this is the case, we want to find out if there is an underlying commonality among all these seemingly distinct practices. Through this paper, we want to emphasize that though these techniques and practices may appear different on the periphery, they are all supposed to eventually lead in the same state of experience; a natural meditative state (which is discussed in various spiritual traditions as a goal of meditation). This is where the outer diversity in practices and techniques collapses to an inner unitary state of conscious experience. In this context, we would also like to make a few comments on the way contemporaries regard different practices of meditation
... The TM experts rely on a diving analogy: Repeating the mantra is like leaning over the edge of a pool -the movement downwards then just happens automatically (Pearson, 2013, p. 396;Rosenthal, 2011Rosenthal, /2012. Once transcending has commenced, it will be halted or impeded by applying any effort or control (Rosenthal, 2016(Rosenthal, /2017Shear, 2006cShear, , 2011bTravis, 2011Travis, , 2014. Since the process is automatic, the practitioner has a sense of "just letting the mantra happen" (Rosenthal, 2011(Rosenthal, /2012, quoting a meditator). ...
... • The meditator repeats the mantra and transcends fully or to some extent; • They cease transcending, and then notice that they have lost awareness of the mantra; and • They return to the mantra and the next cycle begins (Faber et al., 2017;Rosenthal, 2011Rosenthal, /2012Rosenthal, , pp. 18-19, 2016Rosenthal, /2017Travis, 2011;. ...
Article
Full-text available
In contentless experience (sometimes termed pure consciousness) there is an absence of mental content such as thought, perception, and mental imagery. The path to contentless experience in meditation can be taken to comprise the meditation technique, and the experiences (“interim-states”) on the way to the contentless “goal-state/s”. Shamatha, Transcendental, and Stillness Meditation are each said to access contentless experience, but the path to that experience in each practice is not yet well understood from a scientific perspective. We have employed evidence synthesis to select and review 135 expert texts from those traditions. In this paper we describe the techniques and interim-states based on the expert texts and compare them across the practices on key dimensions. Superficially, Shamatha and Transcendental Meditation appear very different to Stillness Meditation in that they require bringing awareness to a meditation object. The more detailed and systematic approach taken in this paper indicates that posturally Shamatha is closer to Stillness Meditation, and that on several other dimensions Shamatha is quite different to both other practices. In particular, Shamatha involves greater measures to cultivate attentional stability and vividness with respect to an object, greater focusing, less tolerance of mind-wandering, more monitoring, and more deliberate doing/control. Achieving contentless experience in Shamatha is much slower, more difficult, and less frequent. The findings have important implications for taxonomies of meditation and for consciousness, neuroscientific, and clinical research/practice, and will provide new and useful insights for meditation practitioners.
... • On reviewing the literature on meditation across various cultures and traditions, it appears that awareness is central to any type of meditation (BRABOSZCZ et al., 2010;TRAVIS, 2011;SCHMIDT, 2014 We now have several studies indicating the dominance of the presence of other oscillations such as delta, theta, alpha and beta in meditation (LEE et al., 2018). The dominance of a specific brain wave oscillation in different regions of the brain depends on a particular type of meditation and the cognitive modality involved. ...
... of wakeful state, REM-sleep and non-REM (deep) sleep states over meditation(in line with studies such as -Llinas and Ribary (2006),Cvetkovic and Cosic (2011), Travis (2011), Ferrarelli Rev. Simbio-Logias, V. 10, Nr 13, 2018, Wind and Noreika (2011);Dissanayaka et al. (2015)). Anticipating a deeper connection between sleep and meditation; both being different states associated with the presence of internal awareness, some ancient cultures have already devised methods to transform a normal sleep state into a deeper meditative state called Yoga Nidra(SHARMA, 2004;SARASWATI, 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, a surge of interest came up with studies in terms of the influence of different types of meditation on the brain and body. Lacking the basic understanding as to why ancient cultures conceived this practice primarily in its various facets, most of these studies mainly focus on delineating the underlying mechanisms of influence in terms of wellbeing and the cognitive enhancement. Thus, they not only involve various definitional and taxonomical issues, but also methodological issues. In this concern, here, we share a new perspective and also emphasize on few issues which are fundamental to meditation research that should be addressed in future studies. Journal: Revista Simbio-Logias http://www.ibb.unesp.br/#!/departamentos/educacao/publicacoes-e-produtos-academicos/simbio-logias/revistas/v-10-nr-13---2018/
... Out of such studies, specifically the integration offered in Frederick Travis' "States of Consciousness Beyond Waking, Dreaming and Sleeping" (Travis 2011) and relying on the examination of the textual material, the following typology for meditative techniques is suggested here: ...
Article
Full-text available
The field of mystical and meditative research lacks a basic typology delineating the varied genres and characteristics of the mystical experience and of the meditative practices that may be correlated to those. Such a state hinders the comparative study of mysticism and meditation through different philosophical, religious and spiritual traditions, or along the chronological development in a single tradition. In this article, I introduce phenomenological typological tools developed in a previous monograph dealing with the Jewish meditative tradition and illustrate how these can be used to analyze the adjustments and the variations between previous and contemporary Jewish mystics, through examining the four different types of mystical experience and five different characteristics of meditative techniques.
... If that is the case, then how can a mediator's level of expertise be based and studied in comparison to a novice or other individual? The better choice would be to study a subject during various states say from waking state to deep sleep state and then draw a conclusion on his state of meditative experience (Cvetkovic and Cosic, 2011;Travis, 2011;Dissanayaka et al., 2015). To overcome this, we suggest a new baseline, which is universally present in every individual derived from the abovediscussed perspective. ...
Article
Full-text available
In Bærentsen (2015), various issues have been raised mainly concerning the contemporary view of the practice of meditation with respect to the actual philosophical perspective. We endorse the idea of referring to ancient traditional sources on meditation - as a practice and thus are in agreement with the many of the points discussed in Bærentsen (2015). As considered in modern scientific experiments on meditation, the practice of meditation is not conceived either for the enhancement of cognitive functions or for the purpose of well-being (Rao, 2011; Awasthi, 2013; Nash and Newberg, 2013; Schmidt, 2014; Tomasino et al. 2014; Bærentsen, 2015), though it appears to show the promising influence on these aspects (Ospina et al. 2007; Braboszcz et al. 2010; Schmidt and Walach, 2014; Tang et al. 2015; Tomasino and Fabbro, 2015). It is utilized mainly as a tool to realize one’s true nature by attaining Buddhahood or Samadhi or any similar experiential state as quoted in different spiritual traditions (which we refer to here as a natural meditative state for a valid reason: Woods, 1927/2003; Rao, 2011; Schmidt, 2014; Schmidt and Walach, 2014; Tomasino et al. 2014). (....for full article follow the link - https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00248...)
Article
Full-text available
Данная статья посвящена понятию «изменённые состояния сознания», используемому апологетами против медитативных практик. В некоторых исследованиях авторы утверждают, что медитация вводит человека в такое состояние, что может указывать на ложность духовного опыта и опасность для психического здоровья практикующего. Цель статьи — показать, почему сложно использовать понятия «изменённые состояния сознания» для критики медитативных практик. В работе проводится анализ основных аргументов за и против применения данного понятия, указаны слабые места такого рода аргументации и предлагается направление, в котором можно построить дальнейшую критику. В начале автор даёт общее определение изменённых состояний сознания, основанных на понятии базисного состояния. Далее указывает основные сложности: малоизученность темы, использование учёными различных моделей её описания, указание ряда исследований на пользу таких состояний, в том числе медитации, факт отнесения исследователями некоторых духовных переживаний святых к изменённым состояниям сознания. Автор приходит к выводу, что изменённые состояния сознания должны использоваться как аргумент против медитативных практик с осторожностью и применяться вместе с другими видами обоснования. Доводы при диспуте с практикующими медитацию предлагаются автором с учётом конкретной цели, вида практики и мировоззрения человека.
Article
Full-text available
Contentless experience involves an absence of mental content such as thought, perception, and mental imagery. In academic work it has been classically treated as including states like those aimed for in Shamatha, Transcendental, and Stillness Meditation. We have used evidence synthesis to select and review 135 expert texts from within the three traditions. In this paper we identify the features of contentless experience referred to in the expert texts and determine whether the experiences are the same or different across the practices with respect to each feature. We identify 65 features reported or implied in one or more practices, with most being reported or implied in all three. While there are broad similarities in the experiences across the traditions, we find that there are differences with respect to four features and possibly many others. The main difference identified is that Shamatha involves substantially greater attentional stability and vividness. Another key finding is that numerous forms of content are present in the experiences, including wakefulness, naturalness, calm, bliss/joy, and freedom. The findings indicate that meditation experiences described as contentless in the academic literature can in fact involve considerable variation, and that in many and perhaps most cases these experiences are not truly contentless. This challenges classical understandings in academic research that in these so-called contentless experiences all content is absent, and that the experiences are therefore an identical state of pure consciousness or consciousness itself. Our assessment is that it remains an open question whether the experiences aimed for in the three practices should be classed as pure consciousness. Implications of our analysis for neuroscientific and clinical studies and for basic understandings of the practices are discussed.
Chapter
In this chapter, we are accumulating significant experimental results of the quorum sensing mechanism over the last few decades. We summarise important experimental observations of several talking bacteria such as Vibrio fischeri,Pseudomomonas aeruginosa, Agrobacterium tumefaciens, Erwinia carotovora, Vibrio harveyi, Vibrio anguillarum, Yersinia enterocolitica, Yersinia pseudotuberculosis, Aeromonas hydrophila,Aeromonas salmonicida, Serratia liquefaciens, Salmonella typhimurium, Ralstonia solanacearum, Rhizobium etli, Rhodobacter sphaeroides, Rhizobium leguminosarum,Burkholderia cepacia, Chromobacterium violaceum, Erwinia chrysanthemi, Escherichia coli, Vibrio cholerae, Streptococcus pneumonic, Bacillus subtilis and Staphylococcus aureus.
Article
This article explores transcendental experiences during meditation practice and the integration of transcendental experiences and the unfolding of higher states of consciousness with waking, dreaming, and sleeping. The subject/object relationship during transcendental experiences is characterized by the absence of time, space, and body sense-the framework that gives meaning to waking experiences. Physiologically, transcendental experiences during Transcendental Meditation practice are marked by slow inhalation, along with autonomic orientation at the onset of breath changes and heightened α1 (8-10 Hz) frontal coherence. The integration of transcendental experiences with waking, dreaming, and sleeping is also marked by distinct subjective and objective markers. This integrated state, called Cosmic Consciousness in the Vedic tradition, is subjectively marked by inner self-awareness coexisting with waking, sleeping, and dreaming. Physiologically, Cosmic Consciousness is marked by the coexistence of α1 electroencephalography (EEG) with delta EEG during deep sleep, and higher brain integration, greater emotional stability, and decreased anxiety during challenging tasks. Transcendental experiences may be the engine that fosters higher human development.
Article
Full-text available
Bei 5 Versuchspersonen (Vpn) wurden polygraphisch EEG, EOG und Herzfrequenz vor, während und nach der Transzendentalen Meditation (TM) und während des Nachtschlafes je zweimal analysiert. Während der TM waren die α-Wellen höher als davor, kontinuierlich und es traten bilaterale Gruppen von ϑ-Wellen auf. Dieselben α-Veränderungen fanden sich während der Entspannung mit geschlossenen Augen. Auch die Diskriminanzanalyse aus den Spektralwerten ergab keine signifikanten Unterschiede der dominanten Frequenz, Intensität und Varianz von 5 festgelegten Frequenzbändern des von C3 abgeleiteten EEG gegenüber der TM. Die Diskriminanzanalyse zeigte auch keine Unterschiede zwischen ähnlichen Abschnitten des während der TM und dem Einschlafen abgeleiteten niedrigen EEG. Wenn ein flaches EEG (Stadium 1) während der Meditation oder während des Einschlafens auftrat, war die Herzfrequenz signifikant niedriger als während des α-Rhythmus. Die Vigilo-Somnogramme ergaben bei allen Vpn einen normalen Verlauf. Subjektiv erlebten die Vpn während der TM eine Zunahme an Entspannung, Wachheit und nicht objektbezogenen Bewußtheit. Die Vpn konnten EEG-Stadien verringerter Vigilanz (W1; l0) ungewöhnlich lange Zeit gleichförmig beibehalten.
Article
Full-text available
A field model of waking, sleeping, and dreaming, called the junction point model, portrays waking, nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, and REM dreaming as expressions of a single, undifferentiated state. EEG data from 7 right-handed men are presented that support this model. In the transitions between waking, NREM sleep, and REM dreaming, bursts of similar frequency EEG were seen that did not seem generated by known sleep mechanisms, suggesting a common experience available at these transitions. EEG spectra during Transcendental Meditation practice, designed to lead to the experience of the underlying field, an experience called transcendental consciousness (TC), were similiar to those reported during the waking/sleeping transition. During the coexistence of TC and sleep, called witnessing sleep, the characteristic alpha activity of TC was seen along with the delta activity of deep sleep. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
A sequel to the popular Zen and the Brain further explores pivotal points of intersection in Zen Buddhism, neuroscience, and consciousness, arriving at a new synthesis of information from both neuroscience research and Zen studies. This sequel to the widely read Zen and the Brain continues James Austin's explorations into the key interrelationships between Zen Buddhism and brain research. In Zen-Brain Reflections, Austin, a clinical neurologist, researcher, and Zen practitioner, examines the evolving psychological processes and brain changes associated with the path of long-range meditative training. Austin draws not only on the latest neuroscience research and new neuroimaging studies but also on Zen literature and his personal experience with alternate states of consciousness. Zen-Brain Reflections takes up where the earlier book left off. It addresses such questions as: how do placebos and acupuncture change the brain? Can neuroimaging studies localize the sites where our notions of self arise? How can the latest brain imaging methods monitor meditators more effectively? How do long years of meditative training plus brief enlightened states produce pivotal transformations in the physiology of the brain? In many chapters testable hypotheses suggest ways to correlate normal brain functions and meditative training with the phenomena of extraordinary states of consciousness. After briefly introducing the topic of Zen and describing recent research into meditation, Austin reviews the latest studies on the amygdala, frontotemporal interactions, and paralimbic extensions of the limbic system. He then explores different states of consciousness, both the early superficial absorptions and the later, major "peak experiences." This discussion begins with the states called kensho and satori and includes a fresh analysis of their several different expressions of "oneness." He points beyond the still more advanced states toward that rare ongoing stage of enlightenment that is manifest as "sage wisdom." Finally, with reference to a delayed "moonlight" phase of kensho, Austin envisions novel links between migraines and metaphors, moonlight and mysticism. The Zen perspective on the self and consciousness is an ancient one. Readers will discover how relevant Zen is to the neurosciences, and how each field can illuminate the other.
Article
[opening paragraph]: Buddhist inquiry into the natural world proceeds from a radically different point of departure than western science, and its methods differ correspondingly. Early pioneers of the scientific revolution, including Copernicus, Kepler, and Galileo, expressed an initial interest in the nature of physical objects most far removed from human subjectivity: such issues as the relative motions of the sun and earth, the surface of the moon, and the revolutions of the planets. And a central principle of scientific naturalism is the pure objectification of the natural world, free of any contamination of subjectivity. This principle of objectivism demands that science deals with empirical facts testable by empirical methods entailing testability by third-person means; and such facts must, therefore, be public rather than private, which is to say, they must be accessible to more than one observer.
Article
This book provides a foundation to the principles of psychology. It draws upon the natural sciences, avoiding metaphysics, for the basis of its information. According to James, this book, assuming that thoughts and feelings exist and are vehicles of knowledge, thereupon contends that psychology, when it has ascertained the empirical correlation of the various sorts of thought or feeling with definite conditions of the brain, can go no farther as a natural science. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The method of obtaining, and some of the characteristics of, auditory driving were given. Points that need further study can be deduced from the results of this experiment. The best uses of auditory driving in psychological research and medicine should be ascertained. Finally the correspondence (which was examined in an unpublished paper by the author) between auditory driving and response to drum beats in primitive dance ceremonies should be investigated in the field.