ArticlePDF Available

Predictors of Media Effects on Body Dissatisfaction in European American Women

Springer Nature
Sex Roles
Authors:

Abstract

This study involved a sample of 81 European American women viewing either appearance-related or non-appearance-related magazine advertisements. Participants completed measures of demographics and objectified body consciousness prior to viewing these images and a measure of body dissatisfaction prior to and after viewing the images. Body dissatisfaction scores worsened after viewing images of women who exemplified cultural standards of the thin beauty ideal. Neither objectified body consciousness nor body mass index predicted degree of change in body dissatisfaction after viewing the images. In other words, the impact of viewing the images was the same for all women, despite varying body mass index levels and varied degrees of objectified body consciousness. Implications are discussed.
BRIEF REPORT
Predictors of Media Effects on Body Dissatisfaction
in European American Women
Emily A. Hamilton &Laurie Mintz &
Susan Kashubeck-West
#Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2007
Abstract This study involved a sample of 81 European
American women viewing either appearance-related or non-
appearance-related magazine advertisements. Participants
completed measures of demographics and objectified body
consciousness prior to viewing these images and a measure of
body dissatisfaction prior to and after viewing the images.
Body dissatisfaction scores worsened after viewing images of
women who exemplified cultural standards of the thin beauty
ideal. Neither objectified body consciousness nor body mass
index predicted degree of change in body dissatisfaction after
viewing the images. In other words, the impact of viewing the
images was the same for all women, despite varying body
mass index levels and varied degrees of objectified body
consciousness. Implications are discussed.
Keywords Body image .Body consciousness .
Media images
The mass media are the most available sources of
information in the United States today (Thompson &
Heinberg, 1999). Womens magazines, one popular medi-
um, have been criticized for furthering womens unrealis-
tically thin ideal body type. Harrison (2003) reported that
the standard of female beauty is a woman who wears a size
4 in the hips, size 2 in the waist, and a size 10 in the bust.
This body type is one that is both thinner than the average
woman and genetically impossible for most women to
attain (Wiseman, Gray, Mosimann, & Agrens, 1992).
Recent research has linked such unrealistic media
images to poor body image among women. In a meta-
analysis of the effect of experimental manipulations of the
thin beauty ideal, Groesz, Levine, and Murnen (2002)
reported that womens body image was significantly more
negative after viewing slender media images than it was
after viewing images of average-sized models, houses or
cars, or plus-sized models. The overall effect size (d) was
.31, a small to medium effect. Several moderating effects
were examined, and the findings indicated that the effect of
viewing thin models was stronger for participants who were
younger than 19 years old, who reported greater body
dissatisfaction prior to the manipulation, and in between-
subjects designs.
Although these meta-analytic results support the socio-
cultural perspective that the mass media promulgate a
slender ideal that elicits body dissatisfaction, more studies
are needed. Stice (2001) noted that the deleterious effects of
media exposure seem to be stronger for at-risk individuals.
Groesz et al. (2002) recommended research on how
images of slender beauty affect ... some females in
particular(p. 12). Although past research strongly
suggests that the negative effect of media images is
enhanced when the females exposed to them are vulnerable
because they have already internalized the slender beauty
ideal(pg. 12), Groesz et al. called for more experimental
research with participants with various pre-existing levels
of pertinent constructs. For example, the meta-analysis by
Groesz et al. indicated that women who already had a high
level of body dissatisfaction were more prone to the
negative effects of viewing thin media images. Such a
Sex Roles
DOI 10.1007/s11199-006-9178-9
E. A. Hamilton :L. Mintz
Department of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology,
University of Missouri-Columbia,
Columbia, USA
S. Kashubeck-West (*)
Division of Counseling & Family Therapy, College of Education,
University of Missouri-St. Louis,
One University Blvd.,
St. Louis, MO 63121-4400, USA
e-mail: SusanKW@umsl.edu
finding suggests that these women are more cognitively
prepared to think of themselves in regard to shape, weight,
and beauty, and, therefore, they suffer more negative effects
of exposure.
One relevant construct is self-objectification. Objectifi-
cation theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) posits that
because women are socialized to see themselves as objects
to be looked at and evaluated, they are likely to feel shame
and anxiety for not meeting cultural standards. Whereas
objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts; McKinley &
Hyde) posits that exposure to unrealistic images of
womens beauty is a cause of women objectifying
themselves (i.e., viewing themselves as an object, having
shame about their bodies) and whereas this theorizing has
been supported by previous research (Morry & Staska,
2002), no researchers have examined whether such self-
objectification puts women in a state of readiness to further
react to media images. Such a study would be a clear step
in addressing the need in the literature noted by Groesz et
al. (2002) for continued study of which women may be
most vulnerable to media images.
Numerous studies have shown that the heavier women
are, the more dissatisfied they are with their bodies (e.g.,
Cattarin & Thompson, 1994). Much as self-objectification
might prime women to react negatively to the thin media
ideal, it is possible that women who are heavier may react
more strongly to media images of thin women than women
who are thinner because heavier women are subjected to
more oppression in the form of weightism and to more
pressure to be thinner. On the other hand, it is possible that
women who are heavier are less likely to react to media
images, as they know they do not come close to meeting
these ideals. Women who are closer to the media images
might feel worse for not actually meeting them. Thus, in the
present study we also investigated whether body mass
index predicted changes in body dissatisfaction after
viewing attractive, thin, media models.
In sum, the purpose of the present study was to examine
self-objectification and weight as predictors of changes in
body image that resulted from media exposure in a sample
of European American women. Specifically, we sought to
answer the following question: Does degree of self-
objectification predict change in body esteem upon expo-
sure to media images of slender, attractive women? Three
aspects of self-objectification were studied: (1) the degree
to which a woman experiences her body from an observers
perspective; (2) the degree to which a woman feels shame
for not fulfilling cultural standards; and (3) the degree to
which a woman believes that she can control her weight
with enough effort. Self-objectification was selected be-
cause it seemed clearly related to Groesz et al.s (2002)
suggestion that some women are cognitively prepared to
think about themselves in terms of the cultural standards of
beauty. Such cognitive preparation would seem to encom-
pass the self-objectification concepts of the experience of
objectification, body shame, and control. In addition, we
examined whether a womans body mass index predicted
change in body dissatisfaction after exposure to thin media
ideals. European American women were selected for
participation because the proportion of ethnic minority
students at our university was too small to provide a
meaningful comparison group. Further, some research (e.g.,
Milkie, 1999) suggests that ethnic minority women react
differently to media images that feature women from the
dominant culture. Rather than include a small number of
ethnic minority women in the study (and ignore the
possibility that they might respond differently to the stimuli
than the European American participants), we chose to
focus on European American women, with a clear
statement that similar research needs to be conducted with
ethnic minority women.
Method
Participants
The initial sample was composed of 93 European American
female undergraduates who were attending a large public
midwestern university and were enrolled in one of three
psychology classes. Forty-eight participants were in the
experimental condition (viewed the appearance-related
images) and 45 participants constituted the control group
(viewed neutral images). Due to missing data, seven cases
from the experimental group and one case from the control
group were deleted. Outliers were identified by using SPSS
GLM to generate and plot values for CooksDfor the
repeated measure (BES score). Four outlier cases were
identified by this procedure and these cases were removed
from the analysis, which resulted in a final sample of 37
participants in the experimental group and 44 participants
in the control group. The average age of the participants
was 20.27 years (SD=1.98), and the majority of the sample
self-identified as single (84%). Mean weight for the sample
was 141.23 lbs (SD=32.54), mean height was 65.69 in.
(SD=2.63), and mean body mass index (BMI) was 22.99
(SD=4.77), which is within the normal range (20.125) for
female college students (National Heart Lung and Blood
Institute, 1998).
Instruments
Objectified body consciousness scale (OBC; McKinley &
Hyde, 1996)The OBC is a 24-item scale that was based
upon social constructionism. The OBC has three factors
(eight items per factor): (a) Surveillance represents the
Sex Roles
degree to which someone watches her body or experiences
her body from an observers perspective; (b) Body Shame
reflects the degree to which someone feels shame for not
fulfilling internalized cultural standards for body size and
shape; and (c) Control Beliefs reflects the degree to which a
person believes she can control her weight and appearance
if she tries hard enough. Participants respond on a 7-point
scale that ranges from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly
disagree) to statements such as During the day, I think
about how I look many times(Surveillance), I feel like I
must be a bad person when I dont look as good as I could
(Body Shame), and I think a person can look pretty much
how they want to if they are willing to work at it(Control
Beliefs). Average scores on the three subscales can range
from 1 to 7; higher scores indicate higher levels of each
construct (e.g., higher body surveillance, greater shame,
greater control beliefs).
McKinley and Hyde (1996) reported psychometric data
across three studies. Specifically, internal consistencies
were: Surveillance, .76.89; Body Shame, .70.84; Control
Beliefs, .68.72. Two-week testretest reliability for Sur-
veillance, Body Shame, and Control Beliefs were .79, .79,
and .73, respectively. Validity evidence included findings
that both surveillance and body shame were negatively
related to body esteem (r=.26 and .46, respectively), and
that surveillance correlated strongly with public self-
consciousness (r=.46) but had no relationship with private
self-consciousness or social anxiety (evidence of discrim-
inant validity). Surveillance also correlated with appearance
orientation and public body consciousness (r= .64 and .46,
respectively). Control beliefs correlated positively with
dieting, restricted eating, exercising to control weight, and
wearing clothes to make oneself look thinner. Cronbachs
alphas obtained for the participants in the present study
were .83, .66, and .76 for Surveillance, Body Shame, and
Control Beliefs, respectively.
Body esteem scale (BES; Franzoi & Shields, 1984)The
BES measures an individuals body esteem across three
empirically-derived dimensions. For women, these factors
include attitudes toward sexual attractiveness, weight
concern, and physical condition. Both factor scores and an
overall score can be calculated; the overall score was used
in the present study. The BES contains 35 body-related
items that participants are asked to rate on a 5-point scale
that ranges from 1 (have strong negative feelings) to 5
(have strong positive feelings). The overall body esteem
score can thus range from 35175; higher scores indicate
more positive body esteem. Cronbachs alphas from .78 to .87
have been reported for the BES, which indicates adequate
internal consistency (Franzoi & Shields, 1984). Significant
correlations between the BES and the Rosenberg Self-
Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) provide evidence for
convergent validity (Franzoi & Herzog, 1986). High test
retest correlations have been found; coefficients range from
.75 to .87 (Franzoi, 1994). However, in previous research,
scores on the BES have proven to be somewhat changeable
and reactive to stimuli, and, thus, it has been suggested that
the BES can best be conceptualized as a state scale
(Tiggemann, 2001). For the purposes of the present study,
the BES was used to measure body satisfaction as a state,
rather than as a stable trait. A Cronbachs alpha of .89 was
obtained with the BES for the present sample.
Demographics Participants were asked to complete a short
demographics questionnaire in which information about
age, race and ethnicity, height and weight, relationship
status, and education level was requested. Height and
weight information were used to calculate each partic-
ipants body-mass index (BMI).
Procedure
Undergraduate psychology students were approached in
classes and invited to take part in a study on how
advertising affects people. Of approximately 150 women
solicited, 100 agreed to participate. Of those, seven
participantsdata were not analyzed due to self-identifica-
tion as a racial/ethnic minority. Participants were assigned
to a group through random distribution of the question-
naires in class. Extra credit was provided and students who
did not wish to participate were given other alternatives for
extra credit.
Participants were given two packets and instructed to
complete the first packet without looking ahead at the
second. The initial packet contained the informed consent
form, a demographic questionnaire, the OBC, the BES, and
several distracter measures. After they completed the first
packet, participants were instructed to begin looking at the
second packet, which contained the images used as visual
stimuli and a second copy of the BES. The participants
were instructed first to look at the images for 13 min (self-
estimated) and then to complete the measures without going
back to the first packet. Finally, participants were debriefed.
The stimuli consisted of ten magazine advertisements
per group. The experimental group received five appear-
ance-related images and five neutral images and the control
group received ten neutral images (including the same five
neutral images viewed by the experimental group). The
appearance-related stimuli were advertisements that pic-
tured European American women who exemplify cultural
ideals of thinness and attractiveness. Faces were visible in
all of these stimuli, with some showing women as whole
body images and others showing partial body images. The
neutral stimuli did not include people but instead marketed
Sex Roles
cars, toilet paper, jewelry, lotion, make-up, a retail store,
gum, nail polish and liquor. All of the images were initially
chosen by the first author and then rated by three experts in
research design and media psychology to assure that the
images portrayed what was intended (i.e., cultural standards
of the thin beauty ideal or neutral images). All of the
advertisements were from magazines that targeted a
youthful, female, European American audience.
Results
Preliminary analyses
Means, standard deviations, and correlations between the
primary variables in the present study are presented in
Table 1. Preliminary analyses were conducted to determine
whether differences existed between the experimental and
control group members on demographic variables, OBC
scales, and on the BES given prior to viewing the images
(i.e., pre-intervention scores). There were no differences
between these groups with regard to: age, t(81) = 1.51, p> .14;
height, t(81)=.47, p>.64; weight t(81) = .51, p> .61; or
BMI, t(81)=.75, p>.46. Also, pre-intervention scores did
not differ significantly between the experimental and the
control groups on the BES, t(81)= .12, p> .91, or on the
three OBC scales of Surveillance, t(81) = 1.50, p> .14, Body
Shame, t(81)=.18, p>.86, and Control Beliefs, t(81) = 1.25,
p>.22. A power analysis indicated that the power for the
time X condition analysis was .55 to detect a small effect
size; this level was judged to be adequate.
Main analyses
To examine our research questions concerning the effect on
body esteem of viewing thin media images and whether this
effect was predicted by level of self-objectification and
body mass index, we conducted a repeated measures
ANCOVA with BES score as the dependent variable,
condition (experimental versus control) and time (pretest
versus posttest) as the blocking variables, and BMI and the
three self-objectification variables as covariates. By includ-
ing the three self-objectification variables and BMI as
covariates, we could examine whether these variables
played a role in the effect of the experimental manipulation.
The analysis showed that the effect of time X condition was
significant, F(1, 75)=4.14, p< .04, η
2
=.06. Thus, the
experimental manipulation of viewing images of thin
women had an effect.
1
Post hoc t-tests indicated that the
experimental group showed a significant decline in body
satisfaction from the pre-intervention measurement (M=
111.76, SD=18.61) to the post-intervention measurement
(M=109.00, SD=19.83), t(37) = 3.17, p< .003. Examination
of the BES scores for the control group found no significant
change from the pre-intervention level (M=112.20, SD=
16.46) to the post-intervention level (M=111.93, SD=
17.25), t(44)=.36, p>.72.
To assess whether self-objectification and body mass
index predicted the degree of change in body dissatisfaction
experienced after viewing cultural images of thinness and
attractiveness or neutral control images, the strength of the
effect of the OBC and BMI covariates in the ANCOVA was
examined. None of the time X OBC scale effects were
significant: Surveillance, F(1, 75)= .31, p> .58, η
2
=.00;
Body Shame, F(1, 75)=.11, p>.74, η
2
=.00; and Control
Beliefs, F(1, 75)=.07, p>.79, η
2
=.00. Thus, self-objecti-
fication did not have an effect on the results of the
manipulation, as women with both high and low levels of
self-objectification were equally affected by viewing the
images of thin women. In a similar vein, the effect of time
X BMI was not significant, F(1, 75) = .01, p> .91, η
2
=.00,
which indicates that both heavy and thin women were
equally affected by viewing the pictures of thin women. In
sum, the manipulation of viewing pictures of thin women
was powerful and the manipulations effects were not
affected by body mass index or self-objectification.
Discussion
The findings of the current study are consistent with
previous research as summarized in a recent meta-analysis
of the effects of experimental manipulations of the thin
beauty ideal (Groesz et al., 2002). Specifically, womens
body image was significantly more negative after viewing
thin media images than after viewing images of neutral
objects. Our study further confirms the conclusions reached
by Groesz et al.: media images of attractive, thin women
can have deleterious effects on the women who view them.
In addition to confirming such findings, our study
addressed an important question raised by Groesz et al. at
the conclusion of their meta-analysis: What makes some
women more vulnerable than others to media images?
We investigated two constructs (i.e., objectified body
consciousness and weight) that we thought might relate to
an increased vulnerability to media images of thin,
attractive women. Although objectification theory suggests
that exposure to unrealistic images of womens beauty
results in women seeing themselves as objects and feeling
shame about their bodies, no researchers to date had
examined whether such self-objectification then primes
women to react more negatively to these unrealistic images.
1
The same analysis was also conducted as an ANOVA, without the
covariates. The results of the ANOVA (including post-hoc tests) were
very similar to those of the ANCOVA.
Sex Roles
The results of the present study indicate that objectified
body consciousness is not a factor that increases womens
vulnerability to the deleterious effects of seeing media
images that reflect the cultural thin ideal. The women in our
study showed increased body dissatisfaction after viewing
appearance-related images, regardless of their prior levels
of self-objectification. In other words, the effect of viewing
such media images is not more powerful for women with
higher levels of objectified body consciousness.
In addition, the results also showed that women who were
heavier (had larger body mass indexes) were not prone to
more negative body dissatisfaction after viewing images of
thin women than participants who were thinner. Numerous
studies indicate that women who are heavier report greater
body dissatisfaction than other women do, and it makes
sense to think that if a woman is heavier and sees pictures of
thin women, she might feel even worse than a thin woman
who is exposed to the same images. Nevertheless, the results
of our study do not support such conjecture.
The overall purpose of the present study was to
investigate constructs that might explain why some women
in particular are affected by the mass medias promulgation
of the thin ideal. Based on our data, we conclude that
exposure to media images of thin women has a significant
impact and that objectified body consciousness and body
mass index do not enhance or detract from this effect. Thus,
there is a continued need for research to identify factors that
might make some women more vulnerable to the impact of
media images that depict thin women. Alternatively, such
research may find that, because these media images are so
pervasive and so powerful, few other factors may impact
womens reactions; in other words, just being a woman in
our culture may be the primary vulnerability factor. After
all, the women in our study were exposed to just five
images of thin women (mixed in with five neutral images)
for only 13 min yet they reported less body satisfaction
than women not exposed to those images. When one thinks
about how such a brief exposure had an effect, it seems
clear that the impact of these images is very powerful.
Future researchers should also examine the effects of
media on ethnic minority women. In the present study, we
included only European American women based on
findings that ethnic minority individuals do not identify
with White media images at the same level as European
American women do (Milkie, 1999) and the related notion
that social comparison processes underlie the medias
impact on women (Groesz et al., 2002; H. Posavac, S.
Posavac, & Weigel, 2001). As far as we can tell, researchers
have yet to examine how media images of ethnic minority
women affect ethnic minority or White women. Such future
research is critical, as are examinations of media effects on
lesbian women, women of diverse ages, and women with
disabilities.
Our current results suggest that interventions specific to
groups of women based on their level of objectified body
consciousness or body mass index are not needed. Past
interventions have focused on interrupting the social
comparison process (Posavac et al., 2001) and on reducing
acceptance of media images (Shaw & Waller, 1996); our
findings suggest that these interventions should be helpful
to women regardless of their objectified body conscious-
ness or body mass index.
Our research has several limitations. First, we examined
the effects of only one type of media (magazine advertise-
ments). Clearly, additional research with various forms of
media is needed to examine whether objectified body
consciousness or weight would have predictive power.
Second, the amount of time images were viewed was not
strictly controlled, although each participant was asked to
examine the pictures for 13 min. However, most studies
suggest that the impact of viewing such images is achieved
with very brief exposure times, and we simply wanted to
ensure that participants had more than just a brief exposure
to the images. Third, the sample size was small, which
resulted in decreased power. Nevertheless, this power level
was consistent with a number of other published studies in
this area, and it was high enough to find an effect of
viewing media images on women in general, which
suggests that, if there were effects of objectified body
consciousness and body mass index, they would have been
found. Fourth, it is possible that by using a pretest that
asked about height, weight, objectified body consciousness.
and body dissatisfaction, we may have primed participants
to be more sensitive to the media images, especially those
participants in the experimental condition. However, it
seems likely that the use of several distracter measures in
Table 1 Means, standard
deviations (SD) and correla-
tions for the main study
variables.
Note. BMI=body mass index;
BES= Body Esteem Scale
score.
*p<.05. **p< .01.
Variables Mean(SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Surveillance 38.30 (8.11)
2. Body shame 27.72 (8.08) .55**
3. Control beliefs 37.47 (7.28) .07 .01
4. BMI 22.99 (4.79) .01 .22 .13
5. BEStime 1 112.00 (17.37) .33** .38** .24* .25*
6. BEStime 2 110.59 (18.41) .30** .36** .21 .24* .96**
Sex Roles
the pretest reduced the possibility of this priming effect.
Future researchers should consider asking for that informa-
tion after the completion of the study tasks.
In conclusion, the media are one of the most direct routes
for information travel, and young women spend a significant
amount of time viewing media images. The results of the
present study confirm past conclusions that the visual media
unfortunately are very effective at causing European Amer-
ican women to feel less positively about their bodies. Our
data also suggest that all European American women, not
just those who are heavy or who have internalized cultural
messages that womens bodies are to be viewed as objects,
are vulnerable to the effect of media messages. Additional
research to investigate which women are most vulnerable to
the medias effect is needed. Although it is an admittedly
idealistic and more grandiose goal, we also hope that our
findings can help to influence the culture as a whole to
examine the detrimental impact of unrealistic images in
advertisements on womens body image.
References
Cattarin, J. A., & Thompson, J. K. (1994). A three-year longitudinal
study of body image, eating disturbance, and general psychological
functioning in adolescent females. Eating Disorders, 2, 114125.
Franzoi, S. L. (1994). Further evidence of the reliability and validity
of the Body Esteem Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50,
237239.
Franzoi, S. L., & Herzog, M. E. (1986). The Body Esteem Scale: A
convergent and discriminant validity study. Journal of Personal-
ity Assessment, 50, 2431.
Franzoi, S. L., & Shields, S. A. (1984). The Body Esteem Scale:
Multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college
population. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 173179.
Fredrickson, B., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory:
Toward understanding womens lived experiences and mental
health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173206.
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of
experimental manipulation of the thin media images on body
satisfaction: A meta-analytical review. International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 3, 116.
Harrison, K. (2003). Television viewersideal body proportions: The
case of the curvaceously thin woman. Sex Roles, 48, 255264.
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The Objectified Body
Consciousness Scale: Development and validation. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 20, 181215.
Milkie, M. A. (1999). Societal comparisons, reflected appraisals, and
mass media: The impact of pervasive beauty images on Black
and White girls self concepts. Social Psychology Quarterly, 62,
190210.
Morry, M., & Staska, S. (2002). Magazine exposure: Internalization,
self-objectification, eating attitudes, and body satisfaction in male
and female university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioral
Science, 33, 269279.
National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (1998). Clinical guidelines
on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and
obesity in adults (NIH Publication No. 98-4083). Washington,
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Posavac, H., Posavac, S., & Weigel, R. (2001). Reducing the impact
of media images for women at risk for body image disturbance:
Three targeted interventions. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 20, 324340.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Prince-
ton, NJ: Princeton University.
Shaw, J., & Waller, G. (1996). The medias impact on body image:
Implications for prevention and treatment. Eating Disorders, 3,
115123.
Stice, E. (2001). Risk factors for eating pathology: Recent advances
and future directions. In R. H. Striegel-Moore, & L. Smolak
(Eds.), Eating disorders: Innovative directions in research and
practice (pp. 5173). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Tiggemann, M. (2001). Person*situation interactions in body dissat-
isfaction. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 6570.
Thompson, J. K., & Heinberg, L. J. (1999). The medias influence on
body image disturbance and eating disorders: Weve reviled
them, now can we rehabilitate them? Journal of Social Issues, 55,
339353.
Wiseman, C. F., Gray, J. J., Mosimann, J. E., & Ahrens, A. H. (1992).
Cultural expectation s of thinness in women: An update.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 8589.
Sex Roles
... Frederickson and Roberts (1997) proposed the objectification theory, which argues that women's perceptions as mere objects instead of human beings in the sight of others accounts for the sense of shame, disapproval, or even hatred they feel toward their bodies. Hamilton, Mintz, and Kashubeck-West (2007) did not produce findings that supported the objectification theory's ability to predict body dissatisfaction. Stormer and Thompson (1996) proposed four other potential explanations for the development of negative body image. ...
... Numerous studies show that advertisements have effects on individuals' perceptions of beauty and their levels of body satisfaction. Hamilton, Mintz, and Kashubeck-West (2007), as well as Glauert, Rhodes, Byrne, Fink, and Grammar (2009) found that priming their participants with advertisements of thin, American women increased their levels of body dissatisfaction. Evans and McConnell (2003) achieved similar results with Chinese participants by using advertisements of an ideal American woman (blonde hair, blue eyes, fair skin); these participants experienced decreased ratings in self-esteem and increased desires to conform to the Caucasian standard. ...
Article
Full-text available
This pilot study compared the perceptions of beauty among Chinese women who were exchange students in the United States with Chinese women who were students in their homeland. We interviewed 19 women in China and 19 women in the United States to determine differences in responses. In accordance with the sociocultural approach and the social comparison approach, we expected Chinese women in the United States to have a be more acculturate, more frequently conclude that American women were more beautiful than Chinese women, be more likely than those studying in China to report body dissatisfaction, be more likely to dislike and desire to alter body parts that specifically reflect American beauty ideals, and express a greater desire to surgically alter their bodies. Results indicated that participants in the United States group were more likely to reflect some American beauty standards (particularly their desires to lose weight and to be taller), while maintaining those of their own culture (the importance of facial appearance), as well. Suggestions for future research and practice, particularly for mental health workers on college campuses with growing populations of Chinese exchange students, are discussed.
... Appearance anxiety has also been reported to increase after viewing advertisements featuring idealized images (Monro & Huon, 2005). Pictures of female models in magazines have indeed been found to exert a powerful influence on a woman's feelings about her own body, regardless of her own weight and size (Hamilton, Mintz, & Kashubeck-West, 2007). According to Groesz, Levine and Murnen (2002), mass media promulgates a slender ideal that elicits body dissatisfaction. ...
Article
Full-text available
Past research has highlighted the role of media for portraying thinning standards of beauty and causing body dissatisfaction among young women. The present research experimentally investigates the effect of the thin ideal, as portrayed in the media, on body dissatisfaction and negative affect in females. 96 undergraduate female students were randomly assigned to two groups that were shown and asked to rate the attractiveness of thin idealized images of women (experimental group) or pens (control group). After viewing the images, participants were required to complete the Adapted Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction with Body Parts Scale (Stice, Maxfield & Wells, 2003) and PANAS-X (Watson & Clark, 1991). Participants were briefly interviewed about their conceptions of beauty. Age, BMI and the number of stimulus presentations were matched. Results showed no significant differences between the two groups, as assessed by t-tests, in body dissatisfaction and negative affect (or any of its sub dimensions, fear, hostility, guilt or sadness). Content analysis of interview responses revealed that participants described a beautiful woman as having “inner beauty/mind” than “being slender”. Contrary to popular research, it may be concluded that not all young women are affected by media exposure. Body dissatisfaction and preference for thinness are affected by socio-cultural and personal factors.
... For example, multiple studies have demonstrated the detrimental effects of the media on body image (M. K. Gordon, 2008;Hamilton et al., 2007;Milkie, 1999;Tiggemann, 2006;Tiggemann & McGill, 2004). Gunther et al. (2006) found that the media played a role in the perpetuation of perceived peer norms. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study uses a Foucauldian discourse analysis to explore media reporting on the role of nurses as being consistently positioned ‘heroes’ during COVID‐19. In so doing, it highlights multiple intersecting discourses at play, with the caring discourse acting as a central one in negatively impacting nurses' ability to advocate for safe working conditions during a public health emergency. Drawing on media reports during the outbreak of COVID‐19 in Ontario, Canada in the spring of 2020 and on historical information from SARS, this study seeks to establish caring as a discourse and examine if the caring discourse impedes nurses' ability to protect themselves from harm. The results of this analysis explicate how public media discourses that position nurses as caring, sacrificial and heroic may have impacted their ability to maintain their personal safety as a result of the expectations put upon the nursing profession.
... The need for the current research included the global influences of selfobjectification, attempting to ameliorate its consequences not limiting to restrained eating, depression, and body image disturbances (Aubrey & Gerding, 2015;Hamilton et al., 2007;Mercurio & Rima, 2011). The traits associating to self-objectification and objectified body consciousness are having a severe negative influence on anorectics and those experiencing depression (Gervais & Davidson, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this current quantitative predictive correlational research was to determine if and to what extent self-objectification predicted body surveillance and body shame, and if state mindfulness mediated those predictive relationships for female Pilates and yoga practitioners from the northeast United States. The theoretical foundation for the present study included objectification theory, cognitive theory of mindfulness (CTM), and objectified body consciousness theory. A sample of 95 female northeast United States Pilates or yoga practitioners completed an online survey measuring self-objectification, state mindfulness, body surveillance, body shame, and demographic information. The validated scales used in the current research included the self-objectification beliefs and behaviors scale (SOBBS), the state mindfulness scale for physical activity (SMS-PA), and the objectified body consciousness scale (OBCS). The findings of the regression analyses determined a significant positive association to exist between self-objectification and body surveillance (b = .3546, p = .0422) and body shame (b = .8804, p = .0000). However, state mindfulness did not mediate the predictive relationship between self-objectification and body surveillance (LLCI -.1048 ULCI .0523) or body shame (LLCI -.0544 ULCI .0271.). Additional research may be necessary to examine the mediating effects of state mindfulness on self-objectification, body surveillance, and body shame. Future research should make use of the positive findings of the current research to explore other methods of diminishing the negative outcomes of high self-objectification.
... For the first time in the video game market where the main heroes are usually males, the main character being a female represents a major transformation in this market. The current studies on the portrayal of women in the media have found that women are often portrayed as sex objects (Lavine, Sweeney and Wagner, 1999;Hamilton, Mintz and Kashubeck-West, 2007). Tracy L. Dietz's (1998) study on gender, role attitudes or stereotypical images of men and women in video games is among the pioneering works in this field. ...
Article
Full-text available
Internet and related new communication environments have also caused many changes in social and cultural structures. Games which are a mass communication medium, provide a space where players can easily obtain images, ideas, and evaluations to determine their own behavioral schedules. There are a number of studies that have included a variety of methodologies and data sources to determine whether or not video games, are also a direct influence on people’s thoughts and behaviors such as traditional media environments that address millions of people. Today, MMORPGs are becoming more and more difficult with increasingly difficult tasks and wars. Different types of media such as visual and print media are the platforms that people can meet different stereotypes. Stereotypes are common thoughts that they are not private and carried on by sharing within the same community. While stereotype has a more uniform quality, prejudice can encompass a large number of stereotypes (ex, race, religion, sex stereotypes). This study aims to determine whether or not there is a relationship between gender and racial stereotypes of video game characters and variables such as race, gender, body types, and sexual portrayal-attire. Of the 107 RPG games that were examined, there were a total of 273 (46.2%) male characters and 318 (53.8%) female characters. Results of the chi-square analysis indicated that there were significant distribution differences between dependent and independent variables.
... Many women also compare their own appearance to images of women they see in the media and in their everyday lives. Research across cultures has revealed that, when exposed to images of women who appear to embody beauty and thinness ideals, female participants report higher levels of body dissatisfaction compared to women who view neutral images (Anixiadis et al., 2019;Betz et al., 2019;Brown & Tiggemann, 2016;Hamilton, Mintz, & Kashubeck-West, 2007;Nagar & Virk, 2017). Although the bulk of these images have been digitally altered to create the illusion of bodily perfection, research suggests that mere awareness that these photos have been altered does not mitigate their influence on body dissatisfaction (Frederick, Sandhu, Scott, & Akbari, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The development and maintenance of positive body image in women may be disrupted by sociocultural appearance-related pressures. Therefore, it is critical to explore factors that may safeguard women's positive body image. A recent study by Homan and Tylka (2015) found that in a large sample (N = 263) of female MTurk workers and university-aged women, both appearance-contingent self-worth and body-based social comparisons were linked to less positive body image, but these links were attenuated in the face of high self-compassion. This research, an independent direct replication of the original study, supported the original findings. In a new, larger sample (N = 363 female-identified MTurk workers), signals were detected that were similar in size and magnitude to the original study. Specifically, while appearance-contingent self-worth and body-based social comparisons were negatively linked to body appreciation, those who endorsed higher levels of self-compassion reported a more positive body image, even in the presence of these potential threats. Findings are extended to eating- and exercise-based social comparisons. All materials, including the replication protocol, data management plan, dataset, SPSS syntax, and output are publicly available on the Open Science Framework at: https://osf.io/r274y/. Implications for research and practice are discussed.
Article
Genital self-image describes the perception of one's genital appearance as being "normal" or "abnormal," and a disharmonious image leads to an increasing number of women seeking esthetic genital surgery each year. The concept of what constitutes "normal" is strongly influenced by the media, cultural norms, and sexual relations. In reality, the extent of normalcy is highly variable and overall patient education regarding extremes of size and shape should be provided to all patients considering surgery. When performed with appropriate training, expertise, and attention to detail in a properly selected patient, esthetic genital surgery is associated with minimal complications or sequelae.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has shown that exposure to the ideal thin standard of female beauty commonly presented in advertising and the broader media contributes to body image disturbance among women. Researchers have proposed that social comparison processes underlie this phenomenon; women may routinely compare their bodies with images of feminine beauty contained in the media and consequently become less satisfied with their own bodies. This study reports the evaluation of three interventions that were designed to prevent women from experiencing body image disturbance when exposed to media images by interrupting the social comparison process. Experimental data suggest that all three interventions (a) decreased the likelihood of female participants comparing their bodies with media images, and (b) prevented media-induced body image disturbance. The content of the interventions, and implications for the treatment and prevention of body image disturbance and eating disorders are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This article offers objectification theory as a framework for understanding the experiential consequences of being female in a culture that sexually objectifies the female body. Objectification theory posits that girls and women are typically acculturated to internalize an observer's perspective as a primary view of their physical selves. This perspective on self can lead to habitual body monitoring, which, in turn, can increase women's opportunities for shame and anxiety, reduce opportunities for peak motivational states, and diminish awareness of internal bodily states. Accumulations of such experiences may help account for an array of mental health risks that disproportionately affect women: unipolar depression, sexual dysfunction, and eating disorders. Objectification theory also illuminates why changes in these mental health risks appear to occur in step with life-course changes in the female body.
Article
Results of the present study indicate that over a 3-month period, the multidimensional Body Esteem Scale (BES) exhibits high test-retest reliability. Findings further indicate that the BES is not susceptible to a tendency to unrealistically deny negative self-attributes and is only very slightly susceptible to a tendency to unrealistically attribute positive self-attributes. Overall, the present study supports the continued use of the BES as a reliable and valid measure of male and female body esteem.
Article
An investigation of current American society's depiction of the ideal female body was performed. Body measurements of Playboy magazine centerfolds and Miss America contestants for 1979-1988 indicated body weight 13-19% below expected weight for women in that age group. Miss America contestants showed a significant decrease in expected weight between 1979 and 1988. Comparisons were made with an earlier study which had demonstrated that body measurements of both groups had decreased during the period 1959-1978. Diet-for-weight-loss and exercise articles in six women's magazines were tabulated for 1959-1988. A significant increase in both diet articles and exercise articles occurred during this period. These findings suggest that the overvaluation of thinness continues and thinness is now sought through both dieting and exercise.
Article
Recent research has demonstrated that media images of “ideal” female models have an impact upon women's body image, leading to dissatisfaction and perceptual distortion. The evidence for this link between media presentation and body image distortion is reviewed, and theoretical models are advanced to explain the link. In particular, women's use of social comparison in establishing their self-concept seems to be an important psychological construct in understanding the impact of the media upon body image. Based on empirical evidence and proposed psychological mediators, a number of measures are suggested that might prevent media effects or that might be used to treat those effects. In particular, psychoeducational preventative measures and group-based treatments seem to offer the greatest promise.
Article
Content analyses and experimental studies often indicate strong, usually negative effects of media on the self In contrast, qualitative work suggests that individuals may exercise considerable influence in selecting, interpreting and criticizing media content. This literature, however, does not adequately consider or specify how "interpreted" media content still might affect self-concept negatively. Incorporating social comparison and reflected appraisal processes, this study shows how media affect self-esteem indirectly, despite criticism, through beliefs about how others use and are affected by media. In-depth interviews with 60 white and minority girls, complemented by quantitative measures from a larger study, help to clarify how girls are affected by prominent images of females pervasive in media. Most girls see the images as unrealistic; many prefer to see "real" girls. White girls, despite their criticism, are still harmed by the images because they believe that others find the images important and that others in the local culture, especially boys, evaluate them on the basis of these images. Minority girls do not identify with "white" media images, nor believe that significant others are affected by them; thus their critical interpretations succeed in thwarting negative feelings. The study increases our understanding of media effects on the self-concept and suggests that researchers consider how media images may be part of social comparison and reflected appraisal processes.