ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

In this article, the authors review basic conceptual issues in research on children’s emotion-related self-regulation, including the differentiation between self-regulation that is effortful and voluntary and control-related processes that are less amenable to effortful control. In addition, the authors summarize what researchers know about developmental changes in self-regulatory capacities, give examples of various methods used to assess these abilities, and summarize findings on the relations between self-regulation or effortful control and positive adjustment and maladjustment. Finally, the authors discuss some strategies for effectively teaching students about emotion regulation.
Emotion-Related Self-Regulation in Children
Nancy Eisenberg1 and Michael J. Sulik1
1Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Abstract
In this article, the authors review basic conceptual issues in research on children’s emotion-related
self-regulation, including the differentiation between self-regulation that is effortful and voluntary
and control-related processes that are less amenable to effortful control. In addition, the authors
summarize what researchers know about developmental changes in self-regulatory capacities, give
examples of various methods used to assess these abilities, and summarize findings on the
relations between self-regulation or effortful control and positive adjustment and maladjustment.
Finally, the authors discuss some strategies for effectively teaching students about emotion
regulation.
Keywords
self-regulation; effortful control; emotion
Developmental psychologists study change in most processes of interest to psychologists,
including processes involved in social, emotional, perceptual, and cognitive systems. For
decades, behavioral and social-learning approaches to understanding socioemotional
development dominated the field. Until the 1980s, developmental psychologists did not
focus much on emotion and emotion regulation. By the 1990s, however, the study of
emotion and its regulation had become quite popular, and today it is common to find articles
on emotion regulation in developmental psychology journals. In particular, many
investigators have been interested in how people regulate their emotion and behavior related
to emotion—what Eisenberg and Spinrad (2004) labeled emotion-related self-regulation.
Controversies About the Definition and Purview of Emotion Regulation
Eisenberg, Hofer, and Vaughan (2007) defined emotion-related regulation as “processes
used to manage and change if, when, and how (e.g., how intensely) one experiences
emotions and emotion-related motivational and physiological states, as well as how
emotions are expressed behaviorally” (p. 288). Investigators, however, differ in regard to
what processes they include in emotion regulation. For example, there is disagreement
regarding whether the same term should include both extrinsic processes (e.g., parents’
helping children to manage their emotions; see Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004; Thompson,
1994) and intrinsic processes (e.g., attention, inhibition, cognitions; Eisenberg & Spinrad,
2004) that modulate emotions and emotion-related behavior. Clearly, modulation from both
external and internal sources is important for children’s experience of emotion and related
© The Author(s) 2012
Corresponding Author: Nancy Eisenberg, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104,
Nancy.Eisenberg@asu.edu.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
Published in final edited form as:
Teach Psychol
. 2012 January 1; 39(1): 77–83. doi:10.1177/0098628311430172.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
behavior. Nonetheless, Eisenberg and Spinrad (2004) argued that it is useful to differentiate
between regulation that comes from outside the self and self-regulation that involves
intrinsic processes. It is difficult to differentiate emotion from its regulation (Cole et al.,
2004), and researchers differ in the degree to which they are willing to use levels of emotion
displayed by individuals as indices of emotion regulation. Some investigators consider, for
example, parents’ reports of their children displaying low levels of anger as indicative of
being well regulated; others would say that the child who is low in anger simply may not
feel the emotion much.
Because of the difficulty differentiating emotion from its regulation, investigators often find
it useful to focus on the processes involved in emotion regulation rather than on the amount
of emotion experienced or expressed. Some capacities involved in emotion-related
regulation appear to have a temperamental basis. Rothbart and Bates (2006) labeled
temperamental self-regulatory capacities as effortful control, defined as “the efficiency of
executive attention—including the ability to inhibit a dominant response and/or to activate a
subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors” (p. 129). Effortful control includes the
abilities to shift and focus attention as needed, to inhibit behavior when needed (i.e.,
inhibitory control), to activate or perform an action when there is a tendency to avoid it (i.e.,
activation control; Evans & Rothbart, 2007; Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001), and
some executive functioning skills involved in integrating information and planning.
People who are able to regulate their emotional reactivity in social or nonsocial contexts
deal well with stressful events. For instance, shifting attention away from a distressing
stimulus (e.g., a horror film) or a tempting object can decrease arousal. Focusing attention
on positive aspects of the situation or on other means of coping also can decrease negative
emotion. Moreover, focusing attention on nonthreatening ideas or objects can be a
distraction from a distressing event or cognition. Consider Mischel’s (1974) classic delay-
of-gratification studies, in which he videotaped children who could choose a smaller treat
that was right in front of them or wait for an experimenter to bring back a larger treat.
Mischel found that some children tried to distract themselves by talking or singing. Others
kept their attention on the rewards. The children who shifted their attention away from the
treat were better able to delay gratification than the children who attended to the treat.
Presumably, this self-distraction lessened children’s desire for attractive items, which
enhanced their ability to delay gratification. Similarly, the abilities to shift and refocus
attention have been associated with lower levels of distress, frustration, and other negative
emotions (Bridges & Grolnick, 1995; Derryberry & Rothbart, 1988; Eisenberg et al., 1993;
Rothbart, Ziaie, & O’Boyle, 1992).
Many processes involved in the modulation of emotion are typically automatic and may be
difficult to control consciously. Thus, some researchers have viewed emotion regulation as
spanning a continuum from conscious, effortful, and controlled regulation to unconscious,
effortless, and automatic regulation (Gross & Thompson, 2007). Although automatic,
unconscious, or uncontrolled responding to emotion-relevant stimuli or cognitions may be
more reliable and effective than conscious responses (Bargh & Williams, 2007), Eisenberg
and colleagues (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2004), like some coping theorists (Compas, Connor,
Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, 2001), have argued that it is useful to differentiate
willful or effortful forms of self-control from those aspects of control that are less amenable
to willful control. Eisenberg et al. (2004) thus suggested a distinction between effortful
control and reactive control.
The primary distinction between these constructs is that effortful control, although often
automatic, can be subject to conscious control. For example, people often drive their cars in
a fairly automatic mode. But when they need to be conscious and careful about their driving
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 2
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
because of bad weather, bad drivers, or some other factor, they can switch into a very
conscious and willful mode of action. The same is likely true with many processes used to
regulate attention, behavior, and emotion. In contrast, reactive control refers to relatively
involuntary motivational approach or avoidance systems of response reactivity that, at the
extremes, reflect impulsive undercontrol or rigid overcontrol (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2004;
Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; also see Carver’s, 2005, discussion of impulse vs. constraint).
Consistent with the distinction between effortful and reactive control, the two constructs can
be differentiated empirically (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2004; Valiente et al., 2003). There is
ongoing debate in the field, however, about how to parse out and label these various control
processes.
Development of Self-Regulation and Temperamentally Based Effortful
Control
For a long time, developmental psychologists assumed that young children had little
capacity for self-regulation. Yet researchers are increasingly identifying early indicators of
rudimentary self-regulation or precursors of effortful control and have found evidence that
self-regulatory processes continue to develop through adolescence.
Early in life, caregivers help infants to regulate their emotion by modulating their exposure
to stimulating events and by acting to alleviate infants’ distress, frustration, or fear with
techniques such as soothing or distraction. But the ability to regulate one’s own behavior
develops rapidly in the toddler and preschool years (Kopp, 1982; Kopp & Neufeld, 2003;
Rothbart & Bates, 2006), and infants and preschoolers improve in aspects of executive
functioning that are part of, or related to, effortful control (see Garon, Bryson, & Smith,
2008). For example, infants demonstrate the capacity to sustain attention by 8 to 10 months
of age (Kochanska, Coy, Tjebkes, & Husarek, 1998), and between 9 and 18 months of age,
attention becomes more voluntary (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). Moreover, on a Stroop-like task
that required toddlers to switch attention and inhibit behavior, Posner and Rothbart (1998)
found improvement in performance by 30 months and high levels of accuracy by 36 to 38
months.
The ability to inhibit behavior when a child does not want to do so (inhibitory control) is
typically absent until 24 to 36 months of age (Gerardi-Caulton, 2000). Kochanska, Murray,
and Harlan (2000) found significant performance gains on tasks involving inhibitory control
between 22 to 33 months of age. Other researchers have shown that the ability to inhibit
behavior on tasks similar to “Simon says” (when children must inhibit behavior in response
to one command or cue and activate behavior in response to another) improves between 3
and 4 years (Jones, Rothbart, & Posner, 2003; Posner & Rothbart, 1998; Reed, Pien, &
Rothbart, 1984). Increases in the length of time children can wait for a treat are also evident
from 24 months to 4 years (Carlson, 2005; Kochanska et al., 2000; Li-Grining, 2007).
There are further improvements in effortful control and executive functioning in the late
preschool and early school years. For example, Diamond and colleagues (Diamond, Prevor,
Callender, & Druin, 1997; also see Diamond & Taylor, 1996) found improvement in
children’s responses to the day– night task between 3.5 and 7 years; on this task, children
say “day” when the adult holds up a black card with stars and “night” when the adult holds
up a white card with a bright sun. Carlson (2005) also found an increase in performance
between 3 and 5 years for the majority of 17 executive functioning tasks (also see
Mezzacappa, 2004). Effortful control continues to improve during the school years and, at a
slower pace, into adulthood (Crone, Somsen, Zanolie, & van der Molen, 2006; Leon-
Carrion, García-Orza, & Pérez-Santamaría, 2004; Murphy, Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, &
Guthrie, 1999; Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999).
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 3
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Measurement of Emotion-Related Self-Regulation, Including Effortful
Control
The study of emotion-related self-regulation in childhood has been characterized by
multireporter, multimethod approaches. Attention tomeasurement issues can help students
understand the nature of the skills involved in emotion-related self-regulation.
A variety of excellent questionnaires are available to measure effortful control and emotion
regulation (Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006; Shields &
Cicchetti, 2001; Windle & Lerner, 1986). For example, the Child Behavior Questionnaire
(Rothbart et al., 2001) assesses multiple aspects of effortful control, including attention
focusing (e.g., “When drawing or coloring in a book, shows strong concentration”) and
inhibitory control (e.g., “Can lower his/her voice when asked to do so”). In addition,
researchers have used a variety of behavioral measures that assess effortful control and some
aspects of executive functioning involved in emotion-related self-regulation. Tasks include
the day–night executive functioning task, which we discussed earlier (see Garon et al.,
2008), and Kochanska et al.’s (2000) comprehensive battery of tasks designed to measure
five components of effortful control: delaying, slowing down motor activity, suppressing or
initiating activity to signal, lowering one’s voice, and effortful attention. For example,
children may have to wait to take an attractive snack until a bell rings; they might also play a
Simon-says-type task, in which they must inhibit or activate behaviors based on different
verbal commands (Kochanska, Aksan, Prisco, & Adams, 2008).
An emerging trend is to measure self-regulatory processes with physiological measures.
Although psychophysiological variables provide a different perspective on regulation than
other methods, it is not yet clear the extent to which psychophysiological responses tap
processes involved in emotion regulation. For instance, physiological responses often appear
to tap some combination of reactivity and regulation. Further complicating this issue, there
are a number of ways people can regulate emotions (Gross, 1998), including cognitive
strategies (e.g., distraction) and response modulation (e.g., trying to control behavioral
responses), and they may differ in which physiological responses are associated with them.
Here we focus on parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) function as an example of how
researchers can use psychophysiological data to understand emotional reactivity and its
regulation and to illustrate the complexity of the findings.
The sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and PNS jointly enervate many of the body’s organs
and have opposing effects. For example, SNS activity increases heart rate, whereas the PNS
decreases heart rate. When faced with a stressful situation, SNS activity increases and PNS
activity decreases, resulting in rapid physiological changes that are collectively known as the
fight–flight response. PNS influence on the heart waxes and wanes with respiration,
decreasing during inspiration and increasing during expiration. This results in a rhythmic
pattern of rising and falling heart rate called respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) that is
sometimes also referred to as vagal tone because the vagus nerve primarily mediates PNS
influence on the heart (Porges, 2007). Students can verify the presence of this pattern of
accelerating and decelerating heart rate by taking their pulse while breathing slowly.
RSA measured under resting (baseline) conditions seems to reflect dispositional levels of
PNS activity (El-Sheikh, 2005). Researchers have also examined RSA changes in response
to a stimulus or task. Sustaining attention and responding to stress typically elicit a decrease
in RSA from baseline; this decrease is referred to as RSA suppression.
Because low baseline RSA is associated with fight–flight behavior, high RSA may reflect
self-regulation. Some studies have found a positive relation between baseline RSA and
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 4
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
attention regulation (Hansen, Johnsen, & Thayer, 2003; Suess, Porges, & Plude, 1994). In
contrast to baseline RSA, some have conceptualized RSA suppression as an index of the
ability to engage with the environment. As such, the inability to suppress RSA in response to
situations that demand attention may indicate self-regulatory problems (Porges, Doussard-
Roosevelt, Portales, & Greenspan, 1996). Infants who sustain attention to a stimulus show
greater RSA suppression (Richards, 1985). Infants and childrenwith highRSAsuppression
are also rated by theirmothers as having higher attention (Huffman et al., 1998; Richards,
1987) and emotion regulation (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996). Children with greater RSA
suppression to cognitively and emotionally challenging tasks have higher status with peers
(Graziano, Keane, & Calkins, 2007). Moreover, high RSA suppression has been associated
with lower levels of externalizing problems (Boyce et al., 2001; Calkins, Graziano, &
Keane, 2007), although the findings are not consistent and may change with age
(Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007).
These studies suggest that RSA suppression indexes the capacity for self-regulation. But
perseverative cognitions (i.e., rumination) that reflect excessive worry about stressors may
indicate a risk for anxiety (Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer, 2006), and worry about stressors is
associated with suppression of RSA (Brosschot, Van Dijk, & Thayer, 2007). Moreover, high
RSA suppression has been associated with elevated internalizing problems (Calkins et al.,
2007). The context in which researchers measure RSA suppression may play an important
role in whether it reflects the adaptive ability to regulate attention as needed to engage with
the environment or the inability to disengage one’s attention and cognition from stressful
circumstances.
The Relation of Emotion-Relevant Regulation to Quality of Social
Functioning
There is a rapidly accumulating body of literature indicating that individual differences in
the regulation of emotion and behavior driven by emotion are linked to variations in social
competence and maladjustment. For example, there is strong evidence that individual
differences in self-regulation are negatively associated with externalizing problems (e.g.,
aggression, defiance; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Rothbart & Bates, 2006) and also predict
change in externalizing problems over time (Eisenberg et al., 2004). In addition, effortful
control may be a stronger predictor of externalizing problems for children prone to
experience negative emotions (Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 2000; see Eisenberg, Spinrad, &
Eggum, 2010). Furthermore, although the data are mixed, there is also evidence that some
aspects of emotion-related regulation and/or effortful control—especially skills involving
effortful attention—are associated with low levels of internalizing problems (see Eisenberg,
Spinrad, et al., 2010). Individual differences in emotion-relevant regulation also have been
associated with social competence, conscience, sympathy, and prosocial behavior
(Eisenberg et al., 1993; Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, et al., 1996; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003;
Rydell, Berlin, & Bohlin, 2003; Spinrad et al., 2006; Spinrad et al., 2007; also see
Eisenberg, Eggum, Sallquist,& Edwards, 2010). Again, such relations may be stronger for
children prone to negative emotions (Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie,& Reiser, 2000; Eisenberg,
Fabes, Murphy, et al., 1996). In general, measures of self-regulation, including effortful
control, have been consistently related to better adjustment and less maladjustment in
childhood and adolescence. Associations such as these ensure that the current interest in
emotion-related self-regulatory processes will not abate quickly.
How to Teach About Emotion Regulation
We suggest that teachers help students understand the nature of emotion-related self-
regulation by having students try simple questionnaire and behavioral measures on
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 5
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
themselves, friends, or family members. Students can discuss what processes are being
regulated in each task and how that type of regulation might affect feelings and their
expression and/or social behavior. Information on Kochanska’s tasks (e.g., Kochanska et al.,
2000; Kochanska et al., 2008) can be obtained from her at Grazyna-kochanska@uiowa.edu.
Rothbart and colleagues’ questionnaires (e.g., Rothbart et al., 2001) are available from
Samuel Putnam at http://www.bowdoin.edu/~sputnam/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/
request-forms/.
Typical self-regulation tasks tap skills associated with executive attention (e.g., the ability to
shift and focus attention), inhibiting behavior, delaying gratification (as described above),
and modulating the expression of emotion. Attention regulation can be measured using tasks
such as the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), which is computerized. The CPT presents
a number of images to the participant, who then presses a key when a particular image
appears on the screen. Interested persons can obtain an example of a CPT program by
emailing the first author at Nancy.Eisenberg@asu.edu. As another example, the Attention
Network Test (ANT; Fan, McCandless, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002) tests three aspects of
attention: orienting, alerting, and executive function. The ANT computer programis
available at http://www.sacklerinstitute.org/ users/jin.fan/.
Examples of measures of inhibitory control include games similar to Simon says
(Kochanska et al., 2008) as well as executive function tests such as the knock–tap task
(Luria, 1966), in which participants first imitate the experimenter’s actions (knocking on a
table with a closed fist when the experimenter knocks and tapping with an open palm when
the experimenter taps) and then perform tricky trials (knocking when the experimenter taps
and tapping when the experimenter knocks). Stroop tasks, such as the day–night task
(Diamond & Taylor, 1996), are also measures of inhibitory control.
Delay tasks measure a child’s ability to inhibit responding in the face of temptation. One
example is Mischel’s “marshmallow” delay-of-gratification task (e.g., Mischel, Ebbesen, &
Zeiss, 1972), which we mentioned earlier and for which clips are widely available online,
but there are numerous adaptations. For instance, in the gift-wrap task (Kochanska et al.,
2000), the experimenter noisily wraps a gift for 2 minutes behind a child, who is asked not
to peek.
Persistence tasks measure activational control, which is the ability to persist at a difficult or
unpleasant task. Examples of this kind of task include persisting at a nearly impossible,
frustrating task (e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, et al., 1996) and persisting on unsolvable
puzzles (e.g., Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998).
Summary
In summary, self-regulation skills are basic skills that develop from the early years into
adulthood. They predict numerous aspects of social competence and maladjustment. Thus,
self-regulation is an important construct for understanding processes that contribute to both
normative development and individual differences in diverse aspects of functioning.
Acknowledgments
Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article:
Writing of this article was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National
Institute of Child Health and Development.
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 6
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Biographies
References
Bargh, JA.; Williams, LE. The nonconscious regulation of emotion. In: Gross, JJ., editor. The
handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford; 2007. p. 429-445.
Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited
resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998; 74:1252–1265. [PubMed: 9599441]
Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology:
Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological
Psychology. 2007; 74:174–184. [PubMed: 17045726]
Boyce WT, Quas J, Alkon A, Smider NA, Essex MJ, Kupfer DJ. MacArthur Assessment Battery
Working Group of the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopathology and
Development. Autonomic reactivity and psychopathology in middle childhood. British Journal of
Psychiatry. 2001; 179:144–150. [PubMed: 11483476]
Bridges, LJ.; Grolnick, WS. The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early
childhood. In: Eisenberg, N., editor. Review of personality and psychology. Vol. Vol. 15. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage; 1995. p. 185-211.
Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF. The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry,
prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research.
2006; 60:113–124. [PubMed: 16439263]
Brosschot JF, Van Dijk E, Thayer JF. Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability during waking
and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007; 63:39–
47. [PubMed: 17020787]
Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Keane SP. Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for
behavior problems. Biological Psychology. 2007; 74:144–153. [PubMed: 17055141]
Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children.
Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005; 28:595–616. [PubMed: 16144429]
Carver CS. Impulse and constraint: Perspectives from personality psychology, convergence with
theory in other areas, and potential for integration. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
2005; 9:312–333. [PubMed: 16223354]
Cole PM, Martin SE, Dennis TA. Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological
challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development. 2004; 75:317–333.
[PubMed: 15056186]
Compas BE, Connor JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME. Coping with stress during
childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research.
Psychological Bulletin. 2001; 127:87–127. [PubMed: 11271757]
Crone EA, Somsen RJ, Zanolie K, van der Molen MW. A heart rate analysis of developmental change
in feedback processing and rule shifting from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology. 2006; 95:99–116. [PubMed: 16674970]
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 7
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Derryberry D, Rothbart MK. Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 55:958–966. [PubMed: 3216290]
Diamond A, Prevor M, Callender G, Druin D. Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated
early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development.
1997; 62:1–208.
Diamond A, Taylor C. Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to
remember what I said and to “do as I say not as I do.”. Developmental Psychobiology. 1996;
29:315–334. [PubMed: 8732806]
Eisenberg, N.; Eggum, ND.; Sallquist, J.; Edwards, A. Relations of self-regulatory/control capacities to
maladjustment, social competence, and emotionality. In: Hoyle, RH., editor. Handbook of
personality and self-regulation. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell; 2010. p. 21-46.
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Bernzweig J, Karbon M, Poulin R, Hanish L. The relations of emotionality
and regulation to preschoolers’ social skills and sociometric status. Child Development. 1993;
64:1418–1438. [PubMed: 8222881]
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Guthrie IK, Murphy BC, Maszk P, Holmgren R, Suh K. The relations of
regulation and emotionality to problem behavior in elementary school children. Development and
Psychopathology. 1996; 8:141–162.
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Guthrie IK, Reiser M. Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in
predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;
78:136–157. [PubMed: 10653511]
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Karbon M, Murphy B, Wosinski M, Polazzi L, Juhnke C. The relations of
children’s dispositional prosocial behavior to emotionality, regulation, and social functioning.
Child Development. 1996; 67:947–992.
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Murphy B, Karbon M, Smith M, Maszk P. The relations of children’s
dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning.
Developmental Psychology. 1996; 32:195–209.
Eisenberg, N.; Hofer, C.; Vaughan, J. Effortful control and its socioemotional consequences. In: Gross,
JJ., editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford; 2007. p. 287-306.
Eisenberg, N.; Morris, AS. Children’s emotion-related regulation. In: Kail, R., editor. Advances in
child development and behavior. Vol. Vol. 30. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic; 2002. p.
190-229.
Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition. Child Development.
2004; 75:334–339. [PubMed: 15056187]
Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s
maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010; 6:495–525.
Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Reiser M, Cumberland A, Shepard SA, Thompson M. The
relations of effortful control and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child
Development. 2004; 75:25–46. [PubMed: 15015673]
El-Sheikh M. Stability of respiratory sinus arrhythmia in children and young adolescents: A
longitudinal examination. Developmental Psychobiology. 2005; 46:66–74. [PubMed: 15690389]
Evans D, Rothbart MK. Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in
Personality. 2007; 41:868–888.
Fan J, McCandless BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of
attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002; 14:340–347. [PubMed: 11970796]
Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative
framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008; 134:31–60. [PubMed: 18193994]
Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior
questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development. 2003; 26:64–86.
Gerardi-Caulton G. Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children
24–36 months of age. Developmental Science. 2000; 3:397–404.
Gottman JM, Katz LS, Hooven C. Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of
families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology. 1996; 10:243–
268.
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 8
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Graziano PA, Keane SP, Calkins SD. Cardiac vagal regulation and early peer status. Child
Development. 2007; 78:264–278. [PubMed: 17328704]
Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General
Psychology. 1998; 2:271–299.
Gross, JJ.; Thompson, RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: Gross, JJ., editor.
Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford; 2007. p. 3-24.
Hansen AL, Johnsen BH, Thayer JF. Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International
Journal of Psychophysiology. 2003; 48:263–274. [PubMed: 12798986]
Huffman LC, Bryan YE, del Carmen R, Pedersen FA, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Porges SW. Infant
temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks of age. Child Development.
1998; 69:624–635. [PubMed: 9680676]
Jones LB, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. Development of executive attention in preschool children.
Developmental Science. 2003; 6:498–504.
Kochanska G, Aksan N, Prisco TR, Adams EE. Mother-child and father-child mutually responsive
orientation in the first two years and children’s outcomes at preschool age: Mechanisms of
influence. Child Development. 2008; 79:30–44. [PubMed: 18269507]
Kochanska G, Coy KC, Tjebkes TL, Husarek SJ. Individual differences in emotionality in infancy.
Child Development. 1998; 69:375–390. [PubMed: 9586213]
Kochanska G, Knaack A. Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children:
Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality. 2003; 71:1087–1112. [PubMed:
14633059]
Kochanska G, Murray KT, Harlan ET. Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change,
antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology. 2000; 36:220–
232. [PubMed: 10749079]
Kopp CB. Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology.
1982; 18:199–214.
Kopp, CB.; Neufeld, SJ. Emotional development during infancy. In: Davidson, R.; Scherer, KR.;
Goldsmith, HH., editors. Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press;
2003. p. 347-374.
Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the
executive functions in children and adolescents. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;
114:1291–1311. [PubMed: 15370187]
Li-Grining CP. Effortful control among low-income preschoolers in three cities: Stability, change, and
individual differences. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43:208–221. [PubMed: 17201520]
Luria, AR. Higher cortical functions in man. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1966.
Mezzacappa E. Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and
sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child
Development. 2004; 75:1373–1386. [PubMed: 15369520]
Mischel, W. Processes in delay of gratification. In: Berkowitz, L., editor. Progress in experimental
personality research. Vol. Vol. 3. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1974. p. 249-292.
Mischel W, Ebbesen EB, Zeiss AR. Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1972; 21:204–218. [PubMed: 5010404]
Murphy BC, Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Shepard S, Guthrie IK. Consistency and change in children’s
emotionality and regulation: A longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1999; 46:413–444.
Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology. 2007; 74:116–143. [PubMed:
17049418]
Porges SW, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Portales AL, Greenspan SI. Infant regulation of the vagal “brake”
predicts child behavior problems: A psychobiological model of social behavior. Developmental
Psychobiology. 1996; 29:697–712. [PubMed: 8958482]
Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Attention, self-regulation, and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society of London B. 1998; 353:1915–1927.
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 9
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler
temperament: The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development.
2006; 29:389–401.
Reed MA, Pien DP, Rothbart MK. Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly. 1984; 30:131–147.
Richards JE. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia predicts heart rate and visual responses during visual
attention in 14 and 20 week old infants. Psychophysiology. 1985; 22:101–109. [PubMed:
3975314]
Richards JE. Infant visual sustained attention and respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Child Development.
1987; 58:488–496. [PubMed: 3829789]
Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey K, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years:
The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development. 2001; 72:1394–1408. [PubMed:
11699677]
Rothbart, MK.; Bates, JE. Temperament. In: Damon, W.; Lerner, RM.; Eisenberg, N., editors.
Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development. New
York, NY: John Wiley; 2006. p. 99-166.(Vol. Ed.)
Rothbart, MK.; Ziaie, H.; O’Boyle, CG. Self-regulation and emotion in infancy. In: Eisenberg, N.;
Fabes, RA., editors. Emotion and its regulation in early development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-
Bass; 1992. p. 7-23.
Ruff, HA.; Rothbart, MK. Attention in early development: Themes and variations. London, UK:
Oxford University Press; 1996.
Rydell AM, Berlin L, Bohlin G. Emotionality, emotion regulation, and adaptation among 5- to 8-year-
old children. Emotion. 2003; 3:30–47. [PubMed: 12899315]
Shields A, Cicchetti D. Parental maltreatment and emotion dysregulation as risk factors for bullying
and victimization in middle childhood. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2001; 30:349–363.
[PubMed: 11501252]
Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Fabes RA, Valiente C, Shepard S, Guthrie IK. Relation of
emotion-related regulation to children’s social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion. 2006;
6:498–510. [PubMed: 16938090]
Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Gaertner B, Popp T, Smith CL, Kupfer A, Hofer C. Relations of maternal
socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence.
Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43:1170–1186. [PubMed: 17723043]
Suess PE, Porges SW, Plude DJ. Cardiac vagal tone and sustained attention in school-age children.
Psychophysiology. 1994; 31:17–22. [PubMed: 8146250]
Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for
Research on Child Development. 1994; 59:25–52.
Valiente C, Eisenberg N, Smith CL, Reiser M, Fabes RA, Losoya S, Murphy BC. The relations of
effortful control and reactive control to children’s externalizing problems: A longitudinal
assessment. Journal of Personality. 2003; 71:1172–1196.
Williams BR, Ponesse JS, Schachar RJ, Logan GD, Tannock R. Development of inhibitory control
across the life span. Developmental Psychology. 1999; 35:205–213. [PubMed: 9923475]
Windle M, Lerner RM. Reassessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life
span: The revised dimensions of temperament survey (DOTS-R). Journal of Adolescent Research.
1986; 1:213–230.
Eisenberg and Sulik Page 10
Teach Psychol
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 October 23.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
... Emotion regulation refers to intentional or spontaneous processes that affect the impact and time of an emotional response on physiological, behavioral and cognitive levels (Gross, 2013). Development of emotion regulation starts in infancy, and continues into adulthood (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012). The development of emotion regulation abilities is in uenced by child temperamental factors, and goes hand-in-hand with increasing cognitive and social capacities (Zeman et al., 2006). ...
... Emotion regulation capabilities develop at a rapid pace during the rst few years of life. In infancy, children are primarily dependent on their caregivers in the regulation of their emotions (Zeman et al., 2006) while in toddlerhood there is a gradual progress from external regulation to more intrinsic processes that support the regulation of emotions (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012). The way parents support their children in the regulation of their emotions in early childhood, is predictive of the emotion regulation strategies children will develop later (Kiss et al. 2014). ...
... The way parents support their children in the regulation of their emotions in early childhood, is predictive of the emotion regulation strategies children will develop later (Kiss et al. 2014). In preschool years, children's repertoire of emotion regulation strategies increases, becoming better able to adjust their emotional behavior to the speci c situation (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012). Children's emotion regulation capabilities not only impact on children's social competency (Blair at al., 2004), but also their academic success and productivity (Graziano et al., 2007). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Parental mind-mindedness is an important predictor of child development. It is not clear whether parental mind-mindedness is also related to child emotion regulation in school-aged children or whether child mind-mindedness plays a role. The current study aimed to examine maternal, paternal and children’s mind-mindedness in relation to child emotion regulation and to examine the importance of the valence of mind-related comments (positive, neutral or negative). The study had a cross-sectional design. Mothers, fathers and children from eighty-three families with children aged 8–18 years participated in a mind-mindedness interview, and the children completed a questionnaire measuring adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation. There was no significant association between maternal or paternal mind-mindedness and children’s mind-mindedness; however, when valence was taken into account, maternal positive and negative mind-mindedness were significantly associated with paternal positive and negative mind-mindedness, respectively. Additionally, children’s mind-mindedness toward their mother and father were significantly associated. Maternal mind-mindedness was the only significant predictor of children’s adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation: positive and neutral mind-related comments predicted more adaptive emotion regulation, and neutral and negative mind-related comments predicted more maladaptive emotion regulation. Although it appears possible to reliably measure children’s mind-mindedness, the question remains regarding the meaningfulness of measuring this concept in childhood. The results of the current study suggest that, in school-aged children, mothers’ representational mind-mindedness plays a more significant role in the development of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation than fathers’ representational mind-mindedness. The valence of mind-related comments seems to matter. However, additional research is needed to determine the directionality of these effects.
... Both emotional self-regulation and prosocial behavior are connected to an array of cognitive (e.g., attention and goal orientation) and moral (e.g., sympathy, perspective taking, and moral reasoning) developmental changes (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012;Hinnant et al., 2013). Adolescents' emotional self-regulatory abilities can lead to an increased propensity for prosocial behavior over time (Memmott-Elison et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction This study investigated the longitudinal effects between adolescent emotional self‐regulation and prosocial behavior toward powerful people (i.e., actions aimed at benefiting or supporting individuals who hold significant influence or authority within a group or society), based on the perspective of positive socialization cycle. Methods We recruited 543 Chinese adolescents (284 girls, M age at Time 1 = 11.27 years) and collected three waves of data over 3 years, each approximately 1 year apart (n Time 2 = 449, n Time 3 = 417). Traditional cross‐lagged panel model was conducted to test the longitudinal relationship between emotional self‐regulation and prosocial behavior toward powerful people. Then, a random‐intercept cross‐lagged panel model was performed to disentangle the between‐person differences from the within‐person predictive processes. Results The results showed that, emotional self‐regulation in the previous year was positively associated with prosocial behavior toward powerful people in the next year, whereas earlier prosocial behavior toward powerful people was not predictive of subsequent emotional self‐regulation. Conclusions Findings advocate the domain‐specific nature of self‐regulation and prosocial behavior and deepen our understanding of the relationship between the two from the culture‐specific perspective. This study also highlights the value of emotional self‐regulation intervention as a viable way to promote adolescent prosocial development. Theoretical and empirical implications are discussed.
... Therefore, future research should not only explore styles of emotion regulation and learning from failure but also examine the socialization of emotion regulation in relation to responses to failure and in relation to learning, more generally. This work may build on existing research on the socialization of emotion regulation in relation to self-regulation (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012;Spinrad et al., 2020), prosocial behaviour (Eisenberg et al., 2001) and close relationships (Roth & Assor, 2012). ...
Article
Background: Research on learners' reactions to failure finds negative emotions may present an obstacle for learning; a painful experience of failure may result in disengagement and avoidance. However, research on styles of emotion regulation and learning from failure is scarce. Self-determination theory's (SDT) conception of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation differentiates among three styles of regulation: integrative emotion regulation (IER), suppressive emotion regulation (SER) and amotivated emotion regulation. Aims: Two studies were conducted, one cross-sectional and one longitudinal, to test the associations between IER and learning from failure among adolescents. Sample: Study 1 comprised 184 adolescents (mean age = 16.55; SD = 1.2). Study 2 comprised 565 adolescents (8-12 graders). The main analysis was based on 192 adolescents' perceptions of failing math grades. Method: Study 1 surveyed adolescents on their emotion regulation styles, adaptive and maladaptive coping practices when dealing with failure and tendency to learn from failure. Study 2 was longitudinal and focused on failure in math. We approached participants twice, before and after the math test. Conclusions: In both studies, IER was related to adaptive coping practices and the tendency to learn from failure. In Study 2, adaptive coping practices mediated relations between IER and learning from failure in math and learning from failure mediated relations between IER and future engagement. These findings suggest that styles of emotion regulation play an important role in learning from failure.
... Selfregulation points to the processes that enable us to maintain optimal levels of emotional, motivational, and cognitive desires. (Liew, 2011) (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012). It refers mainly to managing and organizing one's feelings (Eisenberg et al., 2010) (Mischel & Ayduk, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this paper is to determine whether job satisfaction, organizational citizenship behavior, and self-regulation influence improving employee performance during the Covid-19 pandemic. This study was conducted at an agrochemical company in Banten province. The entire population was permanent employees with a minimum of 1 year of service. A sample of 80 respondents was obtained by using the Slovin formula. The instrument for data processing was SmartPLS. The tests carried out were convergent validity, discriminant, reliability, significance, and R2 tests. Based on the analysis result, the study found that there was an effect of job satisfaction on performance, there was an effect of organizational citizenship behavior on performance, and self-regulation does not affect performance. Simultaneously, there was a notable impact between the variables job satisfaction, organizational citizenship behavior, and self-regulation on performance by 22.00%, the rest is influenced by other variables that aren't investigated in the study.
... Emotion regulation is multifaceted, comprising a diffuse network of processes that have physiological, cognitive, and behavioral bases (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010;Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012;Thompson, Lewis, & Calkins, 2008). It is most commonly viewed as an integrative process involving attention and cognition (Blair, 2002;Thompson et al., 2008;Ursache, Blair, & Raver, 2012). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This entry reviews evidence on the development of children's recognition and understanding of emotions. As described by the broad‐to‐differentiated hypothesis, children initially understand emotion in terms of valence (feels good vs. feels bad). The developmental challenge is to differentiate within these broad valence‐based categories until the adult taxonomy of emotion is acquired. This process is gradual and occurs over the course of childhood.
... Warm and supportive parents are good models of social competences, and thus are more likely to foster the development of good social skills in children resulting in quality peer relationships, not only in childhood, but throughout life. Additionally, as mentioned in the context of structured style, the parental role in the development of these dimensions of resilience can also be realized through the development of emotional regulation which is also associated with social competence (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012). Hence, it can be expected that those who are better at regulating their emotions have better social skills that help them in building quality interpersonal relationships. ...
Article
Full-text available
The number of studies on resilience is rapidly growing, and so is the interest in their implications aiming at fostering resilience of all children; not only those at risk. Many of these studies focus on protective factors that increase the probability of positive developmental outcomes. Protective factors can be recognized at all environmental levels, and the crucial level is family. More specifically, parents are those who have a key role in building children's resilience through parental practices and behaviours. Another important environmental level for fostering resilience from an early age is kindergarten, particularly kindergarten teachers who can also be a relevant protective factor during childhood. Most studies linking parental behaviour and resilience explore this relationship in childhood and adolescence. Additionally, studies on the resilience of kindergarten teachers are also lacking. Therefore, the aim of this study was to obtain insight into kindergarten teachers' resilience and its relationship with the parental behaviour of their mothers and fathers. The results obtained indicate a rather high level of resilience among kindergarten teachers in general. The results also revealed that those who grew up with supportive parents are more resilient compared to those whose parents were restrictive.Key words: dimensions of resilience in adulthood; protective factors; restrictive parents; supportive parents.---SažetakSve su brojnija istraživanja koja se bave otpornošću i sve je veće zanimanje za praktične implikacije otpornosti s ciljem razvoja otpornosti u sve djece, a ne samo u one koja su prepoznata kao rizična. Mnoga od tih istraživanja usmjerena su na istraživanje zaštitnih faktora, odnosno onih koji povećavaju vjerojatnost pozitivnih razvojnih ishoda. Ti faktori mogu se prepoznati na svim razinama okoline, ali je obitelj od svih najvažnija. Točnije, roditelji su oni koji svojim roditeljskim ponašanjem imaju ključnu ulogu u razvoju djetetove otpornosti. Još jedna razina okoline važna za razvoj otpornosti je vrtić, posebno odgojitelji/ce koji također mogu biti važan zaštitni faktor tijekom djetinjstva. Većina istraživanja koja se bave odnosom roditeljskog ponašanja i otpornosti tu vezu istražuje tijekom djetinjstva i adolescencije. Nedostaju istraživanja o otpornosti odgajatelja. Cilj ovog istraživanja bio je dobiti uvid u otpornost odgojiteljica, kao i ispitati postoje li razlike u otpornosti s obzirom na roditeljsko ponašanje njihovih majki i očeva. Rezultati su pokazali kako odgojiteljice imaju prilično visoku razinu otpornosti, te kako su one koje su odrastale s podržavajućim roditeljima otpornije od onih čiji su roditelji bili restriktivni.Ključne riječi: dimenzije otpornosti u odrasloj dobi; podržavajući roditelji; restriktivni roditelji; zaštitni faktori
... 27-28). The development of this competency is influenced by several factors including caregivers' reactions toward their child's emotions and cultural norms (Cole et al., 1994;Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012;Keltner et al., 2018). Adverse life events can also have a strong impact on the development of emotion regulation. ...
Article
Child sexual abuse (CSA) is a worldwide phenomenon that has been linked to deleterious consequences. Adverse life events, such as sexual abuse, can compromise the development of emotional competencies, an important dimension of children's psychosocial development. This study aimed at evaluating emotion recognition competencies in sexually abused and non-abused children. The sample consisted of 97 sexually abused children (65 girls) and 78 non-abused children (56 girls) aged between 6 and 12 years. They were recruited in specialized intervention centers and elementary schools from the Montreal area. Recognition of joy, anger, fear, sadness, and neutral expressions was assessed using the Developmental Emotional Faces Stimulus Set (DEFSS; Meuwissen et al., 2017). Results of an ANCOVA revealed that the total scores of emotion recognition were significantly lower for victims of SA (M = 18.12, SE = 0.33) relative to non-abused children (M = 19.36, SE = 0.37), F(1,170) = 5.70, p < .05. Analyses performed on specific expressions yielded lower scores for the recognition of anger, F(1, 170) = 6.12, p = .014, partial η2 = .03, and joy, F(1, 170) = 8.04, p =.005, partial η2 = .04. Our findings highlight the importance of assessing emotion recognition competencies to improve intervention programs provided to sexually abused children and prevent the development of severe psychosocial problems.
Article
Determining how narrative identity relates to psychological functioning is important for understanding how psychopathology develops and is maintained. The present study investigated whether transdiagnostic factors mediate and/or moderate associations of narrative identity with psychological functioning. We analysed data from n = 245 University students who completed an online survey measuring turning point narratives, transdiagnostic factors and psychological functioning. Results indicated that rumination and emotion dysregulation, but not overgeneral memory, mediated the relationship between lower causal coherence and psychological functioning. Contrary to predictions, neither attachment security nor memory tone moderated any relationships between narrative identity and psychological functioning. These pathways may be particularly important for understanding the development and maintenance of psychopathology.
Article
Kutatásunkban kevert módszertannal(mixed method), azaz kvantitatív és kvalitatív adatgyűjtési módszereket egyaránt használva, az egyéni, illetve csapatsportolók önregulációs szintjét kívántuk összehasonlítani nagyszámú mintán. A kevert módszertani kutatások típusai közül a magyarázó, egymásra épülő felépítés módszertanát követve, előbb egy kérdőívcsomagot vettünk fel (kvantitatív módszer) 18. életévüket betöltött, heti rendszerességgel sportoló felnőttekkel (N=762), majd az adatok elemzése után félig strukturált egyéni interjúkat (kvalitatív módszer) készítettünk 11 fővel. A vizsgálati mintánk mind az életkor, mind a sportolási szint, mind maguk a különböző sportágak tekintetében is igen heterogénnek, széleskörűnek mondható. A kérdőíveket 2019 tavaszán, míg az interjúkat 2020 őszén vettük fel. A kérdőívek felvétele „offline” és online térben egyaránt zajlott, míg a kutatási interjúkat eredetileg személyesen terveztük felvenni, azonban a Covid19 világjárvány miatt valamennyi interjút online vettünk fel. A kitöltők bevonása hozzáférési alapon történt, illetve „hólabda”-módszerrel, a vizsgálatunk során keresztmetszeti eljárást alkalmaztunk. A csapat-, illetve egyéni sportolók önregulációs szintje a Mentális Egészség Teszt (MET) (Vargha és mtsai, 2020) segítségével került összehasonlításra, a kapott eredmények alapján pedig tendenciaszintű különbség mutatkozott a két csoport között, az egyéni sportolóknak volt magasabb az önregulációja, mely eredményt az interjús tapasztalatok is alátámasztják Kirschenbaum (1987) ötlépcsős modellje mentén. Eredményeink a hazai sportolók általános önregulációs szintjéről adnak egy tájékoztató képet, további kutatásoknak adhat lehetőséget az önreguláció sportágspecifikus mérése, illetve annak sportteljesítménnyel való kapcsolatának széleskörű vizsgálata.
Article
Full-text available
The course, antecedents, and implications for social development of effortful control were examined in this comprehensive longitudinal study. Behavioral multitask batteries and parental ratings assessed effortful control at 22 and 33 months (N = 106). Effortful control functions encompassed delaying, slowing down motor activity, suppressing/initiating activity to signal, effortful attention, and lowering voice. Between 22 and 33 months, effortful control improved considerably, its coherence increased, it was stable, and it was higher for girls. Behavioral and parent-rated measures converged. Children's focused attention at 9 months, mothers' responsiveness at 22 months, and mothers' self-reported socialization level all predicted children's greater effortful control. Effortful control had implications for concurrent social development. Greater effortful control at 22 months was linked to more regulated anger, and at 33 months, to more regulated anger and joy and to stronger restraint.
Article
Full-text available
The stop-signal procedure was used to examine the development of inhibitory control. A group of 275 participants, 6 to 81 years of age, performed a visual choice reaction time (go) task and attempted to inhibit their responses to the go task when they heard a stop signal. Reaction times to the stop and go signals were used to assess performance in inhibition and response execution, respectively. Results indicated the speed of stopping becomes faster with increasing age throughout childhood, with limited evidence of slowing across adulthood. By contrast, strong evidence was obtained for age-related speeding of go-signal reaction time throughout childhood, followed by marked slowing throughout adulthood. Hierarchical regression confirmed that the age-related change in inhibitory control could not be explained by general speeding or slowing of responses. Findings are discussed in regard to the contrast between the development of inhibition and response execution and the utility of the stop-signal procedure.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary models of human temperament have been based on the general constructs of arousal, emotion, and self-regulation. In order to more precisely investigate these constructs, they were theoretically decomposed into 19 subconstructs, and homogeneous scales were developed to assess them. The scales were constructed through an item-selection technique that maximized internal consistency and minimized conceptual overlap. Correlational and factor analyses suggested that arousal can be usefully assessed in terms of its central, autonomic, and motor components. The emotions of sadness, relief, and low-intensity pleasure were most closely related to the measures of central arousal. Emotions of fear, frustration, discomfort, and high-intensity pleasure were more closely related to measures of attentional control. We discuss these findings in terms of the functional relations between arousal, emotion, and attention.
Book
This book provides both a review of the literature and a theoretical framework for understanding the development of visual attention from infancy through early childhood, including the development of selective and state-related aspects in infants and young children as well as the emergence of higher controls on attention. They explore individual differences in attention and possible origins of ADHD.
Article
Individual differences in emotionality and regulation are central to conceptions of temperament and personality. In this article, conceptions of emotionality and regulation and ways in which they predict social functioning are examined. Linear (including additive) and nonlinear effects are reviewed. In addition, data on mediational and moderational relations from a longitudinal study are presented. The effects of attention regulation on social functioning were mediated by resiliency, and this relation was moderated by negative emotionality at the first, but not second, assessment. Negative emotionality moderated the relation of behavior regulation to socially appropriate/prosocial behavior. These results highlight the importance of examining different types of regulation and the ways in which dispositional characteristics interact in predicting social outcomes.
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of research on choice preferences for delayed, larger versus immediate, smaller gratifications. In spite of the widespread recognition of the important role of delay of gratification in human affairs, previous experimental research on the topic has been limited. At the empirical level, extensive experimental work has been done on delay of reward in animals. Surprisingly, although voluntary delay behavior has been assumed to be a critical component of such concepts as “ego strength,” “impulse control,” and “internalization,” prior to the present research program relatively little systematic attention had been devoted to it in empirical work on human social behavior. The chapter presents, in greater detail, selected studies that focus on the role of cognitive processes during self-imposed delay. Many theorists have paid tribute abstractly to the importance of cognition for the phenomena of personality in general and for self-regulatory processes in particular. These tributes have been accompanied by some correlational research that explores, for example, the links between intelligence, self-control, cognitive styles, and other dispositional. The chapter offers a further theoretical analysis of the determinants of delay behavior.