ArticlePDF Available

Alternative Explanations of Emotional Numbing of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: An Examination of Hyperarousal and Experiential Avoidance

Authors:

Abstract

The mechanisms that underlie the emotional numbing symptoms associated with PTSD are not well understood. Studies of Vietnam combat veterans have demonstrated that hyperarousal symptoms predict emotional numbing symptoms more strongly than do other symptoms of PTSD. This study sought to extend these findings through the self-report of 170 female sexual assault survivors. The study also examined whether the relationship between hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms was the result of the relationship of each of these to another variable, the tendency to engage in experiential avoidance. Results were consistent with and extended previous findings. Hyperarousal symptoms were also found to predict emotional numbing symptoms above and beyond experiential avoidance, as well as all other symptoms of PTSD.
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Vol. 25, No. 3, September 2003 ( C
°2003)
Alternative Explanations of Emotional Numbing
of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: An Examination
of Hyperarousal and Experiential Avoidance
Matthew T. Tull1,2and Lizabeth Roemer1
Accepted October 25, 2002
The mechanisms that underlie the emotional numbing symptoms associated with PTSD are not well
understood. Studies of Vietnam combat veterans have demonstrated that hyperarousal symptoms
predict emotional numbing symptoms more strongly than do other symptoms of PTSD. This study
soughttoextendthesefindingsthroughtheself-reportof170femalesexualassaultsurvivors.The study
also examined whether the relationship between hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms was
theresult of therelationship of eachof these toanother variable, thetendencyto engage inexperiential
avoidance.Resultswereconsistentwithandextendedpreviousfindings.Hyperarousalsymptomswere
also found to predict emotional numbing symptoms above and beyond experiential avoidance, as well
as all other symptoms of PTSD.
KEY WORDS: posttraumatic stress disorder; emotional numbing; experiential avoidance; sexual assault.
Individuals suffering from posttraumatic stress dis-
order (PTSD) often experience deficits in the ability to
express and experience emotion, otherwise referred to as
emotional numbing. The Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
ual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. (American Psychiatric
Association [APA], 1994) describes emotional numbing
as a composite of three symptoms: a markedly diminished
interest in significant activities (Criterion C-4), feelings
of detachment or estrangement from others (C-5), and re-
stricted range of affect (C-6). Though many symptoms of
PTSD (i.e., hyperarousal, intrusive thoughts, and avoid-
ance) are shared with other anxiety disorders, emotional
numbing symptoms are a characteristic feature of PTSD
that distinguish it from the other anxiety disorders (Foa,
Zinbarg, & Rothbaum, 1992).
Research has demonstrated that emotional numbing
symptoms play an integral role in the development and
maintenance of posttraumatic psychopathology. Reports
1Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston,
Boston, Massachusetts.
2To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of
Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, 100 Morrissey
Boulevard, Boston, Massachusetts 02125; e-mail: matthewttull@
aol.com.
of emotional numbing 1 month following traumatic ex-
posure have been found to be the strongest predictor of
PTSD 5 months later (Harvey & Bryant, 1998). Sever-
ity of emotional numbing symptoms 2 weeks after a
traumatic incident has been found to predict severity of
PTSD 3 months later (Feeny, Zoellner, Fitzgibbons, &
Foa, 2000). Foa, Riggs, and Gershuny (1995) found that
emotional numbing symptoms, as compared to all other
symptoms of PTSD, best distinguished individuals with a
PTSD diagnosis from those without, and Jaycox, Foa, and
Morrall (1998) demonstrated that low levels of emotional
engagement in exposure therapy (which can be concep-
tualized as reflecting emotional numbing) are associated
with poor treatment outcomes. Despite these preliminary
findings, the emotional numbing symptoms associated
with PTSD, as well as the mechanisms underlying these
symptoms, remain largely underresearched and not well
understood.
In the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), emotional numbing
symptoms are included with the avoidance symptoms of
PTSD. Researchers have suggested, however, that emo-
tionalnumbing andavoidancesymptoms ofPTSD are dis-
tinct, with separate mechanisms underlying each. Several
studies analyzing the factor structure of PTSD symptoms
have demonstrated that emotional numbing symptoms
147
0882-2689/03/0900-0147/0 C
°2003 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
148 Tull and Roemer
are separate from avoidance symptoms (e.g., Foa et al.,
1995; King & King, 1994). It has also been suggested
that avoidance behaviors may be driven by strategic psy-
chological processes (i.e., the chronic, active avoidance
of cues that elicit painful emotions) while emotional
numbing may be operating through a more automatic
and passive psychobiological mechanism such as cate-
cholaminedepletion (e.g., vanderKolk,Greenberg,Boyd,
& Krystal, 1985) or conditioned opioid-mediated analge-
sia (Foa et al., 1992).
Litz (1992) has proposed that emotional numbing
may occur as a result of attempts to manage the reex-
periencing and hyperarousal symptoms that accompany
PTSD. He suggests that individuals with PTSD expend
considerable cognitive, behavioral, and emotional energy
in an attempt to manage their hyperarousal symptoms as-
sociated with reexposure. These individuals may reduce
theiremotional resources tosuchan extentthata loss orre-
duction of emotional responsiveness and affective capac-
ity,oremotional numbing, isexperienced. Consistent with
this theory, studies have shown that hyperarousal symp-
tomsofPTSD predict emotionalnumbingsymptoms more
strongly than do active avoidance symptoms (and all other
symptoms associated with PTSD) in samples of Vietnam
combat veterans (Flack, Litz, Hsieh, Kaloupek, & Keane,
2000; Litz et al., 1997).
Litz et al. (1997) have also noted that the demon-
strated relationship between hyperarousal and emotional
numbing may be explained by the relationship of each
of these to another variable—strategic emotional suppres-
sion or, more generally, experiential avoidance. Experien-
tial avoidance is a general term that includes both cogni-
tive and emotional avoidance and refers to any attempt
to alter the form or frequency of internal experiences
(Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996). Al-
though experiential avoidance may initially result in re-
duced distress (and thereby be negatively reinforced), the
chronic use of experiential avoidance is likely to have
a paradoxical, negative effect, as attempts to avoid or
alter internal experiences may actually increase the in-
tensity and severity of the very emotions and thoughts
being avoided (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999; see
also Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997; Wegner, Schneider,
Carter, & White, 1987).
Inregard to therelationship of experientialavoidance
to emotional numbing, a process similar to strategic emo-
tional suppression has been observed among individuals
suffering from PTSD. Roemer, Litz, Orsillo, and Wagner
(2001)found thatVietnamcombatveterans with PTSD,as
comparedtowell-adjustedveterans, reported intentionally
withholding their emotions with greater frequency and
intensity. As a result of this chronic, intentional avoidance
of emotions (i.e., experiential avoidance), individuals
with PTSD may exhibit symptoms of emotional numbing.
As for the relationship between experiential avoidance
and hyperarousal symptoms, hyperarousal may also be
associated with experiential avoidance. The suppression
of emotional expression has been found to result in in-
creased physiological arousal (Gross & Levenson, 1993,
1997), and therefore, it is possible that this form of experi-
ential avoidance among individuals suffering from PTSD
may be accompanied by (or contribute to) hyperarousal
symptoms.3
The above findings suggest an alternative explana-
tion for the relationship between hyperarousal and emo-
tional numbing symptoms of PTSD. Experiential avoid-
ance, rather than hyperarousal, may serve as a major risk
factor for the development of emotional numbing among
individuals who have experienced a potentially traumatic
event. Hyperarousal may only be found to be associated
with emotional numbing as a result of its relationship with
experiential avoidance. That is, hyperarousal may be a
proxy risk factor. According to Kraemer, Stice, Kazdin,
Offord, and Kupfer (2001), any variable (in this case,
hyperarousal) that is associated with a strong risk fac-
tor (experiential avoidance) may also be found to have a
relationship with the same outcome (emotional numbing).
However, in actuality, the relationship between this vari-
able or proxy risk factor and the outcome only exists as a
result of both being associated with the strong risk factor.
This type of third variable relationship is different from
that explained through a mediator or moderator model. In
a mediational model, a variable (i.e., a mediator) is found
to account for the relationship between a predictor and
an outcome (in that the predictor is thought to cause the
mediator, which in turn causes the outcome) whereas in
a moderation model, a third variable (i.e., a moderator)
determines under what conditions another variable will
be associated with an outcome (Baron & Kenny, 1986;
Kraemer et al., 2001). A third variable relationship in-
volving a proxy risk factor, on the other hand, explains a
relationship where a variable is found to be highly asso-
ciated with an outcome only because of its relationship to
3Itisimportant to note that the studies described (i.e., Gross &Levenson,
1993, 1997; Roemer et al., 2001) do not directly examine the construct
of experiential avoidance. Instead, they describe the deliberate with-
holding of emotion or the suppression of emotional expression, which
may not be the same as the avoidance of emotional experience or expe-
riential avoidance (a relatively new and recently researched construct).
However, even though few studies have examined the direct conse-
quences of engaging in experiential avoidance, one can assume that
a similar effect is occurring as that observed among individuals who
withhold the expression of emotion, as withholding expression is likely
an important component of avoiding emotional experience.
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
Alternative Explanations of Emotional Numbing 149
another variable strongly associated with that outcome. In
terms of the present study and according to Kraemer and
colleagues’ guidelines (Kraemar et al., 2001) for testing
whether or not a variable is a proxy risk factor, if experi-
ential avoidance is the dominant risk factor for emotional
numbing and hyperarousal is only associated with emo-
tional numbing as a proxy risk factor, then the predictive
ability of hyperarousal should disappear or be weakened
when experiential avoidance is taken into account.
Past studies have not specifically examined whether
the demonstrated relationship between hyperarousal and
emotional numbing symptoms is actually the result of
the relationship of both sets of symptoms to experien-
tial avoidance. Given that an understanding of the pre-
cise nature of the relationship between hyperarousal and
emotional numbing symptoms would likely have impli-
cations for treatment, research investigating the nature of
this relationship is important. Using a sample of female
sexual assault survivors, this study was conducted to ex-
tendthepreviouslydemonstrated findings ofarelationship
between hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms
within a sample of combat veterans. This study also ex-
plored whether the association between hyperarousal and
emotional numbing symptoms is best explained by the
relationship of each of these symptoms to experiential
avoidance (i.e., whether hyperarousal serves as a proxy
risk factor in the potential relationship between experien-
tial avoidance and emotional numbing).
METHODS
Participants
This study included 170 female sexual assault sur-
vivors who were a subset of a larger sample (N=924
women) that participated in a questionnaire study at the
University of Massachusetts Boston. Within the current
sample, 49.4% reported experiencing sexual assault in
childhood only, 37.1% in adulthood only, 11.2% in both
childhood and adulthood, and 2.4% reported the experi-
ence of a sexual assault though not the age in which it had
occurred. In terms of frequency of assault, 69.4% reported
experiencing one incident of sexual assault, 11% reported
two separate incidents, 8.2% reported three separate inci-
dents, and 12.2% reported experiencing continuous sex-
ual assault, or greater than three incidents. Participants
ranged in age from 18 to 61 years with an average age of
24 (SD =6.88). The racial/ethnic background of partici-
pants was 59.4% White, 18.2% Black/African American,
6.5%Hispanic, 2.9%Asian, 0.6%Native American, 5.9%
biracial, and 6.5% of another or unspecified racial/ethnic
background.
Measures
PTSD Checklist
The PTSD Checklist (PCL; Orsillo, 2001; Weathers,
Litz,Herman, Huska, &Keane, 1993) isa self-report mea-
sure including 17 statements that each correspond to a
PTSD symptom as outlined by the DSM-IV. The measure
assesses the severity of intrusive (e.g., repeated distress-
ing memories, thoughts, or images about the potentially
traumatic event), hyperarousal (e.g., hypervigilance, diffi-
cultyconcentrating), avoidance(i.e.,efforts made to avoid
situations or thoughts associated with the potentially trau-
matic event), and emotional numbing (i.e., loss of interest
or pleasure, feelings of detachment, restricted range of af-
fect) symptoms that may occur as a result of experiencing
a stressful life event. Using a 5-point Likert scale (1 =
not at all,5=extremely), participants rate each question
according to the extent to which the symptom has both-
ered them in the past month. Strong internal consistency
has been found among a variety of populations including
VietnamandPersianGulfveterans,motorvehicleaccident
victims, and sexual assault survivors (r’s range from .94
to .97; Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & Forneris,
1996; Weathers et al., 1993). The PCL has been demon-
strated to have strong test–retest reliability across a 3-day
period (r=.96) as well as moderate to strong correla-
tions with other PTSD measures (Weathers et al., 1993).
A cutoff score of 50 has been used with military samples
to predict PTSD diagnoses (Weathers et al., 1993); how-
ever, Blanchard et al. (1996) found a lower cutoff score
of 44 to be associated with greater diagnostic efficiency
among a predominantly female population of trauma vic-
tims (motor vehicle accident victims and sexual assault
survivors).
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire
The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ;
Hayes et al., 2003), is a self-report measure of expe-
riential avoidance, or the tendency of an individual to
avoid unwanted internal experiences, such as emotions,
thoughts, or bodily sensations. This study uses an earlier
16-item version of the measure, which has been found to
be highly correlated with the final 9-item version (Hayes
et al., 2003). Higher scores on the AAQ correspond to
high experiential avoidance, or the unwillingness to ex-
perience certain thoughts and feelings. Sample items in-
clude, “If I could magically remove all the painful experi-
ences I’ve had in my life, I would do so” and “Anxiety is
bad” (Hayes et al., 2003). The AAQ has adequate internal
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
150 Tull and Roemer
consistency (α=.70) and has been found to be correlated
with a tendency to engage in the suppression of thoughts
in both clinical and nonclinical populations (r’s =.44 to
.50; Hayes et al., 2003). The AAQ has also been found
to be correlated with a number of measures of general
psychopathology, including measures of depression (av-
erage r=.57), anxiety (average r=.47), specific fears
(average r=.47), and trauma-related beliefs (r=.68)
andsymptoms (r=.55;Hayeset al., 2003).Providing ev-
idence for its discriminant validity, the AAQ is negatively
correlated with escape-avoidant coping (r=−.38), sug-
gestingthat the AAQ measuresageneral tendency toavoid
internal experience, regardless of the context, in contrast
to avoidance behaviors that are contextually determined
(Hayes et al., 2003).
Life Events Checklist
Participants also completed a life events checklist
(LEC; Blake et al., 1995). The checklist describes a vari-
ety of potentially traumatic events (e.g., combat exposure,
natural disasters, motor vehicle accidents, sexual assault,
etc.).Participantswere asked toindicatewhich eventsthey
had experienced, the number of times the events were ex-
perienced, and whether the events occurred during child-
hood, adulthood, or both. Of particular interest to this
study were participants’ responses to whether a sexual
assault had been experienced. On the checklist, sexual as-
sault was defined as forced, unwanted oral, anal, or vagi-
nal penetration. As assessed with the LEC, approximately
18.4% of the total sample (N=924) reported experienc-
ing a sexual assault at some point in their life. This rate is
consistentwith previous findings obtainedfrom a largena-
tionalsample ofhigher education female students (15.4%;
Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987) using a validated as-
sessmentof sexualassault, the SexualExperiences Survey
(SES; Koss & Gidycz, 1985; Koss & Oros, 1982).
Procedure
Students and staff from the University of
Massachusetts Boston were recruited through ques-
tionnaire distribution tables located in public areas on
the university campus. Prior to participation, participants
were informed fully, both verbally and in writing, about
the purpose of the study and potentially distressing
subject matter of the questionnaires. Students who
chose to participate in the study filled out a battery of
questionnaires that included the PCL (Weathers et al.,
1993), the AAQ (Hayes et al., 2003), and the LEC (Blake
et al., 1995). Inclusion in the present study required
endorsement of at least one sexual assault experienced in
childhood, adulthood, or both.
RESULTS
Within the current sample of female sexual assault
survivors, 34.9% of the sample met or exceeded the cutoff
score of 44 on the PCL, thereby suggesting that these
individuals may experience trauma-related symptoms to
such severity that a diagnosis of PTSD is warranted.
As assessed by the PCL, all of the PTSD symptoms
(avoidance, intrusion, hyperarousal, and emotional numb-
ing)were significantlyand positively correlated. Allof the
PTSD symptoms were significantly and positively associ-
ated with experiential avoidance, as assessed by the AAQ
(see Table I).
In order to determine whether a relationship between
hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms existed
among female sexual assault survivors similar to that pre-
viously found among combat veterans (see Flack et al.,
2000; Litz et al., 1997), a hierarchical regression analysis
wasperformed. Severityofemotional numbing symptoms
was the dependent variable while the other PTSD symp-
tom clusters served as the predictor variables. The predic-
tor variables entered in the first step of the equation were
severity of intrusion and avoidance symptoms. Similar to
analysesperformed by Litzet al. (1997),severityof hyper-
arousalsymptoms was enteredinthe final stepso as topro-
videthe mostconservativetest of the relationship between
emotional numbing and hyperarousal symptoms. Results
demonstrate that severity of hyperarousal symptoms sig-
nificantlypredicted emotionalnumbing above and beyond
the avoidance and intrusion symptoms of PTSD (β=.41,
p<.001; see Table II), accounting for an additional 10%
of the variance in the severity of emotional numbing
symptoms. Intrusion, though not avoidance, symptoms
were also found to be a significant predictor of emotional
Table I. Correlations Between PTSD Symptoms and Experiential
Avoidance
Experiential
Variable Avoidance Intrusion Hyperarousal EN avoidance
Avoidance 1.00
Intrusion .721.00
Hyperarousal .60.571.00
EN .53.61.631.00
Experiential .42.40.42.371.00
avoidance
p<.01.
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
Alternative Explanations of Emotional Numbing 151
Table II. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Emotional
Numbing With Avoidance, Intrusion, and Hyperarousal Symptoms
Step and predictor variables Adj. R21R2β(final step)
Step 1 .38 .39
Avoidance .01
Intrusion .37
Step 2 .48 .10
Hyperarousal .41
p<.001.
numbing in the final model (β=.37, p<.001). Given
this finding, an additional hierarchical regression analy-
sis was performed where severity of hyperarousal symp-
toms was entered in the first step followed by the avoid-
ance and intrusion symptom severity scores in the second
step. Severity of emotional numbing symptoms was again
the dependent variable. This analysis served to further
test the strength of the relationship between hyperarousal
and emotional numbing symptoms by examining whether
symptoms of avoidance and intrusion make a significant
contributiontothe prediction ofemotional numbing above
and beyond that of hyperarousal. Symptoms of avoidance
and intrusion significantly improved the model above and
beyond severity of hyperarousal symptoms (1R2=.09,
p<.001) though this was largely due to the fact that
severity of intrusion, not avoidance, symptoms was found
to be a highly significant predictor of emotional numb-
ing symptoms in the final model (β=.37, p<.001; see
Table III). Severity of hyperarousal symptoms also re-
mained a significant predictor of emotional numbing in
the final model (β=.41, p<.001).
To examine if the demonstrated relationship between
hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms was a
result of hyperarousal serving as a proxy risk factor in
the relationship between experiential avoidance and emo-
tional numbing, participants’ scores on the AAQ were in-
troduced in the second step of the regression equation
Table III. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Emotional
Numbing With Hyperarousal Symptoms Entered First Followed by
Avoidance and Intrusion Symptoms
Step and predictor variables Adj. R21R2β(final step)
Step 1 .39 .39
Hyperarousal .41
Step 2 .48 .09
Avoidance .01
Intrusion .37
p<.001.
Table IV. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Emotional
Numbing With Avoidance, Intrusion, and Hyperarousal Symptoms and
Experiential Avoidance
Step and predictor variables Adj. R21R2β(final step)
Step 1 .37 .38∗∗
Avoidance .02
Intrusion .35∗∗
Step 2 .39 .02
Experiential avoidance .07
Step 3 .48 .10∗∗
Hyperarousal .42∗∗
p<.05.∗∗p<.001.
(i.e., after the intrusion and avoidance symptoms). As be-
fore, severity of hyperarousal symptoms was entered in
the final (third) step. If the relationship between hyper-
arousaland emotionalnumbing symptomswas merelythe
result of the relationship of each of these to experiential
avoidance, hyperarousal symptoms would not continue
to predict emotional numbing symptoms (or the rela-
tionship would weaken) when controlling for experien-
tial avoidance. Results indicate that, without the inclusion
of hyperarousal symptoms, experiential avoidance signif-
icantly predicted emotional numbing above and beyond
the avoidance and intrusion symptoms of PTSD (β=.14,
p<.05), accounting for an additional 2% of the variance
in the severity of emotional numbing symptoms. When
severity of hyperarousal symptoms was entered into the
model in the third and final step of the regression equa-
tion, experiential avoidance no longer remained a signifi-
cant predictor of emotional numbing. Instead, the severity
of hyperarousal symptoms once again significantly pre-
dicted emotional numbing symptoms above and beyond
theavoidanceand intrusionsymptoms of PTSD,as wellas
experiential avoidance (β=.42, p<.001; see Table IV),
again accounting for an additional 10% of the variance in
the severity of emotional numbing symptoms. As before,
intrusion, but not avoidance, symptoms remained a signif-
icant predictor of emotional numbing in the final model
(β=.35, p<.001).
DISCUSSION
Using a sample of female sexual assault survivors,
this study extended Litz and colleagues’ previous findings
(Litz et al., 1997) obtained from a sample of Vietnam
combat veterans. Symptoms of hyperarousal significantly
predictedemotionalnumbing above and beyondtheavoid-
ance and intrusion symptoms of PTSD. Results further
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
152 Tull and Roemer
demonstrated that hyperarousal symptoms remained a
significant predictor of emotional numbing when con-
trolling for the relationship between emotional numbing
and experiential avoidance, suggesting that experiential
avoidance is not a major risk factor for the development of
emotional numbing symptoms with hyperarousal serving
only as a proxy risk factor due to its association with
experiential avoidance. In other words, the relationship
between hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms
is not the result of the relationship of each of these
to experiential avoidance. The present study does not
provide support for an alternative explanation of the pro-
posed and previously demonstrated relationship between
hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms. Despite
this, it warrants mention that experiential avoidance was
found to explain a significant (albeit minimal) percentage
of the variance in emotional numbing symptoms above
and beyond the intrusion and avoidance symptoms of
PTSD (although it did not remain a significant predic-
tor upon inclusion of hyperarousal symptoms in the
model).
Despite consistency with previous findings, several
limitations need to be taken into consideration when in-
terpreting results. Information pertaining to participants’
sexual assault histories, PTSD symptoms, and experien-
tial avoidance was obtained through self-report data. In-
dividuals who have encountered a potentially traumatic
event may experience difficulty, or even unwillingness, in
openly expressing their thoughts and feelings about that
event through self-report. A tendency to engage in expe-
riential avoidance may also interfere with awareness of
internal experiences and, subsequently, accurate report-
ing of these experiences. However, it appears as though
the use of self-report data was not associated with any
more limitations than those present in interview data, es-
pecially given that results were consistent with previous
studies using interview data to examine the relationship
between hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms
(e.g., Flack et al., 2000; Litz et al., 1997).
Even though all of the participants reported experi-
encing a sexual assault, they were not required to respond
to the PCL for symptoms specifically related to their sex-
ual assault. The PCL asks participants to rate the extent to
which they experience a variety of symptoms associated
with PTSD but does not require participants to specify the
traumatic event to which those symptoms are related. Par-
ticipantsin this studymayhavebeen describingsymptoms
associated with another traumatic event. This is possible
given that the majority of the participants in the current
sample reported at least one other stressful experience on
the LEC. For example, 4.1% reported past combat expo-
sure, 67.1% reported experiencing a physical assault at
some point in their life, and 22.9% reported experiencing
an assault involving a weapon (although it is important to
keepinmind that thelattertwomay have been experienced
concurrent with the sexual assault). Even though the re-
sultswould still providesupportfor the previouslydemon-
strated relationship between hyperarousal and emotional
numbingsymptoms, the relationship may not be exclusive
to the experience of sexual assault, but instead, to a wide
variety of traumatic events.
Results indicated that 34.9% of the present sam-
ple experienced PTSD-related symptoms to such sever-
ity that a diagnosis of PTSD may be warranted, as based
upon the cutoff score proposed for nonmilitary popula-
tionson thePCL. While this percentage issimilar toprevi-
ously reported rates from the National Comorbidity Study
(45.9% of women who reported rape as their most upset-
ting trauma developed PTSD; Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet,
Hughes, & Nelson, 1995), it is important to note that the
PCL is a self-report measure of symptom severity only
and therefore, cannot be used to diagnose PTSD. How-
ever, this finding does speak to the extent of symptom
severity experienced by the present sample.
Another limitation concerns the measure of expe-
riential avoidance, the AAQ. Given theoretical support
for the role of experiential avoidance in the development
and maintenance of a variety of psychological disorders
(see Hayes et al., 1996) and its theoretical relevance to
the development and maintenance of PTSD in particu-
lar, it seemed important to explore the role of experien-
tial avoidance in the relationship between hyperarousal
and emotional numbing symptoms. However, though ex-
periential avoidance is receiving increasing attention as a
construct of empirical interest, it is still a relatively new
construct, and therefore, the only measure developed thus
far to assess it, the AAQ, is not yet published. Preliminary
data (Hayes et al., 2003) nonetheless support its validity,
suggesting it is an adequate measure of an individual’s
tendency to avoid emotional experiences.
Individuals suffering from PTSD may not always
be in an emotionally numb or unresponsive state. Litz
(1992) has suggested that deficits in emotional processing
(or emotional numbing) occur as a result of episodes of
hyperemotionality associated with exposure to a trauma
cue. This study assessed trait rather than state levels of
these symptoms and thus may have obscured important
relationships between state occurrences of hyperarousal,
emotional numbing, and experiential avoidance. Experi-
mental studies where hyperarousal, emotional numbing,
and experiential avoidance are assessed after the presenta-
tion of a trauma-related cue would aid in determining the
exact nature of the relationship between these phenomena
in a trauma-cued state.
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
Alternative Explanations of Emotional Numbing 153
Similar to previous studies, the correlational and
cross-sectional nature of the data prohibits stating with
certainty the actual direction of the relationship between
hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms. Experi-
mentalstudies where hyperarousalisactivelymanipulated
in the lab would further elucidate the role of hyperarousal
symptoms in the development and maintenance of emo-
tional numbing symptoms in PTSD. Longitudinal studies
would also aid in determining the developmental course
of trauma symptoms and their interrelationships.
Despite these limitations, the findings from the
presentstudy suggestthathigh levelsofarousal may result
in the depletion of emotional resources, thereby leading
toemotional numbing,consistent withLitz (1992).Symp-
toms of intrusion were also found to be a significant pre-
dictor of emotional numbing symptoms suggesting that
their role in the development and maintenance of emo-
tional numbing symptoms warrants further investigation.
Although the present study does not provide support for
the exclusive role of experiential avoidance in emotional
numbing, it would be premature to rule out the impor-
tance of the intentional avoidance of emotional experi-
ence in emotional numbing symptoms, as well as PTSD
in general. The intentional suppression or avoidance of
distressing thoughts or emotions may become automatic
over time, such that individuals may not be able to accu-
rately report the extent of their engagement in these strate-
gies.Experimentalstudies in which suppressionisactively
manipulated may shed further light on the role of emo-
tional suppression or avoidance in the development and
maintenance of emotional numbing symptoms. Experien-
tial avoidance also demonstrated modest correlations with
all PTSD symptom clusters suggesting that a tendency to
avoid internal experience may be related to all aspects of
PTSD. Consequently, further research on the relationship
between experiential avoidance and all PTSD symptoms
(for example, intrusions, given evidence from the thought
suppression literature that intrusions may occur as a result
of suppression attempts) is warranted. Experiential avoid-
ance is a broad term encompassing avoidance of a vari-
ety of internal experiences (e.g., emotions, thoughts, and
bodilysensations), andfuture studies may benefit fromfo-
cusing on more specific aspects of experiential avoidance
and their possible influence on the relationship between
hyperarousal and emotional numbing symptoms.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported in part by National In-
stitute of Mental Health grant MH-59044 and an inter-
nal grant from the University of Massachusetts Boston
(both to the last author). Portions of this study were pre-
sented at the 35th Annual Convention of the Association
for Advancement of Behavior Therapy in Philadelphia,
PA (2001). We thank Sue Orsillo, Amy Wagner, and Kim
Gratz for their valuable suggestions in the preparation of
this manuscript.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association [APA] (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: Author.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic,
and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,51, 1173–1182.
Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman,
F. D., Charney, D. S., et al. (1995). The development of a Clini-
cian Administered PTSD Scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress,8, 75–
90.
Blanchard, E. B., Jones-Alexander, J., Buckley, T. C., & Forneris, C. A.
(1996). Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL). Be-
haviour Research and Therapy,34, 669–674.
Feeny, N. C., Zoellner, L. A., Fitzgibbons, L. A., & Foa, E. B. (2000).
Exploring the roles of emotional numbing, depression, and disso-
ciation in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress,13, 489–498.
Flack,W.F.,Jr.,Litz,B.T.,Hsieh, F. Y.,Kaloupek,D.G.,&Keane,T.M.
(2000). Predictors of emotional numbing, revisited: A replication
and extension. Journal of Traumatic Stress,13, 611–618.
Foa,E.B.,Riggs,D.S.,& Gershuny,B.S.(1995).Arousal,numbing,and
intrusion: Symptom structure of PTSD following assault. American
Journal of Psychiatry,152, 116–120.
Foa, E. B., Zinbarg, R., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1992). Uncontrollability
and unpredictability in post-traumatic stress disorder: An animal
model. Psychological Bulletin,112, 218–238.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physi-
ology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,64, 970–986.
Gross,J.J., & Levenson,R.W.(1997). Hidingfeelings:Theacuteeffects
of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology,106, 95–103.
Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship between acute
stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A prospective
evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Con-
sulting and Clinical Psychology,66, 507–512.
Hayes,S. C., Bissett, R. T., Strosahl, K., Follette, W.C.,Polusney,M.A.,
Pistorello, J., et al. (2003). Psychometric properties of the Accep-
tance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ). Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and
commitmenttherapy:Anexperientialapproachto behavior change.
New York: Guilford.
Hayes,S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V.,Follette,V. M., & Strosahl, K.
(1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A func-
tional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology,64, 1152–1168.
Jaycox, L. H., Foa, E. B., & Morral, A. R. (1998). Influence of emotional
engagementandhabituationonexposuretherapy for PTSD. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,66, 185–192.
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & Nelson, C. B.
(1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity
Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry,52, 1048–1060.
King, L. A., & King, D. W. (1994). Latent structure of the
Mississippi Scale for Combat-related Posttraumatic Stress Disor-
der: Exploratory and higher-order confirmatory factor analyses.
Assessment,1, 275–291.
P1: GDX
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp835-joba-464659 April 22, 2003 12:54 Style file version June 25th, 2002
154 Tull and Roemer
Koss, M. P., & Gidycz, C. A. (1985). Sexual Experiences Survey: Relia-
bility and validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
53, 422–423.
Koss, M. P., Gidycz, C. A., & Wisniewski, N. (1987). The scope of rape:
Incidenceandprevalenceof sexualaggressionandvictimizationina
nationalsample of higher education students. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology,55, 162–170.
Koss, M. P., & Oros, C. J. (1982). Sexual Experiences Survey: A re-
search instrument investigating sexual aggression and victimiza-
tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,50, 455–
457.
Kraemer, H. C., Stice, E., Kazdin, A., Offord, D., & Kupfer, D. (2001).
How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and
independent,overlapping,andproxyriskfactors.AmericanJournal
of Psychiatry,158, 848–856.
Litz, B. T. (1992). Emotional numbing in combat-related post-traumatic
stress disorder: A critical review and reformulation. Clinical Psy-
chology Review,12, 417–432.
Litz, B. T., Schlenger, W. E., Weathers, F. W., Caddell, J. M., Fairbank,
J. A., & LaVange, L. M. (1997). Predictors of emotional numbing
in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress,10,
607–618.
Orsillo, S. M. (2001). Measures for acute stress disorder and posttrau-
matic stress disorder. In M. M. Antony, S. M. Orsillo, & L. Roemer
(Eds.), Practitioner’s guide to empirically based measures of anx-
iety (pp. 255–307). New York: Kluwer.
Roemer,L.,Litz,B.T.,Orsillo, S.M.,&Wagner, A. W.(2001).A prelim-
inary investigation of the role of strategic withholding of emotions
in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress,14, 143–150.
Weathers, F. W, Litz, B. T., Herman, D. S., Huska, J. A., & Keane, T. M.
(1993,November).ThePTSD Checklist (PCL): Reliability,validity,
and diagnostic utility. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, Chicago, IL.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987).
Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,53, 5–13.
van der Kolk, B., Greenberg, M., Boyd, H., & Krystal, J. (1985). In-
escapable shock, neurotransmitters, and addiction to trauma: To-
ward a psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Biological Psychia-
try,20, 314–325.
... These two items had relatively low mean scores but were the top-ranked centrality indices and support the notion that avoidance symptoms are not openly expressed but represent an important diagnostic indicator of PTSD (32, 68). It should be noted that although avoidant coping could provide short-term psychological protection and reduce distress of those affected by traumatic events (66), chronic avoidance over extended durations is considered to be maladaptive and contributes to sustained stress symptoms and impaired post-traumatic recovery (33,69). ...
... Additionally, emotional numbing can result from avoidance of traumatic triggers rather than exposure to traumatic events per se; avoidance of such triggers might lead, not only to reduced exposure to emotional provoking situations, but also to a "shut down" of affective system (75,77,78), per the central role of emotional numbing within the network model from this study. Finally, prospective studies have found that emotional numbing is related to increased future risk for the development of PTSD (33,67) and depression (72). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) are commonly reported by psychiatric healthcare personnel during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic and negatively affect quality of life (QOL). However, associations between PTSS and QOL at symptom level are not clear. This study examined the network structure of PTSS and its connection with QOL in psychiatric healthcare personnel during the COVID-19 pandemic. Methods This cross-sectional study was carried out between March 15 and March 20, 2020 based on convenience sampling. Self-report measures including the 17-item Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Checklist – Civilian version (PCL-C) and World Health Organization Quality of Life Questionnaire - Brief Version (WHOQOL-BREF) were used to measure PTSS and global QOL, respectively. Network analysis was used to investigate the central symptoms of PTSS and pattern of connections between PTSS and QOL. An undirected network was constructed using an extended Bayesian Information Criterion (EBIC) model, while a directed network was established based on the Triangulated Maximally Filtered Graph (TMFG) method. Results Altogether, 10,516 psychiatric healthcare personnel completed the assessment. “Avoidance of thoughts” (PTSS-6), “Avoidance of reminders” (PTSS-7), and “emotionally numb” (PTSS-11) were the most central symptoms in the PTSS community, all of which were in the Avoidance and Numbing domain. Key bridge symptoms connecting PTSS and QOL were “Sleep disturbances” (PTSS-13), “Irritability” (PTSS-14) and “Difficulty concentrating” (PTSS-15), all of which were within the Hyperarousal domain. Conclusion In this sample, the most prominent PTSS symptoms reflected avoidance while symptoms of hyper-arousal had the strongest links with QOL. As such, these symptom clusters are potentially useful targets for interventions to improve PTSS and QOL among healthcare personnel at work under pandemic conditions.
... Experiential avoidance is a key factor in the development and maintenance of negative psychological outcomes among people with NSE histories (Batten et al., 2001;Boeschen et al., 2001;Marx & Sloan, 2005;Tull & Roemer, 2003). Specifically, experiential avoidance appears to intensify the negative effects of victim-blaming responses (i.e., AMASA) to NSE disclosure (i.e., NSE acknowledgment) on depressive symptoms in people with NSE histories (Bhuptani et al., 2019). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Nonconsensual sexual experiences (NSEs) may take many forms including rape and sexual assault. NSE acknowledgment has been linked to several positive (e.g., Valdespino-Hayden et al., 2022) and negative outcomes (e.g., Layman et al., 1996) for people with NSE histories. A person’s willingness or ability to acknowledge their NSEs may be, at least in part, due to the extent they accept myths about sexual aggression (AMASA, i.e., rape myths; e.g., Watters & Yalch, 2023). However, AMASA does not fully account for NSE acknowledgement (Reed et al., 2019). As a result, research on possible moderators of the relationship between AMASA and NSE acknowledgement relationship is necessary (Newins et al., 2018). The primary aim of this study is to advance literature by examining psychological flexibility and inflexibility, both of which deal with the relationship between thoughts and feelings and valued behavioral outcomes, as potential moderators of the relationship between NSE acknowledgment and AMASA. The current study is poised to provide preliminary data on two psychological constructs that may serve as target areas for developing interventions to support people with NSE histories, by decreasing AMASA, increasing acknowledgment, and buffering the negative mental health outcomes associated with sexual violence.
... Thompson (1994) defined ER as the capacity to influence one's own emotional experience in terms of quality, intensity, timing, and dynamics in an intended direction. Mental health issues, such as depression (Berking et al., 2014), generalized anxiety disorder (Mennin et al., 2007), post-traumatic stress disorder (Tull, 2003), social anxiety (Kashdan and Breen, 2008) or eating disorders (Piran and Cormier, 2005), have been linked to difficulties in ER. Difficulties in ER, or emotion dysregulation, refer to dysfunctional patterns of ER. ...
Article
Backgrounds Complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CPTSD) is closely related to childhood trauma, yet there is a dearth of studies that have explored the symptoms of CPTSD among children in adversity. Objective The current study aimed to explore the structure of the CPTSD network and to identify central symptoms in a sample of de facto unattended children in China. Methods In total, 244 children and adolescents were included in the analysis, ranging from 9 to 16 years old (11.72 ± 1.64), with 45.49 % girls and 53.67 % boys. The International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ) was used to assess CPTSD symptoms. We used a regularized partial correlation network to identify which symptoms had a high level of strength centrality or bridge centrality, and a Bayesian network to identify the upstream symptoms. Results The results showed that avoidance and emotional numbing had the highest level of strength centrality. Exaggerated startle response and emotional numbing had the highest level of bridge centrality. Last, re-experiencing was identified as upstream in the Bayesian network. Developmental differences are observed in CPTSD symptom centrality compared to adult populations. Conclusions These findings offer insight into key symptoms of CPTSD in vulnerable children and adolescents. We revealed that children and adolescents in adversity who exhibit avoidance, emotional numbing, and re-experiencing should be given more attention.
Article
Full-text available
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a severe mental disorder causing high individual and societal costs. The use of maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategies has been identified as a potential contributing factor. This meta-analysis aimed to quantify the associations between PTSD symptoms and rumination, thought suppression and experiential avoidance. The systematic literature search resulted in 5574 studies, 75 of which were included in the analysis. From those eligible studies 189 effect sizes were obtained. For symptoms of posttraumatic stress, large effects were found for associations with rumination (r = .52) and experiential avoidance (r = .48), whereas a medium effect size was found for thought suppression (r = .29). With respect to different PTSD symptom clusters, associations ranged between r = .35 and r = .41 for associations with intrusive re-experiencing, between r = .39 and r = .41 for associations with avoidance, between r = .50 and r = .53 for associations with alterations in cognitions and mood and between r = .41 and r = .45 for associations with alterations in arousal and activity. Few available studies provide some evidence that associations might be somewhat reduced but still substantial in longitudinal compared to cross-sectional studies. These findings provide valuable targets for future investigations with the long-term goal of improving targeted interventions for the prevention and treatment of PTSD symptoms.
Preprint
Full-text available
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a severe mental disorder causing high individual and societal costs. The use of maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategies has been identified as a potential contributing factor. This meta-analysis aimed to quantify the associations between PTSD symptoms and rumination, thought suppression and experiential avoidance. The systematic literature search resulted in 4486 studies, 44 of which were included in the analysis. From those eligible studies 135 effect sizes were obtained. For symptoms of posttraumatic stress, large effects were found for associations with rumination (r = .53) and experiential avoidance (r = .47), whereas a medium effect size was found for thought suppression (r = .27). With respect to different PTSD symptom clusters, associations ranged between r = .35 and r = .38 for associations with intrusive re-experiencing, between r = .39 and r = .40 for associations with avoidance, between r = .48 and r = .56 for associations with alterations in cognitions and mood and between r = .42 and r = .43 for associations with alterations in arousal and activity. Few available studies provide some evidence that associations might be somewhat reduced but still substantial in longitudinal compared to cross-sectional studies. These findings provide valuable targets for future investigations with the long-term goal of improving targeted interventions for the prevention and treatment of PTSD symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
Developed a survey capable of reflecting hidden cases of rape and of documenting a dimensional view of sexual aggression/sexual victimization. This survey examines degrees of coercion used or experienced in sexual activity. Results obtained from 3,862 university students support a dimensional view. The viability of a survey approach to sample selection for future rape research is discussed. (4 ref)
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A series of factor analyses evaluated the dimensionality of the Mississippi Scale for Combat-Related Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Over 2,200 Vietnam theater and era veterans were divided into three random subsamples, each of which was used in a separate stage of analysis. Initial exploratory factor analyses suggested an underlying single-factor solution. In the second subsample, a second-order solution comprised of a general factor subsuming several first-order factors was supported using chi-square difference testing. This model was successfully replicated with the third subsample. Cumulative evidence suggests that the latent structure of the Mississippi Scale is best represented as an umbrella Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) factor leading to four subsidiary facets or dimensions.
Article
Full-text available
Syndromal classification is a well-developed diagnostic system but has failed to deliver on its promise of the identification of functional pathological processes. Functional analysis is tightly connected to treatment but has failed to develop testable, replicable classification systems. Functional diagnostic dimensions are suggested as a way to develop the functional classification approach, and experiential avoidance is described as 1 such dimension. A wide range of research is reviewed showing that many forms of psychopathology can be conceptualized as unhealthy efforts to escape and avoid emotions, thoughts, memories, and other private experiences. It is argued that experiential avoidance, as a functional diagnostic dimension, has the potential to integrate the efforts and findings of researchers from a wide variety of theoretical paradigms, research interests, and clinical domains and to lead to testable new approaches to the analysis and treatment of behavioral disorders. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
An ACT Approach Chapter 1. What is Acceptance and Commitment Therapy? Steven C. Hayes, Kirk D. Strosahl, Kara Bunting, Michael Twohig, and Kelly G. Wilson Chapter 2. An ACT Primer: Core Therapy Processes, Intervention Strategies, and Therapist Competencies. Kirk D. Strosahl, Steven C. Hayes, Kelly G. Wilson and Elizabeth V. Gifford Chapter 3. ACT Case Formulation. Steven C. Hayes, Kirk D. Strosahl, Jayson Luoma, Alethea A. Smith, and Kelly G. Wilson ACT with Behavior Problems Chapter 4. ACT with Affective Disorders. Robert D. Zettle Chapter 5. ACT with Anxiety Disorders. Susan M. Orsillo, Lizabeth Roemer, Jennifer Block-Lerner, Chad LeJeune, and James D. Herbert Chapter 6. ACT with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Alethea A. Smith and Victoria M. Follette Chapter 7. ACT for Substance Abuse and Dependence. Kelly G. Wilson and Michelle R. Byrd Chapter 8. ACT with the Seriously Mentally Ill. Patricia Bach Chapter 9. ACT with the Multi-Problem Patient. Kirk D. Strosahl ACT with Special Populations, Settings, and Methods Chapter 10. ACT with Children, Adolescents, and their Parents. Amy R. Murrell, Lisa W. Coyne, & Kelly G. Wilson Chapter 11. ACT for Stress. Frank Bond. Chapter 12. ACT in Medical Settings. Patricia Robinson, Jennifer Gregg, JoAnne Dahl, & Tobias Lundgren Chapter 13. ACT with Chronic Pain Patients. Patricia Robinson, Rikard K. Wicksell, Gunnar L. Olsson Chapter 14. ACT in Group Format. Robyn D. Walser and Jacqueline Pistorello
Article
Litz et al. (1997), theorizing that emotional numbing (EN) is the result of emotional depletion caused by chronic hyperarousal, demonstrated that a cluster of hyperarousal symptoms was a robust predictor of EN symptoms. In the present study, these findings were replicated and extended in two multiple regression analyses of data from a large, multisite investigation (T. M. Keane et al., 1998) of psychophysiological responding by male combat veterans. The arousal (D) cluster of symptoms was again the most robust predictor of EN symptoms, whereas physiological indices of arousal and reactivity accounted for negligible amounts of variance in both regression equations. These findings underscore the possible link between disturbances related to arousal and the capacity of traumatized individuals to express and experience pleasant feelings.
Article
Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) experience deficits in emotional responding, yet to date these deficits have been poorly understood. This study is an initial investigation of the role of strategic, intentional withholding of emotional responses among individuals with PTSD. In a sample of 61 combat veterans, veterans with PTSD reported significantly more frequent and intense withholding of their emotional responses than did combat veterans without PTSD. The tendency to withhold emotional responses was associated with PTSD, beyond measures of comorbid distress. The implications of these findings for future research and interventions aimed at deficits in emotional responding are discussed.
Article
Several interviews are available for assessing PTSD. These interviews vary in merit when compared on stringent psychometric and utility standards. Of all the interviews, the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS-1) appears to satisfy these standards most uniformly. The CAPS-1 is a structured interview for assessing core and associated symptoms of PTSD. It assesses the frequency and intensity of each symptom using standard prompt questions and explicit, behaviorally-anchored rating scales. The CAPS-1 yields both continuous and dichotomous scores for current and lifetime PTSD symptoms. Intended for use by experienced clinicians, it also can be administered by appropriately trained paraprofessionals. Data from a large scale psychometric study of the CAPS-1 have provided impressive evidence of its reliability and validity as a PTSD interview.
Article
Emotional numbing symptoms are considered in the clinical literature as cardinal signs of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and have been formally codified in DSM-III-R. However, the term has not been consistently defined nor adequately researched. The present paper critically reviews the extant empirical and theoretical literature in combat-related PTSD that has explored emotional numbing symptoms. A theoretical framework, based on Levanthal's (1984) perceptual-motor theory of emotion, is posited to account for the parameters of emotional processing in PTSD, and specific hypotheses concerning selective or differential emotional processing deficits in PTSD are described in order to clarify empirical issues about the development and maintenance of emotional processing deficits in PTSD and to stimulate future research in this underexplored, yet clinically important area.