ChapterPDF Available

Neighborhood Sense of Community and Social Capital: A multi-level analysis

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In many ways, social capital (SC) is to political science, sociology, applied economics, and community development what sense of community (SOC) and empowerment have been to community psychology. SC is the norms, networks, and mutual trust of “civil society” facilitating cooperative action among citizens and institutions (Coleman, 1988) and has had considerable influence on political thinking and action over the past decade. It is generally observed and analyzed as a characteristic (or lack) of communities or societies, rather than individuals... We have four main goals for this chapter. One is to inform researchers and program planners in community development, urban policy, and social services that many concepts thoroughly studied by community psychologists (sense of community, collective efficacy/empowerment, citizen participation, neighboring) are part of SC. Our second goal is to introduce more community psychologists to SC. Third, to both audiences, we expect to show that residential neighborhood sense of community is at least as strongly related to other SC dimensions as are demographics and other widely studied community-focused cognitions (place attachment, community satisfaction, community confidence, and communitarianism -- or community values). In addition to those interdisciplinary aims, our fourth goal is to explore SOC and its relationships to SC using multi-level analysis. The relationship between SOC and SC -- whether they operate together, separately, or nested one within the other -- and on what level(s) they operate are critical to our understanding of both concepts.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Perkins, D.D., & Long, D.A. (2002). Neighborhood sense of community
and social capital: A multi-level analysis. In A. Fisher, C. Sonn, &
B. Bishop (Eds.), Psychological sense of community: Research,
applications, and implications (pp. 291-318). New York: Plenum.
Chapter 15
NEIGHBORHOOD SENSE OF COMMUNITY
AND SOCIAL CAPITAL
A Multi-Level Analysisi
Douglas D. Perkins and D. Adam Long
Vanderbilt University
INTRODUCTION
In many ways, social capital (SC) is to political science, sociology,
applied economics, and community development what sense of community
(SOC) and empowerment have been to community psychology. SC is the
norms, networks, and mutual trust of “civil society” facilitating cooperative
action among citizens and institutions (Coleman, 1988) and has had
considerable influence on political thinking and action over the past decade.
It is generally observed and analyzed as a characteristic (or lack) of
communities or societies, rather than individuals.
By contrast, SOC has been conceived of and measured by most
researchers as an individual-level construct. Some studies have examined it
at the group or community level (Buckner, 1988; Fisher & Sonn, 1999;
Kingston, Mitchell, Florin, & Stevenson, 1999; Perkins, Brown, & Taylor,
1996; Perkins, Florin, Rich, Wandersman, & Chavis, 1990; Sampson, 1991).
A very few have used it in multi-level analyses (Brodsky, O’Campo, &
Aronson, 1999; Hyde, 1998; Kingston, et al., 1999; Perkins & Long, 2001;
Sampson, 1991). But we found no previous study that analyzed sense of
community at multiple levels simultaneously to see whether it operates
differently at each level.
We have four main goals for this chapter. One is to inform
researchers and program planners in community development, urban policy,
and social services that many concepts thoroughly studied by community
psychologists (sense of community, collective efficacy/empowerment,
292 Perkins and Long
citizen participation, neighboring) are part of SC. Our second goal is to
introduce more community psychologists to SC. Third, to both audiences, we
expect to show that residential neighborhood sense of community is at least
as strongly related to other SC dimensions as are demographics and other
widely studied community-focused cognitions (place attachment, community
satisfaction, community confidence, and communitarianism -- or community
values). In addition to those interdisciplinary aims, our fourth goal is to
explore SOC and its relationships to SC using multi-level analysis. The
relationship between SOC and SC -- whether they operate together,
separately, or nested one within the other -- and on what level(s) they operate
are critical to our understanding of both concepts.
Social Capital: Community-Focused Cognitions and
Behaviors
In observing that Americans are generally now “bowling alone”
rather than in the leagues so popular a generation ago, Putnam (2000) was
less concerned with the disappearance of recreational clubs, per se, than
what he saw as the loss of the glue that binds together the social fabric of our
local communities and, ultimately, our society. His obituary for the
American community may be exaggerated, but the importance of SC to the
functioning and quality of community life seems indisputable.
The bipartisan and multidisciplinary popularity of SC has led to
many different, and often vague, definitions. Until recently, psychologists
have largely ignored SC despite, or perhaps because of, its being little more
than a collection of more specific community-focused behaviors and
cognitions long studied by community psychologists. We, therefore, may be
skeptical of a term from outside the discipline which seems to cover ground
we feel we already know well, and for which there appears to be no clear,
precise, and agreed upon meaning. The only advantage we see in SC, as a
construct, is that it speaks to economists and policy makers and draws their
attention to non-economic assets (Kretzmann & McKnight, 1993). But that is
also the danger in SC: as with empowerment (Perkins, 1995), anti-
government neo-conservatives are co-opting SC to justify reducing public
spending on critical social services under the misguided assumption that the
overburdened private community service sector can suffice. As SC seems to
have strong appeal and staying power, the challenge to researchers is to try
to unpack the construct and make it as useful as possible while being fully
aware of the political ramifications: that is, what issues can SC address
directly and, where government intervention is required, how can SC be
turned into political clout?
Given the expanse of theory and research on SOC over more than a
quarter-century,ii it may provide the greatest contribution to understanding
SC. Yet much of the usage of the term SOC is also vague and
Sense of Community/Social Capital 293
counterproductive. The original subtitle of this chapter was “All the things
you are” to make the point that, similar to “community” and “empowerment”
(Perkins, 1995), both SC and SOC have meant, if not all things to all people,
then too much and too varied to too many.
While there is power in such ambiguity, SC would benefit
conceptually, empirically, and practically from a more precise definition. In
particular, it is important to measure and analyze the specific behavioral and
intrapsychic dimensions of SC separately to gain a clearer understanding of
what aspects of SC operate in what ways and under what conditions. There is
a critical need to dissect, examine, and understand, not only the differences
between various forms of SC, but also the many different factors and
processes that make up, and are related to, each form. Only with careful
attention to the construct and predictive validity of SC can we develop a
more psychological and complex, yet clearly defined, conception of SC.
Dimensions of Social Capital
Saegert and Winkel (1998) were among the first psychologists to
study SC, and found that it significantly predicted the successful
revitalization and maintenance of distressed inner-city housing. They
distinguish two measures of informal SC (neighboring and perceived pro-
social norms) and two formal factors (leadership activity and basic voluntary
participation). The emphasis on leadership is particularly important,
especially for maintaining the momentum and effectiveness of voluntary
organizations. Neighboring is the instrumental help we provide, or get from,
other community members (e.g., watching after a neighbor’s house or child;
Perkins, et al., 1990; 1996; Unger & Wandersman, 1985). Ordinary social
interaction with one’s neighbors, especially as it encourages more
community involvement, either formally or informally, may also be included
as a form of neighboring.
We appreciate, and generally agree with, the utility of Saegert and
Winkel’s (1998) and Putnam’s (2000) emphasis on behavioral definitions of
SC; but as long as the dimensions are analyzed separately, there may be
some added utility in considering possible intrapsychic dimensions or
predictors. Community psychologists have researched many attitudes,
emotions, and perceptions related to SC. The most exhaustive attention has
been paid to two constructs: empowerment (Perkins, et al., 1996; Saegert &
Winkel, 1996; Speer & Hughey, 1995) and SOC. Empowerment is about
perceived control. A primary benefit of SOC is social support from one’s
community. (Briggs (1998) identified social leverage (information) and other
forms of social support as key dimensions of SC. Thus, SC provides at least
three forms of social support: communal (SOC), instrumental (neighboring),
and informational. The fourth form of support, emotional, may also be
involved, depending on the quality of one’s relationships with community
294 Perkins and Long
members.) Control and social support are two of the strongest and most
consistent predictors of positive individual outcomes. The same may be true
of community-level outcomes as well.
Thus, we define SC in terms of four distinct components: (1) trust in
one’s neighbors (SOC) and (2) in the efficacy of organized collective action
(empowerment), (3) informal neighboring behavior, and (4) formal
participation in community organizations (see Figure 1). This four-part
definition adds the idea of formal and informal community “trust” to formal
and informal pro-social community behaviors (cf. Saegert & Winkel, 1998).
SOC and collective efficacy are the cognitive or intrapsychic components of
SC. Citizen participation in grassroots community organizations and
neighboring are the behavioral components of SC. Each dimension of SC is
consistently related to the others.
Cognition/Trust Social Behavior
Informal Sense of community
Neighboring
Formally
Organized
Collective efficacy Citizen participation
Figure 1. Four Dimensions Of Social Capital
Sense of community is a consistent and widely valued indicator of
quality of community life and a catalyst for both behavioral dimensions of
SC: organized participation and informal neighboring (Beckman, et al.,
1998; Chavis & Wandersman, 1990; Hughey, Speer, & Peterson, 1999;
Perkins, et al., 1996; Wandersman & Giamartino, 1980). The link between
organized participation and SOC has been found at both the individual and
community levels of analysis (Brodsky, et al., 1999; Perkins, et al., 1996). It
makes sense that a group of residents must have at least some SOC to be
interested in organizing an association and working together to solve
common problems (Ahlbrandt, 1984). Chavis and Wandersman (1990) found
that, over time, SOC leads to greater self- and collective-efficacy and
neighboring, which all increase participation. Their results suggest that
participation, in turn, enhances SOC. SOC has also been related to
community satisfaction, collective efficacy, neighboring, communitarianism,
and informal social control, less fear of crime, litter and graffiti (Perkins, et
al., 1990) and better-maintained yards (Varady, 1986).
Interest in SOC has been international as have empirical findings on
its psychometric properties (Chipuer & Pretty, 1999: Australia) and its
relationship to participation and neighboring (Garcia, Giuliani, &
Wiesenfeld, 1999: Venezuela; Itzhaky, & York, 2000: Israel; Prezza, Amici,
Sense of Community/Social Capital 295
Roberti, & Tedeschi, 2001: Italy), type of common land (Li, 1998: Taiwan),
investment in home and community building processes (Garcia, et al., 1999:
Venezuela), community satisfaction and local friendships (Sampson, 1991:
U.K.); life satisfaction and loneliness (Prezza, et al., 2001: Italy), minority
community identity (Sonn & Fisher, 1998: Australia), and university
residence social climate and well-being (Pretty, 1990; Pretty, Conroy,
Dugay, Fowler, & Williams, 1996: Canada).
Collective efficacy, or trust in the effectiveness of organized
community action, is closest to the concept of empowerment among all the
social capital dimensions and their predictors. Some definitions of individual
psychological empowerment are little different from traditional theories of
self-efficacy or locus of control. In order to distinguish it from those
concepts, we argue that a necessary component of empowerment, even at the
individual level, should be its connection to collective action and
organizational and community levels of empowerment. Empowerment is
thought both to lead to participation in community organizations and to
result from it. Perceived efficacy of collective action is important for
maintaining participation in a community organization (Florin &
Wandersman, 1984; Perkins, et al., 1990; 1996) and may be important for
initiating it.
Note that our definition of collective efficacy differs importantly
from that of Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls (1997). They define it as
“social cohesion among neighbors combined with their willingness to
intervene on behalf of the common good” (p. 918) and operationalize it as a
combination of SOC and informal social control (ISC).iii We do not adopt
this definition because (1) we think it conceptually sound to separate the
intrapsychic and more general SOC from the narrower, more behavioral ISC
and (2) collective efficacy should be an appraisal of group behavior that is,
as the term suggests, both collectively organized and efficacious. ISC is, by
definition, unorganized and is undemocratic, unrelated to formal
participation (Perkins, et al., 1990), and inconsistently effective in reducing
crime (Perkins, Wandersman, Rich, & Taylor, 1993).
Neighboring behavior is informal mutual assistance and information
sharing among neighbors. In a stress and coping framework, it can be
considered a form of local “instrumental social support.” Some researchers
include non-instrumental social contact as neighboring (e.g., Brown &
Perkins, 2001). All forms of neighboring allow residents to become better
acquainted and discuss shared problems (Unger & Wandersman, 1985).
Prezza, et al. (2001) found that women, long-term residents, those with more
children, those living with a spouse, those with less education, and members
of community groups had more neighboring relationships. Unger and
Wandersman (1983), using a similar survey measure of neighboring to that
used in the present study, found that greater neighboring prior to organizing
a block may facilitate subsequent efforts towards forming a block
296 Perkins and Long
association. In turn, they found that once a block organized, association
members engaged in more social interaction, which may lead to more
neighboring. Perkins, et al. (1996) found that neighboring was, generally, the
strongest single predictor of participation in community organizations in
three cities, cross-sectionally and one year later, at both the individual and
block levels of analysis.
It is surprising that, despite the important role of neighboring to the
quality of community life, so few studies have related neighboring to other
community-focused behaviors and cognitions. Brown and Werner (1985)
found neighboring to be related to community satisfaction. In Time-1 of the
present data, controlling for demographics, block-level neighboring was
related to participation, sense of community, communitarianism, block
satisfaction, and informal social control (Perkins, et al., 1990).
Citizen participation in block, neighborhood, and building (tenant or
co-op) associations, faith-based community service or advocacy committees
and coalitions, school-based associations, and other grassroots community
organizations are examples of formal social capital behavior. These
organizations address a wide variety of local needs, from housing, planning
and traffic issues to cleaning up residential property, vacant lots, and parks to
youth and recreation programs and block parties to crime prevention.
Research on civic participation has been a staple of sociology and
political science from their beginning (or even longer: Tocqueville, 1935/
1969). But the emphasis in much of the research has been on demographic
predictors. For example, replicating their own 1958 study, Hyman and
Wright (1971) found that greater resources (income), investment in the
community (home ownership, length of residence) and skills and knowledge
(education) motivate or permit greater participation. More recently, poor and
middle-class mothers’ participation in block clubs, neighborhood or tenant
groups, and other community organizations was associated with greater
education and income, but not with age, employment, marital status, number
of children, or tenure in neighborhood (Rankin & Quane, 2000).
The psychological research on participation generally controls for
these demographic differences, but goes beyond them to find that
participants, and their organizations and communities, have a greater sense
of collective efficacy or empowerment (Florin & Wandersman, 1984;
Perkins, et al., 1996; Saegert & Winkel, 1996; Speer & Hughey, 1995), SOC
(Chavis & Wandersman, 1990; Perkins & Long, 2001), neighboring
(Perkins, et al., 1996; Unger & Wandersman, 1985), community satisfaction
(Perkins, et al., 1990), and other positive community attachments and
organizational activities (Perkins, et al., 1996).
Psychological Predictors of Social Capital
Place attachment is an important construct in its relationship to SOC
Sense of Community/Social Capital 297
and SC, but one that is often overlooked by community psychologists. It
refers to emotional bonding, developed over time from behavioral, affective,
and cognitive ties to a particular socio-physical environment (Brown &
Perkins, 1992). These bonds are integral to individual and community
aspects of self-identity and provide a source of stability and change for
individuals and communities alike. Place attachments are a resource that
individuals (especially women, minorities, lower-income people, and elders)
and communities can draw on to help revitalize all aspects of home and
neighborhood environments (Brown & Perkins, 2001; Saegert, 1989).
Politically, place attachment may motivate residents to participate in
community organizations (Saegert, 1989). Participation, at both the
individual and community levels, also leads to greater community
attachment (Zhao, 1996). Socially, place attachment can help bring residents
together to address social problems as well as environmental threats (Brown
& Perkins, 1992). Economically, where residents, through their history in,
and attachments to, a place discover what is unique about their community,
they can preserve or develop places and events that generate tourism and
other business opportunities. Those who feel no particular attachment to the
place they live invest little time, energy, or money in it and are more likely to
move (Vinsel, Brown, Altman, & Foss, 1980).
Place attachment and SOC are closely related. The Sense of
Community Index includes four items measuring attachment to place (one’s
block; in the present analyses, these items were removed to create a new
place attachment scale). The two constructs were combined with block
satisfaction and knowing one’s neighbors in an analysis of participation in
neighborhood improvement organizations (Perkins, et al., 1996). In all three
cities studied, that combination was significantly correlated with
participation at the individual and block levels, both cross-sectionally and
over a one-year lag. In multivariate analyses, however, it was a significant
predictor in two cities and only at the individual level.
Cuba and Hummon (1993) identify three loci of place identity --
home, community, and region -- and find that formal organizational
participation, not sense of community, is key to community identity.
Puddifoot (1996) argues that psychological theory supports the analysis of
“community identity,” based on a combination of place identity or
attachment, SOC, and community satisfaction. Pretty (this volume) expands
on that argument, suggesting that SOC and place attachment are part of the
same overarching self-in-community psychological framework with
emotional, cognitive, spiritual, and behavioral dimensions all contributing to
the development of individuals’ community identity.
Despite these connections, we view place attachment as distinct
from SOC because the former is a spatially-oriented emotional construct
(Brown & Perkins, 1992) and the latter is more of a socially-oriented
cognitive construct. Furthermore, keeping the concepts separate allows us to
298 Perkins and Long
consider how one may lead to the other or whether different community
changes might affect place and social attachments differently. For example,
there is intriguing evidence that SOC may be encouraged by “New Urbanist”
planned communities that minimize the impact of automobile traffic and
emphasize walkable, mixed residential/commercial space (Nasar & Julian,
1995; Plas & Lewis, 1996). But more research is needed to determine
whether SOC gains are due to increased social interaction in private and
public outdoor spaces, increased place attachment, both, or neither (people
attracted to New Urbanist communities may be predisposed to more SOC).
Community Satisfaction is also related to place: Brown and Werner’s
(1985) research showing that block satisfaction and neighboring behaviors
are related also found such community ties to be stronger on cul-de-sacs than
through streets. Perkins, et al. (1990) found block satisfaction to be higher on
blocks with more attached homes as well as SOC, collective efficacy, and
neighboring and (surprisingly) fewer trees, gardens and shrubs as well as less
criminal victimization, disorder, and fear. Block satisfaction was also the
strongest predictor of block association (BA) participation in their
multivariate analyses. It remains to be seen whether that relationship was as
strong at Time 2 and in a multilevel analysis at both times.
Chavis and Wandersman (1990) also found block satisfaction to be
associated with BA participation, neighboring, collective efficacy, and SOC.
Using data from the same Nashville project, Florin and Wandersman (1984)
found perceived community problems and community dissatisfaction to load
as one factor and so combined them into “encoding strategies,” which was
modestly associated with individual BA participation. But satisfaction is
very different than a lack of perceived problems. In fact, Perkins, et al.
(1990) found that two of the strongest predictors of participation were
community satisfaction and more perceived disorder (again, a physical
environmental concern). Residents who are very attached to their community
may have high satisfaction, but because they care about it so much, they are
also the most critical of community problems.
Communitarianism is the value placed on one’s community and on
working collectively to improve it (Perkins, et al., 1990). This is the original
meaning before Etzioni (1993) politicized the term as a compromise position
among competing ideologies of autonomous individualism vs. communal
socialism and Liberalism vs. Conservativism. If residents participate more in
communities they value, a communitarian climate should encourage greater
collective participation. Florin and Wandersman (1984) used the cognitive
social learning concept of “subjective stimulus values” to encompass a
variety of constructs, including communitarianism, self-efficacy, collective
efficacy, and SOC. This composite predicted individual participation in BAs
far better than any other variable they considered. At the block level of
analysis, Perkins, et al. (1990) found communitarianism alone to be related
to blocks with more minorities, less income, more home owners,
Sense of Community/Social Capital 299
neighboring, collective efficacy, and to various features of the block physical
environment, but only marginally to SOC, and not significantly to
participation in BAs.
Community Confidence is another vital cognition, especially in older
neighborhoods that may be deteriorating and considered “transitional” due to
changes in local businesses or residential demography (income, tenure, racial
composition; Ahlbrandt 1984; Varady, 1986). As residents perceive their
neighborhood declining, if they still have confidence in its future, they may
stay and upgrade their own property and pressure neighbors and the city to
do likewise. A lack of community confidence, however, may spell
commercial and residential disinvestment and flight and may explain why
many urban policies and revitalization projects have failed (Varady, 1986).
As other, more objective, development indicators -- such as building permits,
residential stability, higher owner occupancy and property values -- are
slower to appear, confidence is considered by many to be a benchmark
indicator of a community’s capacity to revitalize.
Varady (1986) examined the impact of a major federal “urban
homesteading” program on neighborhood confidence and property
upgrading. Program spillover effects on neighbors’ upgrading and
confidence were negligible. Nor were home improvements related to
confidence at the individual/household level, a result confirmed in a more
recent study (Brown & Perkins, 2001). But neighborhoods in better physical
condition had residents who were more confident about the future of the
neighborhood (Varady, 1986). Confidence was also associated with
neighboring, SOC, and resident decisions to move or stay.
MULTI-LEVEL ANALYSIS OF SENSE OF COMMUNITY
Almost all studies of SOC, other community cognitions, or social
capital behaviors (as opposed to organizations), while targeting the block,
neighborhood, or vaguely defined community level, have analyzed
individual level data. There is no doubt that we need more and better data
collected at the community level (Fisher & Sonn, 1999; Puddifoot, 1996;
Shinn, 1990; Theodori, 2000). But another approach to more ecologically
valid research is multi-level analysis. Social scientists have long aggregated
individual perceptions to the group level to create contextual or social
climate variables. With the advent of multi-level analytic statistical
programs, this practice is becoming even more common. Yet psychologists’
individualistic bias has made us slower to respond to these powerful new
techniques. The criteria for validating aggregate individual perceptions as
group climate variables are clear and simple, however (Shinn, 1990).
Climate variables must (1) exhibit adequate inter-rater agreement among
members of the same group, (2) show reliable differentiation, or variance,
between groups, and (3) correlate significantly with other variables at the
300 Perkins and Long
group or individual level.
There have been just a few recent multilevel studies of SOC.
Brodsky, et al. (1999) used multilevel analysis to identify individual- and
community-level predictors of individual SOC, but they only compared three
communities and do not report the extent to which SOC varies at the
community level. Kingston, et al. (1999) show that perceptions of
neighborhood climate (SOC) vary at the community level. But possibly due
to (a) low neighborhood-level variance, (b) low statistical power at that level,
and (c) using dichotomous predictors, they fail to find a significant
correlation between SOC and either neighborhood organization or the
boundedness of the neighborhood by arterial streets. Their results show the
importance of an adequate sample size at the group as well as individual
level in multi-level analysis. Sampson (1991) used a British nation-wide
sample in finding that neighborhood-level social cohesion increases
individuals’ community satisfaction (independent of personal
characteristics).
A multi-level study by Hyde (1998) made, we believe, another
important advance by analyzing SOC and place attachment separately. She
found significant neighborhood-level variance in both. She also found that
both resident perceptions of disorder and independently assessed disorder
predicted SOC and place attachment, suggesting that physical and social
conditions of place influence community attachments. Similarly, using the
present data, Perkins and Long (2001) found that between 9% and 30% of
variance in individual-level SOC was due to block-level differences and that
SOC was predicted by place attachment and other community-focused
cognitions and behaviors at both the block and individual levels.
None of the above, however, has considered SOC at multiple levels
simultaneously. Multi-level analysis is critical to determine how, and how
much, SC is manifested at the community level vs. the individual level. This
could lead to better targeted interventions to encourage the right form of SC
for a given community or a particular group of its individual members. In
addition, it can identify differences in SC dimensions among individuals
with different social attitudes and demographic profiles living in
communities with different levels of social cohesion and place attachment.
(For example, what does it mean to have a strong SOC in a community
where that is not shared versus one where it is?) And it can address the
critical question of whether, controlling for individual and/or community
demographics, individuals engage in more or less formal SC in communities
with more informal cohesion. That is, do communities with more SOC
encourage, not only neighboring, but also more collective efficacy and
voluntarism, or does it tend to replace and thus lessen the formal forms of
SC?
We aim to unpack the broadly defined and loosely understood
concept by examining the construct validity of the various dimensions of SC
Sense of Community/Social Capital 301
and other variables that are related to SOC and how they are inter-related.
We will present a new analysis of one of the major studies of SOC, blocks,
and block associations (Perkins, et al., 1990).
Community Cognitions and Social Capital: Reanalyzing the
Block Booster Data
The present data were collected as part of the Block Booster Project,
a two-year (1985-86), multimethod, action study of the social effects,
organizational dynamics, and viability of urban residential BAs (Chavis,
Florin, Rich, Wandersman, & Perkins, 1987). The purposes of the Project
were to: (1) examine the role of BAs in community development and crime
control and (2) develop an intervention process and set of training materials
to help voluntary associations maintain and strengthen themselves.
Clustered, resident survey data from 47 street blocks (the homes fronting on
the same street between two cross streets or a cross street and dead end) in
five neighborhoods in Brooklyn and Queens, New York, permit comparisons
over two points in time (T1 n = 1,081, T2 n = 638, panel = 438) using
multilevel analyses (HLM) of the constructs as both individual psychological
and community climate phenomena. (For details of the site selection,
sampling, and survey methods, see Perkins, et al., 1990.)
Measures
The following scales were confirmed in principal components
analyses (PCA) as distinct and coherent constructs. All predictors were
standardized. To reduce skewness, variables were transformed using either
the square root (number of children, neighboring, participation) or the
exponential method (length of residence, SOC, place attachment,
communitarianism, collective efficacy). This was not done in previous
publications of these data (Perkins, et al., 1990; 1993; 1996). All four SC
dimension scales (Sense of Community, Collective Efficacy, Participation,
and Neighboring), items, and reliabilities are displayed in Appendix A. Most
items were dichotomous, which lowered the internal consistency of all
scales, we recommend Likert response scales be used in future. (More
information on the creation of scales and their descriptive statistics is
available from the authors.)
Brief Sense of Community Index (BSCI) is a new eight-item scale
adapted in part from the 12-item Sense of Community Index (SCI; Perkins,
et al., 1990).iv PCAs confirmed SOC as distinct from neighboring behavior,
informal social control, block satisfaction, and communitarianism. But a
PCA of the SCI alone failed to confirm McMillan and Chavis’ (1986)
dimensions of emotional connection, group membership, needs fulfillment,
and influence. One or two factors, which cut across their framework, is, we
302 Perkins and Long
argue, a separate construct, that is, place attachment. After removing four
place attachment items, we added three face-valid SOC items to a second
PCA. Three of the original items failed to load cleanly on a single factor and
were removed. The remaining eight items form the new BSCI and were
included in a third PCA resulting in three subscales, confirmed across two
surveys: social connections, mutual concern, and community values (Perkins
& Long, 2001). Only the total scale was used here.
Place attachment (α (Time 1) = .65, n = 903; α (Time 2) = .63, n =
480) is the mean of four items removed from the SCI (true/false): I think my
block is a good place for me to live; I feel at home on this block; it is very
important to me to live on this particular block; I expect to live on this block
for a long time.
Communitarianism (α = .56, n = 1,053; .62, n = 624) is the value
placed on one’s community and on working collectively to improve it.
Unlike Perkins, et al. (1990), it was measured using the mean of just two
items: the importance to the respondent of what their block is like and the
importance of neighbors working together rather than alone to improve block
conditions (not important, somewhat important, very important).
Community (block) satisfaction (α = .36, n = 946; .39, n = 613) was
measured here using the mean of just two items: satisfaction with the block
as a place to live (satisfied/dissatisfied) and, compared to adjacent blocks,
whether the block is a better or worse place to live or about the same as other
blocks in the area. Using the same data, the satisfaction scale by Perkins, et
al. (1990) combined these items with the following two.
Block confidence (α = .62 n = 923; .63, n = 567) was measured using
the mean of two items: “In the past two years, have the general conditions on
your block gotten worse, stayed about the same, or improved” and “in the
next two years, do you feel that general conditions on your block will get
worse, stay about the same, or improve.”
Demographic variables. The present analyses included the following
control variables: sex, age, income level, education, race, length of
residence, home ownership, and number of children in household.
In order to examine the relationship of SOC, relative to other
community-focused cognitions and demographics, to SC, all the above were
used to predict each of the other three dimensions of SC (see Appendix A):
Collective efficacy was measured here using the mean of six items:
whether it is “not likely, somewhat likely, or very likely” the respondent’s
BA (or a hypothetical association on unorganized blocks) can accomplish
improvement of physical conditions, the persuasion of city officials to
provide better services, getting people on the block to help each other more,
a reduction in crime, getting people to know each other better, and getting
information to residents about where to go for needed services.
Participation in BA activities was a sum of eight items coded zero to
one (all but one item were yes/no): membership and participation in a BA,
Sense of Community/Social Capital 303
whether the respondent had attended, spoken in, served as member or officer
in a BA meeting, or done work for the organization outside a meeting in the
past year, and monthly hours working for the BA outside of meetings.
Neighboring behavior was measured using the mean of five items
indicating how many neighbors (none, one or two, or several) asked: to
watch their home while they were away, to loan food or a tool, to help in an
emergency, to offer advice on a personal problem, and to discuss a block
problem. (This differs from the scale by Perkins, et al. (1990) who used
block aggregates only, including neighboring received as well as given.)
Individual and Block-Level Bivariate Correlations
Table 1 presents individual and block mean level bivariate
correlations at both time points for the four dimensions of SC (collective
efficacy, participation, neighboring, SOC), the other four informal
community cognitions (place attachment, communitarianism, block
satisfaction, block confidence), and eight demographics (number of children,
age, education, white ethnicity, income, sex, resident tenure, home
ownership). Below the diagonal are individual-level coefficients, above the
diagonal are block mean aggregated coefficients; these cells display cross-
sectional coefficients at both time points as follows: T1/T2. On the diagonal
are displayed the T1 by T2 correlations for the respective variables (for data
available at both time points); each cell on the diagonal displays coefficients
as follows: individual-level/block-level. Coefficients are displayed only if
significant at p < .05 for individual-level correlations and p < .10 for block-
level correlations. (It is common to relax the significance criterion when
analyzing group data, which tend to be more stable than individual-level data
(Kenny & LaVoie, 1985).
Interestingly, there is virtually no correlation between collective
efficacy and neighboring. Otherwise, the correlations among the four
dimensions of SC are significant, suggesting some internal consistency to the
overarching construct. Participation in BAs (individual r = .68, block-level r
= .87) and SOC (individual r = .58, block r = .77) were both highly
correlated between T1 and T2. The correlations between T1 and T2 for the
other SC scales (collective efficacy and neighboring) and other predictors
were also significant, with block confidence being the least stable. The five
substantive predictors (SOC, place attachment, communitarianism, block
satisfaction, block confidence) also showed some intercorrelation. As
expected, the relationship between SOC and place attachment was strongest
(individual r = .40(T1), .51(T2); block r = .73(T1), .63(T2)). However,
communitarianism was not significantly related to block satisfaction,
confidence, or (at the block level) place attachment. SOC was the only
predictor to correlate significantly with all the other community cognitions at
both levels.
cial Capital 304 304
Table 1. Individual- and Block-level Time-1 and Time-2 Psychological Correlates of Social Capital Variables with Sense of Community and Other Predictors: Individual Level
Correlations Below Diagonal; Block Level Correlations Above
DEPENDENT
VARIABLES: DEMOGRAPHICS: INDEPENDENT VARIABLES:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
1. Collective Efficacy .27/.28 .29/ns .38/ns -.36/ns -.31/ns .34/.49 ns/.55 .52/.38 ns/.36 ns/.26
2. Participation .19/.25 .68/.87 .39/.59 ns/-.25 ns/-.23 ns/.21 .54/.54 .25/ns .34/.23
3. Neighboring .07/ns .32/.37
.38/.56 .36/.38 .38/.34 .47/.63 .39/.39 .29/ns ns/.32 ns/.35
4. Children .09/ns .07/ns .10/ns -.46 -.44 -.27 .26 -.44/-.36 -.35/-.21 .42/.31 ns/-.22 -.28/ns
5. Age ns/.21 .12/ns -.28 -.23 .26 .50/.50 .34/.27
6. Education -.11/ns .06/ns -.22 .40 .36 -.49/-.28
7. White Ethnicity -.13/ns -.07/-.14 -.24 .10 .10 .36 -.24 .-45/-.27 ns/.36 -.59/-.39 .26/ns .31/ns
8. Income .13/ns .15/ns .34 .15 -.24 -.29/-.23 .28/ns
9. Sex ns/.13 -.06 -.18
10. Length-Residence .21/.13 .24/.24 -.12/-.16 .45/.42 -.10/ns .14/.13 .83/.68 .47/.55 .52/.26 .59/.34 ns/.29
11. Homeowner .26/.24 .30/.25 .09/ns .21/.23 -.13/ns .20/.13 .42/.47
.83/.85 .36/.22 .25/ns
12. Sense-Community .26/.42 .36/.33 .42/.37 .13/.17 .08/ns .23/.17 .24/.22
.58/.77 .73/.63 ns/.29 .37/.49 .29/.28
13. Place Attachment .12/.33 .15/.14 .18/.19 .20/.27 -.16/-.12 .07/ns .19/.16 .16/.15 .40/.51 .37/.50 .31/.65 .43/.47
14. Communitarian .30/.37 .14/.19 .16/.20 .06/.10 .13/.15 -.12/-.08 -.17/-.14 -.08/-.10 ns/.12 .09/.09 .09/.16 .27/.36 .17/.23 .33/.50
15. Block Satisfaction ns/.14 ns/.11 ns/.11 .07/ns -.06/ns .17/.29 .23/.37 .23/.45 .39/.34
16. Block Confidence .16/.28 .17/.14 .09/.13 .09/ns .12/ns .27/.24 .27/.31 .31/.28 .26/.20
Sense of Community/So
Note: For the highlighted diagonal cells, correlations are Time-1 by Time-2 for the respective variable, and are arranged as follows: Individual-evel / Block-
level (significant coefficients at p < .05 appear in boldfaced type on diagonal). For demographics, autocorrelations were not possible and intercorrelations are only at
one point in time, as they were asked only at T1 or T2. For the off-diagonal cells, correlations are arranged as follows: Time-1 / Time-2. All coefficients printed are
statistically significant at p < .05 or better at the individual level or p < .10 or better, block level; empty cells denote no significant correlation at T1 or T2.
Sense of Community/Social Capital 305
Of all the predictors, none showed greater or more reliable (i.e.,
significant at both T1 and T2) correlations with all three dependent variables
than did SOC (individual-level r = .26 to .42; block-level r = .34 to .63).
Like SOC, place attachment, communitarianism, and block confidence
showed significant and reliable correlations to all three dimensions of SC at
the individual level. Due to the much smaller n of blocks than individuals,
several of the corresponding block-level correlations, although larger, were
nonsignificant at either T1 or T2. Curiously, block satisfaction correlated
significantly with the three dimensions of SC at T2, but not at T1. This is
particularly surprising given the finding by Perkins, et al. (1990) that block
satisfaction was one of the strongest block-level predictors of participation at
T1, albeit moreso in multivariate than bivariate analyses. Both variables
were computed differently in the present analysis, however. (In Perkins, et
al. (1990), participation included items from a BA member survey and
satisfaction included block confidence items.)
Among demographics, home ownership and residential stability
were the strongest correlates of SC -- both were significantly related to
participation, neighboring, and SOC, but not to collective efficacy. Other
demographic effects were less consistent. Nonwhite residents and blocks
showed more collective efficacy at T1 (only), but more participation at T2.
In contrast, individual older residents participated more at T1, but felt more
collective efficacy at T2. Individuals and blocks with more children and
(unexpectedly) less education felt more collective efficacy at T1.
Multilevel Models Predicting Social Capital Dimensions
In a series of HLMs, SOC and four other community-focused
cognitions (place attachment, communitarianism, block satisfaction, and
community confidence), at block and individual levels, and individual-level
demographics were tested for their ability to predict collective efficacy,
informal neighboring, and formally organized citizen participation. Each of
the three dependent variables was predicted cross-sectionally at two points in
time, about a year apart, see Table 2).v
Collective Efficacy Time 1. In the HLM predicting collective
efficacy at T1, about six percent (p < .001) of the total variance in
individuals’ sense of the efficacy of BAs was due to block differences. The
only significant block-level predictors were SOC and communitarianism. At
the individual level, communitarianism, SOC, block confidence, block
satisfaction, and education were significant. Surprisingly, block satisfaction
and education were associated with less collective efficacy. The model
explains approximately 50% of block differences in collective efficacy and
306 Perkins and Long
13% of individual variance. In testing for random effects among the
individual-level substantive predictors, SOC was significant (p < .01),
indicating that the slope of the relationship between efficacy and SOC varies
across blocks. In an effort to explain that variation, we tested for significant
cross-level interactions with SOC, but none were found.
Collective Efficacy Time 2. At T2, just over 7% (p < .01) of the total
variance in individual collective efficacy occurred at the block level. SOC
and communitarianism were again significant block-level predictors, but this
time, so too is block confidence. At the individual level, SOC,
communitarianism, block confidence, minority status, and length of
residence were significant. Surprisingly, newer residents showed greater
collective efficacy. The model explains 99% of block differences in efficacy
and 25% of individual variance. There were no random effects.
Table 2. Block and individual-level sense of community and other predictors of three social
capital factors at two points in time: Hierarchical linear models
Collective Efficacy Participation Neighboring
Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2
Block Level: Approx. df 44 40 42 57 42 58
% total variance at block level 5.7*** 7.3** 30.5*** 40.0*** 3.0** 7.2***
% block variance explained 49.4 99.0 40.0 45.4 82.0 95.0
Intercept 13.80*** 12.81*** 1.01*** 0.77*** 1.15*** 1.12***
Sense of Community 1.23* 2.21** 1.36*** 1.32*** 0.17* 0.29***
Place Attachment -0.96** -0.93***
Communitarianism 2.71*** 1.98* 0.11#
Block Satisfaction -0.45# -0.17*
Block Confidence 2.62*** 0.80** 0.56** 0.14* 0.14#
Individual level: Approx. df 1,022 303 996 555 1,060 625
% individual variance explained 13.0 25.2 20.6 16.4 20.9 15.9
Children 0.09*** 0.05*
Age 0.10** -0.09***
Education -0.31* 0.07* 0.09** 0.06***
White Ethnicity -0.53*
Income 0.08*
Length of Residence -0.58* 0.12*** 0.07** 0.10***
Home Owner 0.13*** 0.18*** 0.08*** 0.08**
Sense of Community 0.87*** 1.84*** 0.24*** 0.20*** 0.22*** 0.16***
Place Attachment
Communitarianism 1.23*** 1.14*** 0.06* 0.12** 0.05*
Block Satisfaction -0.46* -0.04*
Block Confidence 0.86*** 0.83**
Note: Fixed effects unstandardized coefficients. #p<.10, *p.05, **p.01, ***p.001.
Participation Time 1. In the HLM predicting T1 participation, about
31% (p < .001) of the total variance in individuals’ participation in BAs is
due to block differences. Significant block-level predictors include SOC,
place attachment, block satisfaction, and block confidence. Unlike the
Sense of Community/Social Capital 307
bivariate correlations, which were modestly positive or nonsignificant, in the
HLM, block-level place attachment and satisfaction were associated with
less participation. At the individual level, SOC, communitarianism, number
of children, age, education, income, resident tenure, and home ownership
were significant. The model explains 40% of block differences in
participation and 21% of individual variance.
In testing for random effects, individual-level SOC was significant
(p < .01), with four significant cross-level interactions emerging. On blocks
with more children, more educated residents, more long-term residents, and
low communitarianism, the positive relation between SOC and participation
was stronger than elsewhere.
Participation Time 2. In the HLM predicting participation at T2,
40% (p < .001) of the total variance in individuals’ participation in BAs was
due to block differences. Significant block-level predictors again included
SOC, place attachment, and block confidence, but block satisfaction was
nonsignificant at T2. Block-level place attachment was again associated with
less participation. At the individual level, SOC, communitarianism,
education, and home ownership were significant. The model explains
approximately 45% of block differences in participation and 16% of
individual variance.
In testing for random effects, individual-level communitarianism
emerged as significant (p < .05). Four significant cross-level interactions
were identified to help explain the variation in slopes across blocks.
Communitarianism and participation were virtually unrelated on most blocks
(even marginally negatively related on some). However, on blocks with few
children, blocks with younger residents, blocks with more ethnic minority
residents, and those with more long-term residents, the relation between
communitarianism and participation was positive and much stronger.
Neighboring Time 1. In the HLM predicting neighboring at T1, three
percent (p < .01) of the total variance in individuals’ neighboring behavior is
due to block differences. Significant block-level predictors included SOC,
communitarianism, block satisfaction and block confidence. Surprisingly, in
the multivariate context, higher block-level satisfaction was associated with
less neighboring. At the individual level, SOC, block satisfaction, number of
children, education, resident tenure, and home ownership were significant.
Like at the block level, block satisfaction was associated with less
neighboring. The model explains 82% of block differences in neighboring
and 21% of individual variance. None of the random effects were significant.
Neighboring Time 2. In the HLM predicting neighboring at T2,
about seven percent (p < .001) of the total variance in individuals’ informal
neighboring behaviors is due to block differences. Significant block-level
predictors included SOC and block confidence. At the individual level, SOC,
communitarianism, age, length of residence, and home ownership were
significant. Neighboring behaviors decrease with age. The model explains
308 Perkins and Long
95% of block differences in neighboring and 16% of individual variance. No
random effects were significant.
CONCLUSIONS
This study represents a new, multi-level analysis of the original
Sense of Community Index data (Chavis, et al., 1987; Perkins, et al., 1990).
The BSCI used in the present analyses is shorter than previous scales and has
adequate psychometric properties (Perkins & Long, 2001). The data and
analyses we present meet the three criteria for validly deriving contextual or
social climate variables from group-aggregated individual responses.
Although, for all variables, block-level variances were less than individual-
level variances, the significance of all six HLM unconditional models, and
the many significant block-level predictors (between two and four out of five
in each model), confirm the existence of: (1) substantial within-block
agreement as to community-focused attitudes and behaviors, (2) significant
block differentiation in those variables (and in half the models, significant
block-level variation in slopes), and (3) predictable relations with other
block-level constructs (above diagonal, Table 1), as well as predictable
effects on individual-level SC outcomes in our HLM models. The variable
showing the most block-level variance was participation, which is not
surprising given that the sample included blocks with BAs of varying
activity, and about a third of the blocks had no BA. What is more noteworthy
is that at T2, SOC was as much a block level variable (30%; Perkins & Long,
2001) as participation was at T1.
Strong evidence was shown for our four-component definition of
SC. Each dimension was significantly correlated with at least two other
dimensions at the individual and block-aggregate levels. The only exception
was the nonsignificant link between collective efficacy and neighboring.
This is not surprising given that efficacy is the formal-intrapsychic
dimension and neighboring is the informal-behavioral dimension. The fact
that SOC (informal-intrapsychic) and participation (formal-behavioral) are
so highly correlated, particularly at the block level, is perhaps more
impressive. SOC emerged as the strongest and most consistent predictor (at
both levels) of the other three dimensions of SC. In fact, it was the only
individual-level predictor, including demographics, that was significant in all
six models and the only block-level predictor that was significant in all six.
Living on a block with higher mean SOC and (whether block SOC is high or
low) having higher individual SOC relative to one’s neighbors was related to
more collective efficacy, more neighboring, and more participation in block
organizations.
Our findings that SOC positively relates to neighboring and
participation in grassroots community organizations corroborate numerous
other studies (Beckman, et al., 1998; Brodsky, et al., 1999; Brown &
Sense of Community/Social Capital 309
Werner, 1985; Chavis & Wandersman, 1990; Hughey, et al., 1999; Itzhaky
& York, 2000; Perkins, et al., 1996; Prezza, et al., 2001; Wandersman &
Giamartino, 1980). What is new, in addition to finding the effects to be
significant at both the individual and community levels simultaneously, are
the cross-level interaction effects at T1: SOC and participation being most
closely linked on blocks with more children, more educated residents, more
long-term residents, and low communitarianism may help community
organizers and leaders target their organizing strategies accordingly. (The T2
cross-level interaction, in which communitarianism and participation were
slightly negatively related on blocks with more new residents but had a
clearly positive slope on more residentially stable blocks, may be due to
communitarians feeling alienated or frustrated on blocks with high turnover).
The link between SOC and collective efficacy (Perkins, et al., 1990)
had not been well established. Thus, the significance of SOC at both levels
and time points represents a major contribution to the literature. There are a
number of publications that deal with SOC and empowerment. But with very
few exceptions (e.g., Itzhaky & York, 2000; Speer, 2000), most of those are
either non-empirical or use both constructs as either independent or
dependent variables, rather than relating the two, which is surprising given
the prominence of both empowerment and SOC in community psychology.
Several other reliable effects (i.e., present at T1 and T2) were noted,
especially for community confidence, a construct that has been largely
ignored by psychologists. Individual and block mean communitarianism and
individual confidence in the block’s future related positively to perceptions
of collective efficacy. Individual resident tenure, home ownership, and a
block climate of community confidence related to higher rates of
neighboring. More confidence and less place attachment at the block level,
as well as individual home ownership and more education, related to higher
participation.
The negative coefficients for block-level place attachment should be
discounted as suppression effects as the bivariate correlations were modest,
but positive. Place attachment was strongly correlated with SOC at both
levels. It likely would be less so if the measures did not derive from items
taken from the same scale, as was necessary here. Place attachment is clearly
an important construct independent of SOC (Brown & Perkins, 1992; Cuba
& Hummon, 1993; Hyde, 1998; Li, 1998; Manzo & Perkins, 2001). Even
discounting the negative suppression effects, however, one of our most
surprising multivariate findings is that place attachment was largely
unrelated to collective efficacy, participation, and neighboring at both the
block and individual levels. It is not surprising that social attachments would
be more closely related to SC than are place attachments. In light of all the
evidence that place issues are critical to community participation and
development (Manzo & Perkins, 2001), however, place attachment deserves
further scrutiny in this context with a stronger measure than we had available
310 Perkins and Long
to us.
The following effects were significant (p < .01), but were less
reliable (i.e., appearing at just one time-point). Higher block-mean
community confidence related to higher individual perceptions of collective
efficacy. Greater resident tenure, more children in the home, and age were
associated with higher rates of participation in organizations. Neighboring
behaviors decreased with age, but increased with education. The correlations
with race suggest the possibility that nonwhite residents and blocks felt more
collective efficacy at T1, which may have resulted in higher participation at
T2. But the racial difference in efficacy was no longer significant at T2,
which may imply a degree of disappointment or frustration with their
organizations.
Collective efficacy has been shown in past research to be related to
organizational participation, both as an effect (Schultz, Israel, Zimmerman,
& Checkoway, 1995) and as a cause (Perkins, et al., 1996). Thus, policies
encouraging collective efficacy will have a positive impact on behavioral
dimensions of SC. In this study, individual perceptions of
communitarianism, SOC, and confidence in the future of the block were
strongly associated with increased collective efficacy. Living on a block with
high average SOC and communitarianism was related to higher individual
perceptions of collective efficacy. Although less reliable findings, collective
efficacy was also shown to increase with higher block mean confidence in
the future of the block, but decrease with individual education, length of
residence, and the proportion of white residents living on the block. This
may be due to longer-term, white, and more educated residents having more
personal ties to power and thus not needing as much formal collective
efficacy.
Like Rankin and Quane (2000), we also found a positive association
between greater education and participation in grassroots organizations.
However, where Rankin and Quane found no relation between participation
and number of children, age, employment status or tenure in the
neighborhood, we found that participation was greater among older, better
off (i.e., higher income), more tenured residents, and those with more
children. Our finding for age and participation is supported in another recent
study (Prezza, et al., 2001). Also like Prezza, et al., we found that
neighboring behavior increased with education and number of children in the
household. Unlike Prezza, et al., we found no relation between neighboring
behavior and sex. Controlling for other predictors, younger residents
engaged in more neighboring which, coupled with the above age-
participation link, suggests a possible developmental strategy for community
organizing: facilitate neighboring among young families (e.g., semi-
formalized baby-sitting co-ops), and later, as residents grow older and have
more time, they may participate in more formal organizations.
There are some constraints on the generalizability of the present
Sense of Community/Social Capital 311
findings. Comparisons between organized and non-organized blocks (not
reported) suggest that there may be unique social processes occurring on the
two types of blocks. The data are now 15 years old and social capital and
political processes may have changed. There are some important cultural,
political and economic differences between the neighborhoods selected for
this study. It may be questionable, therefore, to draw conclusions about the
entire sample (across all three neighborhoods) based on block and
individual-level data. It would be even more questionable to infer anything
about communities unlike those represented here. Some of the exceptional
features of the sample include: (a) two out of three areas being low-income
or working-class and minority yet with a large proportion of homeowners,
(b) all neighborhoods experiencing increasing rates of reported crime while
city-wide rates were holding steady or declining, and (c) a housing density
and architectural style that is more crowded and “urban” than most suburban
areas but less so than most of the rest of New York City or other large inner-
city residential areas. The sample is not unique, however. Each of these
characteristics describes the growing “inner ring” of poor and working-class
neighborhoods that are surrounding the gentrifying city cores throughout the
U.S. and other countries. The inhabitants of these ring neighborhoods have
either moved up and out of poorer inner-city areas or have been forced out of
neighborhoods with rapidly increasing housing costs.
Possibly the greatest concern with the present data is the relatively
weak internal consistency of the predictor scales due to a combination of few
items per scale and limited response options (dichotomous for many items).
SOC’s being most consistently related to the other SC dimensions may be
partly due to its having the most items (thus more variance) and highest α.
But given that its α is substantially higher than only block satisfaction, we
doubt that is the only explanation. With better scales, the already impressive
results would likely have been even stronger.
Puddifoot (1996) and others recommend the use of qualitative
methods. Clearly, the ideal study combines both qualitative and quantitative
methods. But as valuable as ethnographic data are, they have their own
reliability and validity limitations, including the fact that they generally
represent a small sample of individuals. New, truly community-level (not
aggregated individual-level) measures of sense of community and other
social capital constructs are needed (Shinn, 1990). They could be used in
multilevel analyses and provide descriptive or comparative context in
qualitative studies.
Our task was to search for more sharply defined and ecologically
valid conceptual, psychometric, and analytical “needles” in the haystack of
research and vague rhetoric on SC and SOC. We believe the dimensions and
predictors, measures, and multi-level analyses used here, while not perfect,
can only enhance the construct validity of SC and SOC. Both concepts
clearly have individual and community-level (not to mention organizational)
312 Perkins and Long
properties. Multi-level analysis gives us a sense of how much each concept
operates at the community, as well as individual, level and how they operate
at different levels simultaneously. The fact that SOC was such a strong and
consistent predictor at both levels suggests, not only that people with SOC
are more likely to help their neighbors, to join a BA, and to be empowered
by it, but that blocks with more SOC enjoy those same results even for
residents who may not share that SOC, but who get involved for more selfish
reasons. In future studies, we plan to use the other SC dimensions
(collective efficacy, neighboring, and participation) and other community-
focused cognitions, at the individual and block levels, to predict the BSCI
and its subscales. We hope the needles we have identified will help
researchers and community leaders and organizers knit tighter, more
politically effective neighborhood social fabrics.
NOTES
i Collection of the data reported was funded by the Ford Foundation (Co-Principal
Investigators: David Chavis, Paul Florin, Richard Rich, and Abe Wandersman). We thank
Chavis, Adrian Fisher, David McMillan, and Chris Sonn for their comments on the study that
developed the Brief Sense of Community Index (Perkins & Long, 2001) and Fisher and Jo
Lippe for editorial assistance with this chapter.
ii A PsycINFO search of “sense of community” found 398 publications through November,
2001, starting with a 1930 article. Sarason’s 1974 book was the 15th record and thus
something of a watershed. The 398 do not include works referring to “social cohesion,”
“community spirit” or other near synonyms.
iii ISC is the degree to which residents spontaneously regulate everyday public behaviors and
physical conditions within the bounds of their community. Although SOC and ISC are highly
correlated at both the block (r = .65; Perkins, et al., 1990) and neighborhood (r = .80;
Sampson et al., 1997) levels, other studies have generally treated them as separate constructs.
There is also a methodological/conceptual problem with ISC in that it is often thought of as a
behavior but typically measured as a cognition (e.g., prediction of how neighbors would act in
hypothetical situations, e.g., youths painting graffiti). Clearly more work needs to be done
measuring actual ISC behaviors and comparing them to perceived ISC. Given the high
correlations between measures of SOC and ISC, Sampson may be justified in combining the
two, but should perhaps add neighboring items and call it “informal collective efficacy.”
vi The SCI is often incorrectly cited. It was developed in 1985 by Chavis and colleagues for
use with the present dataset and published in the appendix of Perkins, et al. (1990). Although
it was ostensibly based on McMillan and Chavis’ (1986) theory, their four dimensions have
not been found in the SCI factor structure in these and other data. Furthermore, McMillan had
nothing to do with creating the SCI and has challenged its validity. Chavis, Hogge, McMillan,
and Wandersman (1986) used a 46-item scale (including component scale items) called the
Sense of Community Profile, which is much broader than the SCI and includes many other
constructs, such as participation and neighboring behaviors, collective efficacy, community
satisfaction, perceived block conditions, and even demographics, such as home ownership and
length of residence.
v Each procedure began with an “unconditional” model indicating the amount of variance in
the dependent variable due to differences in groups (blocks). In step two, demographic control
variables (income, age, race, sex, education, children, home ownership, length of residence)
Sense of Community/Social Capital 313
were added at the individual level. (Sex was not a significant predictor in any model.) In step
three, all nonsignificant demographics were removed and the five cognitive predictors were
added at both the block and individual (block-mean centered) levels. (Cognitive predictors at
the individual level are each deviations from the mean of one’s block so as to be independent
of their block-level counterparts.) In multi-level analysis, degrees of freedom are more limited
both within groups and across groups. Therefore, in step four, all remaining nonsignificant
(block-level p > .10; individual-level p > .05) predictors were trimmed to produce the most
parsimonious model (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). As this increases the risk of Type-I errors,
each step-four model was compared with the corresponding step-three model and the
correlations in Table 1. In step five, each remaining individual-level cognitive predictor was
tested, one-by-one, for a significant random effect, which would indicate a cross-level
interaction. First, block-level demographic variables were modeled in interaction with the
significant random individual-level predictor. Second, all nonsignificant (at p < .10)
interactional demographics were trimmed before modeling the five block-level cognitive
predictors. Third, any nonsignificant block-level interactional predictors were trimmed from
the model. Interpretation of cross-level interactions used a strategy exemplified by Watson,
Chemers, and Preiser (2001) in which the relation between the individual-level interactional
predictor and the outcome variable was compared differentially between high and low (one
SD above and below the mean) status on the block-level interactional predictor.
REFERENCES
Ahlbrandt, R. S. (1984). Neighborhoods, people and community. New York: Plenum.
Beckman, P. J., Barnwell, D., Horn, E., Hanson, M. J., Gutierrez, S., & Lieber, J. (1998).
Communities, families, and inclusion. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13,
125-150.
Briggs, X. S. (1998). Brown kids in white suburbs: Housing mobility and the many faces of
social capital. Housing Policy Debate, 9, 177-221.
Brodsky, A. E., O’Campo, P. J., & Aronson, R. E. (1999). PSOC in community context:
Multi-Level correlates of a measure of psychological sense of community in low-
income, urban neighborhoods. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 659-679.
Brown, B. B., & Perkins, D. D. (1992). Disruptions in place attachment. In I. Altman and S.
Low (Eds.), Place attachment (pp. 279-304). New York: Plenum.
Brown, B. B., & Perkins, D. D. (2001). Neighborhood revitalization and disorder: An
intervention evaluation. Final Report to the National Institute of Justice. Salt Lake
City: University of Utah.
Brown, B. B., & Werner, C. M. (1985). Social cohesiveness, territoriality, and holiday
decorations: The influence of cul-de-sacs. Environment and Behavior, 17, 539-565.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbusch, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and
data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Buckner, J. C. (1988). The development of an instrument to measure neighborhood cohesion.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 771-791.
Chavis, D. M., Florin, P., Rich, R. C., Wandersman, A., & Perkins, D. D. (1987). The role of
block associations in crime control and community development: The Block Booster
Project. (Final Report to Ford Foundation). New York: Citizens Committee for New
York City.
Chavis, D. M., Hogge, J., McMillan, D., & Wandersman, A. (1986). Sense of community
through Brunswik’s Lens. Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 24-40.
Chavis, D. M., & Wandersman, A. (1990). Sense of community in the urban environment: A
catalyst for participation and community development. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 18, 55-82.
Chipuer, H. M., & Pretty, G. M. H. (1999). A review of the sense of community index:
314 Perkins and Long
Current uses, factor structure, reliability, and further development. Journal of
Community Psychology, 27, 643-658.
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of
Sociology, 94, S95-S120.
Cuba, L., & Hummon, D. M. (1993). A place to call home: Identification with dwelling,
community, and region. Sociological Quarterly, 34, 111-131.
Etzioni, A. (1993). Spirit of community: Rights, responsibilities, and the communitarian
agenda. New York: Crown.
Fisher, A. T., & Sonn, C. C. (1999). Aspiration to community: Community responses to
rejection. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 715-725.
Florin, P., & Wandersman, A. (1984). Cognitive social learning and participation in
community development. American Journal of Community Psychology, 12, 689-
708.
Garcia, I., Giuliani, F., & Wiesenfeld, E. (1999). Community and sense of community: The
case of an urban barrio in Caracas. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 727-740.
Hughey, J., Speer, P. W., & Peterson, N. A. (1999). Sense of community in community
organizations: Structure and evidence of validity. Journal of Community
Psychology, 27, 97-113.
Hyde, M. M. (1998). Local sentiments in urban neighborhoods: Multilevel models of sense of
community and attachment to place. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. University
of Maryland-Baltimore County.
Hyman, H., & Wright, C. (1971). Trends in voluntary association memberships of American
adults: Replication based on secondary analysis of national sample surveys.
American Sociological Review, 36, 191-206.
Itzhaky, H., & York, A. S. (2000). Sociopolitical control and empowerment: An extended
replication. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 407-415.
Kenny, D. A., & Lavoie, L. (1985). Separating individual and group effects. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 339-348.
Kingston, S., Mitchell, R., Florin, P., & Stevenson, J. (1999). Sense of community in
neighborhoods as a multi-level construct. Journal of Community Psychology, 27,
681-694.
Kretzmann, J. P., & McKnight, J. L. (1993). Building communities from the inside out: A path
toward finding and mobilizing a community’s assets. Chicago: ACTA.
Li, C. (1998). The contribution of common land to sense of community. In J. Sanford & B. R.
Connell (Eds.), People, places and public policy. (pp. 57-70). Edmond, OK:
Environmental Design Research Association.
Manzo, L. C., & Perkins, D. D. (2001, July). Neighborhoods as common ground: The
importance of place attachment to community participation and development. Paper
presented to the Environmental Design Research Association, Edinburgh, Scotland.
McMillan, D. W., & Chavis, D. M. (1986). Sense of community: A definition and theory.
Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 6-23.
Nasar, J. L. & Julian, D. A. (1995). The psychological sense of community in the
neighborhood. Journal of the American Planning Association, 61, 178-184.
Perkins, D. D. (1995). Speaking truth to power: Empowerment ideology as social intervention
and policy. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 765-794.
Perkins, D. D., Brown, B. B., & Taylor, R. B. (1996). The ecology of empowerment:
Predicting participation in community organizations. Journal of Social Issues, 52,
85-110.
Perkins, D. D., Florin, P., Rich, R. C., Wandersman, A., & Chavis, D. M. (1990). Participation
and the social and physical environment of residential blocks: Crime and
community context. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 83-115.
Perkins, D. D. & Long, D. A. (2001). Neighborhood sense of community and social capital:
“All the things you are.” Paper presented at the Biennial Conference on Community
Research & Action, Atlanta, June.
Sense of Community/Social Capital 315
Perkins, D. D., Wandersman, A., Rich, R. C., & Taylor, R. B. (1993). The physical
environment of street crime: Defensible space, territoriality and incivilities. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, 13, 29-49.
Plas, J. M., & Lewis, S. E. (1996). Environmental factors and sense of community in a
planned town. American Journal of Community Psychology, 24, 109-143.
Podolefsky, A. (1983). Case studies in community crime prevention. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Pretty, G. (1990). Relating psychological sense of community to social climate characteristics.
Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 16-65.
Pretty, G. M. H., Conroy, C., Dugay, J., Fowler, K., & Williams, D. (1996). Sense of
community and its relevance to adolescents of all ages. Journal of Community
Psychology, 24, 365-379.
Prezza, M., Amici, M., Roberti, T., & Tedeschi, G. (2001). Sense of community referred to
the whole town: Its relations with neighboring, loneliness, life satisfaction, and area
of residence. Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 29-52.
Puddifoot, J.E. (1996). Some initial considerations in the measurement of community identity.
Journal of Community Psychology, 24, 327-336
Putnam, R.D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New
York: Simon & Schuster.
Rankin, B. H., & Quane, J. M. (2000). Neighborhood poverty and the social isolation of inner-
city African American families. Social Forces, 79, 139-164.
Saegert, S. (1989). Unlikely leaders, extreme circumstances: Older Black women building
community households. American Journal of Community Psychology, 17, 295-316.
Saegert, S., & Winkel, G. (1996). Paths to community empowerment: Organizing at home.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 24, 517-550.
Saegert, S., & Winkel, G. (1998). Social capital and the revitalization of New York City’s
distressed inner city housing. Housing Policy Debate, 9, 17-60.
Sampson, R.J. (1991). Linking the micro- and macrolevel dimensions of community social
organization. Social Forces, 70, 43-64.
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A
multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918-926.
Sarason, S. B. (1974). The psychological sense of community: Prospects for a community
psychology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Schultz, A. J., Israel, B. A., Zimmerman, M. A., & Checkoway, B. N. (1995). Empowerment
as a multi-level construct: Perceived control at the individual, organizational and
community levels. Health Education Research: Theory & Practice, 10, 309-327.
Shinn, M. (1990). Mixing and matching: Levels of conceptualization, measurement, and
statistical analysis in community research. In P. Tolan, C. Keys, F. Chertok, & L.
Jason (Eds.), Researching community psychology: Integrating theories and methods
(pp. 111-126). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sonn, C. C., & Fisher, A. T. (1998). Sense of community: Community resilient responses to
oppression and change. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 457-472.
Speer, P. W. (2000). Intrapersonal and interactional empowerment: Implications for theory.
Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 51-61.
Speer, P. W., & Hughey, J. (1995). Community organizing: An ecological route to
empowerment and power. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 729-
748.
Theodori, G. L. (2000). Levels of analysis and conceptual clarification in community
attachment and satisfaction research: Connections to community development.
Journal of the Community Development Society, 31, 35-58.
Tocqueville, A. de (1935/1969). Democracy in America. (G. Lawrence, Trans.; J.P. Mayer,
Ed.). New York: Doubleday.
Unger, D. G., & Wandersman, A. (1983). Neighboring and its role in block organizations.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 291-300.
Unger, D. G., & Wandersman, A. (1985). The importance of neighbors: The social, cognitive,
316 Perkins and Long
and affective components of neighboring. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 13, 139-170.
Varady, D. P. (1986). Neighborhood confidence: A critical factor in neighborhood
revitalization? Environment and Behavior, 18, 480-501.
Vinsel, A., Brown, B. B., Altman, I., & Foss, C. (1980). Privacy regulation, territorial
displays, and the effectiveness of individual functioning. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 39, 1104-1115.
Wandersman, A., & Giamartino, G. A. (1980). Community and individual difference
characteristics as influences on initial participation. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 8, 217-228.
Watson, C. B., Chemers, M. M., & Preiser, N. (2001). Collective efficacy: A multilevel
analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1057-1068.
Zhao, L. (1996). Community attachment in rural Iowa: Multilevel test of a theoretical model
with intervening variables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Iowa State
University.
Sense of Community/Social Capital 317
APPENDIX A: Social Capital Survey Scales
Brief Sense of Community Index (overall scale α Time 1 (T1) = .65, n =
713; α Time 2 (T2) = .74, n = 422):
Social Connections Subscale (α = .55 (T1), .50 (T2)):
Instructions for items 1-5: “I am going to read some things that people might
say about their block. For each one, please indicate whether it is mostly true
or mostly false about your block” (coded 1 = “false”, 2 = “true”; Note: Likert
scale recommended for future research).
1. Very few of my neighbors know me. (Reverse)
2. I have almost no influence over what this block is like. (Reverse)
3. I can recognize most of the people who live on my block.
Mutual Concern Subscale (α = .50 (T1), .64 (T2)):
4. My neighbors and I want the same things from the block.
5. If there is a problem on this block people who live here can get it
solved.
6. In general, would you say that people on your block watch after each
other and help out when they can, or do they pretty much go their own
way? (coded 1 = “go own way”, 2 = “a little of both”, 3 = “watch after”)
Community Values Subscale (Face-valid SOC; α = .51 (T1), .61 (T2):
7. Would you say that it is very important, somewhat important or not
important to you to feel a sense of community with the people on your
block? (coded 1= “not”, 2= “somewhat ”, 3= “very”)
8. Some people say they feel like they have a sense of community with the
people on their block; others don’t feel that way. How about you; would
you say that you feel a strong sense of community with others on your
block, very little sense of community or something in between? (coded
1 = “very little”, 2 = “in between”, 3 = “strong”)
Collective Efficacy Scale (α (T1) = .82, n = 918; α (T2) = .82, n = 270):
“The following are things a block association might try to do. For each
one, indicate whether you think it is very likely, somewhat likely, or not
likely that the association on your block can accomplish that goal”
(coded 1 = “not likely” to 3 = “very likely”).
1. Improve physical conditions on the block like cleanliness or housing
upkeep.
2. Persuade the city to provide better services to people on the block.
3. Get people on the block to help each other more.
4. Reduce crime on the block.
5. Get people who live on the block to know each other better.
6. Get information to residents about where to go for services they need.
Citizen Participation Scale (α (T1) = .78, n = 384; α (T2) = .80, n = 184):
1. Are you currently a member of the block association?
318 Perkins and Long
2. Have you ever taken part in an activity sponsored by the block
association?
3. Thinking about work you might do for the block association outside of
meetings, how many hours would you say you give to the association
each month, if any?
“We would like to know what kinds of things people have done in the
association. In the past year have you:”
4. Attended a meeting,
5. Spoken up during a meeting,
6. Done work for the organization outside of meetings,
7. Served as a member of a committee,
8. Served as an officer or as a committee chair?
Note: Each item was coded 1 for participation and 0 for no participation
(#3 was recoded to match this scale, from 0 = “none” to 1 = “8 or more
hours”).
Neighboring Behavior Scale (α (T1) = .78, n = 1,037; α (T2) = .77, n =
615):
“The following is a short list of things neighbors might do for each other.
Please indicate how many times in the past year, you have been asked to
do each one for a neighbor on this block” (coded 0 = “none”, 1-7 = “exact
number”, and 8 = “eight or more”).
1. Watch a neighbor’s home while they were away.
2. Loan a neighbor some food or a tool.
3. Help a neighbor in an emergency.
4. Offer a neighbor advice on a personal problem.
5. Discuss a problem on the block with a neighbor.
... Thus, the involvement of citizens/residents in city projects is rooted in their sense of belonging (Lebrument et al. 2021). Naturally, citizens with an increased sense of belonging will show an increased level of participation in activities expected to improve the community (Perkins and Long 2002;Soria, Troisi, and Stebleton 2003). As Shotter (1993) places it, the sense of belonging ignites the will to participate in society's development. ...
... Hagerty et al. (1992Hagerty et al. ( , 1996 relate to the sense of belonging, the terms "energy for involvement" and "meaningful involvement," and they expect it to be witnessed through meaningful physical, social, as well as psychological involvement. Therefore, a developed sense of belonging would increase citizens' interest in participating in the community's common issues (Perkins and Long 2002;Soria et al. 2003). For example, the increased desire to participate could boost interest in citizen-council cooperations in the form of co-creation or co-development of services (Cohen and Karatzimas 2023). ...
... De los factores ya mencionados, el sentido de comunidad, apego de lugar y participación, juegan un papel determinante en el nivel de resilencia que alcanzan las comunidades afectadas por desastres (Bird, Gísladóttir y Dominey-Howes, 2011;. Asimismo, el apego de lugar y el sentido de comunidad inciden considerablemente en la posibilidad de revitalización de los vecindarios (Perkins y Long, 2002;Manzo y Perkins, 2006), de modo tal "que en los casos en donde no hay conexión emocional de las personas a los lugares, estas no suelen estar lo suficientemente comprometidas para trabajar con los vecinos e instituciones locales en el mejoramiento del entorno" (Berroeta et al., 2014, p.4). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Catastrofes, conducta colectiva pro activa y antisocial: estado de la cuestion y comentarios al caso chileno
... Additionally, many antenatal classes incorporate practical coping strategies and relaxation techniques, equipping women with tangible skills to manage pain and stress during childbirth 64 . The group setting of many programs may also foster a sense of community and shared experience, further bolstering confidence and reducing isolation-related fears 65 . ...
Article
Full-text available
INTRODUCTION Antenatal education programs aim to prepare expectant mothers for childbirth and early parenthood. This meta-analysis assessed the impact of these programs on maternal psychological outcomes and birth experiences, focusing on childbirth self-efficacy, fear of childbirth, and maternal and neonatal outcomes, including rates of vaginal delivery, cesarean section, Apgar scores, and birth weight. METHODS A systematic search was conducted in PubMed, Web of Science, SCOPUS, and Cochrane Library until July 2024. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) comparing antenatal education to standard care were included. Data were synthesized using meta-analysis with standardized mean differences (SMD) for continuous outcomes and risk ratios (RR) for dichotomous outcomes. RESULTS Forty studies were reviewed, with 31 eligible for meta-analysis. Among 1116 pregnant women, antenatal education significantly increased childbirth self-efficacy (SMD=2.00; 95% CI: 1.06-2.95, p<0.0001) and decreased fear of childbirth (SMD=-1.26; 95% CI:-1.79-0.74, p<0.00001). Maternal outcomes showed higher vaginal delivery rates (RR=1.10; 95% CI: 1.04-1.16, p=0.0004) and lower cesarean rates (RR=0.80; 95% CI: 0.70-0.92, p=0.001). No significant differences were found in episiotomy rates, Apgar scores, or birth weight. CONCLUSIONS Antenatal education improves maternal psychological outcomes and promotes vaginal delivery. However, high heterogeneity and potential bias in the studies limit generalizability. More research is needed on long-term impacts and effectiveness in low-resource settings.
... Additionally, many antenatal classes incorporate practical coping strategies and relaxation techniques, equipping women with tangible skills to manage pain and stress during childbirth 64 . The group setting of many programs may also foster a sense of community and shared experience, further bolstering confidence and reducing isolation-related fears 65 . ...
Article
Full-text available
INTRODUCTION Antenatal education programs aim to prepare expectant mothers for childbirth and early parenthood. This meta-analysis assessed the impact of these programs on maternal psychological outcomes and birth experiences, focusing on childbirth self-efficacy, fear of childbirth, and maternal and neonatal outcomes, including rates of vaginal delivery, cesarean section, Apgar scores, and birth weight. METHODS A systematic search was conducted in PubMed, Web of Science, SCOPUS, and Cochrane Library until July 2024. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) comparing antenatal education to standard care were included. Data were synthesized using meta-analysis with standardized mean differences (SMD) for continuous outcomes and risk ratios (RR) for dichotomous outcomes. RESULTS Forty studies were reviewed, with 31 eligible for meta-analysis. Among 1116 pregnant women, antenatal education significantly increased childbirth self-efficacy (SMD=2.00; 95% CI: 1.06–2.95, p<0.0001) and decreased fear of childbirth (SMD= -1.26; 95% CI: -1.79 – -0.74, p<0.00001). Maternal outcomes showed higher vaginal delivery rates (RR=1.10; 95% CI: 1.04–1.16, p=0.0004) and lower cesarean rates (RR=0.80; 95% CI: 0.70–0.92, p=0.001). No significant differences were found in episiotomy rates, Apgar scores, or birth weight. CONCLUSIONS Antenatal education improves maternal psychological outcomes and promotes vaginal delivery. However, high heterogeneity and potential bias in the studies limit generalizability. More research is needed on long-term impacts and effectiveness in low-resource settings.
... Individuals' attachment to a place also fosters their behaviors and attitudes that reinforce social ties (Brown et al., 2004), although its development requires an extended period (Brown & Perkins, 1992). Moreover, those who feel attached to their immediate neighborhoods develop a sense of community, neighborliness, and mutual support, which are deeply related to SC (Long & Perkins, 2007;Perkins & Long, 2002). Similarly, residents feel a sense of neighborhood cohesion, trust, and normality, which are important elements of SC (Brown et al., 2004;Lewicka, 2005). ...
Article
Liderlik izleyenleri etkileme ile ilgili bir kavram iken öz liderlik kavramı kişinin kendisini etkileyerek kendi performansını artırması olarak ifade edilir. Yenilikçilik, bireysel ve grup düzeyinde fikir üreterek yenilik oluşturmayı amaçlar. Kolektif yeterlilik, ortak bir amacı gerçekleştirmek için üyelerin inançla, birlikte hareket etmesini gerektirir. Öz liderlik bireysel, yenilikçilik bireysel ve grup olarak, kolektif yeterlilik ise bireylerin grup olarak birlikte hareket etmelerini gerektiren kavramlardır. Bu çalışmada kolektif yeterliliğin öz liderlik ile yenilikçilik ilişkisinde aracı etki gösterip göstermediği incelenmiştir. Araştırmada öncelikle literatür taraması yapılmıştır. Anket tekniğinden yararlanılarak Kahramanmaraş’ta faaliyetlerini sürdüren 555 esnaf ve sanatkâr işletme sahibinden veriler toplanmıştır. Veriler SPSS kullanılarak analiz edilmiştir. Analiz kapsamında hipotezleri doğrulayabilmek için hiyerarşik regresyon ve process makro analizleri yapılmıştır. Doğrudan etki analiz bulgularına göre; öz liderliğin yenilikçilik üzerinde anlamlı etkisi olduğu, öz liderliğin kolektif yeterlilik üzerinde anlamlı etkisi olduğu, kolektif yeterliliğin yenilikçilik üzerinde anlamlı etkiye sahip olduğu, öz liderlik ve kolektif yeterliliğin birlikte yenilikçilik üzerinde anlamlı etkisi olduğu ve etki düzeyinin anlamlı olmak üzere azaldığı (β=.485 ten β=.444’e; düzeltilmiş R2 .243’ten .255’e) görülmüştür. Dolaylı etki analizinde ise; öz liderliğin kolektif yeterlilikle birlikte yenilikçilik üzerindeki dolaylı etkisinin de (β = 0,670; güven aralığı değerleri: 0,0216, 0,1148) anlamlı olduğu sunucu elde edilmiştir. Hem doğrudan etki hem de dolaylı etki bulgularının birlikte değerlendirilmesi sonucu kolektif yeterliliğin öz liderlik ile yenilikçilik ilişkisinde aracı etkiye sahip olduğu görülmektedir.
Thesis
Full-text available
This research explores how a Sense of Community (SoC) emerges through the process of citizen participation and the characteristics of space. During her PhD, Francesca developed and applied a theoretical framework based on her original definition of SoC. She combines three dimensions of Sense of Community, a perceived, a social, and a spatial one, and applies it to the study of the Dutch urban fringes. The framework is designed for action-oriented research around participation, focusing on the redeveloped and newly built neighbourhoods of the city of Almere. Recent studies and policies have pointed to the tendency of localism and marginalization of communities in urban fringes. The study shows how methods and tools of participatory practices and urban design help develop place attachment and SoC, especially in the absence of historical community roots, more typical of core cities and historical satellite towns. First, she tested the framework in a pilot case study in Kerckebosch Zeist, The Netherlands, using a mix of qualitative methods, engaging residents through walk-along discussions, interviews, and mental maps. Following those designed methods, she conducted fieldwork in the city of Almere, The Netherlands, focusing on the neighbourhood of Almere Poort, one of those undergoing the most rapid transformation today. As one of the main cities in the Amsterdam Metropolitan Area and amongst the fastest-growing cities in Europe, the extensive development of Almere draws on a polynuclear urban structure of the various neighbourhoods. In this context, Almere Poort represents a shift from the classic top-down approach of suburban expansion to one more led to participatory planning. The fieldwork included walk-along discussions, semi-structured interviews, and art-based and design methods with children, teenagers, and intergenerational groups. The analysis shows the important role that intergenerational spaces of encounter play in facilitating Sense of Community. It also shows that different participatory approaches interact with the specific design features of each neighbourhood and in turn lead to distinct community ties. The research found that neighbourhood centres target the social inclusion of specific age groups, such as the elderly and families with financial problems, while excluding other groups, such as young people and middle-aged adults. The thesis brings forward also a methodological contribution to engaged urban research: co-design and art-based methods help include different age groups in the planning of public spaces and, in turn, to develop place attachment, a necessary condition for the Sense of Community.
Chapter
Full-text available
A study of disruptions in psychological processes can provide unique insight into their predisruption functioning as well as the disruptions themselves and their consequences. Place attachment processes normally reflect the behavioral, cognitive, and emotional embeddedness individuals experience in their sociophysical environments. An examination of disruptions in place attachments demonstrate how fundamental they are to the experience and meaning of everyday life. After the development of secure place attachments, the loss of normal attachments creates a stressful period of disruption followed by a postdisruption phase of coping with lost attachments and creating new ones. These three phases of the disruption process are examined with respect to disruptions due to burglaries, voluntary relocations, and disasters, with special attention to the Buffalo Creek, West Virginia, flood and the Yungay, Peru, landslide. Underlying the diversity of disruptions, dialectic themes of stability-change and individuality-communality provide a coherent framework for understanding the temporal phases of attachment and its disruption.
Article
A framework for measuring psychological sense of community for community organizations was presented, and an instrument to measure community organization sense of community was developed. The framework consisted of four components: Relationship to the Organization, Organization as Mediator, Influence of the Community Organization, and Bond to the Community. Two studies examined the dimensionality, reliability, and validity of the instrument. Study 1 (n = 218) was conducted with participants from three community organizations, and it identified four factors, matching the framework, with alpha coefficients from .61 to .85. Study 2 (n = 1,676) was conducted with participants from five community organizations. Study 2 participants were 48% African American, 42% White, 6% Latino/Hispanic, and 3% Other. Also for the Study 2 sample, 69% were female; 31% were male. Study 2 confirmed three factors for the Community Organization Sense of Community scale (COSOC): Relationship to the Organization, Organization as Mediator, Bond to the Community; alpha coefficients ranged from .82 to .87. In three subsamples of Study 2, convergent validity of the instrument was explored by correlating total COSOC scores and subscale scores with two other measures of sense of community, political participation, community involvement, community organization involvement, and perceived safety. The patterns of correlation among the variables indicated, with one exception: strong support for validity of the instrument. Findings are discussed in terms of implications for development of sense of community in community organizations, and community participation. © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Article
The short form of the Sense of Community Index (SCI) (Chavis, Hogge, McMillan, & Wandersman, 1986) was assessed in terms of the four dimensions of psychological sense of community (PSC) proposed by McMillan and Chavis (1986). Four sets of data were used. They measured PSC in the neighborhood for adults and adolescents, and workplace PSC for adults, using true/false and three-point response formats. Reliabilities for the total SCI scores ranged from .64 to .69. Most subscale reliabilities were below acceptable levels, ranging from a low of .16 to a high of .72. Factor analyses showed some support for the existence of the four dimensions of the McMillan and Chavis PSC model in the SCI. However, they were not consistent across data sets. Further work to develop the SCI as a measure representative of the PSC model is outlined, with implications for adult and adolescent populations. © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Article
The aim was to explore the relationships between sense of community and various factors with respect to a fairly broad area (town, city, or large quarter of a metropolis). Degree of neighboring, life satisfaction, loneliness, and area of residence were also considered. Subjects included 630 men and women, aged 20-65 years, with different educational levels. They were individually administered a sociodemographic questionnaire, the Italian Sense of Community Scale, the Satisfaction with Life Scale, the University of California Loneliness Scale, and a Neighborhood Relations Scale. The subjects all live in Central Italy. They were divided into six groups as follows: one group living in a quarter of Rome, three groups living in three different areas of Grottaferrata (a hill town near Rome) and two groups living in two areas of Spoleto (the historical center and a working class suburb), a town in the Umbria region. Multiple regression analysis revealed the following: Neighborhood relations are stronger for women, for members of large families, for those with less education, for those living in the community for many years and for members of groups or associations. The strongest predictor of sense of community is neighborhood relations, although years of residence, being married, group participation, and area of residence are also significant factors. Sense of community is related to life satisfaction and loneliness in both the large and small town and in the city. Moreover, although sense of community is strongly associated with area of residence in Spoleto, this is not true for Grottaferrata. Overall, the results confirm the usefulness of conceptualizing the sense of community construct separately }}}{{{� Journal of Community Psychology, January 2001 }}}{{{from degree of neighboring. © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Book
1 A Community Focus.- Concept of Community.- The Research Context.- The Survey.- What Follows.- 2 The Community of Action.- Discussion of the Research Findings.- Personal Network.- Role of Place.- Socioeconomic Constraints.- Discussion.- Appendix A. Analysis of Various Aspects of Community by the Socioeconomic Characteristics of the Residents.- Utilization of Neighborhood Facilities.- Neighboring.- Organizational Involvement.- Kin in Neighborhood.- Friends in Neighborhood.- Work in Neighborhood.- Primary Social Relationships.- Intimate Ties.- 3 Attitudes about Neighborhoods.- Affective Sentiments.- Analysis.- Discussion of Results.- Quality of Life.- Social Fabric.- Respondent Characteristics.- Conclusion.- Appendix B. Description of the Independent Variables.- Quality of Life Variables.- Social Fabric Variables.- Respondent Characteristics.- 4 Religion, Life Cycle, and Race.- Reasons for Moving to the Neighborhood.- Things Liked Best about the Neighborhood.- Things Liked Least about the Neighborhood.- Planning to Remain in the Neighborhood.- Additional Analysis.- Conclusion.- 5 The Impact of Neighborhood Support Systems on the Individual.- The Model.- Description of the Variables.- Happiness and Life Satisfaction.- Health.- Socioeconomic and Demographic Characteristics.- Social Support System.- Neighborhood Attributes.- Satisfaction with Housing and Neighborhood.- Results.- Discussion.- 6 Neighborhood Social Fabric.- Internal and External Ties.- Relationships among the Variables.- Discussion.- Relationship to Neighborhood Attitudes.- Neighborhood Attachment.- Satisfaction.- Relationship to Life Happiness and Life Satisfaction.- Discussion.- 7 Social Fabric and Neighborhood Change.- The Model.- Analysis.- Lower-Income Neighborhoods.- Discussion.- 8 The Neighborhood Context.- Neighborhood Classification.- Affective Sentiments.- Use of Neighborhood Facilities.- Neighboring.- Social Supports.- Organizational Involvement.- Life Happiness.- Discussion.- 9 The Implications of Income.- High-Income Neighborhoods.- Discussion.- Middle-Income Neighborhoods.- Discussion.- Moderate-Income Neighborhoods.- Primary Ties.- Affective Sentiments.- Neighboring.- Participation.- Happiness and Life Satisfaction.- Use of Neighborhood Facilities.- Characteristics of Residents.- Discussion.- Lower-Income Neighborhoods.- Conclusion.- 10 The Neighborhood and Beyond.- Discussion.- Policy Implications.- Appendix C. Survey Questionnaire.
Article
A small but noteworthy increase in the percentage of American adults who belong to voluntary associations has occurred since the mid-1950's (the date of an earlier study of this topic by the authors), as documented through several replications of national sample surveys. These same replications also confirm a major generalization of the earlier study that such membership is less common than had been assumed; indeed, voluntary association membership is not characteristic of the majority of American adults. Data from the replications confirm the previously demonstrated relationship between membership and major socioeconomic characteristics; but subgroup trends suggest that during the more recent period there has been a sharper growth in associational membership among the lower status groups. Although the findings are not completely consistent, there also seems to have been a sharper growth in association memberships on the part of Negroes. All of these findings, like those in the earlier study which has been replicated, are based on secondary analysis of national sample surveys. These social trends should be systematically measured and documented in the future by additional replications and new primary surveys.
Article
Although social isolation has been posited as a critical structural mechanism linking neighborhood disadvantage to the reduced life chances of inner-city residents, there have been few empirical tests of this proposition. We examine the relative importance of neighborhood poverty and individual and family characteristics on social-network composition and community organizational participation of inner-city Chicago African American families. The results of the multilevel analysis indicate that, while most of the variation in isolation outcomes is due to individual-level respondent characteristics, social-network composition and some forms of organizational participation are affected by neighborhood poverty. Implications of the findings and directions for future research are discussed.