Content uploaded by Michael W Kraus
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Michael W Kraus on Apr 29, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Psychological Science
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/05/30/0956797611434537
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611434537
published online 31 May 2012Psychological Science
Cameron Anderson, Michael W. Kraus, Adam D. Galinsky and Dacher Keltner
The Local-Ladder Effect : Social Status and Subjective Well-Being
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Association for Psychological Science
can be found at:Psychological ScienceAdditional services and information for
http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
What is This?
- May 31, 2012OnlineFirst Version of Record >>
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Psychological Science
XX(X) 1 –8
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611434537
http://pss.sagepub.com
The pursuit of social status is a powerful motive that drives
much of social behavior. But does achieving higher status
bring happiness? Dozens of studies suggest that social status
plays little role in subjective well-being (SWB). For example,
within countries, there is only a weak association between
socioeconomic status (SES) and dimensions of SWB, includ-
ing life satisfaction and the experience of positive and nega-
tive emotions (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). In fact,
individuals who more strongly value wealth and material pos-
sessions, which are components of SES, tend to experience
lower SWB (Kasser & Ryan, 1993). The literature seems to
suggest that attaining high status provides little benefit for
one’s SWB.
However, prior research linking status and SWB has
focused almost exclusively on SES—material dimensions of
status that arise from income and wealth—so it remains an
open question whether other forms of status may have a stron-
ger impact on SWB. Sociometric status is a distinct form of
social status. It represents the respect and admiration individu-
als have in their face-to-face groups, such as among their
neighbors, coworkers, or classmates (Anderson, John, Keltner,
& Kring, 2001). A long tradition of research has documented
that rank-order differences in sociometric status emerge in all
kinds of face-to-face groups (Bales, Strodtbeck, Mills, &
Roseborough, 1951), just as they do in nonhuman species,
with some individuals attaining more respect and admiration
than others.
One reason why SWB may be affected more by sociometric
status than by SES is that sociometric status is defined locally,
in the context of face-to-face groups, whereas SES is typically
defined as global status within one’s country. Individuals’
comparisons with others immediately around them affect their
happiness more than do distant comparisons (Festinger, 1954).
As Bertrand Russell (1930) noted, “beggars do not envy mil-
lionaires, though of course they will envy other beggars who
are more successful” (p. 90). Supporting our argument that
local status matters more to SWB than global status does, a
prior study showed that individuals with higher income rela-
tive to others in their own county reported higher life satisfac-
tion (Boyce, Brown, & Moore, 2010).
Corresponding Author:
Cameron Anderson, University of California, Berkeley—Haas School of
Business, 545 Student Services Bldg. #1900, Berkeley, CA 94720-1900
E-mail: anderson@haas.berkeley.edu
The Local-Ladder Effect: Social Status and
Subjective Well-Being
Cameron Anderson1, Michael W. Kraus2, Adam D. Galinsky3,
and Dacher Keltner4
1Haas School of Business, Universit y of California, Berkeley; 2Department of Psychology, Universit y of Illinois,
Urbana-Champaign; 3Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University; and 4Department of Psychology,
University of California, Berkeley
Abstract
Dozens of studies in different nations have revealed that socioeconomic status only weakly predicts an individual’s subjective
well-being (SWB). These results imply that although the pursuit of social status is a fundamental human motivation, achieving
high status has little impact on one’s SWB. However, we propose that sociometric status—the respect and admiration one
has in face-to-face groups (e.g., among friends or coworkers)—has a stronger effect on SWB than does socioeconomic
status. Using correlational, experimental, and longitudinal methodologies, four studies found consistent evidence for a local-
ladder effect: Sociometric status significantly predicted satisfaction with life and the experience of positive and negative
emotions. Longitudinally, as sociometric status rose or fell, SWB rose or fell accordingly. Furthermore, these effects were
driven by feelings of power and social acceptance. Overall, individuals’ sociometric status matters more to their SWB than
does their socioeconomic status.
Keywords
social structure, socioeconomic status, well-being
Received 7/20/11; Revision accepted 11/24/11
Research Article
Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on May 31, 2012 as doi:10.1177/0956797611434537
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
2 Anderson et al.
However, we hypothesize that sociometric status matters
even more for SWB than does SES rank in the local environ-
ment because sociometric status is connected to a set of psy-
chological and social processes that shape SWB. Sociometric
status is based in peer respect rather than income or wealth
(Berger, Rosenholtz, & Zelditch, 1980; Blau, 1964). As a
reflection of respect and admiration among peers, sociometric
status is likely to strongly influence the personal sense of
power and feelings of social acceptance, which are both criti-
cal determinants of psychological well-being (Baumeister &
Leary, 1995; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). Individ-
uals higher in sociometric status have more control over group
decisions, more autonomy, and more influence over others’
opinions (Berger et al., 1980). Sociometric status is thus likely
to determine the personal sense of power and control. More-
over, individuals higher in sociometric status have more
friends and are more frequently included in others’ social
activities (Thibault & Kelley, 1959). Sociometric status is thus
a specific form of status that should boost the sense of belong-
ingness and interpersonal connection. Although SES can also
shape the sense of power (Anderson, John, & Keltner, in
press), these effects tend to be weaker. In addition, people with
higher SES show signs of impoverished social connections
(Kraus & Keltner, 2009).
In light of this analysis, we propose a local-ladder effect,
whereby higher sociometric status leads to higher SWB. We
expected to observe this effect because sociometric status
shapes two important determinants of psychological well-
being: an increased sense of power and a sense of social accep-
tance. Further, given that some prior research has found
significant (albeit modest) effects of SES on SWB, we also
thought it important to test our hypothesis that the effects of
sociometric status on SWB are stronger than the effects of
SES.
To triangulate on our central research question concerning
the link between status and SWB, we conducted four studies
using a diverse set of complementary designs. Study 1 exam-
ined status and SWB in intact groups and used multiple mea-
sures of sociometric status, including peer reports. Study 2
examined a broader national sample and tested power and
social acceptance as mediators of the link between status and
SWB. Whereas Studies 1 and 2 established ecological validity,
Study 3 used experimental methods to test causal effects of
sociometric status relative to SES. Study 4 used a longitudinal
design that allowed us to assess whether changes in status lead
to changes in SWB: We predicted that as an individual’s socio-
metric status rises or falls after a significant life transition, his
or her SWB rises or falls accordingly.
Study 1: Status and Well-Being in
Extant Groups
In Study 1, we examined the associations between status—
both sociometric and socioeconomic—and well-being in col-
lege student groups, such as sororities and Reserve Officers’
Training Corps (ROTC) groups. College students value their
membership in these kinds of groups and spend considerable
time with fellow group members. Moreover, this design
allowed us to collect multiple measures of sociometric status,
including peer and self-report as well as leadership data.
Method
Participants. Eighty-eight members of 14 college student
groups participated (53% male, 47% female; average age =
20.4 years, SD = 1.3). Participants were asked to select all
racial-ethnic categories to which they belonged; 56% selected
White, 18% selected African American, 10% selected Latino,
24% selected Asian American, 1% selected Native American,
and 10% selected “other.” Two groups provided unreliable
peer ratings of status (see the next paragraph) and were
excluded from the analyses. Thus, the final analyses included
80 participants from 12 separate groups.
Sociometric status. We measured sociometric status with
three indices. First, participants rated each fellow group mem-
ber on whether he or she was respected, admired, and looked
up to in the group, using a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to
7 (strongly agree). We used Kenny and LaVoie’s (1984) social
relations model (SRM) to analyze these peer ratings. Two
groups showed very low consensus in their peer ratings of sta-
tus (αs of .00 and .08) and were therefore excluded from fur-
ther analysis. There was high consensus among the remaining
participants, α = .71. Second, participants rated their own sta-
tus by responding to five items: “I have a high level of respect
in others’ eyes,” “Others admire me,” “Others look up to me,”
“I have high social standing,” and “I am held in high regard by
others.” Responses were made on a scale from 1 (strongly dis-
agree) to 7 (strongly agree). Ratings on these items were com-
bined into an overall measure of self-perceived status (α =
.93). Third, we measured the number of leadership positions
participants had held in their group (e.g., president, rush chair-
person; M = 1.71, SD = 1.56). We formed an overall index of
sociometric status by standardizing scores on each of the three
indicators and then averaging across the standardized scores
(α = .60). We centered this variable around the group mean to
control for group effects.
Family income. SES was measured using a standard scale of
family income (Adler, Epel, Castellazzo, & Ickovics, 2000).
Participants rated their “total household income,” which
included their parents’ combined income, according to the fol-
lowing scale: 1 = $15,000 or less, 2 = $15,001–$25,000, 3 =
$25,001–$35,000, 4 = $35,001–$50,000, 5 = $50,001–
$75,000, 6 = $75,001–$100,000, 7 = $100,001–$150,000, and
8 = over $150,000. The average rating was 6.17 (SD = 1.44);
that is, the average family income was between $75,000 and
$100,000. Family income was centered around the group
mean to reflect participants’ local income relative to other
group members.
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Sociometric Status and Subjective Well-Being 3
SWB. We measured all three main components of SWB
by administering the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS;
Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), in which respon-
dents provide a global, cognitive assessment of their life as a
whole, and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, which
measures the experience of positive and negative emotions
(Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). As in previous research
(Sheldon, King, Houser-Marko, Osbaldiston, & Gunz, 2007),
we averaged SWLS scores (α = .77, M = 5.38, SD = 0.94) with
Positive Affect (PA) scores (α = .89, M = 3.84, SD = 0.72) and
Negative Affect (NA) scores (α = .83, M = 1.80, SD = 0.53),
after reverse-scoring the latter. This SWB variable was also
centered around the group mean.
Results
Because gender and ethnicity (in particular, minority status)
sometimes predict sociometric status (Berger et al., 1980) and
SWB (Diener et al., 1999), we controlled for both gender and
ethnicity in all analyses.
In a simultaneous regression, we found that sociometric
status predicted SWB (β = 0.35; b = 0.33, SE = 0.10, p = .002),
whereas SES (family income rank, locally defined relative
to other group members) did not (β = 0.02; b = 0.01, SE = 0.06,
p = .85). SWB was not predicted by either gender (β = 0.05;
b = 0.08, SE = 0.15, p = .63) or ethnicity (White/non-White;
β = −0.04, b = −0.06, SE = 0.15, p = .72). (See the Supplemen-
tal Material available online for analyses of each of the indi-
vidual components of SWB in all four studies.)
To examine whether sociometric status predicted SWB
more strongly than did income, we compared the residual
from an unrestricted regression model, in which sociometric
status and income were entered into the regression separately,
with the residual from a restricted regression model, in which
the predictors were combined (i.e., sociometric status and
income were summed; Adler et al., 2000). The unrestricted
model had significantly less error variance than the restricted
model, F(1, 78) = 14.15, p < .001; thus, the relation between
sociometric status and SWB was stronger than the relation
between SES and SWB.
Study 2: Status and Well-Being in
a National Sample
The link between SES and SWB tends to be weakest in popu-
lations with the highest average levels of income (Howell &
Howell, 2008). Because Study 1 used a college-student sam-
ple with a high average income, the link between SES and
SWB might have been reduced. Therefore, in Study 2, we
examined a national online sample that had a wider range of
income, education, and backgrounds, which allowed us to test
whether the findings from Study 1 would generalize to a
broader population. We also examined the sense of power and
social acceptance as possible mediators. Finally, we controlled
for the personality trait of extraversion, which predicts both
sociometric status (Anderson et al., 2001) and SWB (Diener
et al., 1999), to rule out the possibility that extraversion might
have driven a spurious link between sociometric status and
SWB in Study 1.
Method
Participants. Participants were 315 individuals (36% male,
64% female; average age = 32.8 years, SD = 11.0) recruited
online from around the United States via Amazon’s Mechani-
cal Turk. Participants were asked to select all racial-ethnic cat-
egories to which they belonged; 74% selected White, 5%
selected African American, 6% selected Latino, 7% selected
Asian American, 9% selected Native American, and 10%
selected “other.”
Sociometric status. Participants rated the respect and admira-
tion they received in the three most important groups to which
they belonged (e.g., friends, family, work group). For each
group, participants indicated their agreement (1 = strongly dis-
agree, 7 = strongly agree) with four items: “I have a high
level of respect in others’ eyes,” “Others admire me,” “I have
high social standing,” and “Others look up to me.” Ratings on
these four items correlated with each other (average α = .94 in
the three groups). Furthermore, participants’ sociometric status
in the three groups was intercorrelated, α = .62, which indicated
that individuals had either consistently high or consistently low
sociometric status in their three groups. We therefore calculated
a combined sociometric-status score (average) for each partici-
pant across his or her groups (α = .62, M = 5.16, SD = 0.93).
SES. Total household income was measured as in Study 1. The
mean was 4.12 (SD = 1.94); that is, the average income was
between $35,001 and $50,000, which is consistent with the
mean U.S. income (DeNavas-Walt, Proctor, & Smith, 2010).
We measured education with an index used in previous studies
(e.g., Willer, 2009; M = 2.66, SD = 0.75). As in prior work
(Kraus, Piff, & Keltner, 2009), we standardized household
income and education and combined these scores to form an
overall measure of SES.
SWB. We measured SWB the same way as in Study 1: with
the SWLS (α = .92, M = 4.29, SD = 1.47), PANAS PA scale
(α = .90, M = 3.38, SD = 0.78), and PANAS NA scale (α = .91,
M = 2.08, SD = 0.82). As in Study 1, these three measures
were combined, after reverse-scoring NA.
Extraversion. We measured extraversion with the Big Five
Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991; M = 3.01, SD =
0.82, α = .88).
Personal sense of power. Participants reported their sense of
power in their relationships within each of their three groups
using the Sense of Power scale (Anderson et al., in press; aver-
age α = .90 in the three groups). Participants’ aggregate scores
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
4 Anderson et al.
correlated across the three groups, α = .54. Therefore, indi-
viduals who felt more (or less) powerful in one group tended
to feel more (or less) powerful in their other groups. The scores
for the three groups were averaged to form an overall measure
of the sense of power (M = 4.82, SD = 0.75).
Social acceptance. We based our measure of social accep-
tance on previous research (Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs,
1995). Participants rated how much they felt accepted,
included, liked, and welcomed by their fellow members in
each of their three groups (average α = .96 in the three groups).
Participants’ aggregate scores correlated across the three
groups, α = .57. Therefore, individuals who felt accepted in
one group tended to feel accepted in their other groups. The
scores for the three groups were averaged to form an overall
measure of acceptance (M = 5.80, SD = 0.79).
Results
As shown in Table 1, sociometric status predicted SWB, and
this relationship held even after controlling for SES, gender,
ethnicity (White/non-White), and extraversion. Moreover, as
in Study 1, sociometric status predicted SWB more strongly
than did SES, as the unrestricted model had less error variance
than the restricted model, F(1, 313) = 14.13, p < .001.
Mediation analyses demonstrated that sociometric status
predicted SWB through the indirect effects of sense of power
and social acceptance. Sociometric status predicted the sense
of power (β = 0.57; b = 0.47, SE = 0.04, p < .001), and when
sense of power and sociometric status were entered simultane-
ously as predictors of SWB, there was a drop in the effect of
sociometric status (Sobel z = 4.90, p < .001; see Model 3 in
Table 1). Sociometric status also predicted social acceptance
(β = 0.65; b = 0.56, SE = 0.04, p < .001), and when sociometric
status and social acceptance were entered simultaneously as
predictors of SWB, there was a drop in the effect of sociomet-
ric status (Sobel z = 5.89, p < .001; see Model 4 in Table 1).
Thus, individuals higher in sociometric status had higher SWB
because they felt a greater sense of power and greater accep-
tance in their groups.
Social status predicted SWB above and beyond the effect
of the personality dimension of extraversion. Where people
stand in their local hierarchy matters to their happiness.
Study 3: Experimental Manipulation
of Status
Our first two studies were correlational in design, and thus
limited the causal inferences we could draw about the relation-
ship between status and well-being. In Study 3, therefore, we
manipulated the subjective sense of status using a priming
technique in which participants compared themselves with
someone who had either high or low sociometric or socioeco-
nomic status (Kraus, Côté, & Keltner, 2010).
Method
Participants. Two-hundred twenty-eight participants (38%
male, 62% female) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical
Turk. Participants were asked to select all racial-ethnic catego-
ries to which they belonged; 72% selected White, 7% selected
African American, 6% selected Latino, 8% selected Asian
American, 5% selected Native American, and 7% selected
“other.”
Experimental manipulation. Participants were shown a lad-
der with 10 rungs (Kraus et al., 2010). In the sociometric-status
conditions, participants were told: “Think of the ladder above as
Table 1. Study 2: Results From Stepwise Regression Analyses Predicting Subjective Well-Being
Independent variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Sociometric status 0.43** (0.04) 0.30** (0.04) 0.15* (0.05) 0.09 (0.05)
Socioeconomic status 0.07 (0.05) 0.09 (0.05) 0.12* (0.04) 0.12* (0.04)
Gender (1 = male, 0 = female) −0.04 (0.08) −0.02 (0.07) −0.01 (0.07)
Ethnicity (1 = White, 0 = non-White) 0.11 (0.09) 0.07 (0.08) 0.07 (0.08)
Extraversion 0.41** (0.05) 0.35** (0.05) 0.36** (0.05)
Sense of power 0.33** (0.06)
Social acceptance 0.39** (0.06)
R2.250** .404** .456** .476**
ΔR2.153** .052** .073**
F test of model 50.22 40.33 41.49 45.05
Note: The table presents unstandardized regression coefficients, with standard errors in parentheses. ΔR2 for Models 3 and 4
was based on ΔR2 from Model 2.
*p < .01. **p < .001.
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Sociometric Status and Subjective Well-Being 5
representing where people stand in the important groups to
which they belong.” For participants in the high-sociometric-
status condition, the instructions continued as follows:
Now please compare yourself to the people at the very
bottom rung of the ladder. These are people who have
absolutely NO RESPECT, ADMIRATION, and INFLU-
ENCE in ALL of their important social groups. In par-
ticular, we’d like you to COMPARE YOURSELF TO
THESE PEOPLE in terms of your own respect, admira-
tion, and influence in your important groups.
Participants in the low-sociometric-status conditions received
the same instructions, except that they were asked to compare
themselves to the people at the top rung, and the instructions
said that such people have “A GREAT DEAL OF RESPECT,
ADMIRATION, and INFLUENCE” in their social groups.
Participants in the SES conditions were given similar instruc-
tions but compared themselves with someone with more or
less wealth, education, and job status. Following this prompt,
all participants were instructed to think of how “the similari-
ties and differences” between them and the comparison target
would affect a getting-acquainted interaction. As a manipula-
tion check, participants were asked, “Where would you place
yourself on this ladder relative to these people on the very bot-
tom [top] rung?” Responses were made on a scale from 1 (bot-
tom rung) to 10 (top rung).
SWB. We again measured SWB with the SWLS (α = .91, M =
4.28, SD = 1.45), PANAS PA scale (α = .91, M = 2.92, SD =
0.83), and PANAS NA scale (α = .91, M = 1.56, SD = 0.73). In
this study, participants were asked the extent to which they
were feeling each emotion on the PANAS at that time. We
computed overall SWB as in the previous studies.
Results
Manipulation check. A 2 (level: high, low) × 2 (type of sta-
tus: sociometric, socioeconomic) between-participants analy-
sis of variance (ANOVA) showed that participants in the
high-status conditions (M = 6.23, SD = 1.99) reported higher
status than participants in the low-status conditions (M = 5.19,
SD = 1.85), F(1, 224) = 16.39, p < .001. There was no interac-
tion between level and type of status, F(1, 224) = 1.38, p = .24.
This suggests that the sociometric and socioeconomic manipu-
lations were equally effective.
SWB. We next submitted SWB scores to a 2 (level: high, low)
× 2 (type of status: sociometric, socioeconomic) between-
participants ANOVA. There was a main effect of level, F(1,
224) = 5.06, p = .03, but more important, there was also a sig-
nificant interaction between level and type of status, F(1, 224) =
4.73, p = .03 (see Fig. 1). Individuals in the high-sociometric-
status condition had higher SWB than those in the low-
sociometric-status condition, t(115) = 3.05, p = .003. In contrast,
individuals in the high-SES condition did not have higher SWB
than those in the low-SES condition, t(109) = 0.06, p = .96.
Therefore, these findings provide evidence for a causal effect of
sociometric status on SWB that is stronger than the effect of
SES on SWB.1
Study 4: Longitudinal Assessment of
Changes in Status
Study 4 used a longitudinal design to examine whether changes
in sociometric status following a major life transition predict
corresponding changes in SWB. That is, when individuals’
sociometric status rises or falls after a significant life transi-
tion, does their SWB rise or fall accordingly?
To answer this question, we assessed Master of Business
Administration (M.B.A.) students a month before they gradu-
ated (Time 1) and then 9 months after graduation (Time 2).
Graduating from an M.B.A. program involves moving from
one important sociometric-status hierarchy (the cohort of
M.B.A. classmates) to another (typically, the workplace).
Such a move could involve an increase or decrease in socio-
metric status and, we predicted, systematic changes in SWB.
Method
Participants. One hundred fifty-six M.B.A. students partici-
pated at Time 1. Of those, 116 (74%) participated at Time 2
(71% male, 29% female; average age = 30.63 years, SD =
2.71). Participants were asked to select all racial-ethnic cate-
gories to which they belonged; 50% selected White, 1%
selected African American, 6% selected Latino, 37% selected
Asian American, and 11% selected “other.” We focused on
participants who were assessed at both times. These partici-
pants did not differ on any dimension from those who com-
pleted only the first assessment.
Sociometric status. At Time 1, participants rated their socio-
metric status in their M.B.A. cohort with the same five self-
report items used in Study 1 (α = .94, M = 4.63, SD = 1.02). At
–0.30
–0.20
–0.10
0.00
0.10
0.20
0.30
Sociometric
Status
Socioeconomic
Status
Subjective Well-Being
Low High
Fig. 1. Results from Study 3: subjective well-being as a function of condition
(high vs. low sociometric status, high vs. low socioeconomic status).
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
6 Anderson et al.
Time 2, participants indicated their agreement with the same
items, but with respect to their workplace, or their most impor-
tant group if they were unemployed (α = .94, M = 5.16, SD =
0.98). Because none of the significant results changed depend-
ing on whether we included or excluded the few participants
who were unemployed at Time 2, we report analyses including
those participants.
Income. As in Study 1, we focused on participants’ total
household income, which was reported using the same scale as
in that study. At Time 1, the mean income was 4.89 (SD =
2.82), which indicated an average income between $35,001
and $50,000. At Time 2, the mean was 6.89 (SD = 1.46), which
indicated an average income between $75,001 and $100,000.
SWB. For each time of assessment, we standardized the SWLS
scores (Time 1: α = .91, M = 5.07, SD = 1.32; Time 2: α = .89,
M = 5.12, SD = 1.20), PANAS PA scores (Time 1: α = .88, M =
3.68, SD = 0.64; Time 2: α = .90, M = 3.67, SD = 0.65), and
PANAS NA scores (reverse-scored; Time 1: α = .85, M = 1.88,
SD = 0.58; Time 2: α = .86, M = 1.78, SD = 0.59). These stan-
dardized scores were averaged to create an SWB measure for
each time of assessment.
Results
As shown in Table 2, Time 2 sociometric status predicted Time
2 SWB. This relationship held up even after controlling for
Time 1 sociometric status, Time 1 SWB, Time 1 SES, Time 2
SES, gender, and ethnicity (White/non-White; see Model 4).
Therefore, as M.B.A. students’ sociometric status rose or fell
after they graduated, their SWB rose or fell accordingly.
We also used a difference-score approach (Allison, 1990)
to test our hypothesis. This analysis showed that changes in
sociometric status from Time 1 to Time 2 predicted changes in
SWB from Time 1 to Time 2 (β = 0.22; b = 0.14, SE = 0.06,
p = .02).
Furthermore, Time 2 sociometric status predicted Time 2
SWB more strongly than did Time 2 SES, as the unrestricted
model had less error variance than the restricted model, F(1,
154) = 20.17, p < .001. Together, the findings from Study 4
suggest that as M.B.A. students’ sociometric status rose or fell
after they graduated, their SWB rose or fell accordingly. More-
over, with this longitudinal design, we were able to establish
that changes in sociometric status predicted changes in SWB
more strongly than did changes in SES.
Discussion
Four studies, triangulating on our research question using cor-
relational, experimental, and longitudinal designs, found con-
sistent evidence for a local-ladder effect: Increases in
sociometric status were associated with rises in SWB. These
findings were robust regardless of whether we measured
sociometric status with peer or self-ratings, and held up after
controlling for the possible confounding variables of gender,
ethnicity, and extraversion. Individuals higher in sociometric
status experienced elevated SWB because they felt more pow-
erful and more accepted in their social groups. Occupying a
higher position in the local ladder thus created a sense of influ-
ence and control over the social environment, as well as a
sense of belonging and acceptance.
Our findings suggest that possessing high status is more
important to SWB than some prior scholarship has sug-
gested. However, not all forms of status affect SWB equally.
Individuals’ sociometric status in their local, face-to-face
groups predicted SWB more strongly than did SES. These
findings dovetail nicely with work based on social identity
theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), which suggests that variables
related to sociometric status, such as prototypicality within
Table 2. Study 4: Results From Stepwise Regression Analyses Predicting Subjective Well-Being (SWB) at Time 2
Independent variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Sociometric status at Time 2 0.55** (0.09) 0.37** (0.08) 0.38** (0.09) 0.35** (0.09)
Sociometric status at Time 1 −0.08 (0.09) −0.07 (0.10) −0.06 (0.10)
SWB at Time 1 0.49** (0.10) 0.46** (0.10) 0.44** (0.11)
Socioeconomic status at Time 1 −0.03 (0.03) −0.03 (0.03)
Socioeconomic status at Time 2 0.05 (0.08) 0.06 (0.08)
Gender (1 = male, 0 = female) −0.24 (0.19)
Ethnicity (1 = White, 0 = non-White) 0.24 (0.55)
R2.451** .643** .651** .666**
ΔR2.192** .008 .016
F test of model 39.39 27.60 16.39 11.98
Note: The table presents unstandardized regression coefficients, with standard errors in parentheses. ΔR2 for Models 3 and 4 was based on
ΔR2 from Model 2.
**p < .001.
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Sociometric Status and Subjective Well-Being 7
a group, affect self-esteem and one’s sense of meaning,
purpose, and belonging (e.g., Haslam, Jetten, Postmes, &
Haslam, 2009).
Future studies should continue to explore why sociometric
status has a stronger effect on SWB than does SES. One pos-
sibility is that although individuals adapt to their income or
education (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman, 1978), they
might not adapt in the same way to their sociometric status.
The joy that comes with an influx of money wanes quickly as
people become accustomed to how wealth shapes their daily
lives. Yet respect and admiration from one’s face-to-face
groups might bring sustained SWB. Moreover, future work
should examine potential moderators of the link between
sociometric status and SWB. For example, related research
suggests that one’s anticipated future sociometric status
(Jetten, Branscombe, & Spears, 2002) and the sociometric sta-
tus of one’s group relative to other groups (Sani, Magrin, Scri-
gnaro, & McCollum, 2010) might moderate the effects of
one’s current sociometric status.
It is interesting to speculate about the evolutionary origins of
the association between sociometric status and SWB. The find-
ing that elevated status is highly correlated with reproductive
success and SWB in chimpanzees (Weiss, King, & Enns, 2002),
humans’ close primate relatives, parallels the local-ladder effect
we observed. In hominid predecessors, the capacity to enjoy
elevated status in small face-to-face groups was also likely asso-
ciated with greater survival rates and reproductive success
(Buss, 1999). Thus, sociometric status might have become
intrinsically rewarding over humans’ evolutionary history.
Other research has shown that individuals who place more
importance on attaining outcomes related to social status—
such as power, control, and prominence (Kasser & Ryan,
1993, 1996)—exhibit lower SWB than individuals who placed
less importance on those outcomes. Thus, our findings suggest
that whereas longing for status might dampen SWB, possess-
ing status (at least, sociometric status) bolsters SWB (see Gru-
ber, Mauss, & Tamir, 2011).
In sum, the current research highlights the importance of
local status hierarchies to happiness. Individuals’ standing in
their local ladders of respect—their friendship, workplace, or
neighborhood groups—has a strong impact on their life satis-
faction and the degree to which they experience positive and
negative emotion. The respect one commands locally shapes
how one feels globally.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss.sagepub
.com/content/by/supplemental-data
Note
1. We do not believe that demand effects drove the findings in Study
3 for two reasons. First, if demand characteristics were at play, one
would expect the effects for the SES manipulation to have been even
stronger than those for the sociometric-status manipulation. People
tend to believe that if they had more money, they would be happier
(Wilson & Gilbert, 2003), yet there are no documented lay beliefs
about sociometric status and SWB. Second, we asked all participants,
“What ideas or hypotheses do you think the researchers in this
experiment were attempting to study?” No participants correctly
guessed the study’s hypotheses (i.e., that sociometric status affects
SWB and that it has a stronger effect on SWB than does SES).
References
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000).
Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psy-
chological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in
healthy, White women. Health Psychology, 19, 586–592.
Allison, P. D. (1990). Change scores as dependent variables in regres-
sion analysis. In C. C. Clogg (Ed.), Sociological methodology
(pp. 93–114). Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Anderson, C., John, O. P., & Keltner, D. (in press). The personal
sense of power. Journal of Personality.
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who
attains social status? Effects of personality traits and physical
attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81, 116–132.
Bales, R. F., Strodtbeck, F. L., Mills, T. M., & Roseborough, M. E.
(1951). Channels of communication in small groups. American
Sociological Review, 16, 461–468.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva-
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117 , 497–529.
Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M. (1980). Status organizing
processes. Annual Review of Sociology, 6, 479–508.
Blau, P. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York, NY:
Wiley.
Boyce, C. J., Brown, G. D. A., & Moore, S. C. (2010). Money and
happiness: Rank of income, not income, affects life satisfaction.
Psychological Science, 21, 471–475.
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery win-
ners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927.
Buss, D. M. (1999). Human nature and individual differences: The
evolution of human personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.,
pp. 31–56). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
DeNavas-Walt, C., Proctor, B. D., & Smith, J. C. (2010). Income,
poverty, and health insurance coverage in the United States:
2009 (Current Population Report). Washington, DC: Census
Bureau.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment,
49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. E. (1999). Subjec-
tive well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bul-
letin, 125, 276–302.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Relations, 7, 117–140.
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
8 Anderson et al.
Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2011). A dark side of happi-
ness? How, when, and why happiness is not always good. Per-
spectives on Psychological Science, 6, 222–233.
Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & Haslam, C. (2009). Social
identity, health, and well-being: An emerging agenda for applied
psychology. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 58,
1–23.
Howell, R. T., & Howell, C. J. (2008). The relation of economic
status to subjective well-being in developing countries: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 536–560.
Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., & Spears, R. (2002). On being
peripheral: Effects of identity security on personal and col-
lective self-esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology,
32, 105–123.
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. (1991). The “Big Five”
Inventory: Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of Califor-
nia, Institute of Personality and Social Research.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream:
Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American
dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 281–288.
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power,
approach and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–
284.
Kenny, D. A., & LaVoie, L. (1984). The social relations model. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 18, pp. 139–180). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextu-
alism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–
1723.
Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2009). Signs of socioeconomic sta-
tus: A thin-slicing approach. Psychological Science, 20, 99–
106.
Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., & Keltner, D. (2009). Social class, the sense
of control, and social explanation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 97, 992–1004.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995).
Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypoth-
esis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518–530.
Russell, B. (1930). The conquest of happiness. London, England:
George Allen & Unwin.
Sani, F., Magrin, M. E., Scrignaro, M., & McCollum, R. (2010).
Ingroup identification mediates the effects of subjective ingroup
status on mental health. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49,
883–893.
Sheldon, K. M., King, L. A., Houser-Marko, L., Osbaldiston, R., &
Gunz, A. (2007). Comparing IAT and TAT measures of power
versus intimacy motivation. European Journal of Personality,
21, 263–280.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup
conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology
of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Thibault, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of
groups. New York, NY: Wiley.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 1063–1070.
Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Enns, R. M. (2002). Subjective well-being is
heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpan-
zees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 83, 1141–1149.
Willer, R. (2009). Groups reward individual sacrifice: The status
solution to the collective action problem. American Sociological
Review, 74, 23–43.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In
M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(Vol. 35, pp. 345–411). New York, NY: Elsevier.
by Michael Kraus on June 4, 2012pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from