ArticlePDF Available

Sexual selection, male morphology, and the efficacy of courtship signalling in two wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae)

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Males of the brush-legged wolf spider, Schizocosa ocreata (Araneae: Lycosidae), possess a conspicuous male secondary sexual character: dark pigmentation and tufts of bristles on the tibiae of their forelegs. We tested several hypotheses relating to the role of this conspicuous trait in sexual selection. Triad mating experiments suggest that the tufts do not play an obvious role in the operation of sexual selection by either male competition or female choice, as there were no significant differences in the mating success of intact and experimentally shaved males. However, females mated more often with males that initiated courtship first, suggesting that capture of a female’s attention by male signalling may play a critical role. In behavioral experiments that paired a single male with a female in arenas that allowed both visual and vibratory signal transmission during courtship, female receptivity did not vary significantly with the presence or absence of tufts. However, experiments that isolated the visual component of communication (by eliminating vibratory communication) revealed a significant effect of the presence of tufts: females showed receptivity less often to males with tufts removed. Female response to visual signals was much greater in S. ocreata than in its sibling congener, Schizocosa rovneri, which lacks male tufts. We hypothesize that the tufts serve to increase the efficacy of visual displays of S. ocreata, as vibratory communication is constrained by the complex leaf litter habitat of some populations. Such environmental constraints may make visual signalling over distance a critical factor for effective courtship communication, which may in turn strongly influence male fitness.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1996) 38 : 17–23 © Springer-Verlag 1996
Sonja J. Scheffer · George W. Uetz · Gail E. Stratton
Sexual selection, male morphology, and the efficacy of courtship
signalling in two wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae)
Received: 30 September 1994 / Accepted after revision: 4 August 1995
Abstract Males of the brush-legged wolf spider,
Schizocosa ocreata (Araneae: Lycosidae), possess a
conspicuous male secondary sexual character: dark pig-
mentation and tufts of bristles on the tibiae of their
forelegs. We tested several hypotheses relating to the
role of this conspicuous trait in sexual selection. Triad
mating experiments suggest that the tufts do not play
an obvious role in the operation of sexual selection by
either male competition or female choice, as there were
no signicant dierences in the mating success of intact
and experimentally shaved males. However, females
mated more often with males that initiated courtship
rst, suggesting that capture of a female’s attention by
male signalling may play a critical role. In behavioral
experiments that paired a single male with a female in
arenas that allowed both visual and vibratory signal
transmission during courtship, female receptivity did
not vary signicantly with the presence or absence of
tufts. However, experiments that isolated the visual
component of communication (by eliminating vibra-
tory communication) revealed a signicant eect of the
presence of tufts: females showed receptivity less often
to males with tufts removed. Female response to visual
signals was much greater in S. ocreata than in its sib-
ling congener, Schizocosa rovneri, which lacks male
tufts. We hypothesize that the tufts serve to increase
the ecacy of visual displays of S. ocreata, as vibra-
tory communication is constrained by the complex leaf
litter habitat of some populations. Such environmental
constraints may make visual signalling over distance a
critical factor for eective courtship communication,
which may in turn strongly inuence male tness.
Key words Spiders · Schizocosa · Sexual
selection · Courtship communication · Secondary
sexual characteristics
Introduction
Within evolutionary biology, much current debate is
focused on the origin and function of male secondary
sexual characteristics and the evolution of female pre-
ferences for such traits (Andersson 1994). Empirical
investigations of the function of a number of male sec-
ondary sexual characteristics have demonstrated that
these traits can inuence male mating success through
male competition, female choice or both (see
Andersson 1995 for review). In many taxa, male sec-
ondary sexual traits appear to increase the conspicu-
ousness of the bearer. In courtship signalling, increased
conspicuousness may increase the ecacy of commu-
nication between signaller and recipient (Guilford and
Dawkins 1991; Hasson 1991). Traits that enable the
receiver to perceive and/or recognize a signal against
a background of visual or auditory “noise” will have
a selective advantage (Guilford and Dawkins 1991).
Since most signals must travel through the environ-
ment, the signalling environment plays an important
role in determining the form of signals used in com-
munication (Endler 1992). In general, it seems that
animal communication systems have evolved to use
most heavily those sensory modalities best suited for
eective signal transmission within particular habitats
(Krebs and Davies 1993). In addition to inuencing
which of the various sensory modalities are used, the
signalling environment may also inuence the form of
the signals within each modality (Morton 1975; Hunter
and Krebs 1979; Endler 1980, 1992; Fleishman 1992).
S.J. Scheer1· G.W. Uetz (*)
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati,
Cincinnati, OH 45221-0006, USA
G.E. Stratton2
Department of Biology, Albion College, Albion, MI 49224, USA
Present addresses:
1Department of Ecology and Evolution, State University of
New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794-5245, USA
2Department of Biology, Rhodes College, Memphis, TN 38112-
1690, USA
Comparative studies of closely related species are a
powerful tool for investigating how ecological factors
inuence the evolution of courtship (Ryan and Rand
1990, 1993; Basolo 1990; Proctor 1991, 1992). In this
study, we investigate the role played by a conspicuous
male secondary sexual characteristic in the courtship
of the wolf spider Schizocosa ocreata (Hentz) (Araneae:
Lycosidae). The male trait, a conspicuous tuft of bris-
tles on the rst pair of legs, is the only known mor-
phological feature that distinguishes S. ocreata from its
sibling species Schizocosa rovneri (Uetz and Dondale
1979; Uetz and Denterlein 1979; Stratton and Uetz
1986). We rst investigated the current function of the
tufts in S. ocreata, and then compared courtship
between the two species, with particular emphasis on
the sensory modalities used and the ecacy of signal
transmission in the habitat of each species.
Schizocosa ocreata provides a good system in which
to investigate the possible functional signicance of a
male secondary sexual characteristic. Because female
discrimination of courtship cues is important at the
heterospecic level within the genus, similar mecha-
nisms might also allow discrimination among
conspecics. Males and females associate only for the
duration of courtship and copulation. Females receive
no material benets from males, so female preference,
if it occurs, must be based solely on male morphology
and behavior.
Naural history, morphology, and behavior
Schizocosa ocreata and S. rovneri are ground-dwelling
wolf spiders found in the leaf litter of deciduous forests
throughout the eastern United States. In the northern
portion of its range, S. ocreata is most commonly found
in upland forests with complex leaf litter, while in the
same range S. rovneri is usually found on the com-
pacted leaf litter of oodplain forests (Uetz and
Denterlein 1979). Morphologically, these species can
be distinguished only by a male secondary sexual char-
acteristic, a conspicuous tuft of bristles and dark pig-
mentation on the tibia and patella of the rst pair of
legs of mature male S. ocreata, which is lacking in
S. rovneri (as well as in the females and juveniles of
both species; Uetz and Dondale 1979).
The courtship behavior of male lycosid spiders often
includes both a vibratory and a visual component
(Rovner 1967, 1975; Uetz and Stratton 1982). The re-
lative importance of these two components of courtship
can vary among species (Stratton 1982). Conspicuous
tufts on the front legs of male S. ocreata suggest that
visual as well as vibratory communication may be
important in this species. The courtship of male
S. ocreata includes in-unison tapping of the decorated
forelegs, as well as numerous leg waves and leg arches
(Fig. 1). At the same time, substratum-coupled vibra-
tions are produced by stridulatory organs located in
the male pedipalps (Uetz and Stratton 1982). The
courtship of male S. rovneri appears to be much less
visually complex, involving fewer leg movements and
consisting mainly of stationary body pulses. The sub-
stratum-coupled vibrations produced by S. rovneri are
greater in amplitude and dier temporally from those
produced by S. ocreata (see Uetz and Denterlein 1979;
Stratton and Uetz 1981, 1983, 1986 for a complete
description of courtship in these species).
Female Schizocosa can distinguish heterospecic
from conspecic males on the basis of courtship cues
(Stratton and Uetz 1981, 1983). The males of both
species will court heterospecic as well as conspecic
females with almost equal frequency, but female spi-
ders are receptive only to conspecic males (Uetz and
Denterlein 1979; Stratton and Uetz 1981, 1983, 1986;
Uetz and Stratton 1982). Female Schizocosa are slightly
larger than males, and have the ability to reject court-
ing males. Under laboratory conditions, females that
are not sexually receptive may lunge toward a court-
ing male with open fangs, and sometimes even kill and
eat the male. While pre- or post-mating cannibalism is
rare (< 5% of 30–50 matings observed in the labora-
tory, Uetz, unpublished), a male conned with an un-
receptive female will in most cases quickly cease
courtship and remain fairly stationary, suggesting
avoidance of predation. Rarely will copulation occur
until stereotypic receptivity behavior has been dis-
played by the female (Montgomery 1903; Uetz and
Denterlein 1979; Scheer, unpublished data). These
behaviors include a slow 180°[360° turn and settling
into a position with the cephalothorax lowered and the
abdomen raised.
The role of the conspicuous tufts on the forelegs of
male S. ocreata in species recognition and/ or sexual
selection has not been previously investigated. The tufts
may be important in inter-sexual selection via female
mate choice, and/ or intrasexual selection via male-male
interactions. Males conned together in small spaces
at unnaturally high densities appear to establish a lin-
ear dominance hierarchy (Aspey 1977a, b). Many leg-
waving movements were found to be involved in the
dominance interactions, suggesting a possible role for
the tufts in intrasexual communication.
18
Fig. 1 Male of Schizocosa ocreata during courtship display, drawn
from a single frame of lm
Materials and methods
Organisms
All S. ocreata used in this study were collected as immatures at the
Cincinnati Nature Center (Clermont Co., Ohio) during April and
May of 1987 and 1988. Spiders were individually housed in visu-
ally shielded containers in a laboratory with controlled tempera-
ture (23°C), light regime (similar to eld conditions, 13 h light: 11 h
dark) and humidity (RH > 60%).
Several experiments required males whose tufts had been
removed by shaving. This involved anesthetization of males by
exposure to carbon dioxide for 45–75 s. Each spider was placed in
a shallow depression on the stage of a dissecting microscope and
held with a ball of cotton, leaving only the forelegs exposed. The
bristles of the tufts were quickly removed with a microscalpel.
Anesthetization and shaving were usually completed in less than
2 min per spider, and a second dose of carbon dioxide was required
only rarely. The spiders recovered quickly from the shaving process;
most were awake by the time both legs had been shaved and they
immediately began to groom upon release from the shaving depres-
sion. Normal exploratory behaviors were usually exhibited shortly
thereafter. A preliminary study showed that the willingness to court
of anesthetized-and-shaved males, anesthetized-only males, and
control males was not signicantly dierent (as measured by either
the latency of courtship behaviors after exposure to a female or the
presence/absence of courtship after exposure to a female (Kruskal-
Wallis H= 2.105, P> 0.1; v2= 0.3, df = 2, P> 0.1 respectively,
n= 15 spiders per treatment)). As a precaution, unshaved males
used in the experiments were also anesthetized for 45–75 s.
Experiment 1a, b. Inuence of the tufts on male mating
success in S. ocreata
If the male tufts are important in either male interactions or female
choice, then there should be a dierence in the mating success of
shaved and tufted males. Two triadic (two males, one female) mat-
ing experiments testing this hypothesis were conducted:
a. In the rst triadic mating experiment, 20 pairs of S. ocreata
males, each consisting of a shaved male and an intact male, were
used. Males were matched for size; both had cephalothorax
widths within 0.1 mm of each other (x= 3.02 mm, SD = 0.41,
n= 24). A female and her paper cage liner (bearing dragline silk
and silk-bound sex pheromone) were introduced into a circular
culture dish (diameter 15 cm). The female was covered with an
opaque specimen cup (120 cc) and given 5 min to acclimate
before a pair of males was introduced. When both males had
been observed performing at least one “jerky tap” behavior, indi-
cating the onset of courtship, the specimen cup was lifted from
the female. The presence and removal of the specimen cup did
not appear to alter the behavior of the spiders, and it ensured
that the female did not have access to the males until both males
courted. For each triad, the identity of the rst male to court
and the identity of the male that successfully copulated were
recorded.
b. The second experiment tested for the possibility that males
require an extended period of time together to establish a dom-
inance hierarchy based on the presence of tufts. This experiment
was conducted in much the same way as the rst experiment (1a)
except that the paired males were placed in the circular culture
dishes, with crickets and a water vial, approximately 44 h before
the start of the trials. It was not possible to get both males to
initiate courtship before exposing the female to them; the addi-
tion of a female, a cage liner, and a specimen cup to their cul-
ture dish resulted in escape behaviors, and removing them to
another arena risked destroying any site-specic dominance rela-
tions that might have been established. Therefore, females were
carefully placed in the male arenas at an equal distance from
both males. Again, the data recorded included the identity of
the rst male to court in each triadic mating trial as well as the
identity of the male that attained the copulation.
Experiment 2. Relative importance of visual vs vibratory
communication during courtship
Specialized arenas were used to isolate experimentally the vibratory
and visual components of courtship. A male and a female spider
were placed in adjacent rectangular arenas (12 ×6×6cm) that were
separated by ca. 1 cm. Each arena consisted of four sides made
from 0.08-mm thick clear acetate; the bottom was open so that the
spiders were in contact with a cardboard substratum. The dimen-
sions of the cardboard varied with treatment. In the complete-
courtship trials, visual signals were clearly visible through the clear
plastic sides of the arenas while vibratory signals traveled through
the single piece of cardboard that held both the male and the female
arenas. In the vibratory-only trials the insertion of a piece of opaque
paper between the male and female arenas prevented perception of
visual signalling, while vibratory communication remained
unaected. Finally, in the visual-only trials, the arena for each
sex was located on a separate piece of foam-insulated cardboard
thereby preventing the transmission of vibratory signals while still
permitting visual signalling. Only intact males were used in this
experiment.
For each trial, a female was introduced into the female com-
partment and given 5–10 min to acclimate. A piece of female cage
liner (a rectangular card bearing female draglines and silk-bound
pheromone) was placed on top of the cardboard in the male arena
to ensure vigorous courtship. When the male was introduced, video
recording equipment was turned on, and the interaction was video-
taped. The sequence of the trials was determined by a randomized
block design. Each individual was used only once.
The stereotypic receptivity turn of females (Montgomery 1903;
Uetz and Denterlein 1979; Stratton and Uetz 1981) was taken as
the sole indication of female receptivity. Previous studies (Uetz and
Denterlein 1979; Stratton and Uetz 1981, 1983; and triadic mating
trials reported here) have shown that when females are receptive,
this display is usually seen (and copulation proceeds) within 4 min
of male-female interaction. We analyzed the rst 5 min of each
taped interaction for female receptivity to male signals.
Experiment 3. Relative importance of tufts within courtship
components
In this experiment, shaved and intact males courted within the
visual-only and complete-courtship arenas described above. Since
the tufts are a visual characteristic, the vibratory-only arena was
not used. Experimental protocol and data analyses were the same
as that described above, and the two experiments were conducted
together as part of the same randomized block design.
Experiment 4. Signal transmission in the eld
Sound attenuation was measured as a function of distance from
point of origin in both the upland leaf litter habitat of S. ocreata
(Cincinnati Nature Center, Clermont County, Ohio) and in the
oodplain leaf litter habitat of S. rovneri (along the Ohio River
near Taylorsport, Boone County, Ky.). A Bruel and Kjaer
accelerometer (type 4399) and Sound Level meter were used to mea-
sure the sound level (dB) produced by dropping a standard object
(pencil) from a consistent height (5 cm) at 5-cm intervals along tran-
sects in four directions (N, S, E, W) from the point of impact in
both environments (n= 3 recordings at each distance).
The attenuation of the courtship signals produced by live male
spiders of each species was measured as above in both the oodplain
19
habitat of S. rovneri and the upland habitat of S. ocreata. Sound
transmission was measured at 5-cm intervals along transects in four
directions (N, S, E, W) from a courting male (n= 5 to 10 record-
ings at each point).
Results
Inuence of the tufts on male mating success
(experiment 1)
In triadic mating experiments, when pairs of shaved
and intact males were introduced to females, the two
types of males were equally likely to attain copulation
(Table 1). This was true regardless of whether or not
the males had been conned together previously
(G= 0.106, df = 1, P> 0.5). In experiment 1b, in which
the males had prior contact and the females were intro-
duced before the males were courting, 79% (11 of 14)
of females mated with the rst male to court (v2= 4.57,
df = 1, P< 0.05). Since shaved and tufted males were
equally likely to be the rst to court (53% of rst males
to court were shaved while 47% were tufted), the female
bias toward the rst male does not alter the results of
Table 1.
Relative importance of visual vs vibratory
communication during courtship (experiment 2)
In the arena allowing the transmission of both vibra-
tory and visual signals, 14 of 15 (93.3%) of the females
gave receptivity responses to the male. In the vibratory-
alone arena, 9 of 13 (69.2%) of the females responded
positively, while in the visual-alone arena 9 of 14
(64.3%) of the females gave receptive responses
(Table 2a). Although female receptivity was lower in
the arenas restricting visual or vibrational signalling
than in the complete courtship arena, these dierences
were not signicant (Table 2a, G= 4.46, df =2,
P> 0.1).
A comparison of data showing the relative impor-
tance of visual and vibratory communication in S. ocre-
ata from this study with published results of a similar
study for S. rovneri (Uetz and Stratton 1982) reveals a
pronounced dierence in the importance of visual sig-
nals (Table 2b). A three-way log-linear contingency
analysis shows signicant non-independence overall
(G= 16.87, df = 7, P< 0.05), and a signicant inter-
action between species and receptivity based on stim-
ulus (G= 16.57, df = 5, P< 0.001). While there is no
signicant dierence in the response of female S. ocre-
ata to conspecic males courting in the dierent are-
nas (G= 4.46, df = 2, P> 0.1), the response of female
S. rovneri to conspecic males courting in the visual
arena is signicantly less than the responses given to
males in the vibratory or the complete courtship are-
nas (G= 16.64, df = 2, P< 0.001).
Relative importance of tufts within courtship
components (experiment 3)
A three-way log-linear contingency analysis of female
responses to shaved vs intact males shows that the
response depends on the stimulus components avail-
able (overall G= 25.32, df = 4, P< 0.001). When vibra-
tory and visual signalling were permitted, the female
response to shaved males did not dier signicantly
from the response to tufted males (Table 3, G= 0.46,
df = 1, P> 0.3). However, when only visual signalling
was possible, the female response to shaved males was
signicantly lower than the response to tufted males
(Table 3, G= 7.79, df = 1, P< 0.01).
Signal transmission in the eld (experiment 4)
Although the upland (complex) leaf litter microhabi-
tat of S. ocreata and the oodplain (compressed) leaf
litter microhabitat of S. rovneri often occur within the
same deciduous forests, they show a considerable
dierence in the transmission of substratum-conducted
vibration (Fig. 2). Sound levels (measured as decibels)
20
Table 1 The number of S. ocreata females that mated (+) with
either the tufted or the shaved male when given a choice between
paired males. In the two experiments reported here, the male pairs
had either (a) no previous contact, or (b) were conned together
for 44 h prior to introduction of the female
Previous contact
between males Male
Tufted Shaved
(a) None (+) 8 11
% receptive 42.1 57.9
(b) 44 h (+) 9 10
% receptive 47.4 52.6
G= 0.106, df = 1, P> 0.5
Table 2 The number of S. ocreata and S. rovneri females that
responded with a receptivity display (+) or did not display recep-
tivity ([) to the various components of conspecic courtship
Courtship components
Female Vibratory Visual Complete
Species receptivity only only
(a) S. ocreata (+) 9 9 14
([)451
% receptive 69.2 64.3 93.3
(b) S. rovneri (+) 15 6 16
([) 4 10 2
% receptive 78.9 37.5 88.9
(a) S. ocreata, G= 4.46, df = 2, P> 0.1; (b) S. rovneri data from
Uetz and Stratton (1982), G= 16.64, df = 2, P< 0.005
from impact vibration produced by a standard test
weight (pencil drop) are lower at the point of origin
(impact absorption) and attenuate much more rapidly
in the upland leaf litter microhabitat of S. ocreata than
in the oodplain microhabitat of S. rovneri.
Courtship vibrations in the upland deciduous litter
microhabitat of S. ocreata were so rapidly attenuated
as to be undetectable beyond the leaf holding the court-
ing spider (< 10 cm), and therefore we could only use
data for the oodplain microhabitat. Attenuation of
the courtship signals of live male spiders from each
species, measured in this same microhabitat type (com-
pressed oodplain leaf litter) indicate that sound level
produced by substratum-conducted courtship vibra-
tions of the two species are also very dierent (Stratton
and Uetz 1983, 1986). The vibratory signals of S. ocre-
ata are not as loud, and travel a shorter distance than
do signals of S. rovneri. We estimated the ecacy of
signal transmission for each species in its own habitat,
using the information on vibration attenuation from
each microhabitat as a scaling factor applied to the
data from signal transmission of both species in the
oodplain microhabitat (Fig. 3).
Discussion
In S. ocreata the presence or absence of a conspicuous
male secondary sexual characteristic, foreleg tufts, does
not appear to be involved in an obvious way with the
current operation of either female mate choice or male
mating competition. When given a choice, females were
most likely to mate with the rst male that captured
their attention, regardless of the condition of his tufts.
This result is similar to that seen for salticid spiders
(Clark and Uetz 1992), and reinforces the argument
that if a bias toward the rst male to court is a com-
mon feature of female behavior, caution should be used
when analyzing the results of simultaneous choice
experiments (Clark and Uetz 1992). However, respon-
ses of females to male movement may reect sexual
selection based on sensory biases (Guilford and
Dawkins 1991; Endler 1992; Fleishman 1992; Ryan and
Rand 1993).
The results of the triadic mating experiments need
not mean that the tufts are unimportant in courtship
of S. ocreata, nor does it mean that they play no role
in sexual selection. In the absence of vibratory sig-
nalling, removal of the tufts from males resulted in
signicantly fewer receptive responses from females.
When vibratory signals are transmitted to the female,
as in the triadic mating experiments, they are sucient
21
Table 3 The number of S. ocreata females that displayed receptiv-
ity (+) or did not display receptivity ([) to shaved or tufted males
Male treatment
Courtship Female
components receptivity Shaved Tufted
(a) Visual only (+) 2 9
([)125
% receptive 14.3 64.3
(b) Complete (+) 12 14
([)21
% receptive 85.7 93.3
Courtship took place (a) within the visual courtship arena
(G= 7.79, df = 1, P< 0.01); or (b) within the complete courtship
arena (G= 0.46, df = 1, P> 0.3).
Fig. 2 Field transmission of vibrations of a standard test weight
(pencil) dropped along a transect in two deciduous forest habitats.
Each point represents the mean of 12 recordings (3 each from 4
directions from the point of origin) at each distance (± 2 SE)
Fig. 3 Estimation of the ecacy of transmission of vibratory
courtship signals of S. ocreata and S. rovneri in their respective
habitats, from data taken from live male spiders (n= 5–10 record-
ings per spider at each point) in the same microhabitat (compressed
oodplain litter) and scaled by the% attenuation of a test weight
in the particular microhabitat (Fig. 2). Female visual range esti-
mate (arrow) based on predatory responses toward visual cues from
prey at varying distances (Uetz, unpublished data)
to elicit a female response and any eect of the tufts
may be overridden. We did not conduct an explicit
analysis of the function of the tufts in terms of a dom-
inance hierarchy among males (see Aspey 1977a, b),
but such a hierarchy would only be important in intra-
sexual selection if it translated into dierential mating
success. The experiment giving males 44 h to interact
prior to the introduction of a female was designed to
test for this eect, and revealed no dierence.
Intersexual selection seems a more likely explanation
for the presence of tufts, given the behavioral and mor-
phological dierences between species.
The conspicuous tufts present in male S. ocreata
appear to be important for successful courtship under
circumstances in which vibratory signalling is less
eective or not possible. The high frequency of
responses of female S. ocreata to the visual component
of male courtship may be partially explained by the
presence of the male tufts, or alternatively, may be the
selective agent favoring their evolution (Ryan and Rand
1990, 1993; Basolo 1990; Proctor 1991, 1992; Endler
1992; Fleishman 1992). Evidence from experiments iso-
lating the visual component of courtship suggests that
the tufts inuence the ecacy or detectability of male
visual signalling (see Guilford and Dawkins 1991;
Hasson 1991) under environmental conditions that
constrain vibratory communication. Our estimates of
male signalling ecacy (Fig. 3) suggest that for male
S. rovneri, vibrational signalling via substratum-cou-
pled stridulation (Rovner 1975) is an eective means
of communicating, even over what must be consider-
able distances to a 1-cm spider. In contrast, the vibra-
tional signal of S. ocreata does not travel very far at
all in the upland habitat. (Our estimate of 10 cm, based
on transmission in compressed oodplain litter, is prob-
ably an overestimate, since in the upland leaf litter habi-
tat we were unable to detect spider vibrations beyond
the leaf with which the spider was in contact.)
Female Schizocosa are slightly larger than males and
feed on a variety of arthropods including conspecics.
Therefore, it may be essential for males to communi-
cate their identity eectively. Females of both
Schizocosa species react to moving prey stimuli with
typical predatory responses (orient, stalk, lunge) at dis-
tances up to 30 cm and beyond (Rennecker, Valerius,
Cook and Uetz, unpublished), similar to other lycosid
species (Land 1985). Because the vibratory signals of
male S. rovneri in the oodplain habitat are transmit-
ted approximately this distance (Fig. 3), there is no
range within which a courting male is visible to a female
but not heard. In contrast, however, a courting S. ocre-
ata male may come into the visual range of a female
15 to 20 cm before she can detect his vibratory signals.
Visual signals may thus be critically important in mate
identication when the male is within the visual range
of the female but not yet within the range of eective
vibratory communication. If tufts increase the chances
that the male will be detected and recognized by the
female, resulting in receptivity rather than predatory
behavior, there is potential for a strong selective advan-
tage – both in terms of sexual selection by increased
mating success because of more ecient courtship com-
munication, and in terms of natural selection by a
reduction in the probability of predation. Further
experiments will investigate whether the presence of the
tufts inuences female predatory behavior when the
spiders are vibrationally isolated.
Based on the results of this study, we hypothesize
that the conspicuous tufts of male S. ocreata function
to increase the ecacy of courtship communication (see
Guilford and Dawkins 1991) in an environment that
physically constrains the more common mode of vibra-
tional signalling. The complex leaf litter habitat of
S. ocreata may create an important physical constraint
on the eectiveness of vibrational signalling; success-
ful courtship communication may require an enhanced
contribution from the visual component of male
courtship. This appears to have been eected behav-
iorally – by an increased level of courtship movement
(especially of the forelegs) – as well as morphologically
– by the conspicuous male foreleg pigmentation and
tufts. The distribution of male morphology and behav-
ior within the genus Schizocosa is consistent with this
hypothesis. Although the presence of male foreleg tufts
is uncommon within the genus (Dondale and Redner
1978), leg-waving visual displays are an important com-
ponent of courtship in species with tufts or other dec-
orations; such behaviors appear to be absent in species
that lack tufts (Stratton and Lowrie 1984), a trend that
is seen in other lycosid genera as well as in Schizocosa
(E. Hebets, pers. commun.).
Because a complete phylogeny is not yet available
for Schizocosa, we can only speculate as to the evolu-
tionary origin of the tufts and visual signals. Given the
vibrational signalling biology of most lycosid spiders
and the distribution of the tufts within the genus, it is
very possible that the tufts represent an apomorphic
feature of S. ocreata (McClintock and Uetz 1995). If
this is indeed the case, it is possible that the tufts have
arisen as a means of exploiting some pre-existing visual
bias in female wolf spiders, in the manner suggested by
Ryan et al. (1990), Basolo (1990, 1995), and Proctor
(1991, 1992) for other taxa.
Acknowledgements This research was supported in part by a Sigma
Xi Grant-in-Aid of Research (to S.J.S.), and a grant from the
Hewlett-Mellon Fund of Albion College (to G.E.S.). The Cincinnati
Nature Center kindly allowed us to collect spiders from their
grounds. We thank Karen Ausdenmoore and Keith Karoly for
assistance with the spiders. Doug Futuyma, Leo Shapiro, Kevin
Omland, and students in the Uetz laboratory provided valuable
comments on the manuscript.
References
Andersson M (1995) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press,
Princeton
22
Aspey WP (1977a) Wolf spider sociobiology. I. Agonistic display
and dominance-subordinance relations in adult male Schizocosa
crassipes. Behavior 62:103–141
Aspey WP (1977b) Wolf spider sociobiology. II. Density parame-
ters inuencing agonistic behavior in Schizocosa crassipes.
Behavior 62:142–163
Basolo AL (1990) Female preference predates the preference of the
sword in swordtail sh. Science 250:808–810
Basolo AL (1995) Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-exist-
ing bias in sexual selection. Proc R Soc Lond [Biol] 259:307–311
Clark DL, Uetz GW (1992) Morph-independent mate selection in
a dimorphic jumping spider: demonstration of movement bias
in female choice using video-controlled courtship behaviour.
Anim Behav 43:247–254
Dondale CD, Redner JH (1978) Revision of the nearctic wolf
spider genus Schizocosa (Araneida: Lycosidae). Can Ent 110:
143–181
Endler JA (1980) Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia
reticulata. Evolution 34:76–91
Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of
evolution. Am Nat [Suppl] 139:125–153
Fleishman LJ (1992) The inuence of the sensory system and the
environment on motion patterns in the visual displays of ano-
line lizards and other vertebrates. Am Nat [Suppl] 139:S36–S61
Guilford T, Dawkins M (1991) Receiver psychology and the evo-
lution of animal signals. Anim Behav 42:1–14
Hasson O (1991) Sexual displays as ampliers: practical examples
with an emphasis on feather decorations. Behav Ecol 2:189–197
Hunter ML, Krebs JR (1979) Geographical variation in the song
of the great tit (Parus major) in relation to ecological factors.
J Anim Ecol 48:759–785
Krebs JR, Davies NB (1993) An introduction to behavioral
ecology, 3rd edn. Blackwell Scientic Publications, Oxford
Land MF (1985) The morphology and optics of spider eyes. In:
Bartk FG (ed) Neurobiology of Arachmids. Springer, Berlin
Heidelberg New York, pp 53–78
McClintock WJ, Uetz GW (1996) Female mate choice in two wolf
spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae): pre-existing bias or preference for
a novel trait. Anim Behav (in press)
Montgomery TH Jr (1903) Studies on the habits of spiders, par-
ticularly those of the mating period. Proc Acad Nat Sci Phil
55:59–149
Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds.
Am Nat 109:17–34
Proctor HC (1991) Courtship in the water mite Neumania papilla-
tor: males capitalize on female adaptations for predation. Anim
Behav 42:589–598
Proctor HC (1992) Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male
mating behaviour: a cladistic test using water mites (Acari:
Parasitengona). Anim Behav 44:745–752
Rovner JS (1967) Acoustic communication in a lycosid spider
(Lycosa rabida Walckenaer). Anim Behav 15:273–281
Rovner JS (1975) Sound production by nearctic wolf spiders:
a substratum-coupled stridulatory mechanism. Science 190:
1309–1310
Ryan MJ, Rand AS (1990) The sensory basis of sexual selection
for complex calls in the tungara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus
(sexual selection for sensory exploitation). Evolution
44:305–314
Ryan MJ, Rand AS (1993) Species recognition and sexual selection
as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution 47:
647–657
Ryan MJ, Fox JH, Wilczynski W, Rand AS (1990) Sexual selec-
tion for sensory exploitation in the frog Physalaemus pustulo-
sus. Nature 139:6667
Stratton GE (1982) Reproductive behavior and behavior genetics
in Schizocosa wolf spiders. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of
Cincinnati
Stratton GE, Lowrie DC (1984) Courtship behavior and life cycle
of the wolf spider Schizocosa mccooki (Araneae: Lycosidae).
J Arachnol 12:223–228
Stratton GE, Uetz GW (1981) Acoustic communication and repro-
ductive isolation in two species of wolf spiders. Science
214:575–577
Stratton GE, Uetz GW (1983) Communication via substratum-
coupled stridulation and reproductive isolation in wolf spiders
(Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim Behav 31:164–172
Stratton GE, Uetz GW (1986) The inheritance of courtship behav-
ior and its role as a reproductive isolating mechanism in two
species of Schizocosa wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae).
Evolution 40:129–141
Uetz GW, Denterlein G (1979) Courtship behavior, habitat, and
reproductive isolation in Schizocosa rovneri Uetz and Dondale
(Araneae: Lycosidae). J Arachnol 7:121–128
Uetz GW, Dondale CD (1979) A new wolf spider in the genus
Schizocosa (Araneae: Lycosidae) from Illinois. J Arachnol 7:
86–88
Uetz GW, Stratton GE (1982) Acoustic communication and repro-
ductive isolation in spiders. In: Witt PN, Rovner JS (eds) Spider
communication: mechanisms and ecological signicance.
Princeton University Press. Princeton, pp 123–159
Communicated by M. A. Elgar
23
... Furthermore, recent evidence demonstrates that repeated experience with males with average tufts affects adult preferences for small-tufted and large-tufted males (Stoffer and Uetz 2015). While these studies provide evidence for SCAP based on visual experience alone (isolated via video playback), male S. ocreata use multimodal courtship displays with a combination of visual (e.g., tufts, leg tapping) and vibratory (e.g., stridulation, percussion) displays (Stratton and Uetz 1981, 1983, 1986Scheffer et al. 1996)-leaving questions regarding whether SCAP in mate preferences is possible in the vibratory modality as well. ...
... The videos were also muted during scoring to avoid any potential bias in case the scorer thought it sounded like a lower or higher-amplitude signal (while the vibrations are substrateborne, the human ear can hear the vibratory playback). In S. ocreata, females use receptivity displays (pivots, tandem leg extensions, and settles) in response to male courtship to signal their willingness to mate (Montgomery 1903;Uetz and Denterlein 1979;Scheffer et al. 1996;Norton and Uetz 2005). In our current study using vibratory playback, female receptivity was recorded as a behavior directed to a particular male if the female was within the width of the iPod video screen (and therefore, in close proximity to the piezoelectric disc bender). ...
Article
Full-text available
While members of the choosier sex often prefer courting mates with bright, large, or loud phenotypes, social experience can result in variation in mate preferences. Fewer studies, though, have investigated how multiple social parameters might interact to affect such preferences. In the brush-legged wolf spider, Schizocosa ocreata, asynchrony of maturation between sexes provides a time period in which females might be exposed to male courtship prior to making a mating decision. We tested whether adult females demonstrated plasticity in their preferences for vibratory signal amplitude after experience with vibratory playback during their penultimate stage. Penultimate instar females were presented unimodal vibratory courtship signals via piezoelectric disc benders, manipulating the perceived encounter rate (every other day or once per day), the number of males (one or two), and/or the vibratory amplitude (low or high). As adults, each female was presented vibratory playback of a low- or high-amplitude courtship signal in both no-choice and two-choice designs. In no-choice trials, previous experience with different amplitude signals significantly affected adult preferences, while other social parameters did not. Specifically, female S. ocreata preferred high-amplitude signals to low-amplitude signals if previously exposed to high-amplitude signals, while those previously exposed to low-amplitude signals preferred low-amplitude signals. In two-choice trials, however, females preferred high-amplitude signals regardless of their previous social experience, suggesting that innate preferences for high-amplitude signals might outweigh any learned preferences in some contexts. Results from this study complement previous social experience studies in S. ocreata, by clearly demonstrating a second sensory modality through which social learning can occur. Significance statement Social experience affects subsequent mate preferences in a variety of taxa, but in many instances, single parameters are examined at a time. Furthermore, in species that use multimodal communication, there remain questions about whether a single sensory modality is sufficient to elicit such plasticity. In this study, we manipulated multiple parameters using vibratory playback to examine whether social experience during the brush-legged wolf spider’s juvenile stage affected preferences for low- and high-amplitude signals as an adult. Ultimately, only the amplitude of the male’s signals that they were exposed to as a juvenile impacted adult mate preference—not the number of perceived males or how often they encountered the males. These results, along with previous studies, demonstrate that S. ocreata is capable of plasticity in response to social cues in multiple modalities.
... The absence of brushes in diurnally active species such as S. humilis, S. duplex, and S. retrorsa, however, suggests that this type of ornamentation is unnecessary during the day or may be selected against, since tibial brushes also make spiders more conspicuous to predators . Most Schizocosa studies have previously focused on the role of sexual selection in the evolution of tibial ornamentation (pigmentation and brushes; Scheffer et al. 1996;Hebets and Uetz 2000;Uetz et al. Figure 3: Effects of light levels and male and female mass on the probability of copulation in Schizocosa duplex and S. ocreata. In S. duplex mating success increased with female mass, while the effect of male mass was dependent on the light environment. ...
... But variation in these condition-dependent traits often does not predict mating success (Shamble et al. 2009;Hebets 2012, 2015). Experimental manipulations of pigments and brushes may better elucidate the importance of these traits in mate choice, although even studies using addition or removal of these traits have demonstrated limited or no effects on mating success (Scheffer et al. 1996;Hebets 2003;Taylor et al. 2008;Stafstrom and Hebets 2013). Our results suggest that the light environment interacts with these ornaments in an ornament-specific manner, and thus future studies should incorporate variability in the signaling environment into functional studies of ornamentation and sexual selection. ...
Article
Light availability is highly variable, yet predictable, over various timescales and is expected to play an important role in the evolution of visual signals. Courtship displays of the wolf spider genus Schizocosa always involve the use of substrate-borne vibrations; however, there is substantial variation in the presence and complexity of visual displays among species. To gain insight into the role the light environment plays in the evolution of courtship displays, we tested the function of visual courtship signaling across distinct light environments in four species of Schizocosa that vary in their degree of ornamentation and dynamic visual signals. We ran mating and courtship trials at three light intensities (bright, dim, and dark) and tested the hypothesis that ornamentation interacts with light environment. We also examined each species' circadian activity patterns. The effects of the light environment on courtship and mating varied between species, as did circadian activity patterns. Our results suggest that femur pigmentation may have evolved for diurnal signaling, whereas tibial brushes may function to increase signal efficacy under dim light. Additionally, we found evidence for light-dependent changes in selection on male traits, illustrating that short-term changes in light intensity have the potential for strong effects on the dynamics of sexual selection.
... These complex, multimodal (vibratory plus visual) displays facilitate effective communication across variable and complex signaling environments Uetz et al. 2013). Specifically, the foreleg ornaments are high contrast signals , and can act as amplifiers, enhancing the detectability of a male's courtship display in their natural environment (Scheffer et al. 1996;Hebets and Uetz 2000;Uetz et al. 2011). Many of the display components, however, are also condition dependent in their expression. ...
... Signal variations expressed by good condition males, for example, include vibratory signals that are longer, more intense, or faster (Kotiaho 2000;Rundus et al. 2010;Hebets et al. 2021), larger brushes (Uetz et al. 2002;Hebets et al. 2008Hebets et al. , 2021, and/ or darker foreleg pigmentation (Shamble et al. 2009;Rundus et al. 2010;Hebets 2012, 2015). Despite their conditiondependence, however, females do not always prefer more ornamented males (Scheffer et al. 1996;Hebets et al. 2008;Shamble et al. 2009;Stafstrom and Hebets 2013;Watts et al. 2019; but see McClintock and Uetz 1996;Persons and Uetz 2005), raising questions about ornamentation's function as an indicator trait. ...
Article
Animals often communicate in complex, heterogeneous environments, leading to hypothesized selection for increased detectability or discriminability in signaling traits. The extent to which secondary sexual ornaments have evolved to overcome the challenges of signaling in complex environments, however, remains understudied, especially in comparison to their role as indicator traits. This study tested the hypothesis that the condition-dependent secondary sexual ornamentation in the wolf spider Rabidosa rabida functions to increase detectability/discriminability in visually complex environments. We predicted that male ornamentation would interact with the complexity of the signaling environment to affect male mating success. In particular, we expected ornaments to confer a greater mating advantage when males courted in visually complex environments. To test this, we artificially manipulated male foreleg ornamentation (present/absent) and ran repeated-measures mating trials across laboratory microcosms that represented simple versus complex visual signaling environments. Microcosm visual complexity differed in their background pattern, grass stem color, and grass stem placement. We found that ornamented males mated more often and more quickly than unornamented males across both environments, but we found no support for an ornament-by-environment interaction. Male courtship rate, however, did interact with the signaling environment. Despite achieving the same level of mating success across signaling environments, ornamented males courted less rapidly in complex versus simple environments, although environmental complexity had no influence on unornamented male courtship rates. Our results suggest that the visual complexity of the signaling environment influences the interactive influence of ornamentation and dynamic visual courtship on female mate choice.
... Given the background available, we predicted that females would alter their activity in the presence of male chemotactile cues. It is likely that females could quickly identify the added chemotactile cues as belonging to another wolf spider; however, wolf spiders use motion to detect prey and potential mates (Rovner 1993;Persons and Uetz 1997;Scheffer et al. 1996;Stoffer et al. 2016), and they reduce their activity when at risk of predation (Persons et al. 2001;Lehmann et al. 2004). Thus, we predicted that the presence of male chemotactile cues would lead to reduced activity as the female prioritized visual and vibratory sensory modalities in an effort to locate and identify the individual that produced the depositions. ...
... Specifically, we thought that male chemotactile cues would reduce the time needed for the males and females to get together and reduce the aggressiveness of the encounter. Assessment in wolf spiders typically has a visual component (Scheffer et al. 1996;Hebets and Uetz 2000;Uetz and Roberts 2002) and this caused us to predict that augmentation of male chemotactile cues would have a larger effect on courtship interactions in the dark when it was not possible for the females to evaluate courtship visually. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many animals communicate using multiple sensory channels simultaneously and this ability enables them to overcome the challenges of living in variable environments. Studies of wolf spiders have been important in understanding complex communication with a primary focus on the roles of visual and vibratory signals during mating. However, it is well documented that wolf spiders leave silk draglines behind that are often associated with additional chemical residues such as pheromones. We verified that females detect the silk and secretions left on a substrate by male wolf spiders in the species, Tigrosa helluo. We hypothesized that male chemotactile cues play different roles when vision is limited and so we explored the effects of adding male cues prior to courtship in light vs dark situations. Aggression was lower when male chemotactile cues were augmented and when vision was reduced. Males and females came together more quickly and had higher mating success when male cues were augmented under red light, which simulated darkness for these spiders, whereas contact was delayed and mating success was lower in the treatment with added male cues in white light. Thus, the availability of visual signals adjusted the importance of male chemotactile cues either through a shift in sensory priorities of the female or in the evaluative criteria used to make decisions. Chemotactile cues potentially allow for sophisticated information to be conveyed about the male, as such, may be an important communication modality on their own as well as in concert with other signaling channels. Significance statement Selecting the right mate depends on effective communication so that the prospective partner can detect and accurately evaluate key traits. Animals living in variable habitats must overcome the communication challenges by presenting information through multiple sensory channels. The understanding of multimodal communication has been advanced by studies of visual and vibratory signaling in wolf spiders. We found that females detect male silk and chemical secretions and that they alter mating interactions in the wolf spider, Tigrosa helluo. Allowing males to deposit silk and chemical cues before virgin males and females were allowed to interact reduced aggression and increased mating success in red light (low vision) conditions. These results broaden the possibilities for the types of interacting signals that might influence the efficacy of multimodal communication in animals live and mate in dynamic environments.
... Communication in complex environments is difficult and animals have evolved multimodal signalling to adapt to this environment. Each signal can highlight female re- productivity status, but all the signals displayed have a synergistic effect [15]. Eremias argus is therefore likely to use UV fluorescence as a backup signal in courtship. ...
Article
Full-text available
Ornamental traits such as ultraviolet (UV) fluorescence and reflectance can provide honest signals indicating the bearer’s condition as a potential mate. UV fluorescence is widely found in nature and used for multiple functions, such as indicating female maturity in arachnids and acting as a signal enhancer in many insects. Lizards can display a broad range of colours, but the function of some of their ornamental traits remains unclear. Here, we report the presence of UV fluorescence in female Eremias argus specimens, a small lizard ranging across Mongolia, China and the Korean Peninsula. Based on our observation, combined with the literature on the breeding behaviour of the species, we provide two hypotheses, assigning the use of UV fluorescence to: 1) an indicator of female’s body conditions and 2) signal efficacy backup. To verify our hypotheses, we suggest a protocol with three serial trials under low light and enhanced UV light conditions.
... This female behavior could be related to the higher rate of precopulatory sexual cannibalism in P. gallopavo compared to the species mentioned above (Toscano-Gadea & Costa 2016). Finally, recent studies on the sexual behavior of South American Hogna indicate that, similar to what has been observed in the North American species Schizocosa ocreata (Scheffer et al. 1996;Stratton 2005), males display dark hair tufts located at the end of their forelegs and jerk the legs vigorously during courtship (Costa & Gonza´lez 2015) (Table 1A). These outstanding characteristics related to visual communication used during male courtship make the genera Pavocosa and Hogna promising models for studies of sexual selection. ...
Article
Full-text available
The family Lycosidae is one of the spider families with the greatest diversity of species and with varied and striking strategies. Studies on Neotropical wolf spiders have contributed new and valuable information to the field of sexual selection for several decades, having discovered cases that differ markedly from previously known patterns, not only for the family but for spiders in general. Here we provide a review of studies on reproductive biology of South American wolf spiders in recent decades, focusing on the subfamilies Lycosinae, Allocosinae and Sossipinae. The promising possibilities of the spiders of this family to test fundamental hypotheses in sexual selection and reproductive biology are highlighted, and we outline areas of particular interest for future studies. We hope that this review will inspire further studies on a broader range of wolf spider species in the Neotropics.
... Similarly, decades of research on wolf spiders in the genus Schizocosa have explored the form and function of visual (ornamentation and dynamic movements) and vibratory signaling (reviewed in Uetz and Roberts, 2002;Stratton, 2005;Hebets et al., 2013;Uetz et al., 2016;Hebets and McGinley, 2019;Starrett et al., 2022). Despite major advances in our understanding of bimodal signal interactions in select species [e.g., S. ocreata (Scheffer et al., 1996;Uetz et al., 2016); S. uetzi (Hebets, 2005); S. stridulans, ; S. floridana (Rosenthal and Hebets, 2012)], there remains one Schizocosa species where the visual and vibratory signaling appear non-functional from the female's perspective-Schizocosa. retrorsa (Banks, 1911). ...
Article
Full-text available
Wolf spiders within the genus Schizocosa have become a model system for exploring the form and function of multimodal communication. In terms of male signaling, much past research has focused on the role and importance of dynamic and static visual and substrate-borne vibratory communication. Studies on S. retrorsa, however, have found that female-male pairs were able to successfully mate in the absence of both visual and vibratory stimuli, suggesting a reduced or non-existent role of these signaling modalities in this species. Given these prior findings, it has been suggested that S. retrorsa males may utilize an additional signaling modality during courtship—air particle movement, often referred to as near-field sound—which they likely produce with rapid leg waving and receive using thin filiform sensory hairs called trichobothria. In this study, we tested the role of air-particle movement in mating success by conducting two independent sets of mating trials with randomly paired S. retrorsa females and males in the dark and on granite (i.e., without visual or vibratory signals) in two different signaling environments—(i) without (“No Noise”) and (ii) with (“Noise”) introduced air-particle movement intended to disrupt signaling in that modality. We also ran foraging trials in No Noise/Noise environments to explore the impact of our treatments on overall behavior. Across both mating experiments, our treatments significantly impacted mating success, with more mating in the No Noise signaling environments compared to the Noise environments. The rate of leg waving—a previously assumed visual dynamic movement that has also been shown to be able to produce air particle displacement—was higher in the No Noise than Noise environments. Across both treatments, males with higher rates of leg waving had higher mating success. In contrast to mating trials results, foraging success was not influenced by Noise. Our results indicate that artificially induced air particle movement disrupts successful mating and alters male courtship signaling but does not interfere with a female’s ability to receive and assess the rate of male leg waving.
... A prior study in S. stridulans that explored the role of vibratory versus visual signalling in mating success, however, found no support for a role of visual signalling in the absence of vibratory signalling [11]. Furthermore, we found no evidence that either vibratory complexity or visual courtship rate influenced female orientation behaviour; a behaviour used previously as a proxy of a female's interest in mating [41,59,60] (electronic supplementary material, S5). Copulating males in our study also did not increase the occurrence of particular courtship components (idles/leg-taps), as measured by entropy or courtship rate, in the second half of courtship. ...
Article
Full-text available
The evolution of complex signals has often been explored by testing multiple functional hypotheses regarding how independent signal components provide selective benefits to offset the costs of their production. In the present study, we take a different approach by exploring the function of complexity per se. We test the hypothesis that increased vibratory signal complexity-based on both proportional and temporal patterning-provides selective benefits to courting male Schizocosa stridulans wolf spiders. In support of this hypothesis, all of our quantified metrics of vibratory signal complexity predicted the mating success of male S. stridulans. The rate of visual signalling, which is mechanistically tied to vibratory signal production, was also associated with mating success. We additionally found evidence that males can dynamically adjust the complexity of their vibratory signalling. Together, our results suggest that complexity per se may be a target of female choice.
Article
The mitochondrial genome (mitogenome) is useful for identification and phylogenetic analyses among arthropods, but there are no sufficient mitogenome data for wolf spiders. To enrich the mitogenome database of wolf spiders, the complete mitogenome of Pardosa pusiola was sequenced by high‐throughput sequencing. It is 14,284 bp, comprising 13 protein‐coding genes (PCGs), 22 transfer RNA genes (tRNAs), two ribosomal RNA genes (rRNAs), and a control region (CR). It represents a high bias toward A and T nucleotides with an A + T content of 76.49%. The mitogenome exhibited a negative AT skew (−0.13) and a positive GC skew (0.32). Most PCGs started with ATN codons and ended with TAA, TAG, or an incomplete T. In addition, most tRNAs had aberrant secondary structures with the absence of DHU arm or TΨC arm. Analysis performed with CREx software demonstrated that large‐scale rearrangements of tRNAs were observed in the mitogenome of P. pusiola as compared with the putative ancestral mitogenome. The Bayesian inference (BI) and maximum likelihood (ML) phylogenetic trees based on the 13 PCGs of 25 spiders had the same topology, which could be presented as (Araneidae + (Agelenidae + (Dictynidae + Desidae)) + (Salticidae + (Thomisidae + (Oxyopidae + (Pisauridae + Lycosidae))))). This study offers a useful genetic resource for the taxonomy and phylogeny of spiders.
Chapter
Spiders are often underestimated as suitable behavioural models because of the general belief that due to their small brains their behaviour is innate and mostly invariable. Challenging this assumption, this fascinating book shows that rather than having a limited behavioural repertoire, spiders show surprising cognitive abilities, changing their behaviour to suit their situational needs. The team of authors unravels the considerable intra-specific as well as intra-individual variability and plasticity in different behaviours ranging from foraging and web building to communication and courtship. An introductory chapter on spider biology, systematics and evolution provides the reader with the necessary background information to understand the discussed behaviours and helps to place them into an evolutionary context. Highlighting an under-explored area of behaviour, this book will provide new ideas for behavioural researchers and students unfamiliar with spiders as well as a valuable resource for those already working in this intriguing field.
Article
Full-text available
The courtship behaviors of two morphologically similar spider species, Schizocosa ocreata and S. rovneri, are distinctive and prevent interbreeding. We used "forced" copulation between these species to investigate the mode of inheritance of the courtship behavior and to determine whether postmating isolating mechanisms exist. F1 hybrids proved to be behaviorally sterile, but they were capable of producing viable offspring when forced to interbreed. Analysis of the courtship behaviors of F1, F2, and backcross progeny showed that the inheritance of some aspects of these behaviors is consistent with models involving single autosomal loci. The inheritance of secondary sexual characteristics in the males is also investigated. The genes for courtship behavior and secondary sexual characteristics do not assort independently. The origin of the premating isolating mechanisms may be explained by either an initial habitat separation between the two groups, or by a founding event with each group subsequently diverging in slightly different habitats. It is suggested that the differences in the microhabitats may have a profound effect on what type of signal (visual or vibratory) would be effective.
Article
Full-text available
There is a bewildering diversity of signals, sensory systems, and signaling behavior. A consideration of how these traits affect each other's evolution explains some of this diversity. Natural selection favors signals, receptors, and signaling behavior that maximize the received signals relative to background noise and minimize signal degradation. Properties of sensory systems bias the direction of evolution of the signals that they receive. For example, females may prefer males whose signals they can perceive more easily, and this will lead to the spread of more easily perceived male traits. Environmental conditions during signal transmission and detection also affect signal perception. Specific environmental conditions will bias the evolutionary direction of behavior, which affects the time and place of signaling as well as microhabitat preferences. Increased specialization of microhabitats and signaling behavior may lead to biased evolution of the sensory systems to work more efficiently. Thus, sensory systems, signals, signaling behavior, and habitat choice are evolutionarily coupled. These suites of traits should coevolve in predictable directions, determined by environmental biophysics, neurobiology, and the genetics of the suites of traits--hence the term "sensory drive." Because conditions vary in space and time, diversity will be generated
Article
We investigated patterns of mating call preference and mating call recognition by examining phonotaxis of female túngara frogs, Physalaemus pustulosus, in response to conspecific and heterospecific calls. There are four results: females always prefer conspecific calls; most heterospecific calls do not elicit phonotaxis; some heterospecific calls do elicit phonotaxis and thus are effective mate recognition signals; and females prefer conspecific calls to which a component of a heterospecific call has been added to a normal conspecific call. We use these data to illustrate how concepts of species recognition and sexual selection can be understood in a unitary framework by comparing the distribution of signal traits to female preference functions.
Article
The courtship behaviors of two morphologically similar spider species, Schizocosa ocreata and S. rovneri, are distinctive and prevent interbreeding. We used "forced" copulation between these species to investigate the mode of inheritance of the courtship behavior and to determine whether postmating isolating mechanisms exist. F1 hybrids proved to be behaviorally sterile, but they were capable of producing viable offspring when forced to interbreed. Analysis of the courtship behaviors of F1 , F2 , and backcross progeny showed that the inheritance of some aspects of these behaviors is consistent with models involving single autosomal loci. The inheritance of secondary sexual characteristics in the males is also investigated. The genes for courtship behavior and secondary sexual characteristics do not assort independently. The origin of the premating isolating mechanisms may be explained by either an initial habitat separation between the two groups, or by a founding event with each group subsequently diverging in slightly different habitats. It is suggested that the differences in the microhabitats may have a profound effect on what type of signal (visual or vibratory) would be effective.
Article
Male túngara frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus) vocalize to attract females, and enhance the attractiveness of their simple, whine-only call by adding chucks to produce complex calls. Complex calls contain more total energy and are of longer duration. By virtue of the greater frequency range of the chuck, complex calls also simultaneously stimulate both the amphibian papilla and the basilar papilla of the frog's inner ear. Female phonotaxis experiments using synthetic stimuli demonstrate that an increase in the call's acoustic energy is not sufficient to account for the enhanced attractiveness of the complex call. However, the stimulation of either or both of the female's sound-sensitive inner-ear organs is sufficient to elicit her preference. We suggest that the female's sensory system generates selection that equally favors at least three evolutionary alternatives for enhancing call attractiveness and that historical constraints imposed by the male's morphology determined which of the alternatives was more likely to evolve. These data are consistent with our hypothesis of sensory exploitation, which states that selection favors those traits that elicit greater stimulation from the female's sensory system and which emphasizes the nonadaptive nature of female preference.
Article
Male tungara frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus) vocalize to attract females, and enhance the attractiveness of their simple, whine-only call by adding chucks to produce complex calls. Complex calls contain more total energy and are of longer duration. By virtue of the greater frequency range of the chuck, complex calls also simultaneously stimulate both the amphibian papilla and the basilar papilla of the frog's inner ear. Female phonotaxis experiments using synthetic stimuli demonstrate that an increase in the call's acoustic energy is not sufficient to account for the enhanced attractiveness of the complex call. However, the stimulation of either or both of the female's sound-sensitive inner-ear organs is sufficient to elicit her preference. We suggest that the female's sensory system generates selection that equally favors at least three evolutionary alternatives for enhancing call attractiveness and that historical constraints imposed by the male's morphology determined which of the alternatives was more likely to evolve. These data are consistent with our hypothesis of sensory exploitation, which states that selection favors those traits that elicit greater stimulation from the female's sensory system and which emphasizes the nonadaptive nature of female preference.