An Ecological Alternative to Snodgrass & Vanderwart: 360 High Quality Colour Images with Norms for Seven Psycholinguistic Variables

Article (PDF Available)inPLoS ONE 7(5):e37527 · May 2012with51 Reads
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037527 · Source: PubMed
Abstract
This work presents a new set of 360 high quality colour images belonging to 23 semantic subcategories. Two hundred and thirty-six Spanish speakers named the items and also provided data from seven relevant psycholinguistic variables: age of acquisition, familiarity, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual complexity. Furthermore, we also present lexical frequency data derived from Internet search hits. Apart from the high number of variables evaluated, knowing that it affects the processing of stimuli, this new set presents important advantages over other similar image corpi: (a) this corpus presents a broad number of subcategories and images; for example, this will permit researchers to select stimuli of appropriate difficulty as required, (e.g., to deal with problems derived from ceiling effects); (b) the fact of using coloured stimuli provides a more realistic, ecologically-valid, representation of real life objects. In sum, this set of stimuli provides a useful tool for research on visual object- and word-processing, both in neurological patients and in healthy controls.
An Ecological Alternative to Snodgrass & Vanderwart:
360 High Quality Colour Images with Norms for Seven
Psycholinguistic Variables
Francisco Javier Moreno-Martı
´
nez, Pedro R. Montoro*
Departamento de Psicologı
´
aBa
´
sica I, Universidad Nacional de Educacio
´
n a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
Abstract
This work presents a new set of 360 high quality colour images belonging to 23 semantic subcategories. Two hundred and
thirty-six Spanish speakers named the items and also provided data from seven relevant psycholinguistic variables: age of
acquisition, familiarity, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual complexity. Furthermore, we also present
lexical frequency data derived from Internet search hits. Apart from the high number of variables evaluated, knowing that it
affects the processing of stimuli, this new set presents important advantages over other similar image corpi: (a) this corpus
presents a broad number of subcategories and images; for example, this will permit researchers to select stimuli of
appropriate difficulty as required, (e.g., to deal with problems derived from ceiling effects); (b) the fact of using coloured
stimuli provides a more realistic, ecologically-valid, representation of real life objects. In sum, this set of stimuli provides a
useful tool for research on visual object-and word- processing, both in neurological patients and in healthy controls.
Citation: Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Montoro PR (2012) An Ecological Alternative to Snodgrass & Vanderwart: 360 High Quality Colour Images with Norms for Seven
Psycholinguistic Variables. PLoS ONE 7(5): e37527. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527
Editor: Luis M. Martinez, CSIC-Univ Miguel Hernandez, Spain
Received Jan uary 18, 2012; Accepted April 24, 2012; Published May 25, 2012
Copyright: ß 2012 Moreno-Martı
´
nez, Montoro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unres tricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credit ed.
Funding: No current external funding sources for this study.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* E-mail: prmontoro@psi.uned.es
Introduction
Throughout the last 30 years, many clinical and experimental
studies on cognitive processing (i.e., exploring memory, attention
or language) have been performed with the items created by
Snodgrass and Vanderwart (S&V) [1]. These authors standardized
their stimuli in four variables relevant to cognitive processing:
familiarity, image agreement, name agreement and visual
complexity. Experimental control of these variables is essential
because they are known to affect cognitive processing both of
pictorial and verbal material. Thus, more familiar items, those
with higher name and image agreement, as well as those with
lesser visual complexity, are more easily named both by intact and
neurological participants [2–5].
Apart from these variables, other cognitive and psycholinguistic
variables such as age of acquisisiton (AoA) and manipulability and
typicality of items significantly affect cognitive processing. Thus,
AoA is a powerful predictor of object-naming performance both in
normal and brain-injured individuals, with earlier acquired words
being more easily processed than later acquired ones [6,7].
Similarly, there is a significant relationship between the degree of
manipulability of an object; that is, the degree of use of the human
hand that is necessary for an object to perform its function and its
semantic representation (e.g., [8–12]). Indeed, it has been
proposed that differences in manipulability could explain category
effects on object identification, consisting of a better performance
with items from the domain of nonliving things (e.g., tools)
compared to living things (e.g., animals; see [13], for a review).
Lastly, typicality of items (i.e., how typical, or representative, a
member is of a category) is another important psycholinguistic
variable. Classic studies by Eleanor Rosch showed the relevance of
this variable and its strong influence on performance in tasks
assessing cognitive processing and memory, language use and
communication, or development-related phenomena such as
category learning and conceptual development (see, for example,
[14–16]). Similarly, typicality of items has also been found to
significantly impact the performance of neurological patients (e.g.,
aphasics: [17]). Despite the relevance of typicality in normal and
damaged cognitive processing, most of the recent normative works
and new semantic tests have not paid close attention to this
variable (for example, [1,18–28]; but see also [29–31]). Likewise,
only a few recent works have provided ratings of AoA [19] or
manipulability [10], and, to our knowledge, only [25,30] have
presented ratings of both variables concurrently, but with a
relatively sparse number of items, as they only studied 140 [30]
and 112 [25] coloured stimuli.
Some recent concerns respect to S&V corpi are related to the
ecological validity of the stimuli and ceiling effects in the responses.
Items from S&V consist of black and white line drawings. From an
ecological view, the validity of studies using this type of stimuli has
been questioned [28]. Colour is an essential attribute of objects
and, except for unusual pathologies, it is difficult to separate colour
from real world objects [32,33]. Consequently, the number of
works using coloured items, providing a more realistic represen-
tation of objects, as well as studies normalising coloured stimuli,
have been progressively increasing (see, for example, [19–
21,23,27,28,34–42]). Regarding ceiling effects, it has been
observed that most of the items from S&V are easily named by
healthy participants, at least under normal viewing conditions.
This facilitates non-damaged participants showing ceiling effects in
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
studies that involve the processing of objects, especially when using
not very demanding tasks, (e.g., picture naming; see [37,43]). As
shown by Laws and collaborators in studies on category-specificity,
this problem may distort both the degree and type of deficit
reported in patients [37,43].
The goal of the present work was twofold: (a) to present a broad
set of high quality ecological colour photographs, on white
backgrounds, across a difficulty range to deal with problems
derived from ceiling effects; and (b) to give detailed norms, derived
from a large group of healthy participants, of several relevant
psycholinguistic variables, some of them not sufficiently studied in
several previous works: AoA, familiarity, manipulability, name
agreement, typicality and visual complexity, as well as lexical
frequency. Furthermore, indexes of individual item analysis,
including a measure of item difficulty and two indexes of item
discrimination have been included.
Methods
Item selection
Following previous normative and semantic assessment studies,
we selected 23 semantic subcategories (and their items) based on
relevant theoretical and methodological reasons [1,19–21,23,25–
29,30,44,45]. Consequently, we included problematic/atypical
subcategories, such as body parts, musical instruments or foodstuff
[13,46,47], different types of plant life subcategories [48–51];
insects [50]; subcategories differing in their degree of manipula-
bility, such as buildings or tools [10–12]. As a result, we included
ten subcategories from the living domain: animals, birds, body
parts, dried fruits, insects, flowers, fruits, sea creatures, trees and
vegetables; and twelve subcategories from the nonliving domain:
buildings, clothing, foodstuff, furniture, jewellery, kitchen utensils,
musical instruments, office material, sports/games, tools, vehicles
and weapons; plus the subcategory of the nonliving natural things,
such as a mountain or a stone. Table 1 contrasts the present work
with previous normative studies carried out with coloured
stimuli—plus the classic findings by S&V—regarding the number
of categories and items studied.
Following the aforementioned procedure, 360 items were
selected, and colour photographs were obtained for each one.
All the photographs were directly taken by the first author and a
collaborator (Sara Can˜amo´n). Subsequently, the images were
removed from their original backgrounds (except for the nonliving
natural things) and placed on a plain white background; the mean
dimension of the images was 2656223 pixels. Regarding the left-
right orientation of each image, it was decided that, for each
category susceptible to being oriented (i.e., animals, vehicles or
tools), half of the items were left-facing and the other half right-
facing.
The experimental items were displayed to a sample of 236
participants (see Participants and Procedure sections) for naming
the pictures and, then, for evaluating the five psycholinguistic
variables included in the study: AoA, familiarity, manipulability,
typicality and visual complexity. Several examples of items are
presented in Figure 1; the whole set of items are included as
supplemental material (Appendix S1).
Participants
The sample consisted of 236 healthy Spanish-speaking under-
graduate students (119 males; 117 females) with a mean age 36.7
years (SD = 10.9; range 19–63 years; Males M = 37.4, SD = 10.2;
Females M =36 SD = 11.5, F = 1.03, n.s.) and a mean number of
years of education of 14.4 years (SD = 2.5; range 12–17 years;
Males M = 14.6, SD = 2.5; Females M = 14.3, SD = 2.5, F = 1.3,
n.s.). All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and Spanish
was their first language. Any person with a known history of
neurological disease, head trauma, or stroke was excluded. The
student participants were assigned course credit for their
participation in the study. The study was approved by the
Bioethics Committee from the UNED and conforms with the
Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed
consent (approved by the Bioethics Committee from the UNED)
for the collection of data and subsequent analysis. Additionally,
participants were explained that they were free to suspend their
participation in the experiments at any time and for any cause.
Procedure
The 360 images were divided into three groups of items (120
each), namely lists A, B and C. We implemented a pseudorandom
selection in order to ensure that the three resulting lists included a
similar number of exemplars belonging to the 23 subcategories.
The 236 participants were randomly assigned to work with one of
the groups of items. Each group of items was evaluated by n =77
(38 males; 39 females, list A), n = 80 (41 males; 39 females, list B),
and n = 79 (40 males; 39 females, list C). Participants were tested
individually in two sessions. They all carried out the naming
session first and, subsequently, they rated the items for familiarity,
age of acquisition, visual complexity, manipulability and typicality.
The whole experiment, combined across both sessions, lasted
approximately ninety minutes, with self-administered rest periods
during the two sessions and between sessions. Each experimental
session was preceded by the instructions provided by researchers
and a practice phase to enable each participant to become familiar
with the task, and, additionally, to generate the acquisition of
anchor points for the stimulus ratings. In the practice phase, each
participant observed ten pictures that were not included in the
main stimulus set. The pictures were displayed on 19-inch LCD
colour monitors with a screen resolution of 10246768 pixels and a
32-bit color mode controlled by microcomputers running E-Prime
1.2 software (Psychology Software Tools, 1996–2002). Every
monitor was calibrated by means of the Display Color Calibration
tool available in Windows 7 Professional operating system
(Microsoft corporation, 2009) including brightness, contrast, color
balance and Gamma adjustments. Previously to the beginning of
each experimental session, at least 45 minutes were provided to
warm up the monitors. Periodically, the screens were carefully
cleaned in order to ensure an optimal picture quality. Viewing
distance was approximately 60 cm.
During the test phase, the 120 images were presented in a
random order. Each image was preceded by a cross (+) for 500 ms,
and remained on the screen for 3,000 msec (naming task phase) or
until the participant responded (during the item rating phase).
During the latter part of the task, visual complexity and typicality
were always the first and the last variables evaluated, respectively;
the rest of the variables were randomly displayed. To evaluate
visual complexity, participants were asked to ‘‘rate the visual
complexity of the image itself, rather than that of the object it
represents’’. To evaluate the remaining variables (AoA, familiarity,
manipulability and typicality), participants were asked to ‘‘rate the
object represented rather than the image itself’’. When the
participants evaluated the variables AoA, familiarity, manipula-
bility and typicality, experimenters provided them with the
canonical name of the item (i.e., the intended one). Additionally,
when participants evaluated the typicality of the items, they were
also provided with the category of the item on the screen (e.g.,
‘‘animals’’ —category— for ‘‘elephant’’—item).
Naming task. Participants were asked to name each image
by typing its name with the keyboard on the screen. They were
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
told to give the specific—rather than the general—name for the
different items. For example, in the case of the subcategory of
‘‘trees’’, if the participant knew the name of the item, he/she
should give the name of that particular tree, e.g., ‘‘pine tree’’,
instead of the general name of ‘‘tree’’. Participants were asked to
type the initials for ‘‘don’t know’’ (NC = ‘‘No Conozco’’, in
Spanish), if the image was unknown to them, to type ‘‘tip of the
tongue’’ (PL = ‘‘Punta de la Lengua’’, in Spanish) if they were
momentarily unable to remember the name, or to type ‘‘don’t
remember’’ (NR = ‘‘No Recuerdo’’, in Spanish). All their respons-
es were automatically saved by the program. According to this
task, ‘‘name agreement’’ was calculated based on the percentage of
participants who named the item according to its canonical name.
AoA. Participants were asked to estimate the age in years at
which they had learned each word following the same procedure
of other similar previous studies (e.g., [4,7]). Scores were obtained
by asking participants to rate age of acquisition for each word on a
seven-interval scale (range: 1 = 0–2 years; 7 = 13 years or more; see
[25]).
Familiarity. Participants were instructed to rate each item,
assessing ‘‘how usual or unusual the concept is in your realm of
experience’’ on the basis of ‘‘how frequently you think about the
concept, and how frequently you come into contact with the
concept —both directly (e.g., seeing a real-life exemplar) and in a
mediated way (e.g., represented in the media)’’. Participants
provided their responses on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = very
unfamiliar,5= very familiar) by pressing the corresponding number
on the keyboard.
Manipulability. Participants were instructed to rate each
item, assessing ‘‘the degree to which using a human hand is
necessary for this object to perform its function’’. Participants
provided their responses on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never
necessary,5= totally indispensable) by pressing the corresponding
number on the keyboard.
Typicality: This reflects the degree to which a concept is a
representative exemplar of its category. Scores were obtained by
asking participants to rate on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all
prototypical,5=very prototypical) how representative of its category
they thought an exemplar was (e.g., car for vehicles).
Visual Complexity. Instructions from S&W’s study were
adapted to evaluate the visual complexity of the items. Conse-
quently, participants were asked to evaluate ‘‘the amount of detail,
intricacy of lines, pattern and quantity of colours presented in the
image’’. Participants recorded their responses on a 5-point scale
(1 = very simple,5= very complex) by pressing corresponding numbers
on the keyboard.
Lexical frequency. Owing to the unavailability of norms for
all of the words in a standard Spanish corpus (e.g. [52]), we
gathered norms for lexical frequency using an Internet search
engine. This method is a viable alternative to the currently
available databases and may even provide a more representative
[53] as well as a constantly updating measure of word frequency
[19] that has high convergent validity with other more classical
Table 1. Present study compared to previous normative ones with coloured stimuli (Adlington et al., 2008; Brodeur et al., 2010;
Moreno-Martı
´
nez et al., 2011; and Viggiano et al., 2004), plus the one by S&V, concerning the categories and number of items
studied.
Present study Adlington’s Brodeur’s M-Martı
´
nez’s S&V’s Viggiano’s
1. Animals 21 08 --- 10 30 42
2. Birds 20 08 --- --- 08 ---
3. Body parts 20 10 --- 10 12 ---
4. Flowers 12 08 --- 10 --- ---
5. Fruits 21 10 --- 10 11 ---
6. Insects 17 08 --- 10 08 ---
7. Marine creatures 18 --- --- --- --- ---
8. Nuts 11 --- --- --- --- ---
9. Trees 11 --- --- 10 --- ---
10. Vegetables 20 09 --- 10 13 36
11. Buildings 15 10 --- 10 --- ---
12. Clothing 13 11 28 10 19 12
13. Desk material 15 --- 38 --- --- ---
14. Food 15 07 78 --- --- 10
15. Furnitur e 15 12 02 10 14 13
16. Jewellery 12 --- 08 --- --- ---
17. Kitchen utensiles 13 12 60 10 14 12
18. Musical instruments 16 11 04 --- 09 06
19. Sports/Games 16 --- 41 --- 18 ---
20. Tools 15 12 37 10 12 29
21. Vehicles 13 11 --- 10 10 09
22. Weapons 15 --- --- --- 07 ---
23. Nature 16 --- 11 --- --- ---
Note: --- = Category not studied.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.t001
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
databases. Furthermore, search engines permit the gathering of
word frequency values for more unusual items that do not typically
feature in conventional databases (see [19,30,54,55]). With more
than 250 million web pages, the AltaVista search engine (www.
altavista.com) is one of the largest search engines currently
available and, for this reason, it was selected for this process. These
names were entered into the search function of AltaVista, and a
search was performed, specifying that results should be for Spain
Figure 1. Several selected examples of the standardised stimuli (subcategory in brackets).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.g001
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
and in Spanish only. The number of hits returned, after
conversion to their natural logarithm, served as the frequency
estimate for each word [19,31,53,56].
Results
1. Descriptive results
A summary of the rating data for each item is reported in
Appendix S2 of the supplemental material. For each item, the
following information is presented: 1) the number of order of each
item; 2) the most frequent name in English and Spanish; 3) two
measures of name agreement: the statistic H and the percentage of
participants who produced the canonical/dominant name, plus
the percentage of participants who produced the modal name of
the item in those cases in which the latter did not match the
dominant name. Although both indexes are measures of name
agreement (statistic H and the percentage), the latter indicates only
how dominant the most common name is in a sample, whereas H
(or entropy [57]) is sensitive to how widely distributed responses
are over all the unique names that are provided for a picture.
Consequently, index H is more informative than name agreement
(e.g., it gives information about the dispersion of the names). H was
calculated according to the following formula:
H~
X
k
i~1
p
i
log
2
1
p
i

where k is the number of unique names given for a picture, and p
i
is the proportion of the sample providing each unique name. H =0
when there is perfect agreement among participants (e.g., just one
name) and increases as agreement decreases. ‘‘Don’t know’’, ‘‘tip
of the tongue’’ and ‘‘don’t remember’’ responses were not taken
into account to calculate index H; 4) the means and standard
deviation for AoA, familiarity, manipulability, typicality, visual
complexity and lexical frequency values expressed as a natural
logarithm. Appendix S3 reports alternative names of each item;
indexes of individual item analysis, including a measure of item
difficulty and two indexes of item discrimination based on item-test
correlations—point-biserial and biserial— are also included in
Appendix S4. Table 2 presents summary statistics for all the
aforementioned variables. Likewise, Table 3 shows summary
statistics for all the variables for all the subcategories. Lastly,
Table 4 shows Pearson correlations among the variables. In
general, as with other normative studies, the standard psycholin-
guistic variables tend to correlate with each other (see
[1,19,58,59]).
2. Reliability and validity of the study
To establish validity, we compared our norms/stimuli with
those of the classical S&V, collected in USA, plus four recent
studies which, like ours, were conducted with high quality colour
images and coloured pictures, collected in United Kingdom,
Canada, Italy and Spain, respectively: [1,19,20,28,30]. Pearson’s
correlations, including those items sharing the same name in the
four studies (n = 50 with [19], n = 68 with [20], n = 113 with [30],
n = 80 with [28], and n = 106 with [1]) are shown in Table 5. A
high pattern of significant correlations (fluctuating between .25
and .99) was found among the diverse variables observed across
the five studies. So, even where compared across English (different
countries, languages and studies), Italian and Spanish, the ratings
remain highly correlated. Regarding reliability, Cronbach’s alpha
coefficients were also high: a = .83 (name agreement), a = .97
(visual complexity, familiarity and manipulability) and a = .98
(AoA and typicality).
Discussion
The goal of the present work was twofold: (a) to present a broad
set of high quality ecological colour photographs across a range of
difficulty, to deal with problems derived from ceiling effects; and
(b) to give detailed norms, derived from a group of healthy
participants, of several relevant psycholinguistic variables. To the
best of our knowledge, this work is the first to provide such a high
number of quality ecological items (360), pertaining to so many
different (23) subcategories and providing indexes of seven relevant
psycholinguistics variables: age of acquisition, familiarity, lexical
frequency, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual
complexity, gathered from such a large number of participants
(n = 236). Another main contribution of our study, compared to
previous recent normative works, is that it incorporates item
analyses, for those authors interested in selecting the more suitable
items according to their goals and recently collected norms on
typicality.
Recent normative works have provided valuable data from a
high number of coloured items and have also presented ratings for
relevant psycholinguistics variables [19,20,30,28]. However, (i)
they have excluded theoretically relevant subcategories, such as
animals, body parts, buildings and vehicles ([20], also did not
evaluate typicality), (ii) they have provided ratings for only several
psycholinguistics variables: familiarity, name agreement and visual
complexity ([28], did not include body parts) or (iii) the number of
items evaluated is relatively sparse, compared to the 260 items
originally studied by S&V (147: [19]; 140: [30]; 174: [28]). The
category-specific literature has convincingly shown that there are
important differences within the living domain (e.g., animals and
plant life—fruits, flowers and vegetables) between the animals and
plant-life subcategories [13]. Similarly, processing differences have
been reported within the non-living domain (e.g., tools, vehicles
and furniture), between small manipulable objects, such as tools,
and large outdoor objects, such as buildings [12]. In their domain-
specific theory, Caramazza and collaborators posited that, for the
subcategories of items for which rapid identification confers
reproductive advantages, natural selection has produced special-
ized, dissociable neural pathways—modules [48,60,61]. According
to these proposals, such modules exist for animals and plant life,
although the domains of tools and conspecifics have recently been
incorporated into this view [62]. Similarly, within the nonliving
thing domain, Warrington and McCarthy [12] reported a case
that revealed a clear dissociation within this domain: a greater
impairment in identifying small and manipulable objects, com-
pared to large and non-manipulable things. Regarding psycholin-
guistics variables, while familiarity [3], name agreement [4] and
visual complexity [5] have been shown to be significantly relevant
to the processing of pictorial and verbal material, both in control
participants and patients, other no less relevant variables, such as
age of acquisition [2], manipulability [63], typicality [17] and
word frequency [64] have also robustly revealed their impact on
normal and impaired processing of items. Consequently, works
providing normative data for these variables are, in our view,
particularly demanded in the object processing arena. Likewise,
the above commented semantic specialization (i.e., differences
between manipulable and non-manipulable things/biologically
derived modules), strongly recommend having a sufficient number
of items that make it possible to elucidate these theoretical issues.
Validity indexes showed that our stimuli had similar features as
those of other corpi and they presente high internal consistency as
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
well; this suggests that the new corpus has adequate psychometric
characteristics. Likewise, the fact that our scales presented high
cross-language correlations with similar studies indicates that our
stimuli are suitable to be used in countries other than Spain and in
different cultures with different languages.
Table 2. Summary statistics for all the variables.
AoA Fam LF (Log) Man Typ VC NA
H
M
3.63 3.56 15.560 3.11 3.65 2.55 0.72 0.94
SD
1.30 0.88 14.190 1.26 0.86 0.74 0.28 0.87
Median 3.44 3.55 15.520 3.52 3.74 2.46 0.82 0.70
Mode 3.05 4.78 14.190 1.22 3.37 1.53 1.00 0.00
Skew 0.37 20.25 20.019 20.30 20.54 0.21 20.86 0.83
Kurtosis 20.73 20.79 0.380 21.43 20.52 20.77 20.47 20.21
Range 5.36 3.65 11.300 3.87 3.49 3.31 0.99 3.73
Min 1.37 1.32 9.010 1.01 1.46 1.18 0.01 0.00
Max 6.73 4.97 20.310 4.88 4.95 4.49 1.00 3.73
Q1 2.59 2.87 14.310 1.70 3.06 1.94 0.53 0.20
Q3 4.58 4.36 16.630 4.21 4.42 3.14 0.96 1.57
Note: AoA = Age of acquisision; Fam = Familiarity, LF = Lexical frequency (natural logarithm); Man = Manipulability; Typ = Typicality; VC = Visual complexity; %
NA = Percentage of name agreement.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.t002
Table 3. Summary statistics for all the variables for each category.
AoA Fam Man Typicality VC LF (Log) %NA
MSDMSDMSDMSDMSDM SDMSD
1. Animals 3.46 1.39 2.91 0.74 1.42 0.41 3.60 0.87 3.19 0.36 15.22 1.63 0.79 0.25
2. Birds 3.88 1.21 3.01 0.75 1.45 0.28 3.47 0.67 3.25 0.44 15.16 1.10 0.66 0.25
3. Body parts 2.74 1.50 4.13 0.92 2.09 0.82 4.07 0.70 2.36 0.53 16.80 1.56 0.74 0.29
4. Flowers 4.18 1.15 3.43 0.67 2.06 0.25 3.82 0.69 2.73 0.50 15.56 1.80 0.52 0.31
5. Fruits 3.50 1.46 3.92 0.83 3.71 0.21 3.75 0.93 1.79 0.39 15.78 1.44 0.77 0.26
6. Insects 3.28 1.04 3.36 0.80 1.41 0.18 3.87 0.67 2.99 0.48 14.76 1.14 0.75 0.20
7. Marine creatures 3.12 0.99 3.86 0.54 4.26 0.56 3.63 1.67 2.63 0.74 15.67 1.67 0.89 0.12
8. Nuts 3.57 0.72 3.70 0.54 3.67 0.47 3.85 0.70 1.96 0.31 15.10 1.68 0.67 0.25
9. Trees 4.40 .81 3.28 0.53 1.71 0.24 3.71 0.49 2.82 0.43 15.29 0.96 0.37 0.32
10. Vegetables 3.84 1.14 3.93 0.74 3.70 0.27 3.84 0.58 1.97 0.49 15.17 0.94 0.74 0.27
11. Buildings 4.01 1.44 3.18 0.89 2.66 0.50 2.81 1.07 3.35 0.73 16.65 2.14 0.70 0.24
12. Clothing 2.85 1.33 4.30 0.88 3.81 0.48 3.86 1.17 1.89 0.39 15.43 1.46 0.89 0.18
13. Desk material 3.47 1.14 4.25 0.52 4.55 0.20 4.09 0.57 1.83 0.53 15.48 1.74 0.77 0.29
14. Food 3.48 1.35 3.98 0.92 4.03 0.34 3.47 0.90 2.17 0.79 14.54 2.04 0.74 0.27
15. Furnitur e 3.30 1.47 4.23 0.68 3.19 0.54 3.89 0.88 2.45 0.48 15.97 1.72 0.79 0.21
16. Jewellery 4.03 1.12 3.39 0.61 3.88 0.44 3.77 0.89 2.79 0.50 15.97 1.73 0.66 0.23
17. Kitchen utensiles 3.88 1.43 3.84 1.01 4.41 0.30 3.68 1.07 2.20 0.64 14.54 1.33 0.63 0.29
18. Musical
instuments
4.05 1.24 3.09 0.70 4.69 0.13 3.79 0.79 3.13 0.73 15.05 1.74 0.77 0.31
19. Sports/Games 3.12 0.99 3.86 0.54 4.26 0.56 3.63 0.63 2.63 0.73 15.67 1.67 0.89 0.12
20. Tools 4.21 1.20 3.49 0.73 4.53 0.18 3.82 0.69 1.93 0.39 14.74 2.07 0.70 0.28
21. Vehicles 2.93 0.97 3.78 .65 3.93 0.55 3.32 1.18 2.98 0.62 16.83 1.83 0.83 0.15
22. Weapons 3.91 1.01 2.59 .49 4.20 0.52 3.19 1.02 2.56 0.76 15.80 1.89 0.78 0.25
23. Nature 2.85 1.01 3.71 .79 1.49 0.59 3.66 0.73 2.68 0.72 17.50 1.53 0.67 0.29
Note: AoA = Age of acquisision; Fam = Familiarity, LF = Lexical frequency (natural logarithm); Man = Manipulability; VC = Visual complexity; % NA = Percentage of name
agreement.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.t003
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
Although we have attempted to address the methodological
issues of this literature reviewed in the Introduction, there remains
one limitation in the current study: the fact that cognitive/
conceptual effects are able to drive the categorization beyond the
low level features. As mentioned in Introduction, we selected our
categories —and stimuli— in a ‘‘top-down perspective’’, based on
relevant theoretical reasons, and mainly derived from Cognitive
Neuropsychology arena [1,10–13,19–21,23,25–30,44,45,47].
From a different perspective, vision studies from psychophysical
and neurophysiological field have, traditionaly, made used of
accurate low-level quantitative methods to define the physical
parameters of naturalistic photographs, in order to explore basic
aspects of the human visual system (see, e.g., [65–67]). However, it
should be recognized that the human visual system is sufficiently
adaptable to make possible that different low level features in the
stimuli can be compensated to obtain higher level invariant
categorizations. Clearly, this is something that cannot be taken for
granted and should be recognized in any study dealing with
pictorial stimuli.
Beyond the low level properties of objects, another relevant
point has been relatively ignored in the previous literature on
normative and semantic assessment studies. This point is related to
the control of the relations between objects specified by abstract
feature spaces (see [68], for a review). Most of the recently
developed corpi have been designed according to arbitrary criteria
for the selection of the categories and the assigning of their stimuli.
In contrast, an alternative selection method could take advantage
of the semantic structural descriptions derived from hierarchical
Bayesian models, which fits quite well the human performance in
semantic induction tasks [68,69]. This procedure should be
seriously considered by researchers in order to develop more
accurate instruments in this field.
To conclude, the present work provides a useful tool for
researchers examining language, memory, object- and word-
processing, particularly for those interested in comparing healthy
versus neurologically damaged populations. Accordingly, the new
instrument, in combination with other recently developed corpi, is
intended to be an ecological alternative to the corpus developed by
Snodgrass and Vanderwart thirty years ago, especially, but not
exclusively, in a Spanish-speaking population.
Supporting Information
Appendix S1 Colour photographs of the 360 items.
(RAR)
Appendix S2 Normative psycholinguistic ratings for
each item.
(DOC)
Appendix S3 Proportion (in brackets) of target names,
alternative names, acceptable synonyms of each item,
plus ‘‘Don’t know’’ (DK), ‘‘Don’t remember’’ (DR), and
‘‘Tip of the tongue’’ (TOT) responses.
(DOC)
Appendix S4 Indexes of individual item analysis includ-
ing a measure of item difficulty and two indexes of item
discrimination based on item-test correlations (point-
biserial and biserial).
(DOC)
Table 4. Correlation matrix for naming performance and psycholinguistic variables.
AoA Fam LF Man Typ VC %NA
H
AoA 1 2.82* 2.57* 2.05 2.72* .34* 2.68* .67*
Fam 1 .47* .29* .75* 2.57* .63* 2.61*
LF 1 2.05 .45* 2.10 .42* 2.40
Man 1 .10 2.36* .19* 2.20*
Typ 1 2.27* .52* 2.51*
VC 1 2.25* .26*
NA 1 2.93*
H 1
Note: AoA = Age of acquisision; Fam = Familiarity, LF = Lexical frequency; Man = Manipulability; Typ = Typicality; VC = Visual complexity; % NA = Percentage of name
agreement.
*p,.01.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.t004
Table 5. Correlations between current stimuli and those of
Adlington et al. (2009), Brodeur et al. (2010), Moreno-Martı
´
nez
et al. (2011), Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980), and Viggiano
et al. (2004)
1
.
Items (n) AoA Fam LF Man VC %NA
Adlington
et al.’s
50 .84 .78 .74 n.e. .66 .62
Brodeur
et al.’s
68 n.e. .76 n.e. .48 .68 .25
Moreno-
Martı
´
nez
et al.’s
113 .99 .98 .99 .88 .92 .94
S&V 106 .81
2
.79 .62 n.e. .76 .41
Viggiano
et al.’s
(English)
80 n.e. .73 n.e. n.e. .80 .34
Viggiano
et al.’s
(Italian)
80 n.e. .77 n.e. n.e. .84 .46
Note: AoA = Age of acquisision; Fam = Familiarity, LF = Lexical frequency;
VC = Visual complexity; % NA = Percentage of name agreement, n.e. = not
evaluated.
1
= Viggiano et al.’s work studied two samples: English and Italian speakers
evaluated the same items.
2
= 39 items (in their original study, S&V presented AoA data only for some
items).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037527.t005
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
Acknowledgments
We wish to thank Sara Can˜ amo´n for her help taking and selecting
photographs of the items. We also wish to thank Professors Gregory
Francis, Roberto Dell’Acqua and to one anonymous reviewer for their
valuable comments and observations which contributed to improve earlier
versions of the manuscript.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: FJMM PRM. Performed the
experiments: FJMM PRM. Analyzed the data: FJMM PRM. Contributed
reagents/materials/analysis tools: FJMM. Wrote the paper: FJMM PRM.
References
1. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms
for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory 6: 174–215.
2. Ellis AW, Morrison CM (1998) Real age of acquisition effects in lexical retrieval.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 24:
515–523.
3. Funnell E, Sheridan J (1992) Categories of knowledge: Unfamiliar aspects of
living and nonliving things. Cognitive Neuropsychology 9: 135–153.
4. Gilhooly KJ, Gilhooly ML (1979) Age-of-acquisition effects in lexical and
episodic memory. Memory & Cognition 7: 213–223.
5. Humphreys GW, Forde EME (2001) Category-specific deficits: A major review
and presentation of the Hierarchical Interactive Theory (HIT). Behavioral and
Brain Sciences 24: 480–481.
6. Holmes S, Fitch FJ, Ellis A (2006) Age of Acquisition affects object recognition
and naming in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology 28: 1010–1022.
7. Silveri MC, Cappa A, Mariotti P, Puopolo M (2002) Naming in patients with
Alzheimer’s disease: Influence of age of acquisition and categorical effects.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24: 755–764.
8. Allport DA (1985) Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia In:
Newman SK, Epstein R, eds. Current perspectives in dysphasia Edinburgh (pp
32–60): Churchill Livingstone.
9. Buxbaum LJ, Saffran EM (2002) Knowledge of object manipulation and object
function: Dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects. Brain & Language
82: 179–199.
10. Magnie´ MN, Besson M, Poncet M, Dolisi C (2003) The Snodgrass and
Vanderwart set revisited: Norms for object manipulability and for pictorial
ambiguity of objects, chimeric objects and nonobjects. Journal of Clinical &
Experimental Neuropsychology 25: 521–560.
11. Tranel D, Logan CG, Randall JF, Damasio AR (1997) Explaining category-
related effects in the retrieval of conceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete
entities: operationalization and analysis of factors. Neuropsychologia 35:
1329–1339.
12. Warrington EK, McCarthy RA (1987) Categories of knowledge: Further
fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain 110: 1273–1296.
13. Capitani E, Laiacona M, Mahon B, Caramazza A (2003) What are the facts of
semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence.
Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 213–261.
14. Rosch EH (1973) Natural Categories. Cognitive Psychology 4: 328–350.
15. Rosch EH, Mervis CB (1975) Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal
Structure of Categories. Cognitive Psychology 7: 573–605.
16. Rosch EH, Simpson C, Miller RS (1976) Structural Bases of Typicality Effects.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:
491–502.
17. Kiran S, Thomson CK (2003) Effect of typicality on online category verification
of animate category exemplars in aphasia. Brain & Language 85: 441–450.
18. Adlam AL, Paterson K, Bozeat S, Hodges JR (2010) The Cambridge semantic
memory test battery: Detection of semantic deficits in semantic dementia and
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocase 16: 193–207.
19. Adlington RL, Laws KR, Gale TM (2008) The Hatfield Image Test (HIT): A
new picture test and norms for experimental and clinical use. Journal of Clinical
& Experimental Neuropsychology 31: 731–753.
20. Brodeur MB, Dionne-Dostie E, Montreuil T, Lepage M (2010) The BOSS, a
new set of 538 normalized photos of objects to be used as ecological stimuli in
vision and memory paradigms. PLoS ONE 5(5): e10773. doi:10.1371/journal.
pone.0010773.
21. Bunn EM, Tyler LK, Moss HE (1998) Category-specific semantic deficit: The
role of familiarity and property type re-examined. Neuropsychology 12:
367–379.
22. Dimitropoulou M, Dun˜abeitia JA, Blitsas P, Carreiras M (2009) A standardized
set of 260 pictures for Modern Greek: Norms for name agreement, age of
acquisition and visual complexity. Behavior Research Methods 41: 584–589.
23. McKenna P (1998) The category-specific Names Test Hove: Psychological
Press.
24. Moreno FJ, Can˜amo´ n S (2005) Presentacio´n y resultados preliminares de la
Baterı
´
a Nombela (I): Un nuevo instrumento para evaluar el deterioro sema´ntico
categorial . Revista de Psicopatologı
´
a y Psicologı
´
aClı
´
nica 10: 205–219.
25. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Peraita H (2007) Un nuevo conjunto de ı
´
tems para la
evaluacio´n de la disociacio´n ser vivo/ser no vivo con normas obtenidas de
ancianos sanos espan˜oles. Psicolo´gica 28: 1–20.
26. Pe´rez MA, Navalo´n C (2003) Normas espan˜olas de 290 nuevos dibujos: acuerdo
en la denominacio´n, concordancia de la imagen, familiaridad, complejidad
visual y variabilidad de la imagen. Psicolo´gica 24: 215–241.
27. Rossion B, Pourtois G (2004) Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object
pictorial set: the role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition.
Perception 33: 217–236.
28. Viggiano MP, Vannucci M, Righi S (2004) A new standardized set of ecological
pictures for experimental and clinical research on visual object processing.
Cortex 40: 494–509.
29. Hodges JR, Salmon DP, Butters N (1992) Semantic memory impairment in
Alzheimer’s disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge? Neuropsychologia
30: 301–314.
30. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Montoro PR, Laws KR (2011) A set of high quality colour
images with Spanish norms for seven relevant psycholinguistic variables. Aging,
Neuropsychology & Cognition 18: 293–327.
31. Dell’Acqua R, Lotto L, Job R (2000) Naming times and standardized norms for
the Italian PD/DPSS set of pictures: Direct comparisons with American,
English, French, and Spanish published databases. Behavior Research Methods,
Instruments, & Computers 32: 588–615.
32. Price CJ, Humphreys GW (1989) The effects of surface detail on object
categorization and naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology A Human Experimental Psychology 41: 797–827.
33. Tanaka JW, Presnell LM (1999) Colour diagnosticity in object recognition.
Perception and Psychophysics 61: 1140–1153.
34. Adlington RL, Laws KR, Gale TM (2009) Visual processing in Alzheimer’s
disease: Surface detail and colour fail to aid object identification. Neuropsycho-
logia 47: 2574–2583.
35. Done DH, Gale TM (1997) Attribute verification in dementia of Alzheimer type:
Evidence for the preservation of distributed concept knowledge. Cognitive
Neuropsychology 14: 547–571.
36. Grossman M, Robinson K, Biassou N, White-Devine T, D’Esposito MD (1998)
Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease: representativeness, ontologic category,
and material. Neuropsychology 12: 34–42.
37. Laws KR, Gale TM, Leeson VC, Crawford JR (2005) When is category-specific
in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 41: 452–463.
38. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ (2010) Size matters: A study on naming and size
knowledge in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurocase 16: 494–502.
39. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Laws KR (2007) An attenuation of the ‘normal’ category
effect in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a review and bootstrap analysis. Brain
& Cognition 63: 167–173.
40. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Laws KR (2008) No category-specificity in Alzheimer’s
disease: A normal aging effect. Neuropsychology 22: 485–490.
41. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Tallo´n-Barranco A, Frank-Garcia A (2007) Enfermedad
de Alzheimer, deterioro categorial y variables relevantes en la denominacio´n de
objetos. Revista de Neurologı
´
a 44: 129–133.
42. Tyler LK, Bright P, Dick E, Tavares P, Pilgrim L, et al. (2003) Do semantic
categories activate distinct cortical regions? Evidence for a distributed neural
semantic system. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 541–559.
43. Laws KR (2005) Illusions of normality: A methodological critique of category-
specific naming. Cortex 41: 842–851.
44. Laiacona M, Barbarotto R, Trivelli C, Capitani E (1993) Disossociazioni
semantiche intercategoriali: descrizione di una batteria standrdizzata e dati
normativi Archivio di Psicologia. Neurologia e Psichiatria 54: 209–248.
45. Whatmough C, Chertkow H, Murtha S, Templeman D, Babins L, et al. (2003)
The semantic category effect increases with worsening anomia in Alzheimer’s
type dementia. Brain & Language 84: 134–147.
46. Barbarotto R, Capitani E, Laiacona M (2001) Living musical instruments and
inanimate body parts. Neuropsychologia 39: 406–414.
47. Warrington EK, Shallice T (1984) Category specific semantic impairments.
Brain 107: 829–854.
48. Caramazza A, Shelton JR (1998) Domain-specific knowledge systems in the
brain: the animate-inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10:
1–34.
49. Crutch SJ, Warrington EK (2003) The selective impairment of fruit and
vegetable knowledge: A multiple processing channels account of fine-grain
category specificity. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 355–372.
50. McRae K, Cree GS (2002) Factors underlying category-specific semantic
deficits. In EME. Forde, Humphreys, eds. Category-specificity in mind and
brain. East Sussex: Psychology Press. pp 211–249.
51. Samson D, Pillon A (2003) A case of impairment knowledge for fruits and
vegetables. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 373–400.
52. Sebastia´n N, Martı
´
MA, Carreiras MF, Cuetos F (2000) LEXESP, Le´xico
informatizado del Espan˜ol Barcelona: Ediciones de la Universitat de Barcelona.
53. Blair IV, Urland GR, Ma JE (2002) Using internet search engines to estimate
word frequency. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 34:
286–290.
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
54. Baayen RH, Piepenbrock R, Gulikers L (1995) The CELEX lexical database
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.
55. Kucera H, Francis W (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American
English Providence: Brown University Press.
56. Moreno-Martı
´
nez FJ, Montoro PR (2010) Longitudinal patterns of fluency
impairment in dementia: The role of domain and ‘‘nuisance variables’’.
Aphasiology 11: 1389–1399.
57. Cover TM, Thomas JA (2006) Elements of Information Theory (2nd Edition).
Hoboken: Wiley and Sons.
58. A
´
lvarez B, Cuetos F (2007) Objective age of acquisition norms for a set of 328
words in Spanish. Behavior, Research, Methods 39: 377–383.
59. Sanfeliu MC, Ferna´ndez A (1996) A set of 254 Snodgrass-Vanderwart pictures
standardized for Spanish: Norms for name agreement, image agreement,
familiarity, and visual complexity. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and
Computers 28: 537–555.
60. Mahon BZ, Caramazza A (2003) Constraining questions about the organisation
and representation of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20:
433–450.
61. Caramazza A, Mahon BZ (2003) The organization of conceptual knowledge: the
evidence from category-specific semantic deficits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
7: 325–374.
62. Shelton JR, Fouch E, Caramazza A (1998) The selective sparing of body parts
knowledge: A case study. Neurocase 4: 339–351.
63. Filliter JH, McMullen PA, Westwood D (2005) Manipulability and living/non-
living category effects on object identification. Brain & Cognition 57: 61–65.
64. Oldfield RC, Wingfield A (1965) Response latencies in naming objects.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 17: 273–281.
65. Freeman J, Simoncelli EP (2011) Metamers of the ventral stream. Nature
Neuroscience 9: 1195–1201.
66. Girshick AR, Landy MS, Simoncelli EP (2011) Cardinal rules: Visual orientation
perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics. Nature Neuroscience
14: 926–932.
67. Torralba A (2009) How many pixels make an image? Visual Neuroscience 26:
123–131.
68. Tenenbaum JB, Kemp C, Griffiths TL, Goodman ND (2011) How to grow a
mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction. Science 331: 1279–1285.
69. Griffiths TL, Chater N, Kemp C, Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB (2010) Probabilistic
models of cognition: Exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences 14: 357–364.
A New Corpus of 360 High Quality Color Images
PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 May 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 5 | e37527
    • "Several linguistic properties were computed, for one the naming agreement. One way to define naming agreement is as the percentage of participants that gave the dominant name (Brodeur et al., 2010;Moreno-Martinez & Montoro, 2012;Severens, Van Lommel, Ratinckx, & Hartsuiker, 2005;Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980). Another way to define naming agreement is as the percentage of people that gave the intended name (Adlington et al., 2009;Bates et al., 2003). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Researchers in different fields of psychology have been interested in how vision and language interact, and what type of representations are involved in such interactions. We introduce a stimulus set that facilitates such research (available online). The set consists of 100 words each of which is paired with four pictures of objects: One semantically similar object (but visually dissimilar), one visually similar object (but semantically dissimilar), and two unrelated objects. Visual and semantic similarity ratings between corresponding items are provided for every picture for Dutch and for English. In addition, visual and linguistic parameters of each picture are reported. We thus present a stimulus set from which researchers can select, on the basis of various parameters, the items most optimal for their research question.
    Full-text · Article · Oct 2015
    • "Long-term emotional relational memory 9 Stimulus materials A total of 288 neutral objects and 144 background scenes were used in the present experiment. Neutral objects were selected from two different standardized sets: The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS; Brodeur, Dionne-Dostie, Montreuil, & Lepage, 2010), and the ecological adaptation of Snodgrass and Vanderwart (Moreno-Martínez & Montoro, 2012) During encoding, neutral objects were presented superimposed on an emotional or neutral background scene. Participants were presented with one of six encoding lists (for list construction, see Jaeger et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2004a), each consisting of 144 object/background pairs, with no more than two pictures from the same valence presented consecutively. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: There is abundant evidence in memory research that emotional stimuli are better remembered than neutral stimuli. However, effects of an emotionally charged context on memory for associated neutral elements is also important particularly in trauma and stress-related disorders, where strong memories are often activated by neutral cues due to their emotional associations. In the present study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate long-term recognition memory (one week delay) for neutral objects that had been paired with emotionally arousing or neutral scenes during encoding. Context effects were clearly evident in the ERPs: An early frontal ERP old/new difference (300-500 ms) was enhanced for objects encoded in unpleasant compared to pleasant and neutral contexts; and a late central-parietal old/new difference (400-700 ms) was observed for objects paired with both pleasant and unpleasant contexts, but not for items paired with neutral backgrounds. Interestingly, objects encoded in emotional contexts (and novel objects) also prompted an enhanced frontal early (180-220 ms) positivity compared to objects paired with neutral scenes indicating early perceptual significance. The present data suggest that emotional — particularly unpleasant— backgrounds strengthen memory for items encountered within these contexts and engage automatic and explicit recognition processes. These results could help in understanding binding mechanisms involved in the activation of trauma-related memories by neutral cues.
    Full-text · Article · Oct 2015
    • "We selected 18 colored photographs of daily-life objects from two standardized stimulus sets (Brodeur, Dionne-Dostie, Montreuil, Lepage, & Op de Beeck, 2010; Moreno-Martínez & Montoro, 2012). Half of the objects were kitchen utensils whereas the other half were garage tools. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: The purpose of the current study was to investigate to what extent low-level versus high-level effects determine where the eyes land on isolated daily-life objects. We operationalized low-level effects as eye movements toward an object's center of gravity (CoG) or the absolute object center (OC) and high-level effects as visuomotor priming by object affordances. In two experiments, we asked participants to make saccades toward peripherally presented photographs of graspable objects (e.g., a hammer) and to either categorize them (Experiment 1) or to discriminate them from visually matched nonobjects (Experiment 2). Objects were rotated such that their graspable part (e.g., the hammer's handle) pointed toward either the left or the right whereas their action-performing part (e.g., the hammer's head) pointed toward the other side. We found that early-triggered saccades were neither biased toward the object's graspable part nor toward its action-performing part. Instead, participants' eyes landed near the CoG/OC. Only longer-latency initial saccades and refixations were subject to high-level influences, being significantly biased toward the object's action-performing part. Our comparison with eye movements toward visually matched nonobjects revealed that the latter was not merely the consequence of a low-level effect of shape, texture, asymmetry, or saliency. Instead, we interpret it as a higher-level, object-based affordance effect that requires time, and to some extent also foveation, in order to build up and to overcome default saccadic-programming mechanisms.
    Full-text · Article · Apr 2015
Show more