ArticlePDF Available

The Evolutionary Biology of Education: How Our Hunter-Gatherer Educative Instincts Could Form the Basis for Education Today

Authors:

Abstract

Education is broadly defined as the set of processes by which each generation of human beings acquires the culture in which they grow up. By this definition, education is part and parcel of our biological makeup. An analysis of education in hunter-gatherer bands indicates that young humans are designed, by natural selection, to acquire the culture through their self-directed play and exploration. Research at a modern-day democratic school designed to facilitate self-education demonstrates that our hunter-gatherer educative instincts are quite adequate for education today, given an appropriate educational environment. The ideal environment for such education—found both in hunter-gatherer bands and at the school studied—is one in which young people (a) have unlimited free time and much space in which to play and explore; (b) can mix freely with other children of all ages; (c) have access to a variety of knowledgeable and caring adults; (d) have access to culturally relevant tools and equipment and are free to play and explore with those items; (e) are free to express and debate any ideas that they wish to express and debate; (f) are free from bullying (which includes freedom from being ordered around arbitrarily by adults); and (g) have a true voice in the group’s decision-making process. The per-student cost required to create such settings is less than half that of the average for our current public schools. KeywordsEducation–Evolution–Hunter-gatherers–Play–Children–Cultural transmission–Self-directed learning
1 23
Evolution: Education and
Outreach
ISSN 1936-6426
Volume 4
Number 1
Evo Edu Outreach (2011)
4:28-40
DOI 10.1007/
s12052-010-0306-1
The Evolutionary Biology of Education:
How Our Hunter-Gatherer Educative
Instincts Could Form the Basis for
Education Today
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you
wish to self-archive your work, please use the
accepted author’s version for posting to your
own website or your institution’s repository.
You may further deposit the accepted author’s
version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s
request, provided it is not made publicly
available until 12 months after publication.
ORIGINAL SCIENTIFIC ARTICLE
The Evolutionary Biology of Education: How
Our Hunter-Gatherer Educative Instincts Could Form
the Basis for Education Today
Peter Gray
Published online: 26 January 2011
#Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
Abstract Education is broadly defined as the set of
processes by which each generation of human beings
acquires the culture in which they grow up. By this
definition, education is part and parcel of our biological
makeup. An analysis of education in hunter-gatherer bands
indicates that young humans are designed, by natural
selection, to acquire the culture through their self-directed
play and exploration. Research at a modern-day democratic
school designed to facilitate self-education demonstrates
that our hunter-gatherer educative instincts are quite
adequate for education today, given an appropriate educa-
tional environment. The ideal environment for such
educationfound both in hunter-gatherer bands and at the
school studiedis one in which young people (a) have
unlimited free time and much space in which to play and
explore; (b) can mix freely with other children of all ages;
(c) have access to a variety of knowledgeable and caring
adults; (d) have access to culturally relevant tools and
equipment and are free to play and explore with those
items; (e) are free to express and debate any ideas that they
wish to express and debate; (f) are free from bullying
(which includes freedom from being ordered around
arbitrarily by adults); and (g) have a true voice in the
groups decision-making process. The per-student cost
required to create such settings is less than half that of the
average for our current public schools.
Keywords Education .Evolution .Hunter-gatherers .Play .
Children .Cultural transmission .Self-directed learning
Most of us when we hear the term education automatically
think of schooling. For this article, I ask you to keep these
two terms and concepts distinct. Schooling is a relatively
new cultural innovation. It is the deliberate provision, by
adults, of special settings and procedures designed to teach
specific skills, beliefs, and concepts to the young. Educa-
tion, in contrast, is far from new; it is older than our
species.As applied to our species, it is the entire set of
processes by which each new generation of human beings,
in any cultural group, acquires the skills, knowledge,
rituals, beliefs, lore, and valuesin short, the cultureof
the previous generation. To say that we are the supremely
cultural animal is to say that we are the supremely
educative animal.
Beginning well over a million years ago, we (our human
genetic line) began moving along an evolutionary track that
made us ever more dependent on cultural transmission, that
is, upon education. Over time, we developed means of
hunting, gathering, processing foods, protecting ourselves
from predators, birthing, caring for infants, and combating
diseases that depended increasingly on detailed, learned
knowledge and theories about our local environment and on
well-honed skills, including the crafting and using of tools,
that were passed along from generation to generation. We
also came to depend on increasingly high levels of
cooperation within bands and across networks of bands,
which required the cultural transmission of social mores,
rules, rituals, stories, and shared cultural beliefs and values,
all serving to help promote cooperation.
In any human group, children who failed to acquire
crucial aspects of the culture around them would be at a
serious disadvantage for survival and reproduction. They
P. Gray
Department of Psychology, Boston College
Chestnut Hill, MA 02467, USA
P. Gray (*)
340 S. Quinsigamond Ave.
Shrewsbury, MA 01545, USA
e-mail: grayp@bc.edu
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
DOI 10.1007/s12052-010-0306-1
Author's personal copy
would not know how to perform economically essential
tasks, how to get along well with others within their culture,
or how to attract a mate for reproduction. Natural selection,
then, would strongly favor characteristics that promoted
young peoples abilities to acquire the culture. These
characteristics are what I refer to as educative instincts.
The analysis that I present here suggests that the human
educative instincts have much more to do with learning
than with teaching. They are, largely, the characteristics that
lead young people, on their own initiatives, to observe,
explore, and practice essential elements of the culture that
surrounds them.
In this article, I begin by examining how education
occurs in hunter-gatherer cultures. It is in those cultures that
we see most clearly the power of the human educative
instincts. Then I present evidence, from research at a school
designed for self-education, that the same instincts that
worked so well to promote hunter-gathererseducation can
still work effectively today, in our culture, given appropri-
ate conditions. Finally, I point out how our traditional
schools make education difficult by removing the con-
ditions that allow childrens educative instincts to operate
effectively.
Education in Hunter-Gatherer Cultures
Our educative instincts took their present shape during the
hundreds of thousands of years of evolutionary history
when our ancestors were all hunter-gatherers. As one pair
of anthropologists put it, the hunter-gatherer way of life is
the only stable way of life our species has ever known (Lee
and DeVore 1968). By comparison, our post-hunter-
gatherer existence has been very short and turbulent. The
advent of agriculture, beginning roughly 10,000 years ago,
set into motion an ever-accelerating rate of cultural change
in how we humans live; but however we live, we continue
to do so with the biological drives and capacities that were
honed by natural selection during our hunter-gatherer days.
We cant go back in time to observe education in our
pre-agricultural ancestors, but we can make reasonable
inferences about that process by examining it in those
groups of people, in various isolated parts of the world,
who managed to survive as hunter-gatherers into modern
times. Today, the pure hunting and gathering way of life is
almost completely obliterated, wiped out by intrusions from
modern civilizations. But as recently as the 1970s, and to
some degree even later, it was still possible for anthro-
pologists to find and study hunter-gatherers who were
nearly untouched by modern ways. To learn about
education in hunter-gatherer cultures, I have studied the
anthropological literature on childrens lives in such
cultures. To supplement that literature, I (along with
graduate student Jonathan Ogas) identified ten prominent
anthropologists who, among them, had studied seven
different hunter-gatherer cultures on three different con-
tinents and questioned them extensively, with a written
questionnaire, about their observations of childrens lives in
the cultures they observed.
For all of this work, I have focused exclusively on the
variety of hunter-gatherer cultures that anthropologists refer
to as immediate-return or egalitarian hunter-gatherers, or
sometimes as band societies. These are cultures in which
people live in small bands (typically of 20 to 50 persons,
including children) that move from place to place within a
large but circumscribed territory to follow the available
game and vegetation. They have a highly egalitarian social
structure, make decisions by consensus, own little property,
share food and material goods within and even across
bands, do not have means for long-term food preservation,
have little occupational specialization except that based on
gender, and reject violence as a legitimate means of solving
problems (Kelly 1995). Among the cultures of this type that
in one way or another were included in my study are the Ju/
hoansi (also called the !Kung, of Africas Kalahari Desert),
Hazda (of Tanzanian rainforests), Mbuti (of Congos Ituri
Forest), Aka (of rain forests in Central African Republic and
Congo), Efé (of Congos Ituri Forest), Batek (of Peninsular
Malaysia), Agta (of Luzon, Philippines), Nayaka (of South
India), Aché (of Eastern Paraguay), Parakana (of Brazils
Amazon basin), and Yiwara (of the Australian Desert).
I have ignored, for this study, the category of hunter-
gatherer society referred to as delayed-return or non-
egalitarian hunter-gatherers, typified by the Kwakiutl of
the American northwest coast and the Ainu of Japan. These
are relatively sedentary societies where people exploit a
particular local resource for food (commonly fish). They
are characterized by high population densities, food storage,
resource ownership and defense, hierarchical social struc-
tures, inherited status, and relatively high rates of violence
and acceptance of violence as legitimate. Archeological
evidence suggests that these cultures are more recent than
the egalitarian band cultures and are less likely to represent
the predominant living conditions of our pre-agricultural
ancestors (Kelly 1995). In line with the practice of some
anthropologists, when I use the term hunter-gatherer,
unmodified, I am referring specifically to the band,
egalitarian variety.
Despite great differences in their geography and their
specific ways of hunting and gathering, hunter-gatherer
societies (of the band type) are remarkably similar to one
another in their basic social structure, social mores, and
approach to education. Such similarity, across continents,
adds confidence to the view that these societies represent
reasonably well the kinds of hunter-gatherer societies that
preceded the advent of agriculture. The research literature
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 29
Author's personal copy
and our survey concerning childrens lives in hunter-
gatherer cultures reveal a remarkably consistent story, from
culture to culture. Here are four conclusions that appear to
apply to all hunter-gatherer cultures (of the band type) that
have been studied (for more details, see Gray 2009a):
1. Hunter-gatherer children had to learn an enormous
amount to become effective adults.
It would be a mistake to assume that because hunter-
gatherer cultures were simplerthan modern cultures,
children had less to learn. The hunting-and-gathering way
of life was highly knowledge-intensive and skill-intensive,
and because of the absence of occupational specialization,
each child had to acquire the whole culture, or at least that
part of it appropriate to his or her gender.
To become hunters, boysand girls, too, in those
cultures where women as well as men huntedhad to
learn how to identify and track the many dozens of different
species of birds and mammals that their group hunted. They
had to learn how to craft to perfection the tools of hunting,
such as bows and arrows, blowguns and darts, snares, nets,
and so on. And, of course, they had to develop great skill at
using those tools. To become gatherers, girlsand boys
also, to the degree that men also gatheredhad to learn
which of the countless varieties of roots, nuts, seeds, fruits,
and greens in their area were edible and nutritious; when
and where to find them; how to extract the edible portions;
and how to process them. In addition, hunter-gatherer
children had to learn to build huts, make fires, cook, fend
off predators, predict weather changes, navigate their
hunting and gathering grounds, treat wounds and diseases,
assist births, care for infants, maintain harmony in the
group, negotiate with neighboring groups, tell stories, make
music, and engage in the various dances and rituals of their
culture.
2. Hunter-gatherer adults did not direct childrens educa-
tion or in other ways tell them what to do. Children and
even adolescents were free to play and explore, on their
own, in their own chosen ways, from dawn to dusk.
In the words of hunter-gatherer expert Richard Lee
(1988, p. 264), hunter-gatherers were fiercely egalitarian.
They eschewed any attempts by one person to control the
behavior of others. They had no chiefs or big men; they
made all decisions within the band through debate until
consensus was reached. Such egalitarianism was necessar-
ily coupled with extraordinary personal autonomy. Hunter-
gatherers did not tell one another what to do. Each person
was free at any time to leave the band and join another
band, and would do so if he or she felt put-upon. People
were always free, on any given day, to join or not join a
hunting or gathering party. If they joined, it was because
they wanted to, not because they were compelled to. Food
was shared, and they would get the same portion regardless
of whether or not they had taken part in getting the food.
Remarkably, these principles of equality and autonomy
were applied as much to children as to adults. Adults did
not tell children what to do any more than they told other
adults what to do; they believed that childrens own wills
should be their guides. Here is a sample of quotations, from
various researchers and observers of hunter-gatherers, that
reinforce this point (Gray 2009a):
&Aborigine children [of Australia] are indulged to an
extreme degree, and sometimes continue to suckle until
they are four or five years old. Physical punishment for
a child is almost unheard of(Gould 1969, p. 90).
&Hunter-gatherers do not give orders to their children;
for example, no adult announces bedtime. At night,
children remain around adults until they feel tired and
fall asleep.Parakana adults [of Brazil] do not
interfere with their childrens lives. They never beat,
scold, or behave aggressively with them, physically or
verbally, nor do they offer praise or keep track of their
development.Children do not go to parents for help
or to complain about one another.Adults do not give
any indication of being worried about the psychological
future of their children. Whether or not their children
will become effective adults is not an issue(Gosso et
al. 2005, pp. 218, 226).
&The idea that this is my childor your childdoes not
exist [among the Yequana, of Venezuela]. Deciding
what another person should do, no matter what his age,
is outside the Yequana vocabulary of behaviors. There
is great interest in what everyone does, but no impulse
to influencelet alone coerceanyone. The childs
will is his motive force(Liedloff 1977, p. 90).
&Infants and young children [among Inuit hunter-
gatherers of the Hudson Bay area] are allowed to
explore their environments to the limits of their physical
capabilities and with minimal interference from adults.
Thus if a child picks up a hazardous object, parents
generally leave it to explore the dangers on its own. The
child is presumed to know what it is doing(Guemple
1988, p 137).
&Ju/hoan children [of Africas Kalahari Desert] very
rarely cried, probably because they had little to cry
about. No child was ever yelled at or slapped or
physically punished, and few were even scolded. Most
never heard a discouraging word until they were
approaching adolescence, and even then the reprimand,
if it really was a reprimand, was delivered in a soft
voice(Thomas 2006, p 198).
To her description of Ju/hoan child-raising practices,
Thomas (2006, pp. 198199) adds: We are sometimes told
that children who are treated so kindly become spoiled, but
30 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
this is because those who hold that opinion have no idea
how successful such measures can be. Free from frustration
or anxiety, sunny and cooperative, and usually without
close siblings as competitors, the children were every
parents dream. No culture can ever have raised better,
more intelligent, more likable, more confident children.
[Thomass statement about the lack of close siblings as
competitors refers to the fact that births for hunter-gatherer
women are usually spaced at least four years apart. The
continuous, on-demand nursing of children until they are
three or four years old apparently produces a hormonal
effect that delays ovulation in women who are lean, as
hunter-gatherer women are, and serves as a natural means
of birth control.]
Hunter-gatherer children generally stay within eyeshot or
at least hearing distance of their mothers or other adults
until they are about four years old, at which age they begin
to prefer the company of other children and begin to run
freely with other kids. In our survey, we asked the ten
anthropologists how much time children in the culture they
observed were free to play each day, and the answer we
received from all of them was essentially this: They were
free to play almost all of the time, from dawn to dusk.
Here are three typical responses (Gray 2009a):
&Both girls and boys had almost all day every day free
to play(Alan Brainard, concerning the Nharo, of
southern Africa).
&Children were free to play nearly all the time; no one
expected children to do serious work until they were in
their late teens(Karen Endicott, concerning the Batek
of Malaysia).
&Boys were free to play nearly all the time until age 15
17; for girls most of the day, in between a few errands
and some babysitting, was spent in play(Robert
Bailey, concerning the Efé, of central Africa).
These comments are consistent with published reports. In a
formal study of Ju/hoan childrens activities, Patricia Draper
(1976, pp. 210, 213) concluded: Girls are around 14 years
old before they begin regular food gathering and water- and
wood-collecting. This is in spite of the fact that they may be
married before this age. Boys are 16 years old or over before
they begin serious hunting.Children do amazingly little
work.The Hazda (of the Tanzanian rainforests in Africa)
are sometimes cited as an exception to the rule that hunter-
gatherer children engage in little productive work. Hazda
children forage for a good portion of their own food. But a
study of Hazda children, aged 5 to 15, revealed that they
spent only about two hours per day foraging, in the rich
vegetative areas near camp, and that even while foraging
they continued to play (Blurton Jones et al. 1994).
A number of researchers have compared the child-
raising practices of hunter-gatherers with those of agrarian
(farming) communities. While hunter-gatherer parents are
indulgent and permissive, agrarian parents are typically
strict and autocratic (Barry et al. 1959). While hunter-
gatherers value their childrens willfulness and indepen-
dence, agrarian parents value obedience. While hunter-
gatherer children are free to play and explore all day on
their own, agrarian children are required to work a good
portion, if not most of the day, at chores in the home and
field. A study of peoples in Botswana with mixed hunter-
gatherer and agrarian subsistence revealed that the more a
family was involved in hunting and gathering and the less
they were involved in farming, the more time children had
to play (Bock and Johnson 2004).
Researchers have offered a variety of interrelated
explanations of the differences between hunter-gatherers
and farmers in child-raising practices. Perhaps the most
obvious explanation has to do with the immediate econom-
ic benefits, or lack of such benefits, gained from childrens
work. Hunting and gathering are knowledge- and skill-
intensive, but not labor-intensive. The adults hunt and
gather with a sense of play, and they have plenty of time
left over for such leisure activities as gossiping, visiting
friends in neighboring bands, making music, and in other
ways playing (Gray 2009a; Sahlins 1972). Moreover, the
birth rate among hunter-gatherers is relatively low, so there
are relatively few young mouths to feed. Hunter-gatherers
simply dont need child labor. In contrast, farming is highly
labor-intensive, and much of that labor is unskilled and can
be done by children. Farmers typically have more children
than do hunter-gatherers, and to feed and care for them all
the children must work.
Other explanations, quite closely related to this immedi-
ate economic one, focus on the values imparted by the
different methods of child rearing and on parentsgoals in
child raising (Barry et al. 1959; DeVore et al. 1968). The
social and economic life of adult hunter-gatherers requires
assertiveness, creativity, and individual judgment. To have
influence in a society where decisions are made by debate
and consensus, you must be assertive. Hunting and
gathering themselves require creative, diverse methods
and on-the-spot judgments to meet the unpredictable,
ever-changing conditions of nature. As Whiting (in DeVore
et al. 1968) has pointed out, the permissive parenting style
of hunter-gatherers seems ideally designed to promote
assertiveness, creativity, and independence. In contrast,
most agrarian societies have a stratified, hierarchical social
structure, with landowners at the top, where obedience to
lords and masters may be essential to survival. Moreover,
farming itself depends more on adherence to tried and true
routines than on individual creativity. It is no wonder, then,
that farming parents should be motivated to beat the
willfulness out of their children and train them in lessons
of conformity and obedience.
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 31
Author's personal copy
When hunter-gatherer adults were asked about their
child-raising practices, they talked about each persons right
to make their own decisions, but they also pointed out that
young people learned through their freely chosen activities
and would begin to contribute to the bands economy, on
their own free will, when they were ready to do so. Their
view of education seems well summed up by a Ju/hoan
folk expression that can be translated roughly as Children
teach themselves(Bakeman et al. 1990).
3. Hunter-gatherer children acquired the skills of their
culture, and consolidated their knowledge, by playing
at culturally valued activities.
Hunter-gatherer children were never isolated from the
activities of adults in the band. They observed all that went on
and they heard the gossip, stories, and debates of adults. They
incorporated all this into their play, not because any adult told
them to but because they were naturally motivated to.
In response to our survey question about what the
children played at, we received many answers that showed
that children played at activities that were of economic or
social value in their culture (Gray 2009a). Digging up
tubers, fishing, smoking porcupines out of holes, cooking,
caring for infants, climbing trees, building vine ladders,
building huts, using knives and other tools, making tools,
carrying heavy loads, building rafts, making fires, defend-
ing against attacks from make-believe predators, imitating
animals (a means of identifying animals and learning their
habits), making music, dancing, storytelling, and arguing
were all mentioned by one or more respondents. The
specific lists differed from culture to culture, in accordance
with differences in the skills that were exemplified by
adults in each culture.
All of the respondents said that boys in the culture they
studied engaged in a great deal of playful hunting. The two
respondents who studied the Agta (of the Philippines)a
culture in which women as well as men regularly hunt
noted that girls as well as boys in that culture engaged in
much playful hunting. Young children, with little bows and
arrows (or other weapons, depending on the culture), might
in their play shoot at butterflies, toads, and rodents.
Eventually, as they became skilled, they might in their play
begin to kill some small animals and bring them home to
cook. Over time, playful hunting gradually became real
hunting, still in the spirit of play.
Collin Turnbull, who studied and wrote extensively
about the Mbuti (of Africas Ituri Forest), described how
Mbuti children of both sexes (age 9 and up) practice the art
of argument in their play. He wrote:
It may start through imitation of a real dispute the
children witnessed in the main camp, perhaps the
night before. They all take roles and imitate the
adults. It is almost a form of judgment for if the adults
talked their way out of the dispute the children having
performed their imitation once, are likely to drop it. If
the children detect any room for improvement,
however, they will explore that, and if the adult
argument was inept and everyone went to sleep that
night in a bad temper, then the children try and show
that they can do better, and if they cannot, then they
revert to ridicule which they play out until they are all
rolling on the ground in near hysterics. That happens
to be the way many of the most potentially violent
and dangerous disputes are settled in adult life.
(Turnbull 1982, pp. 142143)
Turnbull noted in his writings that Mbuti children would
build a whole village of play huts, some distance away
from their bands actual temporary village, and then use
that as a playground to act out scenes they had witnessed
among the adults or to improvise new ones. In her response
to our survey, Nancy Howell reported the same observation
concerning Ju/hoan children. Apparently, hunter-gatherer
children, in their play, spend a great deal of time practicing
the arts and crafts of adulthood.
4. Hunter-gatherer children and adolescents played and
explored together in age-mixed groups.
The play of hunter-gatherer children occurred almost
always in age-mixed groups. A typical group playing
together at any given time might consist of half a dozen
children ranging in age from 4 to 11, or from 7 to 15. Even
if hunter-gatherer children wanted to segregate by age, they
would rarely find more than one or two playmates within a
year or two of their own age. Because hunter-gatherer
bands were relatively small and births were widely spaced,
the number of potential playmates for any given child was
limited. As Patricia Draper put it, in her response to our
survey: Any child with enough motor and cognitive
maturity could enter into any game. Older teenagers and
adults could and did play as well, though not for as long or
with the same enthusiasm as the children.I will return
later to the issue of age mixing. I think it plays a huge role
in childrens natural means of education.
Education at a Modern Democratic School Designed
for Self-Education
At this point you might be thinking: Well, that system of
education may have been fine for hunter-gatherers, but we
are not hunter-gatherers. The educational needs of our
children today are very different.Indeed, our childrens
educational needs are in some ways quite different from
those of hunter-gatherers. For starters, we have reading,
writing, and arithmeticskills that were foreign to hunter-
32 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
gatherer cultures. Some educational researchers, including
at least one who brings an evolutionary perspective to his
work (Geary 2008), have argued that childrens natural
ways of learning are inadequate for learning the three Rs
and that is one reason why school-imposed drill is essential.
I might agree were it not for my observations and research
at a remarkable alternative school, the Sudbury Valley
School, located in Framingham, Massachusetts.
Sudbury Valley defies the usual conceptions of what a
school must be in our modern era. Here is a brief
description. The school admits students from age 4 on
through high school age, on a first-come first-served basis,
with no attention to measures of ability. It is a private day
school, but it operates on a per-pupil budget less than half
that of the local public schools. In recent years, the school
has generally had between 140 and 200 enrolled students
and nine or ten adult staff members. The school is governed
by the School Meeting, which makes all school rules, at
which each student and staff member has one vote. The
school has no curriculum, gives no tests, does not monitor
studentsprogress, and does not assign students to classes
or special spaces. All students, regardless of age, are
allowedall day long, day after dayto go wherever they
wish in the schools two buildings and ten-acre campus, to
associate with whom they please, and to do whatever they
wish as long as it does not violate any of the schools
democratically legislated rules (which serve to protect the
school community and the rights of individuals).
What the school does provide is access to tools of learning.
There are computers, books, a woodworking shop, a fully
equipped kitchen, a pond, a nearby woods, athletic equipment,
and adult staff members with expertise in a wide variety of
endeavors who provide help and instruction when asked. The
students have access to all of this, but they are never required
or coaxed to take advantage of any of these resources. Just as
is true of hunter-gatherer children and adolescents, they are
free to play and explore on their own, all day, every day, and
that is what they do. The staff at the schooland the parents
who send their children therebelieve, as hunter-gatherers
did, that children who are allowed to play and explore freely
will learn what they need to know to become effective adults.
The school has been operating in this way for 43 years. It
has hundredsof graduates, including many who did all of their
primary and secondary schooling there. Follow-up studies
including one that I conducted manyyears agoshow that the
graduates have done very well in life (Gray and Chanoff 1986;
Greenberg and Sadofsky 1992; Greenberg et al. 2005).
Those who have wanted to go to collegea group that
constitutes the majority of the graduateshave had no
difficulty getting into good colleges and doing well there,
and others have pursued good careers (in such areas as arts,
skilled crafts, information technology, and business startups)
that do not require college. Collectively, the graduates
occupy the whole range of professions that we value in our
society. The great majority of them report no regrets at all
about attending such an unusual school; they feel that the
school gave them many advantages. Relatively recently, a
worldwide Sudbury school movement has been spreading.
Today, worldwide, there are roughly 40 schools explicitly
modeled after Sudbury Valley (a list can be found at the
Sudbury Valley School website).
How do students learn at this school? Research that
my university students and I have conducted indicates
that they learn in very much the same ways that hunter-
gatherer children learn (Gray 2007). They explore and
play, in age-mixed groups as well as alone, at activities
that are valued in the culture around them, with tools that
are crucial to the culture. Whereas hunter-gatherer chil-
dren played with bows and arrows, digging sticks, and
vine ladders, Sudbury Valley students play at computers
and at games that involve the written word and numbers.
They explore not just by examining the immediate
physical and social world around them but also by reading
about the subjects that interest them and by asking
questions of staff members and other students. Sometimes,
a group of students will ask a staff member to lead a
course or regular discussion group on a topic of their
interest. In this environment, no bell tells them to stop
pursuing a passionate interest; they can delve as deeply as
they like, for as long as they like, into any subject that
they like. In the process, they develop the basic skills and
the passionate specific interests that lead, eventually, to a
career decision.
Another pool of children and adolescents learning on
their own in our society are those involved in the rapidly
growing unschoolingmovement (Kirschner 2008). These
are young people who dont attend school at all. They are
usually officially registered as homeschoolers, but in fact
are not subjected to any curriculum or tests at home
because their parents subscribe to the philosophy that
children learn best when they pursue their own interests in
their own chosen ways. I have recently collected many
stories from unschooling parents about how their children
learned to read and to calculate with numbers, and
collectively, those stories make a case very similar to that
resulting from our studies at Sudbury Valley (for an
analysis of the stories, see Gray 2010a,d). Children on
their own initiative, in a literate and numerate culture,
naturally play with words and numbers and thereby become
readers, writers, and proficient enough with math to meet
the demands of life in our culture. Sudbury Valley children
and unschooled children do not develop the fear and
loathing of math that is so common elsewhere in our
culture, so if they choose to study math formallyfor
example, to prepare to take the math SAT or ACT for
college admissionthey learn quickly, eagerly, and effi-
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 33
Author's personal copy
ciently. As is true of people in the larger culture, a few
enjoy numerical and abstract symbol play so much that they
go on to become mathematicians, and a few others enjoy
words so much that they become writers.
The Educative Instincts: Play and Exploration
In 1898, just 39 years after DarwinsOn the Origin of
Species appeared, the German philosopher and naturalist
Karl Groos published a book entitled The Play of Animals
in which he applied Darwins theory to an analysis of
animal play. Like Darwin, Groos had a sophisticated,
modern understanding of the relationship between instinct
and learning. He recognized that learning is not something
that is superimposed upon instinct, but that it itself arises
out of instinct and serves, among other things, to modify
and refine instinctive behavior patterns to suit present
conditions. Grooss central argument, in The Play of
Animals, was that mammals come into the world with
instincts to play at the vary activities that they most need to
develop in order to become effective adults. Predators play
at preying; prey animals play at dodging and darting and
getting away; monkeys play at swinging from branch to
branch in trees; males of most species play at fighting; and
so on. Grooss book has had a great impact on modern
animal behavior research. It has inspired much research into
the functions of animal play, and its thesis, with some
refinements and qualifications, is generally accepted today
by biologists who study animal play.
Three years after the appearance of The Play of Animals,
Groos (1901) published a second book, The Play of Man, in
which he extended his theory of animal play to humans.
Groos recognized that a basic difference between humans and
other mammals lies in our extraordinary dependence on
culture. He recognized that our survival depends on our
abilities, when young, to acquire the skills and knowledge
unique to the culture in which we are born. So, according to
Groos, natural selection enlarged the play drive in humans and
shaped it in a manner to include a strong element of mimicry.
Human children play not just at species-specific skills (the
kinds of skills that characterize the species everywhere), like
other mammals do, but also play at culture-specific skills.
According to Groos, children come into the world with an
innate drive to look around and see what the adults, especially
the most admired and successful adults, are doing and to
incorporate those activities into their play.
Unlike his book on animal play, Grooss book on human
play is little read today and rarely cited. In fact, it has been
allowed to go out of print. My research suggests, however,
that GroossThe Play of Man contains insights that should
be understood by every educator today. In fact, my main
criticism of Grooss work on human play is that he didnt
go far enough. He did not conceive of the idea that the
human instincts to acquire the culture around them are so
powerful that, given appropriate environmental conditions,
they can provide the primary foundation for education even
in our modern world.
In addition to play, the other powerful force for childrens
self-education is curiosity. Groos considered exploration
(motivated by curiosity) to be a category of play, but most play
researchers today consider the two to be distinct (Power 2000).
Learning can be divided at least roughly into two broad
categorieslearning to do (skill learning) and learning about
(information learning). Play serves primarily the former, and
exploration serves primarily the latter. Stated differently,
exploration provides information and play provides practice.
Just as the play drive has expanded in our species to serve
the function of cultural acquisition, so apparently has curiosity.
Children and adolescents (and adults too) everywhere, when
free, show extraordinary curiosity about the world around
them, especially the social and cultural world. That is why
gossip is a central activity to every human culture. It also
explains why children pay special attentionto the activities and
conversations of older children and adults and why, when free
to do so, they ask never-ending questions about what is going
on. Given its extraordinary importance, psychologists have
paid far too little research attention to human curiosity.
Conditions that Maximize the Power of the Educative
Instincts
Because our instincts for self-education were shaped by
natural selection during the long period when we were all
hunter-gatherers, we might expect those instincts to function
best in the context of a hunter-gatherer band or a modern
setting that replicates essential aspects of such a band. The
founders of the Sudbury Valley School did not set out to
model a hunter-gatherer band. Their aim was to create a
school in line with the fundamental tenets of American
democracy (Sudbury Valley School 1970). But, to my eyes,
the school contains precisely those elements of a hunter-
gatherer band that are most essential for childrens self-
educative instincts to operate well. Perhaps this is no
coincidence. It can be argued that hunter-gatherer societies
were the original democracies (Ingold 1999). Here, I offer a
list of what seem to me to be the most crucial conditions for
childrens educationconditions that exist both in hunter-
gatherer bands and at the Sudbury Valley School (also
presented in Gray 2008b).
Time and Space for Play and Exploration
Self-education through play and exploration requires
enormous amounts of unscheduled timetime to do
34 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
whatever one wants to do, without pressure, judgment, or
intrusion from authority figures. That time is needed to
make friends, play with ideas and materials, experience and
overcome boredom, and develop passions. In hunter-
gatherer bands, adults place few demands on children and
adolescents, partly because they recognize that young
people need to explore and play on their own to become
competent adults. The same is true at Sudbury Valley.
Self-education also requires spacespace to roam, to get
away, to explore. That space should, ideally, encompass the
full range of terrains relevant to the culture in which one is
developing. Hunter-gatherer adults trust their children to
use good judgment in deciding how far they should venture
away from others into possibly dangerous areas. At
Sudbury Valley, children are likewise trusted, within the
limits set by prudence in our modern, litigious society.
They can explore the surrounding woods, fields, and nearby
stream, and by signing out to let others know where they
are going, those who have reached a criterion age can
venture as far off campus as they choose.
Free Age Mixing Among Children
As I noted earlier, hunter-gatherer children necessarily play
in age-mixed groups as there are not enough children of any
given age for age-segregated play. At Sudbury Valley, there
are enough children that they could play just with others
close in age, but they dont. In quantitative studies, we have
observed that children commonly, of their own choice, play
across large age rangesoften of four yearsdifference or
more (Gray and Feldman 1997). Much of my own and my
university studentsresearch at Sudbury Valley has focused
on the educative value of age mixing (Feldman and Gray
1999; Gray and Feldman 2004). We have described
evidence that age-mixed play is qualitatively different from
same-age play. It is less competitive, more nurturing, and
offers unique learning opportunities both for the older and
younger participants.
Age mixing allows younger children to play at and learn
from activities that they could not do alone. As illustration,
imagine two four-year-olds trying to play a simple game of
catch (Gray 2008c). They cant do it. Neither can throw the
ball straight enough for the other to catch it, and neither can
catch well enough to snag the others wild throws. In a
world of just four-year-olds, there would be no catch. But a
four-year-old and an eight-year-old can play catch and
enjoy it. The older child can toss the ball gently into the
hands of the four-year-old so he or she has a chance to
catch it, and the older child can run and leap well enough to
catch at least some of the younger childs wild throws. In a
world of just four-year-olds, there is no catch, but in a
world that includes eight-year-olds as well as four-year-
olds, everyone has a chance to learn and enjoy this skilled
game. The same applies to all sorts of activities that stretch
the abilities of younger children, including reading. A
common scene at Sudbury Valley is one in which a group
of childrensome of whom can read and some of whom
cannotare huddled around a computer screen or playing
some other kind of game that involves reading. The older
children read aloud the words that the younger ones cannot
readnot deliberately to teach but just to keep the game
going. The result is that the younger children pick up the
words they see in front of them and hear pronounced, and
soon they are in the initial stages of reading themselves.
Age mixing benefits the older children as well as the
younger. In an age-mixed environment, all children have
the opportunity to practice being matureto practice
leading, guiding, and caring for othersthrough their
interactions with younger children. At Sudbury Valley, we
have observed countless instances in which older children
went out of their way to help much younger ones (Gray and
Feldman 2004). The presence of younger children seems to
draw out the nurturing instincts in children and adolescents
of both sexes and to promote the development of nurturing
behavior (for further evidence of this, see Ember 1973). We
also observed many scenes in which older children
explained concepts to younger onesconcepts such as
rules of a game, rules of the school, the best way to find
lost mittens, and the phonetic sounds of letters. As all
teachers know from experience, explaining a concept to
others is often the best way to stretch and consolidate ones
own understanding of that concept. In an age-mixed
environment, all children have the opportunity to learn
through teaching.
Children learn from the presence of older and younger
children even when they are not directly interacting across
age. Older children provide models that younger ones try to
emulate. At Sudbury Valley, young children become
interested in reading not so much because they see adults
reading but more because they see children a little older
than themselves reading. Five-year-olds arent particularly
interested in emulating adults; adults are too far ahead of
them, too much in a different world. But five-year-olds do
very much want to be like the cool seven- and eight-year-
olds that they see around them. If those seven- and eight-
year-olds are reading and discussing books, or are playing
computer games that require reading, then the five-year-
olds want to do that too. The same applies to the whole
realm of activities that occur at the schooltree climbing,
cooking, playing musical instruments, and so on.
Just as younger children are attracted to the more
sophisticated activities of older ones, older children are
often attracted to the creative and imaginative activities of
younger ones. At Sudbury Valley, we have frequently
observed teenagers playing with paints, clay, or blocks, or
playing make-believe games, often with younger children.
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 35
Author's personal copy
These are activities that most teenagers elsewhere in our
culture would have long since abandoned. Through such
play, the teenagers become wonderful artists, builders,
storytellers, and creative thinkers.
Comparable studies of age mixing in hunter-gatherer
bands have not been conducted, but I suspect that all of the
advantages of age-mixed play that we have observed at
Sudbury Valley would occur in hunter-gatherer bands as
well, though the specific activities practiced would be
different.
Access to Knowledgeable and Caring Adults
In hunter-gatherer bands, the adult world is not segregated
from the childrens world. Children see what adults do and
incorporate that into their play. They also hear the adults
stories, discussions, and debates, and they learn from what
they hear. When they need adult help, or have questions
that cannot be answered by other children, they can go to
any of the adults in the band. All of the adults care for
them. Most of the adults, in fact, are literally their aunts and
uncles.
At Sudbury Valley, too, adults and children mingle
freely, though the ratio of adults to children is much smaller
there than in a hunter-gatherer band. There is no place in
the school where staff members can go but students cannot.
Students can listen into any adult discussions and observe
whatever the adults are doing, and they can join in if they
wish. Students who need help of any kind can go to any of
the staff members. A child who needs a lap to sit on, or a
shoulder to cry on, or personal advice, or the answer to
some technical question that he hasnt been able to find on
his own, or (occasionally) more prolonged help in the form
of a tutorial or course, knows just which adult will best
satisfy his or her need. The adults are not literally aunts and
uncles, but they are much like aunts and uncles. They know
all of the students over the entire span of time that they are
students at the school (unlike teachers in a conventional
school who know each set of students for just one year) and
take pride in watching them develop. Since the staff
members must be reelected each year by vote that includes
all of the students in the school, they are necessarily people
who like kids and are liked by kids.
Access to Equipment and Freedom to Play
with that Equipment
To learn to use the tools of a culture, people need access to
those tools. Hunter-gatherer children played with knives,
digging sticks, bows and arrows, snares, musical instru-
ments, dugout canoes, and all of the other items of
equipment that were crucial to their culture. At Sudbury
Valley, children have access to a wide range of the
equipment that is of most general use to people in our
culture, including computers, woodworking equipment,
cooking equipment, art materials, sporting equipment of
various types, and many walls filled with books.
Free Exchange of Ideas
Intellectual development occurs best in a setting where
people can share ideas freely, without censorship or fear of
being ostracized. According to anthropologistsreports,
hunter-gatherers were non-dogmatic in their beliefs, even in
their religious beliefs (Gray 2009a; Thomas 2006). People
could say what they please, without fear, and ideas that had
any consequence to the group were debated endlessly. The
same is true at Sudbury Valley. The school has deliberately
refrained from becoming aligned with any particular
religious or political ideology. All ideas are on the table.
In this kind of environment, an idea is something to think
about and debate, not something to memorize and feed
back on a test. Daniel Greenberg, the schools leading
philosopher, has described the school as a free market-
place of ideas(Sudbury Valley School 1970). Children
who may not hear much discussion of politics or religion at
home hear it at school, and they hear every side of every
issue.
Freedom from Bullying
To feel free to explore and play, a person must feel safe,
free from harassment and bullying. Such freedom occurred
to a remarkable extent in hunter-gatherer bands, and it does
also at Sudbury Valley. According to anthropologists, the
close-knit personal relationships, the age mixing, and the
non-competitive, egalitarian ethos of hunter-gatherer cul-
tures worked effectively to prevent serious bullying (Gray
2009a). If an older or bigger child seemed to be picking on
a younger or smaller one, others would step in and quickly
stop it. The same occurs at Sudbury Valley, and in addition,
research at the school suggests that the simple presence of
young children has a pacifying effect on older children
(Gray and Feldman 2004). Moreover, at Sudbury Valley,
the schools democratically created rules and judicial
system, in which children of all ages are involved, prevent
serious bullying. Students who feel harassed in any way
can bring upthe offender, to appear before the Judicial
Committee, comprising school members of all ages. This
contrasts sharply with the case in many conventional
schools where bullying is a way of life (Gray 2010c, e).
Students there who report bullying are snitches or tattle-
tales, and teachers can often get away with bullying
because they make the rules and are not subject to them.
Because students make the rules at Sudbury Valley and
have responsibility for enforcing them, they have far more
36 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
respect for the rules than do students in a standard school
who have little or no voice in rule creation or enforcement
(for further elaboration, see Gray 2010c).
Immersion in a Democratic Community
Hunter-gatherer bands and the Sudbury Valley School are,
in quite different ways, democracies. As I noted earlier,
hunter-gatherer bands did not have chiefs who make
decisions for the group. Instead, all group decisions were
made through long discussions until all who cared came to
an agreement. Anybody, including children, could take part
in those discussions. Sudbury Valley is administered
through a formal democratic process, involving discussions
and votes of the School Meeting, where each student and
staff member who chooses to attend has an equal vote.
Immersion in the democratic process endows each person
with a sense of responsibility that helps motivate education.
If my voice counts, if I have a real say in what the group
does and how it operates, then Id better think things
through carefully and speak wisely. Im responsible not just
for myself but also for my community, so thats a good
reason for me to educate myself in the things that matter to
my community.
Why Our Standard Approach to Schooling Has Never
Worked Well
Our standard approach to schooling did not arise from
scientific research on how children learn, and it certainly
did not arise from any research that brought Darwinian
evolutionary insights to bear. It came about through an
historical process, which I will briefly summarize here (for
further detail and documentation, see Gray 2008a).
With the advent of agriculture, childrens as well as
adultslives changed dramatically from what they had been
in hunter-gatherer times. Agriculture brought landowner-
ship, status hierarchies, and, most important for our
considerations, servitude and slavery. In these conditions,
human willfulness and the spirit of individual autonomy
were threats to survival. This cultural change came about
much too rapidly for natural selection to play a role. Babies
continued to be born, and still are, with all of the instincts
for self-determination and self-education that served so well
in hunter-gatherer days. To get children to abide by the new
rules of unquestioned conformity and obedience, willful-
ness had to be beaten out of children. Most children spent
most of each day at unskilled labor, doing much the same
kind of work that they would continue to do for lords and
masters in adulthood. This was the state of affairs through
the feudal Middle Ages and beyond. The Industrial
Revolution did not immediately improve these conditions.
In fact, for a while, it made things worse. Children
continued to be treated essentially as slaves, now working
in unhealthy, dirty, crowded factories rather than in fields
where they at least had experienced fresh air and sunshine.
For a variety of reasons, some religious and some
secular, the idea of universal, compulsory education
gradually arose and spread, beginning in the seventeenth
century in some parts of the world. Education was
understood as inculcation in a prescribed curriculum that
was deemed essential to the saving of souls, the generation
of patriots, or the production of good factory workers. With
the rise of schooling, people began to think of learning as
childrenswork, which they must be forced to do. The same
power-assertive methods that had been used to make
children work in fields and factories were quite naturally
transferred to the classroom. Nobody imagined that
learning could be play. By this time in history, childrens
own willfulness, playfulness, and curiosity were commonly
seen as works of the devil; surely, those aspects of
childhood were worthless for education. The literature of
early education is filled with accounts of the duty of
schoolmasters to beat children who disobeyed or failed to
learn the prescribed lessons. Play was seen as the enemy of
education, not the vehicle. The attitude of eighteenth
century school authorities toward play is well reflected in
John Wesleys rules for Wesleyan schools, which included
the statement: As we have no play days, so neither do we
allow any time for play on any day; for he that plays as a
child will play as a man(quoted by Mulhern 1959, p.
383).
As time has moved on, the methods of schooling have
become less harsh (at least less corporally harsh), but basic
assumptions have not changed. Learning continues to be
defined as childrens work, and power-assertive means are
used to make children do that work. A case can be made
and has been made by Chudacoff (2007)that the peak of
childrens freedom in the United States was reached in the
early to mid-twentieth century when most children had
been freed from long hours of farm or factory work, and
school and school-like activities occupied much less of
their time than they do today. Over the past 50 or 60 years,
schooling and school-like activities (such as organized, age-
segregated adult-directed sports) have expanded to take
over increasing portions of childrens time, leaving less and
less opportunity for children to bring their hunter-gatherer
instincts to bear in their own education. During that same
period, we have seen dramatic increases in childhood
anxiety, depression, suicide, obesity, and other mental and
physical ailments that can be attributed at least partly to the
stress of continuous evaluation by adults and the lack of
play (for documentation, see Gray 2010b).
Despite all of this time in school and our cultures
extreme emphasis on schooling, the evidence is that
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 37
Author's personal copy
children do not learn much in school. I, like other college
professors, must more or less start from scratch in teaching
college courses; I cannot assume that the students remem-
ber anything from comparable courses in high school. For
example, students spend thousands of hours on math drill in
school, and yet, a year or two out, they know little more
math than what they would have learned anyway, just in the
course of life. Worse, many of them, because of their school
experience, have developed such fears of math that they
avoid it like the plague (Burns 1998). Our schools do so
poorly because they are settings that more or less
deliberately deprive children of all of the conditions,
described above, that are essential for self-education:
&Children are not free to play, explore, and roam in
school. They are instead required to follow a curriculum
chosen for them by others and to spend most of the day
sitting in their seats.
&Children are strictly segregated by age in school, almost
completely deprived of opportunities to interact with
older and younger children, from whom they have the
most to learn.
&In the classroom, there is typically only one adult, who
may or may not be a caring person, and who may or
may not be approachable by students seeking help or
comfort. Moreover, that adult is often not seen by the
children as a full person, but as someone playing a
certain role, that of teacher. Models of other professions
and interests are completely lacking.
&Children may be exposed, in school, to some of the
equipment valued by our culture, but the exposure
typically occurs in ways that are controlled by the adults
and do not permit the sort of free play that promotes
deep and lasting learning.
&Free exchange of ideas is cut off in school, by the
prescriptions of the curriculum and by the tests that
provide rightand wronganswers to essentially
every question.
&Because of the age segregation and the competitive
environment and the lack of freedom to leave when one
feels harassed, bullying and anxiety are part and parcel
of many childrens everyday school experience.
&Our standard schools are not, by any stretch of
imagination, democratic communities. They are highly
stratified top-down structures, with students at the very
bottom of the hierarchy.
So, what we do in our schools is to deprive children of
all of the conditions that they need to educate themselves,
and then we try to teach them something. No wonder the
teachers job is so hard. No wonder our children learn and
remember so little of what they are taught. No wonder most
children are less happy in school than almost anywhere else
(Csikszentmihalyi and Hunter 2003; Herman et al. 2009).
Readers familiar with educational theory know that some
of the criticisms of standard schooling presented in this
article are similar to criticisms that have long been raised,
without any explicit consideration of principles of evolu-
tion, by progressive educational theoristsfrom Rousseau
on through Montessori, Dewey, and the modern construc-
tivists (who emphasize Piagets ideas about child develop-
ment). It is noteworthy, however, that none of those
theorists suggest the degree of departure from standard
schooling that is described in this article and represented in
practice in hunter-gatherer cultures and Sudbury schools.
Progressive educational theories all leave adult educators
clearly in charge of childrens learning. In schools founded
on those theories, teachers attempt to use and direct
childrens drives to play and explore in ways designed to
meet the needs of an established curriculum, chosen by
educators, not by children.
The idea that children themselves are motivated to figure
out and learn what they need to know to do well in the
culture and can successfully direct their own education, if
provided with an appropriate environment, is not a
component of any of the educational theories discussed in
contemporary schools of education (see Gray 2009b; Gray
and Chanoff 1984). Moreover, the list of requirements for
an educational setting in which childrens educative
instincts can operate optimally, presented in this article,
cannot be found in any previous writings on education.
That list comes directly from a comparison of the
education-promoting elements of hunter-gatherer bands
with those present in a radically alternative modern-day
school where children have long educated themselves
successfully.
Directions for Further Research
The appropriately skeptical reader no doubt has many
questions concerning the ideas presented in this article, and
at least some of those questions provide directions for
further research.
The most direct evidence presented in this article for the
claim that the hunter-gatherer model of education can work
in modern times derives from studies of the Sudbury Valley
School and its graduates. But Sudbury Valley is just one
relatively small school. Although other Sudbury model
schools exist, systematic research at those schools is thus
far lacking. Moreover, the students at Sudbury Valley are a
self-selected group that, for one reason or another, chose to
attend a very unusual school and whose parents agreed to
that choice. Some enrolled at an early age because their
parents believed in this philosophy of education from the
outset, and others came at some point after starting school
elsewhere, often because of some problem related to their
38 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
standard schooling, such as repeated failure, persistent
rebellion, or general unhappiness (Gray and Chanoff
1986). Although the students seem to me and to the
Sudbury Valley staff to encompass the whole normal range
of human personalityand they seem to learn well in the
school regardless of personalitywe presently have no
way to prove that. Some students at some point leave the
school and return to a traditional public school, and to date
there are no systematic data on why they leave. It is
possible that the school does not work so well for some as
for others, and research aimed at understanding that would
be helpful. Hard evidence that the school works for students
over the whole normal personality range would increase
interest in the school as a general model for contemporary
education. On the other hand, evidence that it works well
for some but not all students would encourage those who
urge more choice in schools, so students could sort
themselves out according to their needs.
Aside from further research at Sudbury schools, the
analysis presented in this article suggests the need for more
studies of childrens self-directed play out of school, aimed
at understanding how such play leads to acquisition of
culturally valued skills, knowledge, and attitudes. A model
for such research is the work of Mitra (2003,2005) who
documented how thousands of impoverished, often illiterate
children in both urban and rural areas of India quickly and
efficiently taught themselves how to use a computer, when
they had access to one, through their social play and
exploration, with no adult guidance at all. Mitras research
has drawn a good deal of attention in the popular press, but
so far has not inspired comparable large-scale research by
others on childrens capacities to educate themselves.
The list of environmental attributes that optimize child-
rens abilities to educate themselves suggests further
directions for research. Most glaring, perhaps, is the need
for more research on the value of age-mixed interactions
among children and adolescents. Today, children are
segregated by age more than they ever have been in the
past. We not only have age-segregated school classrooms
but also age-segregated after-school programs, sports
programs, camping experiences, playgroups, and so on.
Opportunities for free age-mixed play in neighborhoods
have rapidly declined over the last several decades as
parents have become increasingly fearful of allowing
children to play freely outdoors. The reduced size of
nuclear families and reduced significance of extended
families (because of their geographical dispersion) have
also reduced opportunities for age-mixed play.
Perhaps partly because of such age segregation in the
society, very little research has been conducted on child-
rens age-mixed interactions, and almost no research has
been conducted on interactions among children or adoles-
cents varying in age by more than two or three years (see
Gray and Feldman 2004). If it is true, as suggested in this
article, that age mixing is a key component of childrens
abilities to learn through their self-initiated activities, then
age mixing should move to the forefront of topics for
research in developmental psychology and education.
Connections to the EvoS Consortium
As this special issue of Evolution: Education & Outreach is
dedicated to the EvoS (Evolutionary Studies) Consortium,
it seems fitting to end with a note connecting the present
article to EvoS. The inspiration for the article came, in part,
from the positive reactions to a talk I gave to the EvoS
group at Binghamton University in the fall of 2009. EvoS is
dedicated to the principle that an evolutionary perspective
can inform and improve critical thought and research across
the whole range of academic pursuits. The idea that
evolutionary thinking can inform thought and research on
education is doubly relevant to EvoS as education is not
only a topic of study in academia but is also the vehicle by
which academia progresses.
I was impressed in my presentation at Binghamton by
the playful and inquisitive spirit of those who attended my
talk and by the age mixingamong undergraduates,
graduate students, and facultythat occurred during the
long dinner and questioning period that followed the talk.
Here was a place for free exchange of ideas, where people
at various positions on the academic road could learn from
and be inspired by one another. In one of his blog posts
(found at evostudies.org), David Sloan Wilson (2009)
describes well the central role of age mixing in the success
of the EvoS program. As Wilsons thoughts make clear, an
evolutionary approach is valuable for understanding and
improving education across the lifespan.
References
Bakeman R, Adamson LB, Konner M, Barr R. !Kung infancy: the social
context of object exploration. Child Dev. 1990;61:794809.
Barry H, Child I, Bacon MK. The relation of child training to
subsistence economy. Am Anthropol. 1959;61:5163.
Bock J, Johnson SE. Subsistence ecology and play among the
Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana, Hum Nat. 2004;15:6381.
Burns M. Math: facing an American phobia. Sausalito: Math
Solutions Publications; 1998.
Chudacoff HP. Children at play: an American history. New York: New
York University Press; 2007.
Csikszentmihalyi M, Hunter J. Happiness in everyday life: the uses of
experience sampling. J Happiness Stud. 2003;4:18599.
DeVore I, Murdock GP, Whiting JWM. Discussions, part VII: Are the
hunter-gatherers a cultural type? In: Lee R, DeVore I, editors.
Man the hunter. Chicago: Aldine; 1968. p. 3359.
Draper P. Social and economic constraints on child life among the
!Kung. In: Lee R, DeVore I, editors. Kalahari hunter-gatherers:
Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840 39
Author's personal copy
studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press; 1976. p. 199217.
Ember C. Feminine task assignment and the social behavior of boys.
Ethos. 1973;1:42439.
Feldman J, Gray P. Some educational benefits of freely chosen age
mixing among children and adolescents. Phi Delta Kappan.
1999;80:50712.
Geary DD. An evolutionarily informed education science. Educ
Psychol. 2008;43:17995.
Gosso Y, Otta E, de Lima M, Morais M, Ribeiro FJL, Bussab VSR.
Play in hunter-gatherer societies. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK,
editors. The nature of play: great apes and humans. New York:
Guilford; 2005. p. 21353.
Gould RA. Yiwara: foragers of the Australian desert. New York:
Charles Scribner; 1969.
Gray P. Natures powerful tutors: the educative functions of free play
and exploration. Eye Psi Chi. 2007;12(1):1821.
Gray P. A brief history of education. Blog at Psychology Today:
Freedom to Learn, Aug. 20, 2008. http://www.psychologytoday.
com/blog/freedom-learn; 2008a.
Gray P. The natural environment for childrens education: How the
Sudbury Valley School is like a hunter-gatherer band. Blog at
Psychology Today: Freedom to Learn, Sept. 3, 2008. http://www.
psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn; 2008b.
Gray P. The value of age-mixed play. Education Week, 16 April;
2008c.
Gray P. Play as a foundation for hunter-gatherer social existence. Am J
Play. 2009a;1:476522.
Gray P. Rousseaus errors: They persist today in educational theory.
Blog at Psychology Today: Freedom to Learn, Feb. 12, 2009.
http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn; 2009b.
Gray P. Children teach themselves to read. Blog at Psychology Today:
Freedom to Learn, Feb. 24, 2010. http://www.psychologytoday.
com/blog/freedom-learn; 2010a.
Gray P. The dramatic rise in anxiety and depression in children and
adolescents: is it connected to the decline in play and rise in
schooling? Blog at Psychology Today: Freedom to Learn, Jan.
26, 2010. http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn;
2010b.
Gray P. Freedom from bullying: how a school can be a moral
community. Blog at Psychology Today: Freedom to Learn, June
8, 2010. http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn;
2010c.
Gray P. Kids learn math easily when they control their own learning.
Blog at Psychology Today: Freedom to Learn, April 15, 2010.
http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/freedom-learn; 2010d.
Gray P. School bullying: A tragic cost of forced schooling and
autocratic school governance. Blog at Psychology Today:
Freedom to Learn, May 12, 2010. http://www.psychologytoday.
com/blog/freedom-learn; 2010e.
Gray P, Chanoff D. When play is learning: a school designed for self-
directed education. Phi Delta Kappan. 1984;65:60811.
Gray P, Chanoff D. Democratic schooling: what happens to young
people who have charge of their own education? Am J Educ.
1986;94:182213.
Gray P, Feldman J. Patterns of age mixing and gender mixing among
children and adolescents at an ungraded democratic school.
Merrill Palmer Q. 1997;43:6786.
Gray P, Feldman J. Playing in the zone of proximal development:
qualities of self-directed age mixing between adolescents and
young children at a democratic school. Am J Educ.
2004;110:10845.
Greenberg D, Sadofsky M. Legacy of trust: life after the Sudbury
Valley School experience. Framingham: Sudbury Valley School
Press; 1992.
Greenberg D, Sadofsky M, Lempka J. The pursuit of happiness: the
lives of Sudbury Valley alumni. Framingham: Sudbury Valley
School Press; 2005.
Groos K. The play of animals. New York: Appleton; 1898.
Groos K. The play of man. New York: Appleton; 1901.
Guemple L. Teaching social relations to Inuit children. In: Ingold T,
Riches D, Woodburn J, editors. Hunters and gatherers 2:
property, power, and ideology. Oxford: Berg; 1988. p. 13049.
Herman KC, Reinke W, Traylor PJ, KB AG. Childhood depression:
rethinking the role of school. Psychol Sch. 2009;46:43346.
Ingold T. On the social relations of the hunter-gatherer band. In: Lee
RB, Daly R, editors. The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and
gatherers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999. p.
399410.
Blurton Jones N, Hawkes K, Draper P. Differences between Hazda
and !Kung childrens work: original affluence or practical
reason? In: Burch Jr ES, Ellanna LJ, editors. Key issues in
hunter-gatherer research. Oxford: Berg; 1994. p. 189215.
Kelly RL. The foraging spectrum: Diversity in hunter-gatherer
lifeways. Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press; 1995.
Kirschner DH. Producing unshoolers: learning through living in a
U.S. education movement. Doctoral dissertation, Department of
Anthropology, University of Pennsylvania; 2008.
Lee RB. Reflections on primitive communism. In: Ingold T, Riches D,
Woodburn J, editors. Hunters and gatherers 1. Oxford: Berg;
1988. p. 25268.
Lee RB, DeVore I. Problems in the study of hunters and gatherers. In:
Lee RB, Lee I, editors. Man the hunter. Chicago: Aldine; 1968.
p. 312.
Liedloff J. The continuum concept, revised edition. New York: Knopf;
1977.
Mitra S. Minimally invasive education: a progress report on the hole-
in-the-wallexperiments. Br J Educ Technol. 2003;34:36771.
Mitra S. Self-organizing systems for mass computer literacy: findings
from the hole in the wallexperiments. Int J Dev Issues.
2005;4:7181.
Mulhern J. A history of education: a social interpretation. 2nd ed. New
York: Ronald Press; 1959.
Power TG. Play and exploration in children and animals. Mahwah:
Erlbaum; 2000.
Sahlins M. Stone age economics. Chicago: Aldine; 1972.
Sudbury Valley School. The crisis in American education: an analysis and
a proposal. Framingham: Sudbury Valley School Press; 1970.
Thomas EM. The old way. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux; 2006.
Turnbull CM. The ritualization of potential conflict between the sexes
among the Mbuti. In: Leacock EB, Lee RB, editors. Politics and
history in band societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press; 1982. p. 13355.
Wilson DS. On the importance of mixed-age learning and how to
make the most of it in an EvoS program. EvoS Blog at http://
evostudies.org/blog/?p=107; 2009.
40 Evo Edu Outreach (2011) 4:2840
Author's personal copy
... A lack of recognition of these differences, or of the potential value of the approaches, means that education is equated with becoming like the dominant group and leaving their own culture, language, knowledge and values behind. We argue that, rather than being viewed as a barrier to participation in the education system, hunter-gatherer worldviews and the educational approaches that sustain them should be recognised as legitimate in themselves and promoted wherever possible (see also Gray 2011). ...
... Learning, on the other hand, most commonly occurs within hunter-gatherer communities through processes such as observation, imitation, participation and innovation (Lew-Levy et al 2017). Play in mixed age and gender-groups has been recognised as an important aspect of children's socialisation (Lew-Levy et al 2017; see also Gray 2011;Salali et al 2019). Due to small group size, and emphasis on personal autonomy and sociopolitical and gender egalitarianism, hunter-gatherer education has been characterised by little to no coercion on part of the parents, and by children's active role in navigating the learning process, depending on their own motivation, interest and abilities (Boyette & Hewlett 2018;Lew-Levy et al 2017). ...
... Due to small group size, and emphasis on personal autonomy and sociopolitical and gender egalitarianism, hunter-gatherer education has been characterised by little to no coercion on part of the parents, and by children's active role in navigating the learning process, depending on their own motivation, interest and abilities (Boyette & Hewlett 2018;Lew-Levy et al 2017). Importantly, these characteristicspersonal autonomy, strong sense of social belonging, self-motivation -form the foundation of successful educational practices according to recent meta-studies and comprehensive research (see for example Deci & Ryan 1987;Gallois et al 2017;Gray 2011;Ryan & Deci 2000. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article provides an overview of some of the main themes that have emerged in the research on hunter gatherers and education. The term 'education' refers both to schooling, and to the traditional pedagogical modes of knowledge transmission that hunter-gatherer communities have developed and maintained over millennia. Formal education plays a crucial, yet complicated role for contemporary hunter-gatherers; it is considered to be a foundational element for economic and social development, yet also continues to be a tool of assimilation. Participation in schooling can also conflict with local livelihood strategies, culture and knowledge. While this is the case for many marginalised groups, hunter-gatherers are one of the most marginalised and most vulnerable groups, and face some of the most challenging problems with formal education. This paper examines these issues from a human rights perspective, and within a global context. We describe the main challenges that hunter-gatherers face regarding participation in formal education, including physical, financial, social, cultural and structural barriers, and highlight issues of both inclusion and assimilation. We also examine traditional knowledge and educational approaches among hunter-gatherer communities, calling for a much greater appreciation of the importance and relevance to current global concerns.
... It is uncontroversial to describe that the topic of school improvement from an evolutionary perspective is not on the radar for the international field of evolution education, which is a sub-field specialization of science and biology education. Specialized journals including Evolution: Education & Outreach, Science & Education, Journal of Biological Education, and The American Biology Teacher have limited relevant literature on record (the only even partially relevant examples we have been able to find include Gray, 2011;Grobstein & Lesnick, 2011;Eirdosh & Hanisch 2020a, b). This is not surprising, as the field of evolution education overwhelmingly lacks a focus or research infrastructure for engaging general education students in the evolution of human behavior, cognition, and culture, more generally (Ziadie & Andrews, 2018). ...
... In this paradigm, upper grade students interview their peers and other school stakeholders around their perceptions of "a school where students make the rules". Using various design elements from the evolution-informed Self-Directed Education model of schooling (see Gray, 2011), student interviewers explain to participants a range of egalitarian and autonomy-supportive design elements of this school model, explaining that scientists continue to debate whether this school is a good model for all humans on the basis of our evolutionary history or simply "what is best for humans". Interviewers then probe the participants' knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs about this model and perceptions on the scientific debate. ...
Chapter
Human culture changes over time and varies across space. Two main approaches to study cultural evolution have developed in the last fifty years: human behavioural ecology and a suite of perspectives centred on the role of cultural transmission. The latter are often confusingly referred to with the name of the phenomenon they are trying to explain, ‘cultural evolution’. We argue that this is unhelpful and is generating confusion, including the claim that human behavioural ecology disregards cultural evolution. The aim of behavioural ecology is to explain human behaviours, and the vast majority of them are at least to some extent cultural. In addition, culture forms part of the ecology that determines the costs and benefits associated with adopting a behaviour. Thus, human behavioural ecologists have studied cultural evolution from the very beginning, even though they have not focussed on social learning. We explore three examples in detail: kinship systems, religious institutions, and witchcraft belief. We then use the framework offered by Tinbergen’s [1963, Z Tierpsychol, 20(4), 410-433] four evolutionary questions about behaviour to explain how human behavioural ecology and cultural transmission approaches can fruitfully coexist and complement each other. Moreover, we discuss several difficulties with cultural transmission approaches and highlight how the human behavioural ecological view of cultural evolution sometimes diverges from them. We conclude by suggesting that the field can move forward and achieve greater synthesis by exploring how selective processes acting on biological fitness differ from those acting on cultural fitness – and how the two might interact in the cultural evolution of human behaviours. Link to preprint: https://osf.io/u47tw/
... Cognitive. Cognitive ability is a classical benchmark for a child to be considered to master a learning material, so teachers who are one component of education must be able to optimize the learning outcomes of their students, primarily in the cognitive domain (Colorado et al., 2021;Gray, 2011). The process of improving students' cognitive domains can be achieved by applying various approaches and learning models that can increase students' activities, interests, and creativity in a pleasant atmosphere and under various conditions. ...
... The teaching and learning process is carried out by emphasizing various factors, for example, in the learning process after the process of giving awards to students who get the highest grades/scores, whether the grades/scores are obtained from group grades or individual student grades through quizzes given by the teacher on developmental material and growth. The learning process carried out from the observations of students felt very happy when given an award, and students became more active and more motivated to be able to work together in their groups (Gray, 2011;Rice et al., 2011). The process of awarding is critical because it is significant for students to obtain high scores, both individual scores and group scores. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to determine the effect of the STAD-type cooperative model with the help of Crossword Puzzles on students' biology learning outcomes, especially on Development and Growth material in terms of Cognitive aspects. The population in this study amounted to 5 classes, and a sample of 2 classes was taken randomly. The study was conducted in two classes which were grouped as experimental class and control class. In the experimental class, learning is carried out using a STAD-type cooperative model with the help of crossword puzzle worksheets, while in the control class, learning is carried out using a conventional model research data obtained from the test of learning outcomes for cognitive aspects. Student learning outcomes on cognitive aspects were analyzed by t-test. The results of data analysis show that the learning outcomes of cognitive domain biology students whose learning uses the STAD-type cooperative model with the help of crossword puzzle worksheets are higher than students whose learning uses the conventional model.
... Additionally, we are unable to explain these differences in emotional tone by referring to ethnotheories, because chimpanzee societies are hierarchically organized whereas the Aka are more egalitarian, like many hunting and gathering human groups. The high frequencies of joint engagement without emotion could be due to the high tolerance of infants in these two groups (e.g., infants are rarely punished in chimpanzee societies, see Goodall, 1968Goodall, , 1986 or in the Aka communities, see Gray, 2011;Lancy, 2012). Perhaps the lack of emotion during infant joint engagement in some settings simply reflects partners lack of desire to control the infant, allowing them to do as they please. ...
Article
Joint attention (JA) is an early manifestation of social cognition, commonly described as interactions in which an infant looks or gestures to an adult female to share attention about an object, within a positive emotional atmosphere. We label this description the JA phenotype . We argue that characterizing JA in this way reflects unexamined assumptions which are, in part, due to past developmental researchers' primary focus on western, middle‐class infants and families. We describe a range of cultural variations in caregiving practices, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories as an essential initial step in viewing joint attention within inclusive and contextualized perspectives. We begin the process of conducting a decolonized study of JA by considering the core construct of joint attention (i.e., triadic connectedness) and adopting culturally inclusive definitions (labeled joint engagement [JE]). Our JE definitions allow for attention and engagement to be expressed in visual and tactile modalities (e.g., for infants experiencing distal or proximal caregiving), with various social partners (e.g., peers, older siblings, mothers), with a range of shared topics (e.g., representing diverse socialization goals, and socio‐ecologies with and without toys), and with a range of emotional tone (e.g., for infants living in cultures valuing calmness and low arousal, and those valuing exuberance). Our definition of JE includes initiations from either partner (to include priorities for adult‐led or child‐led interactions). Our next foundational step is making an ecological commitment to naturalistic observations (Dahl, 2017, Child Dev Perspect, 11 (2), 79–84): We measure JE while infants interact within their own physical and social ecologies. This commitment allows us to describe JE as it occurs in everyday contexts, without constraints imposed by researchers. Next, we sample multiple groups of infants drawn from diverse socio‐ecological settings. Moreover, we include diverse samples of chimpanzee infants to compare with diverse samples of human infants, to investigate the extent to which JE is unique to humans, and to document diversity both within and between species. We sampled human infants living in three diverse settings. U.K. infants ( n = 8) were from western, middle‐class families living near universities in the south of England. Nso infants ( n = 12) were from communities of subsistence farmers in Cameroon, Africa. Aka infants ( n = 10) were from foraging communities in the tropical rain forests of Central African Republic, Africa. We coded behavioral details of JE from videotaped observations (taken between 2004 and 2010). JE occurred in the majority of coded intervals ( Mdn = 68%), supporting a conclusion that JE is normative for human infants. The JA phenotype, in contrast, was infrequent, and significantly more common in the U.K. ( Mdn = 10%) than the other groups ( Mdn < 3%). We found significant within‐species diversity in JE phenotypes (i.e., configurations of predominant forms of JE characteristics). We conclude that triadic connectedness is very common in human infants, but there is significant contextualization of behavioral forms of JE. We also studied chimpanzee infants living in diverse socio‐ecologies. The PRI/Zoo chimpanzee infants ( n = 7) were from captive, stable groups of mixed ages and sexes, and included 4 infants from the Chester Zoo, U.K. and 3 from the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Japan. The Gombe chimpanzee infants ( n = 12) were living in a dynamically changing, wild community in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania, Africa. Additionally, we include two Home chimpanzee infants who were reared from birth by a female scientist, in the combined U.S., middle‐class contexts of home and university cognition laboratory. JE was coded from videotaped observations (taken between 1993 and 2006). JE occurred during the majority of coded intervals ( Mdn = 64%), consistent with the position that JE is normative for chimpanzee infants. The JA phenotype, in contrast, was rare, but more commonly observed in the two Home chimpanzee infants (in 8% and 2% of intervals) than in other chimpanzee groups ( Mdns = 0%). We found within‐species diversity in the configurations comprising the JE phenotypes. We conclude that triadic connectedness is very common in chimpanzee infants, but behavioral forms of joint engagement are contextualized. We compared JE across species, and found no species‐uniqueness in behavioral forms, JE characteristics, or JE phenotypes. Both human and chimpanzee infants develop contextualized social cognition. Within‐species diversity is embraced when triadic connectedness is described with culturally inclusive definitions. In contrast, restricting definitions to the JA phenotype privileges a behavioral form most valued in western, middle‐class socio‐ecologies, irrespective of whether the interactions involve human or chimpanzee infants. Our study presents a model for how to decolonize an important topic in developmental psychology. Decolonization is accomplished by defining the phenomenon inclusively, embracing diversity in sampling, challenging claims of human‐uniqueness, and having an ecological commitment to observe infant social cognition as it occurs within everyday socio‐ecological contexts. It is essential that evolutionary and developmental theories of social cognition are re‐built on more inclusive and decolonized empirical foundations.
... In this paradigm, upper grade students interview their peers and other school stakeholders around their perceptions of "a school where students make the rules". Using various design elements from the evolution-informed Self-Directed Education model of schooling (see Gray 2011), student interviewers explain to participants a range of egalitarian and autonomy-supportive design elements of this school model, explaining that scientists continue to debate whether this school is a good model for all humans on the basis of our evolutionary history or simply "what is best for humans". Interviewers then probe the participants' knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs about this model and perceptions on the scientific debate. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
A generalized conceptualization of evolutionary processes allows for a view of the cognitive, behavioral, and cultural variation in our everyday lives as elements of diverse evolving systems. Such a view invites questions about how cultural evolutionary processes may favor or hinder the expression of variant thoughts and behaviors, any of which may be more or less valued by any given community. From an educational perspective, this implies an untapped potential for engaging students in understanding the cultural evolutionary dynamics of their everyday lives, schools, and broader communities. As a strategy to engage this potential, the Community Science Lab at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology is developing a unique model of Community-Based Cultural Evolution (CBCE) for inter-institutional collaboration at the intersection of evolution education and applied school improvement efforts. Using advances in teaching for conceptual understanding and transfer of learning, the CBCE model aims to empower students to clarify, investigate, and collaboratively influence the cultural evolutionary dynamics of their own school and surrounding communities. The relationship between students' evolving intuitive theories of school improvement, and the evolving scientific theories of school improvement scientists, provides a framework for understanding the development of student conceptions of cultural (and, perhaps, biological) change more generally. This chapter provides a conceptual foundation for exploring the claim that engaging students in reflecting on the cognitive, behavioral, and cultural evolutionary processes in their everyday lives provides new opportunities for school improvement and interdisciplinary evolution education initiatives. The practical and systemic challenges of this approach are clarified and future directions are outlined.
Chapter
A generalized conceptualization of evolutionary processes allows for a view of the cognitive, behavioral, and cultural variation in our everyday lives as elements of diverse evolving systems. Such a view invites questions about how cultural evolutionary processes may favor or hinder the expression of variant thoughts and behaviors, any of which may be more or less valued by any given community. From an educational perspective, this implies an untapped potential for engaging students in understanding the cultural evolutionary dynamics of their everyday lives, schools, and broader communities. As a strategy to engage this potential, the Community Science Lab at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology is developing a unique model of Community-Based Cultural Evolution (CBCE) for inter-institutional collaboration at the intersection of evolution education and applied school improvement efforts. Using advances in teaching for conceptual understanding and transfer of learning, the CBCE model aims to empower students to clarify, investigate, and collaboratively influence the cultural evolutionary dynamics of their own school and surrounding communities. The relationship between students’ evolving intuitive theories of school improvement, and the evolving scientific theories of school improvement scientists, provides a framework for understanding the development of student conceptions of cultural (and, perhaps, biological) change more generally. This chapter provides a conceptual foundation for exploring the claim that engaging students in reflecting on the cognitive, behavioral, and cultural evolutionary processes in their everyday lives provides new opportunities for school improvement and interdisciplinary evolution education initiatives. The practical and systemic challenges of this approach are clarified and future directions are outlined.
Article
Despite numerous education readjustments by the government, efforts for equal opportunity in education and an increase in present enrolment, many indigenous children in Thailand continue to face serious obstacles in the formal education system. This study focuses on the Moken hunter-gatherer communities in southern Thailand and seeks to analyse and understand the situation of Moken children and the shortcomings of the formal education system within their context. Traditional ways of knowing, especially knowing through nature and through play, are used as a platform to rethink the way we look at indigenous children’s education in Thailand. This paper identifies alternate approaches to sustainable and inclusive education for Moken children in the Thai education context. Traditional practices of knowledge transmission based around play and self-directed learning are looked at to help bridge the gap between Moken learners and the formal education system in order to contextualise and make relevant the curriculum that is taught. While this study only focuses on the Moken, findings and conclusions may be drawn to encompass a larger scope in terms of implications for other indigenous learners in Thailand.
Article
This article provides an overview of some of the main themes that have emerged in the research on hunter gatherers and education. The term ‘education’ refers both to schooling, and to the traditional pedagogical modes of knowledge transmission that hunter-gatherer communities have developed and maintained over millennia. Formal education plays a crucial, yet complicated role for contemporary hunter-gatherers; it is considered to be a foundational element for economic and social development, yet also continues to be a tool of assimilation. Participation in schooling can also conflict with local livelihood strategies, culture and knowledge. While this is the case for many marginalised groups, hunter-gatherers are one of the most marginalised and most vulnerable groups, and face some of the most challenging problems with formal education. This paper examines these issues from a human rights perspective, and within a global context. We describe the main challenges that hunter-gatherers face regarding participation in formal education, including physical, financial, social, cultural and structural barriers, and highlight issues of both inclusion and assimilation. We also examine traditional knowledge and educational approaches among hunter-gatherer communities, calling for a much greater appreciation of the importance and relevance to current global concerns.
Chapter
The concept of ecosystem, mainly used in ecological sciences, refers to the dynamic network of interconnection and interdependencies between living organisms (biotic) and non-living components (abiotic) in the environment. In this chapter, we have tried to apply the concept of ecosystem in the field of education, in order to view the education process as a network of relationships between different biotic (learners, teachers, government bodies, educational associations, institutional bodies, etc.) and abiotic (infrastructure, teaching-learning materials, etc.) entities, shaped by a set of external influencers, defining the social, economic, political, and socio-psychological context. These external influencers heavily determine the performance of an education ecosystem in inculcating effective and quality learning in a specific geographic context. This ecosystemic perspective to education is in direct contrast to the conventional narratives of education, which views production and distribution of education along a linear axis. Subsequently, the chapter proceeds onto demystify the evolution of education ecosystem, starting from ancient and medieval society to the modern education ecosystem. The chapter concludes by narrating the limitations intrinsic to the interactions within modern education ecosystem, which marks the incapacitation of the same in meeting the emerging demands of twenty-first century.
Chapter
Full-text available
O capítulo analisa práticas de inclusão desenvolvidas por professores do ensino regular, focando-se no acesso e na participação de alunos com multideficiência em atividades conjuntas com pares com desenvolvimento típico. Procura-se compreender o modo como estes alunos estão a ser incluídos no ensino regular e como acedem ao currículo, nomeadamente quais os contextos e as atividades que lhe estão acessíveis.
Article
In this study, I explore some of the inherent and lived tensions or paradoxes produced through the principles and practices of the governmental and educational contexts of the neoliberal milieu, through the lens of a contemporary countercultural movement. In the particularities of this movement, a community of practice known to insiders as the “unschooling movement,” families seek to challenge the rationalization and standardization that they perceive as rampant and objectionable in state-overseen education. This is an ethnographic study of the countercultural praxis and identities entailed in cultivating unschooled children through distinctive childhood, familial, and community-based experiences. I consider dimensions of lifestyle that include attachment parenting, the organization of space and time, consumption, community-based education and legitimation (portfolio evaluations) to prove educational equivalence. This study reveals the hidden resources of social capital and educational capital used to sustain a countercultural educational alternative.
Article
The frequencies of age mixing and gender mixing were observed among students age 4 to 19, at an ungraded, democratically structured school where the students move about freely and direct their own activities. Age mixing (across a span of at least 24 months) was more frequent for the 12-to 15-year-olds than for the 8-to 11-or 16-to 19-year-olds and was less frequent for conversations than for activities (mostly play) other than conversation. Gender mixing was less frequent for the 8-to 11-year-olds than for any other age group and was more frequent for conversations than for other activities. For students observed with companions of both sexes, the mean age difference from opposite sex companions was greater than that from same-sex companions. The results are discussed in relation to ideas about the possible developmental functions of such mixing.
Article
If you believe the experts, "child's play"; is serious business. From sociologists to psychologists and from anthropologists to social critics, writers have produced mountains of books about the meaning and importance of play. But what do we know about how children actually play, especially American children of the last two centuries? In this fascinating and enlightening book, Howard Chudacoff presents a history of children's play in the United States and ponders what it tells us about ourselves. Through expert investigation in primary sources-including dozens of children's diaries, hundreds of autobiographical recollections of adults, and a wealth of child-rearing manuals-along with wide-ranging reading of the work of educators, journalists, market researchers, and scholars-Chudacoff digs into the "underground" of play. He contrasts the activities that genuinely occupied children's time with what adults thought children should be doing. Filled with intriguing stories and revelatory insights, Children at Play provides a chronological history of play in the U.S. from the point of view of children themselves. Focusing on youngsters between the ages of about six and twelve, this is history "from the bottom up." It highlights the transformations of play that have occurred over the last 200 years, paying attention not only to the activities of the cultural elite but to those of working-class men and women, to slaves, and to Native Americans. In addition, the author considers the findings, observations, and theories of numerous social scientists along with those of fellow historians. Chudacoff concludes that children's ability to play independently has attenuated over time and that in our modern era this diminution has frequently had unfortunate consequences. By examining the activities of young people whom marketers today call "tweens," he provides fresh historical depth to current discussions about topics like childhood obesity, delinquency, learning disability, and the many ways that children spend their time when adults aren't looking.