ArticlePDF Available

Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement

Authors:
  • Columbia University Teachers College

Abstract and Figures

Identifying the types of mathematics content knowledge that are most predictive of students' long-term learning is essential for improving both theories of mathematical development and mathematics education. To identify these types of knowledge, we examined long-term predictors of high school students' knowledge of algebra and overall mathematics achievement. Analyses of large, nationally representative, longitudinal data sets from the United States and the United Kingdom revealed that elementary school students' knowledge of fractions and of division uniquely predicts those students' knowledge of algebra and overall mathematics achievement in high school, 5 or 6 years later, even after statistically controlling for other types of mathematical knowledge, general intellectual ability, working memory, and family income and education. Implications of these findings for understanding and improving mathematics learning are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Science
23(7) 691 –697
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797612440101
http://pss.sagepub.com
Knowledge of mathematics is crucial to educational and finan-
cial success in contemporary society and is becoming ever
more so. High school students’ mathematics achievement pre-
dicts college matriculation and graduation, early-career earn-
ings, and earnings growth (Murnane, Willett, & Levy, 1995;
National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008). The strength
of these relations appears to have increased in recent decades,
probably because of a growing percentage of well-paying jobs
requiring mathematical proficiency (Murnane et al., 1995).
However, many students lack even the basic mathematics
competence needed to succeed in typical jobs in a modern
economy. Children from low-income and minority back-
grounds are particularly at risk for poor mathematics achieve-
ment (Hanushek & Rivkin, 2006).
Marked individual and social-class differences in mathemat-
ical knowledge are present even in preschool and kindergarten
(Case & Okamoto, 1996; Starkey, Klein, & Wakeley, 2004).
These differences are stable at least from kindergarten through
fifth grade; children who start ahead in mathematics generally
stay ahead, and children who start behind generally stay behind
(Duncan et al., 2007; Stevenson & Newman, 1986). There are
substantial correlations between early and later knowledge in
other academic subjects as well, but differences in children’s
mathematics knowledge are even more stable than differences
in their reading and other capabilities (Case, Griffin, & Kelly,
1999; Duncan et al., 2007).
These findings suggest a new type of research that can con-
tribute both to theoretical understanding of mathematical devel-
opment and to improving mathematics education. If researchers
can identify specific areas of mathematics that consistently pre-
dict later mathematics proficiency, after controlling for other
types of mathematical knowledge, general intellectual ability,
and family background variables, they can then determine why
those types of knowledge are uniquely predictive, and society
can increase efforts to improve instruction and learning in those
areas. The educational payoff is likely to be strongest for areas
that are strongly predictive of later achievement and in which
many children’s understanding is poor.
Corresponding Author:
Robert S. Siegler, Carnegie Mellon University–Psychology, 5000 Forbes Ave.,
Pittsburgh, PA 15213
E-mail: rs7k@andrew.cmu.edu
Early Predictors of High School
Mathematics Achievement
Robert S. Siegler
1
, Greg J. Duncan
2
, Pamela E. Davis-Kean
3,4
,
Kathryn Duckworth
5
, Amy Claessens
6
, Mimi Engel
7
,
Maria Ines Susperreguy
3,4
, and Meichu Chen
4
1
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University;
2
Department of Education, University of California,
Irvine;
3
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan;
4
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan;
5
Quantitative Social Science, Institute of Education, University of London;
6
Department of Public Policy,
University of Chicago; and
7
Department of Public Policy and Education, Vanderbilt University
Abstract
Identifying the types of mathematics content knowledge that are most predictive of students’ long-term learning is essential
for improving both theories of mathematical development and mathematics education. To identify these types of knowledge,
we examined long-term predictors of high school students’ knowledge of algebra and overall mathematics achievement.
Analyses of large, nationally representative, longitudinal data sets from the United States and the United Kingdom revealed
that elementary school students’ knowledge of fractions and of division uniquely predicts those students’ knowledge of
algebra and overall mathematics achievement in high school, 5 or 6 years later, even after statistically controlling for other
types of mathematical knowledge, general intellectual ability, working memory, and family income and education. Implications
of these findings for understanding and improving mathematics learning are discussed.
Keywords
mathematics achievement, cognitive development, childhood development, fractions, division
Received 10/26/11; Revision accepted 1/31/12
Research Report
692 Siegler et al.
In the present study, we examined sources of continuity in
mathematical knowledge from fifth grade through high school.
We were particularly interested in testing the hypothesis that
early knowledge of fractions is uniquely predictive of later
knowledge of algebra and overall mathematics achievement.
One source of this hypothesis was Siegler, Thompson, and
Schneiders (2011) integrated theory of numerical develop-
ment. This theory proposes that numerical development is a
process of progressively broadening the class of numbers that
are understood to possess magnitudes and of learning the func-
tions that connect those numbers to their magnitudes. In other
words, numerical development involves coming to understand
that all real numbers have magnitudes that can be assigned
specific locations on number lines. This idea resembles Case
and Okamoto’s (1996) proposal that during mathematics
learning, the central conceptual structure for whole numbers, a
mental number line, is eventually extended to rational num-
bers. The integrated theory of numerical development also
proposes that a complementary, and equally crucial, part of
numerical development is learning that many properties of
whole numbers (e.g., having unique successors, being count-
able, including a finite number of entities within any given
interval, never decreasing with addition and multiplication)
are not true of numbers in general.
One implication of this theory is that acquisition of fractions
knowledge is crucial to numerical development. For most chil-
dren, fractions provide the first opportunity to learn that several
salient and invariant properties of whole numbers are not true of
all numbers (e.g., that multiplication does not necessarily pro-
duce answers greater than the multiplicands). This understand-
ing does not come easily; although children receive repeated
instruction on fractions starting in third or fourth grade (National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 2006), even high school
and community-college students often confuse properties of
fractions and whole numbers (Schneider & Siegler, 2010;
Vosniadou, Vamvakoussi, & Skopeliti, 2008).
This view of fractions as occupying a central position
within mathematical development differs substantially from
other theories in the area, which focus on whole numbers and
relegate fractions to secondary status. To the extent that such
theories address development of understanding of fractions at
all, it is usually to document ways in which learning about
them is hindered by whole-number knowledge (e.g., Gelman
& Williams, 1998; Wynn, 1995). Nothing in these theories
suggests that early knowledge of fractions would uniquely
predict later mathematics proficiency.
Consider some reasons, however, why elementary school
students’ knowledge of fractions might be crucial for later
mathematics—for example, algebra. If students do not under-
stand fractions, they cannot estimate answers even to simple
algebraic equations. For example, students who do not under-
stand fractions will not know that in the equation 1/3X = 2/3Y,
X must be twice as large as Y, or that for the equation 3/4X = 6,
the value of X must be somewhat, but not greatly, larger than
6. Students who do not understand fraction magnitudes also
would not be able to reject flawed equations by reasoning
that the answers they yield are impossible. Consistent with
this analysis, studies have shown that accurate estimation of
fraction magnitudes is closely related to correct use of frac-
tions arithmetic procedures (Hecht & Vagi, 2010; Siegler
et al., 2011). Thus, we hypothesized that 10-year-olds’ knowl-
edge of fractions would predict their algebra knowledge
and overall mathematics achievement at age 16, even after we
statistically controlled for other mathematical knowledge,
information-processing skills, general intellectual ability, and
family income and education.
Method
To identify predictors of high school mathematics proficiency,
we examined two nationally representative, longitudinal data
sets: the British Cohort Study (BCS; Butler & Bynner, 1980,
1986; Bynner, Ferri, & Shepherd, 1997) and the Panel Study
of Income Dynamics-Child Development Supplement (PSID-
CDS; Hofferth, Davis-Kean, Davis, & Finkelstein, 1998).
Detailed descriptions of the samples and measures used in
these studies and of the statistical analyses that we applied are
included in the Supplemental Material available online; here,
we provide a brief overview.
The BCS sample included 3,677 children born in the United
Kingdom in a single week of 1970. The tests of interest in the
present study were administered in 1980, when the children
were 10-year-olds, and in 1986, when the children were
16-year-olds. Mathematics proficiency at age 10 was assessed
by performance on the Friendly Maths Test, which examined
knowledge of whole-number arithmetic and fractions. Mathe-
matics proficiency at age 16 was assessed by the APU (Applied
Psychology Unit) Arithmetic Test, which examined knowl-
edge of whole-number arithmetic, fractions, algebra, and
probability. General intelligence was assessed at age 10 by
performance on the British Ability Scale, which included mea-
sures of verbal and nonverbal intellectual ability, vocabulary,
and spelling. Parents provided information about their educa-
tion and income and their children’s gender, age, and number
of siblings.
The PSID-CDS included a nationally representative sample
of 599 U.S. children who were tested in 1997 as 10- to 12-
year-olds and in 2002 as 15- to 17-year-olds. At both ages,
they completed parts of the Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-
Educational Battery-Revised (WJ-R), a widely used achieve-
ment test. The 10- to 12-year-olds performed the Calculation
Subtest, which included 28 whole-number arithmetic items (8
addition, 8 subtraction, 7 multiplication, and 5 division items)
and 9 fractions items. The 15- to 17-year-olds completed the
test’s Applied Problems Subtest, which included 60 items on
whole-number arithmetic, fractions, algebra, geometry, mea-
surement, and probability. Applied Problems items 29, 42, 43,
45, and 46 were used to construct the measure of fractions
knowledge, and items 34, 49, 52, and 59 were used to con-
struct the measure of algebra knowledge. Also obtained at
Early Predictors of High School Math Achievement 693
ages 10 to 12 were measures of working memory (as indexed
by backward digit span), demographic characteristics (gender,
age, and number of siblings), and family background (parental
education in years and log mean income averaged over 3
years). Two measures of literacy from the WJ-R, passage com-
prehension and letter-word identification (a vocabulary test),
were obtained both at age 10 to 12 and at age 15 to 17.
Results
The results yielded by bivariate and multiple regression analy-
ses are presented for the British sample in Table 1 and for the
U.S. sample in Table 2. In both tables, results are presented for
algebra scores (Models 1 and 2) and total math scores (Models
3 and 4).
Our main hypothesis was that knowledge of fractions at age
10 would predict algebra knowledge and overall mathematics
achievement in high school, above and beyond the effects of
general intellectual ability, other mathematical knowledge,
and family background. The data supported this hypothesis.
In the United Kingdom (U.K.) data, after effects of all other
variables were statistically controlled, fractions knowledge at
age 10 was the strongest of the five mathematics predictors of
age-16 algebra knowledge and mathematics achievement
(Table 1, Models 2 and 4). A 1-SD increase in early fractions
knowledge was uniquely associated with a 0.15-SD increase in
subsequent algebra knowledge and a 0.16-SD increase in total
math achievement (p < .001 for both coefficients). In the U.S.
data, after effects of other variables were statistically con-
trolled, the relations between fractions knowledge at ages 10
to 12 and high school algebra and overall mathematics
achievement at ages 15 to 17 were of approximately the same
strength as the corresponding relations in the U.K. data (Mod-
els 2 and 4 in Tables 1 and 2). As documented in the Supple-
mental Material (see Tables S5 and S6), in both data sets, the
predictive power of increments to fractions knowledge was
equally strong for children lower and higher in fractions
knowledge.
If fractions knowledge continues to be a direct contributor to
mathematics achievement in high school, as opposed to having
influenced earlier learning but no longer being directly influen-
tial, we would expect strong concurrent relations between high
school students’ knowledge of fractions and their overall math-
ematical knowledge. High school students’ knowledge of frac-
tions did correlate very strongly with their overall mathematics
achievement, in both the United Kingdom, r(3675) = .81, p <
.001, and the United States, r(597) = .87, p < .001. Their frac-
tions knowledge also was closely related to their knowledge of
algebra in both the United Kingdom, r(3675) = .68, p < .001,
and the United States, r(597) = .65, p < .001. Although algebra
is a major part of high school mathematics and fractions consti-
tute a smaller part, the correlation between high school students’
Table 1. Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement: British Cohort Study Data (N = 3,677)
Algebra score Total math score
Predictor
Model 1 (bivariate
regression)
Model 2 (multiple
regression)
Model 3 (bivariate
regression)
Model 4 (multiple
regression)
Age-10 math skills
Fractions 0.42*** (0.02) 0.15*** (0.02) 0.46*** (0.02) 0.16*** (0.02)
Addition 0.20*** (0.02) 0.00 (0.02) 0.26*** (0.02) 0.05** (0.02)
Subtraction 0.22*** (0.02) 0.04* (0.02) 0.24*** (0.02) 0.03 (0.02)
Multiplication 0.32*** (0.02) 0.06*** (0.02) 0.37*** (0.02) 0.08*** (0.02)
Division 0.37*** (0.02) 0.13*** (0.02) 0.40*** (0.02) 0.12*** (0.02)
Age-10 abilities
Verbal IQ 0.39*** (0.02) 0.11*** (0.02) 0.42*** (0.02) 0.10*** (0.02)
Nonverbal IQ 0.41*** (0.02) 0.17*** (0.02) 0.46*** (0.02) 0.19*** (0.02)
Demographic characteristics
Female gender –0.02 (0.02) 0.00 (0.02) –0.01 (0.02) 0.00 (0.01)
Age 0.01 (0.02) –0.03* (0.02) 0.01 (0.02) –0.03* (0.01)
Log mean household income 0.38*** (0.04) 0.08* (0.03) 0.40*** (0.04) 0.09* (0.04)
Parents’ education 0.27*** (0.02) 0.10*** (0.02) 0.29*** (0.02) 0.10*** (0.02)
Number of siblings –0.05** (0.02) –0.01 (0.01) –0.09 (0.02) –0.05*** (0.01)
Mean R
2
.29 .35
Note: This table presents results from regression models predicting algebra and total math scores at age 16 from math skills, cognitive
ability, and child and family characteristics at age 10. All predictors and dependent variables were standardized; therefore, although the
coefficients reported are unstandardized, they can be interpreted much like standardized coefficients. Parameter estimates and stan-
dard errors (in parentheses) are based on 20 multiply imputed data sets. The British Cohort Study data on which these analyses were
based are publicly available from the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London Web site: http://
www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/bcs70.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
694 Siegler et al.
knowledge of fractions and their overall mathematics achieve-
ment was stronger than the correlation between their algebra
knowledge and their overall mathematics achievement in both
the U.K. data, r(3675) = .81 versus .73, χ
2
(1, N = 3,677) = 66.49,
p < .001, and the U.S. data, r(597) = .87 versus .80, χ
2
(1, N =
599) = 15.03, p < .001.
Early knowledge of whole-number division also was consis-
tently related to later mathematics proficiency. Among the five
mathematics variables derived from the elementary school tests,
early division had the second-strongest correlation with later
mathematics outcomes in the U.K. data (Table 1) and the stron-
gest correlation with later mathematics outcomes in the U.S.
data (Table 2). Concurrent correlations between high school stu-
dents’ knowledge of division and their overall mathematics
achievement were also substantial both in the United Kingdom,
r(3675) = .59, and in the United States, r(597) = .69, ps < .001.
To the best of our knowledge, relations between elementary
school children’s division knowledge and their mathematics
proficiency in high school have not been documented
previously.
Regressions like those in Tables 1 and 2 place no con-
straints on the estimated coefficients. Therefore, we reesti-
mated our regression models, first imposing an equality
constraint on the coefficients for fractions and division, and
then imposing an equality constraint on the coefficients for
addition, subtraction, and multiplication (see Table S4 in the
Supplemental Material). Finally, we tested whether the pooled
coefficients for these two sets of skills differed from each
other. The predictive relation was stronger for fractions and
division than for the other mathematical skills in both the U.K.
and the U.S. data—U.K.: F(1, 3664) = 36.92, p < .001, for
algebra and F(1, 3664) = 28.79, p < .001, for overall mathe-
matics; U.S.: F(1, 558) = 7.12, p < .01, for algebra and F(1,
558) = 9.72, p < .01, for overall mathematics (see Table S4).
The greater predictive power of knowledge of fractions and
knowledge of division was not due to their generally predict-
ing intellectual outcomes more accurately. When Models 2
and 4 in Tables 1 and 2 were applied to predicting high school
students’ literacy (spelling and vocabulary from the British
Ability Scale in the BCS; passage comprehension and letter-
word identification from the WJ-R in the PSID-CDS), only
two of the eight predictive relations between fractions and
division knowledge, on the one hand, and literacy, on the
other, were significant: Fractions knowledge predicted vocab-
ulary in the U.K. data, and division knowledge predicted
letter-word identification in the U.S. data (see Tables 3 and 4).
Moreover, in all cases but one, the pooled predictive effect of
fractions and division knowledge on literacy was no greater
than the pooled predictive effect of addition, subtraction, and
multiplication knowledge on literacy (see Table S4 in the Sup-
plemental Material). The one exception was that fractions and
division knowledge more accurately predicted vocabulary in
Table 2. Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement: Panel Study of Income Dynamics Data (N = 599)
Algebra score Total math score
Predictor
Model 1 (bivariate
regression)
Model 2 (multiple
regression)
Model 3 (bivariate
regression)
Model 4 (multiple
regression)
Early math skills
Fractions 0.41*** (0.06) 0.17* (0.08) 0.49*** (0.05) 0.18** (0.06)
Addition 0.26*** (0.06) 0.09 (0.06) 0.30*** (0.06) 0.05 (0.05)
Subtraction 0.26*** (0.05) 0.04 (0.05) 0.39*** (0.05) 0.12* (0.05)
Multiplication 0.31*** (0.05) 0.00 (0.06) 0.43*** (0.05) 0.02 (0.05)
Division 0.40*** (0.05) 0.19*** (0.06) 0.53*** (0.05) 0.26*** (0.06)
Early abilities
Backward digit span 0.29*** (0.06) 0.10 (0.06) 0.33*** (0.05) 0.08 (0.05)
Passage comprehension 0.38*** (0.05) 0.11 (0.06) 0.51*** (0.05) 0.20*** (0.05)
Demographic characteristics
Female gender –0.06 (0.05) –0.09 (0.05) –0.08 (0.05) –0.13*** (0.04)
Age 0.04 (0.05) –0.18*** (0.05) 0.06 (0.05) –0.22*** (0.04)
Log mean family income
(1994–1996)
0.31*** (0.06) 0.05 (0.06) 0.38*** (0.06) 0.12* (0.06)
Parents’ education 0.39*** (0.05) 0.19*** (0.05) 0.41*** (0.06) 0.11 (0.06)
Number of siblings –0.17** (0.06) –0.03 (0.05) –0.18** (0.06) –0.03 (0.04)
Mean R
2
.35 .52
Note: This table presents results from regression models predicting algebra and total math scores at age 15 to 17 from math skills,
cognitive abilities, and child and family characteristics at age 10 to 12. All predictors and dependent variables were standardized;
therefore, although the coefficients reported are unstandardized, they can be interpreted much like standardized coefficients.
Parameter estimates and standard errors (in parentheses) are based on 20 multiply imputed data sets. The data on which these
analyses were based came from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics public-use data set, available at http://psidonline.isr.umich.edu.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Early Predictors of High School Math Achievement 695
Table 3. Results From Regression Models Predicting Literacy at Age 16 From Math Skills and Child and Family
Characteristics at Age 10: British Cohort Study Data (N = 3,677)
Spelling Vocabulary
Predictor
Model 1 (bivariate
regression)
Model 2 (multiple
regression)
Model 3 (bivariate
regression)
Model 4 (multiple
regression)
Age-10 math skills
Fractions 0.16*** (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.38*** (0.02) 0.09*** (0.02)
Addition 0.10*** (0.02) 0.00 (0.02) 0.21*** (0.02) 0.03 (0.02)
Subtraction 0.12*** (0.02) 0.04 (0.02) 0.17*** (0.02) 0.00 (0.02)
Multiplication 0.17*** (0.02) 0.05* (0.02) 0.27*** (0.02) 0.03 (0.02)
Division 0.16*** (0.02) 0.03 (0.02) 0.28*** (0.02) 0.04 (0.02)
Age-10 abilities
Verbal IQ 0.19*** (0.02) 0.09*** (0.02) 0.49*** (0.02) 0.30*** (0.02)
Nonverbal IQ 0.20*** (0.02) 0.08*** (0.02) 0.38*** (0.02) 0.10*** (0.02)
Demographic characteristics
Female gender 0.14*** (0.02) 0.14*** (0.02) 0.00 (0.02) 0.03 (0.02)
Age 0.01 (0.02) 0.00 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) –0.02 (0.02)
Log mean household income 0.19*** (0.03) 0.05 (0.04) 0.39*** (0.03) 0.05 (0.03)
Parents’ education 0.13*** (0.02) 0.04* (0.02) 0.32*** (0.02) 0.14*** (0.05)
Number of siblings –0.09*** (0.02) –0.07*** (0.02) –0.12*** (0.02) –0.05*** (0.02)
Mean R
2
.09 .30
Note: All predictors and dependent variables were standardized; therefore, although the coefficients reported are unstandardized,
they can be interpreted much like standardized coefficients. Parameter estimates and standard errors (in parentheses) are based
on 20 multiply imputed data sets. The British Cohort Study data on which these analyses were based are publicly available from the
Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, University of London Web site: http://www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/bcs70.
*p < .05. ***p < .001.
the U.K. data, F(1, 3664) = 5.53, p < .05. (See Tables 3 and 4
for a summary of predictors of literacy in the two data sets.)
Discussion
These findings demonstrate that elementary school students’
knowledge of fractions and whole-number division predicts
their mathematics achievement in high school, above and
beyond the contributions of their knowledge of whole-number
addition, subtraction, and multiplication; verbal and nonverbal
IQ; working memory; family education; and family income.
Knowledge of fractions and whole-number division also had a
stronger relation to math achievement than did knowledge of
whole-number addition, subtraction, and multiplication; verbal
IQ; working memory; and parental income. These results were
consistent across data sets from the United Kingdom and the
United States. The fact that the relations of the predictor vari-
ables to algebra knowledge and overall mathematics achieve-
ment were similar in strength in the two samples, despite
differences in the samples, the tests, and the times at which the
data were obtained, is reason for confidence in the generality of
the findings.
The correlation between knowledge of fractions in elemen-
tary school and achievement in algebra and mathematics over-
all in high school was expected, but the relation between early
division knowledge and later mathematical knowledge was
not. Fractions and division are inherently related (N/M means
N divided by M), but the finding that early knowledge of
fractions and early knowledge of division accounted for inde-
pendent variance in later algebra knowledge and overall math-
ematics achievement indicated that neither relation explained
the other.
There are several likely reasons why knowledge of division
uniquely predicted later mathematics achievement. Mastery of
whole-number division, like mastery of fractions, is required
to solve many algebra problems (e.g., to apply the quadratic
equation). Also, as is the case with fractions, high percentages
of students fail to master division; thus, when high school stu-
dents in the PSID-CDS were presented with a seemingly easy
problem in which a boy wants to fly on a plane that travels
400 miles per hour in order to visit his grandmother who lives
1,400 air miles away, only 56% of the students correctly
indicated how long the flight would take. More speculatively,
poor knowledge of both division and fractions might lead stu-
dents to give up trying to make sense of mathematics, and
instead to rely on rote memorization in subsequent mathemat-
ics learning.
An alternative interpretation is that the unique predictive
value of knowledge of fractions and knowledge of division
stems from those operations being more difficult than addi-
tion, subtraction, and multiplication, and thus measuring more
advanced thinking. Some of our results are inconsistent with
this interpretation, however. First, knowledge of fractions and
knowledge of division were not uniquely predictive of most
subsequent literacy skills (see Tables 3 and 4), as should have
been the case if their predictive value was due solely to their
696 Siegler et al.
greater difficulty. Second, spline tests (see Tables S5 and S6 in
the Supplemental Material) showed that the predictive strength
of early knowledge of fractions and division did not differ
between students with greater and lesser mathematics achieve-
ment in high school. Thus, the unique predictive value of early
fractions and division knowledge seems to be due to many
students not mastering fractions and division and to those
operations being essential for more advanced mathematics,
rather than simply to fractions and division being relatively
difficult to master.
Over 30 years of nationwide standardized testing, mathe-
matics scores of U.S. high school students have barely budged
(National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008). The present
findings imply that mastery of fractions and division is needed
if substantial improvements in understanding of algebra and
other aspects of high school mathematics are to be achieved.
One likely reason for students’ limited mastery of fractions
and division is that many U.S. teachers lack a firm conceptual
understanding of fractions and division. In several studies, the
majority of elementary and middle school teachers in the
United States were unable to generate even a single explana-
tion for why the invert-and-multiply algorithm (i.e., a/b ÷
c/d = ad × bc) is a legitimate way to solve division problems
with fractions. In contrast, most teachers in Japan and China
generated two or three explanations in response to the same
question (Ma, 1999; Moseley, Okamoto, & Ishida, 2007).
These and the present results suggest that improved teaching
of fractions and division could yield substantial improvements
in students’ learning, not only of fractions and division but of
more advanced mathematics as well.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
National Science Foundation Grant 0818478 and Department of Edu-
cation, Instructional and Educational Sciences Grants R324C100004
and R305A080013 supported this research.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss.sagepub
.com/content/by/supplemental-data
References
Butler, N., & Bynner, J. M. (1980). British Cohort Study: Ten-Year
Follow-up, 1980. UK Data Archive [distributor]. Retrieved from
http://www.esds.ac.uk/findingData/bcs70.asp
Butler, N., & Bynner, J. M. (1986). British Cohort Study: Sixteen-
Year Follow-up, 1986. UK Data Archive [distributor]. Retrieved
from http://www.esds.ac.uk/findingData/bcs70.asp
Bynner, J., Ferri, E., & Shepherd, P. (Eds.). (1997). Twenty-something
in the 1990s: Getting on, getting by, getting nowhere. Aldershot,
England: Ashgate.
Table 4 . Results From Regression Models Predicting Literacy at Age 15 to 17 From Math Skills and Child and Family
Characteristics at Age 10 to 12: Panel Study of Income Dynamics Data (N = 599)
Letter-word identification Passage comprehension
Predictor
Model 1 (bivariate
regression)
Model 2 (multiple
regression)
Model 3 (bivariate
regression)
Model 4 (multiple
regression)
Early math skills
Fractions 0.40*** (0.05) 0.03 (0.05) 0.41*** (0.05) 0.04 (0.06)
Addition 0.22*** (0.05) –0.05 (0.04) 0.19*** (0.06) –0.06 (0.05)
Subtraction 0.38*** (0.06) 0.07 (0.06) 0.35*** (0.05) 0.05 (0.05)
Multiplication 0.47*** (0.06) 0.14* (0.06) 0.43*** (0.05) 0.11* (0.05)
Division 0.49*** (0.06) 0.14* (0.07) 0.44*** (0.05) 0.08 (0.05)
Early abilities
Backward digit span 0.30*** (0.05) 0.03 (0.04) 0.36*** (0.05) 0.10* (0.04)
Passage comprehension 0.65*** (0.06) 0.46*** (0.06) 0.65*** (0.04) 0.43*** (0.05)
Demographic characteristics
Female gender 0.15** (0.05) 0.05 (0.04) 0.13* (0.05) 0.03 (0.04)
Age 0.17*** (0.05) –0.08 (0.05) 0.13** (0.05) –0.08 (0.04)
Log mean family income
(1994–1996)
0.33*** (0.05) 0.08 (0.05) 0.39*** (0.06) 0.05 (0.05)
Parents’ education 0.32*** (0.07) 0.05 (0.06) 0.47*** (0.05) 0.23*** (0.05)
Number of siblings –0.06 (0.06) 0.06 (0.04) –0.15*** (0.05) 0.03 (0.04)
Mean R
2
.51 .53
Note: All predictors and dependent variables were standardized; therefore, although the coefficients reported are unstandardized,
they can be interpreted much like standardized coefficients. Parameter estimates and standard errors (in parentheses) are based on
20 multiply imputed data sets. Models were weighted by 2002 child-level weights and adjusted for the clustering of children within
the same family. The data on which these analyses were based came from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics public-use data set,
available at http://psidonline.isr.umich.edu.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Early Predictors of High School Math Achievement 697
Case, R., Griffin, S., & Kelly, W. M. (1999). Socioeconomic gra-
dients in mathematical ability and their responsiveness to inter-
vention during early childhood. In D. P. Keating & C. Hertzman
(Eds.), Developmental health and the wealth of nations: Social,
biological, and educational dynamics (pp. 125–152). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). Exploring the microstructure of
children’s central conceptual structures in the domain of number.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
61(1–2, Serial No. 246), 27–58.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston,
A. C., Klebanov, P., . . . Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and
later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446.
Gelman, R., & Williams, E. (1998). Enabling constraints for cogni-
tive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigen-
esis. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of
child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception & language (5th
ed., pp. 575–630). New York, NY: Wiley.
Hanushek, E. A., & Rivkin, S. G. (2006). School quality and the
black-white achievement gap (NBER Working Paper No. 12651).
Washington, DC: National Bureau of Economic Research.
Hecht, S. A., & Vagi, K. J. (2010). Sources of group and individual
differences in emerging fraction skills. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 102, 843–858.
Hofferth, S., Davis-Kean, P. E., Davis, J., & Finkelstein, J. (1998).
The Child Development Supplement to the Panel Study of Income
Dynamics: 1997 user guide. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan,
Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research. Retrieved
from https://psidonline.isr.umich.edu/CDS/cdsi_userGD.pdf
Ma, L. (1999). Knowing and teaching elementary mathematics:
Teachers’ understanding of fundamental mathematics in China
and the United States. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Moseley, B. J., Okamoto, Y., & Ishida, J. (2007). Comparing U.S. and
Japanese elementary school teachers’ facility for linking rational
number representations. International Journal of Science and
Mathematics Education, 5, 165–185.
Murnane, R. J., Willett, J. B., & Levy, F. (1995). The growing impor-
tance of cognitive skills in wage determination. Review of Eco-
nomics and Statistics, 78, 251–266.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2006). Curriculum
focal points for prekindergarten through grade 8 mathematics: A
quest for coherence. Washington, DC: Author.
National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations for suc-
cess: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory
Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Schneider, M., & Siegler, R. S. (2010). Representations of the magni-
tudes of fractions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 36, 1227–1238.
Siegler, R. S., Thompson, C. A., & Schneider, M. (2011). An inte-
grated theory of whole number and fractions development. Cog-
nitive Development, 62, 273–296.
Starkey, P., Klein, A., & Wakeley, A. (2004). Enhancing young
children’s mathematical knowledge through a pre-kindergarten
mathematics intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
19, 99–120.
Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction
of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child
Development, 57, 646–659.
Vosniadou, S., Vamvakoussi, X., & Skopeliti, I. (2008). The frame-
work theory approach to conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou
(Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change
(pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wynn, K. (1995). Infants possess a system of numerical knowledge.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 172–177.
... Frações é conteúdo nuclear na Matemática e sua aprendizagem está relacionada ao desempenho futuro dos alunos na Matemática mais avançada (Siegler et al., 2012;Booth & Newton, 2012), notadamente na Álgebra. Assim, o conhecimento amplo e profundo de números fracionários torna-se essencial, principalmente durante as séries iniciais. ...
... Mais ainda, ao compararem frações com numeradores iguais, assinalam como maior fração aquela com maior denominador (e.g., 2/5<2/8). Igualmente, não compreendem frações impróprias, pois, para eles, a quantidade de partes tomadas deve ser sempre menor do que a quantidade total de partes (Siegler et al., 2012). Essas percepções ocorrem, provavelmente porque os alunos aplicam sem discernimento propriedades dos números inteiros aos números fracionários. ...
... Ainda que a perspectiva de partição seja dominante nas escolas brasileiras (Scheffer & Powell, 2019) e que tenha algum valor pedagógico, ela não é a única e nem o modo inicial mais indicado de se desenvolver o conceito de frações (Siegler et al., 2012). A ideia é a de desenvolver esse conceito pela perspectiva de medição. ...
Book
Full-text available
O presente e-book registra a memória dos trabalhos apresentados por professores pesquisadores durante a edição inaugural do Seminário Internacional de Lesson Study no Ensino de Matemática (SILSEM), realizado pela Universidade de Brasília (UnB) e pela Universidade Estadual de Campinas (Unicamp), com apoio de instituições e associações, como a Universidade Federal de São Carlos (UFSCar), o Instituto Federal do Espírito Santo (IFES), a Universidade Federal de Campina Grande, a Universidade Federal da Fronteira do Sul (UFFS), a Universidade Federal do Tocantins, a Sociedade Brasileira de Educação Matemática (SBEM), tendo como organizadores o Grupo de Investigação em Ensino de Matemática (GIEM – UnB), o Grupo de Pesquisa Prática Pedagógica em Matemática (PRAPEM – Unicamp) e o Grupo de Sábado (GdS – Unicamp). Durante o evento, que ocorreu em maio de 2021, foram realizadas seis (6) Mesas-Redondas e 63 Comunicações Científicas, organizadas e apresentadas em 14 Salas Virtuais, além de três (3) atividades culturais. Das 937 pessoas que se inscreveram no evento, 648 participaram regularmente das atividades, sendo este público composto por estudantes de graduação e de pós-graduação, professores da educação básica e do ensino superior, representando 8 países. Tudo isso revelou-nos o interesse e a busca por espaços de estudo, de socialização de pesquisas e de aprendizagem em contextos de Lesson Study, aproximando pesquisadores da América do Sul a de outros continentes. De modo especial, o SILSEM possibilitou a presença de precursores do LS no Japão, na América Latina e, particularmente, no Brasil, registrando aspectos históricos valiosos desse processo de formação e aprendizagem colaborativa de professores que tem o trabalho docente como foco principal de estudo. O SILSEM, realizado em formato on-line devido á pandemia da Covid-19, possibilitou a participação de muitos professores, estudantes e pesquisadores que usufruíram desse formato, sendo esta uma oportunidade ímpar de diminuir distâncias geográficas com os grandes centros internacionais de pesquisa sobre/em Lesson Study e de conhecer seus principais expoentes como, por exemplo: Masami Isoda (Japão), que destacou a significância do Lesson Study para o desenvolvimento da Educação Matemática e seu impacto na educação escolar mundial; João Pedro da Ponte (Portugal), que apresentou experiências portuguesas de Estudos de Aula com professores em serviço; Raimundo Olfos e Soledad Estrella (Chile), que apresentaram as primeiras experiências com Lesson Study na América Latina; Carl Winslow, Klaus Rasmussen e Jacob Bahn (Dinamarca), que descreveram e discutiram três contextos diferentes de trabalho com Lesson Study na Dinamarca, na escola primária, na escola secundária e no ensino superior, envolvendo a formação de professores. Pesquisadores brasileiros, chilenos, colombianos, americanos e portugueses também tiveram a oportunidade de apresentar e discutir duas experiências investigativas com Lesson Study em mesas redondas e nas comunicações científicas, envolvendo contextos escolares de ensino relativos: aos anos iniciais e finais do Ensino Fundamental; ao Ensino Médio; e, também, ao contexto do Ensino Superior, incluindo, sobretudo, a formação inicial e continuada de professores que ensinam matemática. Por outro lado, a não presencialidade limitou diálogos, vínculos acadêmicos e, especialmente, o contato humano tão essencial ao nosso desenvolvimento. Assim, vislumbramos que o SILSEM desenvolva-se enquanto espaço de colaboração, pesquisa e ajuda mútua, integrando momentos on-line e presenciais, de forma a estabelecer um espaço/ tempo na América do Sul para o avanço do Lesson Study e das compreensões que ele possibilita quanto à melhoria da qualidade do ensino e da aprendizagem, desde os anos iniciais do ensino fundamental até o ensino superior e aos estudos pós-graduados. Os resumos expandidos publicados neste e-book reafirmam o compromisso das Comissões Organizadora e Científica de registrar e socializar as experiências investigativas e os conhecimentos produzidos no âmbito das diferentes modalidades de Lesson Study que vêm sendo desenvolvidas pelo mundo, sobretudo na América do Sul. Logo, além de preservar a memória dessas experiências e dar civilidade a esses estudos pioneiros de LS na América do Sul, certamente motivará os investigadores a criar grupos ou comunidades locais de investigação e a estabelecer redes de intercâmbio e pesquisa entre os mesmos. Desejamos a todos/as uma excelente leitura!
... Several studies find that general cognitive abilities (e.g., working memory, attention, and fluid intelligence) and students' prior mathematics knowledge predict achievement in mathematics (Geary et al., 2018;Nunes et al., 2012;Rittle-Johnson et al., 2017;Siegler et al., 2012). However, research considering the combined predictive role of such variables is limited, and these variables' relative contribution to mathematics performance across grades is still debated (Fuchs et al., 2010;Lee & Bull, 2016;Nunes et al., 2012;Träff et al., 2020). ...
... Natural number division and fractions are key contents for mathematics learning in the early grades of secondary school (Argentina's Ministry of Education, 2012;Siegler & Lortie-Forgues, 2017;Siegler et al., 2011). A study with US and UK students aged 10 to 12 years old indicated that both contents were the prior knowledge that contributed the most to overall mathematics achievement 5 to 6 years later when the effect of other mathematical (addition, subtraction, and multiplication with natural numbers), cognitive (WM, verbal, and non-verbal intelligence), and sociodemographic variables (age, income level, and parental education) were controlled (Siegler et al., 2012). Similarly, reported that sixth-grade fraction knowledge represented the math-specific variable that contributed the most to grade-by-grade increase in general mathematics knowledge from 6 to 8th grades (controlling for natural numbers knowledge, arithmetic skills, WM, intelligence, and reading ability). ...
... Our results regarding the importance of division ability for later mathematics achievement are consistent with those reported in UK and US high school students (Siegler & Pyke, 2013;Siegler et al., 2012). This study contributes to the literature by showing that division ability is relevant for general mathematics learning in other educational systems and early school years. ...
Article
The aim of this study was to analyze the joint, relative, and unique predictive value of students’ prior knowledge of mathematics (knowledge of fractions and ability to divide natural numbers) and general cognitive ability (fluid intelligence and working memory) upon general mathematics achievement in the last year of primary school. Seventy-five students participated (M age = 11.2 years old, SD = 0.40). Hierarchical regression analysis showed that the ability to divide and fractions knowledge accounted for 41% of the variance in mathematics achievement, both acting as significant predictors. By incorporating working memory and fluid intelligence into the model, fraction knowledge showed to be no longer a significant predictor. These general cognitive skills explained an additional 8% of the variance in mathematics knowledge, both being significant predictors and contributing to mathematics achievement in a unique way. The implications of these results for mathematics teaching are discussed.
... In the research literature, there is a general agreement that a good understanding of rational numbers is foundational for more advanced mathematics. For example, Siegler et al. (2012) showed thatcontrolling for natural number knowledge, reading achievement, IQ, working memory, family income and family educationfifth graders' fraction knowledge predicts algebra and overall mathematics scores in high school. Despite their importance, rational numbers are found to be a huge stumbling block in education for a large group of learners (Depaepe et al., 2015;Gabriel et al., 2013;Gómez et al., 2014;McMullen & Van Hoof, 2020;Siegler et al., 2012;Vamvakoussi et al., 2013;Vamvakoussi & Vosniadou, 2010;Van Hoof et al., 2017). ...
... For example, Siegler et al. (2012) showed thatcontrolling for natural number knowledge, reading achievement, IQ, working memory, family income and family educationfifth graders' fraction knowledge predicts algebra and overall mathematics scores in high school. Despite their importance, rational numbers are found to be a huge stumbling block in education for a large group of learners (Depaepe et al., 2015;Gabriel et al., 2013;Gómez et al., 2014;McMullen & Van Hoof, 2020;Siegler et al., 2012;Vamvakoussi et al., 2013;Vamvakoussi & Vosniadou, 2010;Van Hoof et al., 2017). ...
Chapter
Fractions are fundamental in students’ mathematical development. However, for many students, they are known to be a major stumbling block. In this chapter, we examine the obstacles elementary school children face when they learn fractions, through the lens of numerical cognition. We start by discussing the discrepancy between children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge of fractions, and we review studies showing that the concept of fraction magnitude is particularly difficult to learn. This has wider implications as understanding fraction magnitude has been shown to be a strong predictor of achievement in algebra and overall mathematics achievement in later years. We then discuss the natural number bias (NNB), a well-characterised misconception linked to fraction learning whereby learners are inclined to apply natural number characteristics when reasoning about fractions without considering whether it is appropriate. The NNB is persistent, appearing early in the fraction learning process and lasting through secondary school and beyond. We conclude this chapter by describing interventions aimed at improving fraction learning and we provide suggestions on how to introduce the concept of magnitude more intentionally when teaching fractions.KeywordsFractionsNatural number biasConceptual knowledgeProcedural knowledge
... To provide a model of teacher implementation of SRSD, the teachers watched a commercially produced video that explained and showed examples of the steps in the SRSD instructional framework (Harris & Graham, 2012). Second, the trainers presented the importance of teaching fractions using multiple representations (Hallet et al., 2010;Namkung & Fuchs, 2016;Siegler et al., 2012). The approach adopted in the intervention was the Concrete-Representational-Abstract (CRA) framework, where students represented their understanding of the mathematics concept or procedure by using concrete manipulatives to represent the problem (e.g., fraction block and paper folding), progressing to two-dimensional representations (e.g., area models and number lines), and then representing the concept or procedure using abstract representations (e.g., using mathematical notation to find the least common multiple to solve 1 2 + 2 3 ) (Witzel, 2005;Witzel et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Incorporating argument writing as a learning activity has been found to increase students’ mathematics performance. However, teachers report receiving little to no preservice or inservice preparation to use writing to support students’ learning. This is especially concerning for special education teachers who provide highly specialized mathematics instruction (i.e., Tier 3) to students with mathematics disabilities (MLD). The purpose of this study was to examine the effectiveness of teachers providing content-focused open-ended questioning strategies, which included both argument writing and foundational fraction content, using Practice-Based Professional Development (PBPD) and Self-Regulated Strategy Development (SRSD) for implementing a writing-to-learn strategy called FACT-R²C². We report the relative number of higher-order mathematical content questions that teachers asked during instruction, from among three different-level question types: Level 1: yes/no questions focused on the mathematics content; Level 2: one-word responses focused on the mathematics content; and Level 3: higher-order open-ended responses centered around four mathematical practices from the Common Core State Standards for Mathematics. Within a well-controlled single-case multiple-baseline design, seven special education teachers were randomly assigned to each PBPD + FACT-R²C² intervention tier. Results indicated that: (1) teachers’ relative use of Level 3 questions increased following the introduction of the FACT intervention; (2) this increase was apart from the professional development training that the teachers had initially received; and (3) students’ writing quality improved to some extent with the increase in teachers’ relative use of Level 3 questions. Implications and future directions are discussed.
... As the performance pressure in secondary schools increases, math teachers often do not have the time to address prior deficits and unlock students' full potential regarding basic arithmetic operations. Hence, especially students who have problems understanding basic arithmetic operations are at risk of falling behind because basic arithmetic skills form the basis of understanding more complex mathematics (Andersson 2010;Bailey et al. 2014;Hansen et al. 2017;Jordan et al. 2013) and are a strong predictor of later achievement (Bailey et al. 2012; Barbieri et al. 2021;Duncan et al. 2007;Siegler et al. 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Math learning programs were expected to revolutionize students’ learning, but their effects so far have mostly been disappointing. Following the debate about why to continue research on math learning programs, we aimed to reformulate this question into how to continue this research. Investigations to date have neither considered a sufficiently wide set of outcome variables nor differentiated between performance measures (e.g., measuring addition and subtraction performance separately) and affective-motivational variables. Moreover, as students can only benefit from a program if they use it, researchers need to take practice behavior into account. Thus, we investigated whether the adaptive arithmetic learning program Math Garden fostered students’ addition and subtraction performance, their math self-concept, and a reduction of their math anxiety. We also investigated how practice behavior (practiced tasks/weeks) affected these outcomes. We used a randomized pretest-posttest control group design with 376 fifth-grade students in Germany. Students in the experimental condition practiced with Math Garden for 20.7 weeks and had an increase in math self-concept. The more subtraction tasks the students practiced, the more they improved their subtraction performance. We found no effects on math anxiety. The results are discussed in terms of providing a starting point for new directions in future research.
... A student who experiences mere memorization and mimicking of rules, facts, and operations in elementary school mathematics is unlikely to understand the power of mathematics or to be interested in mathematics in higher grades (Reys & Fennell, 2003). On the contrary, students' conceptual understanding of mathematics topics in elementary school is an indicator of their success in high school mathematics (Bailey et al., 2014;Siegler et al., 2012). Thus, providing high cognitive demand tasks that will contribute to the students' understanding of elementary school mathematics is essential (Huinker & Bill, 2017;Van de Walle et al., 2019). ...
Article
As a result of the COVID-19 pandemic, distance education started at K-12 levels in the spring semester of the 2019-2020 school year. The Ministry of National Education had also published instructional tasks to be used in distance education at all grade levels in order to create mathematics learning opportunities for students and to provide resources for teachers. Well-structured and high-quality instructional tasks play an important role in students' learning mathematics. The aim of this study is to examine the quality of the elementary school mathematics tasks recommended for distance remedial education from multiple perspectives, in particular their cognitive demand levels. A total of 85 tasks focusing on 79 critical objectives in grades 1-4 mathematics were examined using document analysis. Results of this study showed that the majority of the tasks were at low cognitive demand level, cognitive demand levels did not show a balanced distribution, and some tasks had mathematical errors.
... Proficiency with fractions is foundational for success in algebra and other advanced mathematics domains, and therefore a gateway to careers in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM; Sadler & Tai, 2007;Siegler et al., 2012). In 2008, the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (NMAP) identified improving fractions performance as a national priority. ...
Article
This study explored whether initial skill moderated outcomes of Promoting Algebra Readiness, a Tier 2 sixth-grade mathematics intervention targeting conceptual and procedural knowledge of fractions. The study analyzed data from a quasi-experimental pilot study in which at-risk students ( n = 198) were assigned to the treatment or control condition at the school level. Proximal and distal measures of math proficiency were collected in the fall (pretest) and spring (post-test). Analyses examined initial student achievement as a moderator of mathematics outcomes. Results indicated that intervention outcomes were not moderated by initial skill. Implications for tiered mathematics instruction and future mathematics intervention research are discussed.
Chapter
Despite considerable investment in research in mathematical cognition and learning over the past decade, students with mathematics learning difficulties are losing ground. Fractions are a particular barrier for many of these learners. Development of evidence-based fraction interventions for students who are still struggling in middle school is essential to help prevent cascading difficulties, particularly when algebra becomes a primary focus. Addressing this need, our research team developed a fraction sense intervention (FSI) for low-performing middle schoolers. To make learning last, the FSI explicitly incorporates general techniques backed by evidence from cognitive science. In this chapter, we address fraction intervention for low achievers in three areas: (a) domain specific concepts, procedures, and representations; (b) general techniques that support learning across domains; and (c) lesson-specific details about how information is presented in the FSI. We describe the iterative development of the FSI and discuss its effectiveness in two contexts: small and larger group settings.KeywordsFractionsMathematics learning difficultiesIntervention
Article
Full-text available
This article continues the work that we have been doing for many years in France and Quebec on the use of a preventive support scheme intended to help students with special needs solve math problems. The scheme is comprised of three didactic mechanisms: a principal didactic mechanism (the classroom session) and two auxiliary mechanisms in which small groups of students are gathered before and after the class. Prior modelling has helped us identify the structure and functions of the scheme and has underscored the importance of continuity between the three mechanisms. The aim of this article is to describe the entire continuum and examine it from a mesogenetic perspective. To do this, we use a case study involving a lesson about fractions in Quebec. The analysis allows us to discover new insights about the preventive support scheme. ---------------------------------------------------------------------------Résumé Cet article s'inscrit dans la continuité des travaux que nous menons depuis plusieurs années en France et au Québec, concernant un dispositif d'aide « préventif» adressé aux élèves en difficulté dans la résolution de problèmes mathématiques. Ce dispositif est constitué de trois systèmes didactiques: un système didactique principal (la classe) et deux systèmes didactiques auxiliaires regroupant un petit nombre d'élèves, situés en amont et en aval de la séance de classe. La modélisation antérieurement réalisée a permis d'identifier la structure et les fonctions de ce dispositif et a souligné l'importance d'une certaine continuité entre les trois systèmes didactiques. Le but de cet article est de révéler l'ensemble de ce continuum et de l'étudier d'un point de vue mésogénétique. Nous réalisons pour ce faire une étude de cas dans le cadre d'un enseignement portant sur le concept de fraction au Québec. Les analyses nous permettent de dégager de nouveaux éléments de compréhension du dispositif d'aide préventif.
Article
Mathematics skills are associated with future employment, wellbeing and quality of life. However, many adults and children fail to learn the mathematics skills they require. To improve this situation, we need to have a better understanding of the processes of learning and performing mathematics. Over the past two decades there has been a substantial growth in psychological research focusing on mathematics. However, to make further progress we need to pay greater attention to the nature of, and multiple elements involved in, mathematical cognition. Mathematics is not a single construct; rather, overall mathematics achievement is comprised of proficiency with specific components of mathematics (e.g., number fact knowledge, algebraic thinking), which in turn recruit basic mathematical processes (e.g., magnitude comparison, pattern recognition). General cognitive skills and different learning experiences influence the development of each component of mathematics as well as the links between them. Here I propose and provide evidence for a framework that structures how these components of mathematics fit together. This framework allows us to make sense of the proliferation of empirical findings concerning influences on mathematical cognition and can guide the questions we ask, identifying where we are missing both research evidence and models of specific mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
All theories of cognitive development make assumptions about constraints on learning, even those which commonly are seen as nonconstraint theories. We do not ask whether a constraint theory or a nonconstraint theory is better. Instead, we ask what kind of theory best accommodates key facts about cognitive development and concept learning. Our answer follows in three sections. We begin by reviewing what we consider these relevant facts to be, in the form of seven postulates about cognitive development and learning. In the second section, we turn to a review of theories and the notions of constraint embedded within them. This allows us to bring out key sources of misunderstanding and confusion about what constraints are and how they function. In the third section, we move to a combined consideration of evidence and theory, and develop our reasons for favoring a rational-constructivist theory of cognitive development. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Using cognitive ethnography as a guiding framework, we investigated US and Japanese fourth-grade teachers' domain knowledge of key fraction representations in individual interviews. The framework focused on revealing cultural trends in participants' organization of knowledge and their interpretations of that organization. Our analyses of the interviews, which included a representation sorting task, indicated three major differences that defined US and Japanese teachers' approaches to rational number representation: (1) Japanese teachers interpreted all rational number representations as conveying primarily mathematical information, whereas US teachers interpreted only some representations as conveying primarily mathematical information; (2) the US teachers focused more intently on part-whole relations than Japanese in their interpretations; and (3) Japanese teachers more easily linked rational number representations to more advanced upcoming content in the curriculum. A review of US textbooks used by the teachers reflected their consistency with US teachers' interpretations of the representations. These findings imply that strong cultural differences underlay the approaches that teachers in both nations take to rational number representation and that these differences may help explain established cross-national differences in student reasoning.
Article
Full-text available
There is growing evidence that socioeconomic (SES)-related differences in mathematical knowledge begin in early childhood, because young children from economically disadvantaged families receive less support for mathematical development than their middle-class peers receive. A pre-kindergarten mathematics intervention, including a pre-kindergarten mathematics curriculum, was developed and implemented in public and private preschools serving low- and middle-income families. Mathematical knowledge of intervention and comparison children was comprehensively assessed. A significant SES-related gap in mathematical knowledge was found at the beginning of the pre-kindergarten year. The intervention significantly enhanced the mathematical knowledge of children at both levels of SES. Low-income children acquired more knowledge, relative to their starting point, than middle-income children. The extent of mathematical knowledge was similar in low-income intervention children and middle-income comparison children. Implications of this research for early childhood education curricula and educational policy are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Results from a two year longitudinal study of 181 children from fourth through fifth grade are reported. Levels of growth in children's computation, word problem, and estimation skills using common fractions were predicted by working memory, attentive classroom behavior, conceptual knowledge about fractions, and simple arithmetic fluency. Comparisons of 55 participants identified as having mathematical difficulties to those without mathematical difficulties revealed that group differences in emerging fraction skills were consistently mediated by attentive classroom behavior and conceptual knowledge about fractions. Neither working memory nor arithmetic fluency mediated group differences in growth in fraction skills. It was also found that the development of basic fraction skills and conceptual knowledge are bidirectional in that conceptual knowledge exerted strong influences on all three types of basic fraction skills, and basic fraction skills exerted a more modest influence on subsequent conceptual knowledge. Results are discussed with reference to how the identification of potentially malleable student characteristics that contribute to the difficulties that some students have with fractions informs interventions and also will contribute to a future theoretical account concerning how domain general and domain specific factors influence the development of basic fraction skills.
Article
This article proposes an integrated theory of acquisition of knowledge about whole numbers and fractions. Although whole numbers and fractions differ in many ways that influence their development, an important commonality is the centrality of knowledge of numerical magnitudes in overall understanding. The present findings with 11- and 13-year-olds indicate that, as with whole numbers, accuracy of fraction magnitude representations is closely related to both fractions arithmetic proficiency and overall mathematics achievement test scores, that fraction magnitude representations account for substantial variance in mathematics achievement test scores beyond that explained by fraction arithmetic proficiency, and that developing effective strategies plays a key role in improved knowledge of fractions. Theoretical and instructional implications are discussed.