Conference PaperPDF Available

LED lighting flicker and potential health concerns: IEEE standard PAR1789 update

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The IEEE Standards Working Group, IEEE PAR1789 “Recommending practices for modulating current in High Brightness LEDs for mitigating health risks to viewers” has been formed to advise the lighting industry, ANSI/NEMA, IEC, EnergyStar and other standards groups about the emerging concern of flicker in LED lighting. This paper introduces power electronic designers for LED lighting to health concerns relating to flicker, demonstrates that existing technologies in LED lighting sometimes provide flicker at frequencies that may induce biological human response, and discusses a few methods to consider when trying to mitigate unintentional biological effects of LED lighting. The paper represents on-going work in IEEE PAR1789 that is vital to designing safe LED lamp drivers.
Content may be subject to copyright.
LED Lighting Flicker and Potential Health Concerns:
IEEE Standard PAR1789 Update
Arnold Wilkins
Dept. of Psychology
University of Essex; UK
Jennifer Veitch
National Research Council Canada
Ottawa; Canada
Brad Lehman
Dept. Elect. & Comp. Eng
Northeastern University; USA
Abstract -- The IEEE Standards Working Group, IEEE
PAR1789 "Recommending practices for modulating current in
High Brightness LEDs for mitigating health risks to viewers"
has been formed to advise the lighting industry, ANSI/NEMA,
IEC, EnergyStar and other standards groups about the
emerging concern of flicker in LED lighting. This paper
introduces power electronic designers for LED lighting to
health concerns relating to flicker, demonstrates that existing
technologies in LED lighting sometimes provide flicker at
frequencies that may induce biological human response, and
discusses a few methods to consider when trying to mitigate
unintentional biological effects of LED lighting. The paper
represents on-going work in IEEE PAR1789 that is vital to
designing safe LED lamp drivers.
Index Terms-- LED, health risk, flicker, lighting, power
electronics, ergonomics, drivers, headache, seizure, standards
I. NOMENCLATURE
Flicker: a rapid and repeated change over time in the
brightness of light.
Modulation: a measure of light variation that is often
applied to periodic oscillations. This report refers to
modulation as variation in luminance as a proportion of the
average luminance (commonly referred to as Percent Flicker,
Peak-to-Peak Contrast, Michelson Contrast, or Depth of
Modulation). For a time-varying luminance with maximum
and minimum values:
Modulation = (Lmax - Lmin) / (Lmax + Lmin)
(Lighting Design Glossary)
Visible Flicker: Flicker that is consciously perceivable by
a human viewer.
Invisible or Imperceptible Flicker: Flicker that is not
consciously perceivable by a human viewer.
The effects of flicker can range from decreased visual
performance to non-specific malaise to the onset of some
forms of epilepsy.
II. INTRODUCTION
This paper summarizes a public report created by the IEEE
Standards PAR1789 group on LED lighting that is examining
biological effects of flicker in emerging LED lighting
technologies. (The full length version of the report can be
found at http://grouper.ieee.org/groups/1789/ ) The intention
of this document is to provide an objective summary of the
effects on human health for both visible and invisible flicker
and to draw attention to implications for the design of LED
lighting. Specifically, contributions of this paper include
making the reader aware of
1. Risks of seizures due to flicker at frequencies within the
range ~3- ~70Hz;
2. Human biological effects due to invisible flicker at
frequencies below ~165Hz;
3. The differences between “visible” flicker and “invisible”
flicker and any relation to health risks;
4. A few, typical driving approaches in LED lighting that
may produce flicker.
This report does not attempt to make recommendations on
safe flicker frequencies or modulation depths for LED
lighting. Its purpose is to describe possible health
implications of flicker. By bringing these issues to the power
electronic and lamp designers now, it will permit better
ethical discussions and decisions to be made on development
of future LED lamps as the market continues to have
explosive growth. This report endorses no technology for
driving LED lamps. Specifically, Section III of the report
gives tutorial surveys on health effects of flicker. Section IV
introduces a few typical LED driving methods that introduce
flicker in frequency ranges of interest.
III. FLICKER
The health effects of flicker can be divided into those that
are the immediate result of a few seconds’ exposure, such as
epileptic seizures, and those that are the less obvious result of
long-term exposure, such as malaise, headaches and impaired
visual performance. The former are associated with visible
flicker, typically within the range ~3- ~70Hz, and the latter
with invisible modulation of light at frequencies above those
at which flicker is perceptible (invisible flicker). Human
biological effects are a function of flicker frequency,
modulation depth, brightness, lighting application, and
several other factors.
A. Photosensitive Epilepsy
About one in 4000 individuals is recognized as having
photosensitive epilepsy. Repetitive flashing lights and static
repetitive geometric patterns may induce seizures in these
individuals, and in perhaps as many again who have not been
diagnosed and may be unaware that they are at risk.
The seizures reflect the transient abnormal synchronized
activity of brain cells, affecting consciousness, body
movements and/or sensation. The onset of photosensitive
epilepsy occurs typically at around the time of puberty; in the
age group 7 to 20 years the condition is five times as common
as in the general population. Three quarters of patients remain
photosensitive for life (Harding and Jeavons, 1994; Wilkins,
1995; Fisher et al. 2005). Many factors [see Fisher et al.,
2005 for extensive reference list] may combine to affect the
likelihood of seizures including:
Flash Frequency. Any repetitive change in a visual
stimulus within the frequency range 3 Hz to 70 Hz, is
potentially a risk and the greatest likelihood of seizures is for
frequencies in the range 15 Hz to 20 Hz, see Fig. 1. The
flashes do not have to be rhythmic.
Brightness. Stimulation in the scotopic or low mesopic
range (below about 1 cd/m2) has a low risk and the risk
increases monotonically with log luminance in the high
mesopic and photopic range.
Contrast with background lighting. Contrasts above
10% are potentially a risk.
Distance between the viewer and the light source and its
location which determine
Total area of the retina receiving stimulation. The
likelihood of seizures increases according to the
representation of the visual field within the visual cortex of
the brain. The cortical representation of central vision is
greater than that of the visual periphery, and so
Location of stimulation within the visual field is
important: Stimuli presented in central vision pose more of a
risk than those that are viewed in the periphery, even though
flicker in the periphery may be more noticeable.
Wavelength of the light. Deep red flicker and
alternating red and blue flashes may be particularly
hazardous.
Open or closed eyes. Bright flicker can be more
hazardous when the eyes are closed, partly because the entire
retina is then stimulated. However, if flickering light is
prevented from reaching the retina of one eye by placing the
palm of a hand over that eye, the effects of the flicker are
very greatly reduced in most patients.
Figure 1. Percentage of patients with photosensitive epilepsy exhibiting
epileptiform EEG responses to the flicker from a xenon gas discharge lamp
shown as a function of flash frequency. After Harding and Jeavons (1994).
Figure 2. Escalator stair tread
In addition, a substantial minority of patients (usually
those who are sensitive to flicker) are sensitive also to spatial
patterns; see Fig. 2 for an example. About one third of
patients are sensitive to patterns even when there is no
flicker, and most are more sensitive to flicker if it is patterned
(Harding and Jeavons, 1994; Wilkins, 1995; Fisher et al.,
2005; Wilkins et al. 1979). The worst patterns are those of
stripes in which one cycle of the pattern (one pair of stripes)
subtends at the eye an angle of about 15 minutes of arc.
B. Imperceptible Flicker
The frequency of the alternating current electricity supply
is 60Hz in America and 50Hz in Europe; in Japan, both 50Hz
and 60Hz are used in different regions. The circuitry in older
fluorescent lamps with magnetic ballasts operate so as to
flash the lamps at twice the supply frequency (100Hz or
120Hz). However, as the lamps age, the flashes that occur
with one direction of current may not equal those that occur
with the other direction, and the lamps may emit flicker with
components at the frequency of the electricity supply. It has
been determined that photosensitive seizures should not occur
if fluorescent lamps are operating properly. However, when
the lamps malfunction giving flicker below 70Hz,
electroencephalographic recordings indicate a risk of seizures
(Binnie et al., 1979). Nevertheless some photosensitive
patients do complain of normally functioning (older)
fluorescent lighting (with magnetic ballasts).
Measurements of the electroretinogram have indicated that
modulation of light in the frequency range 100-160Hz and
even up to 200 Hz is resolved by the human retina although
the flicker is too rapid to be seen (Burns et al. 1992, Berman
et al., 1991). In an animal (cat), 100Hz and 120Hz
modulation of light from fluorescent lamps has been shown
to cause the phase-locked firing of cells in the lateral
geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, part of the brain with
short neural chains to the superior colliculus, a body that
controls eye movements (Eysel and Burandt, 1984). There are
several studies showing that the characteristics of human eye
movements across text are affected by modulation from
fluorescent lamps and cathode ray tube displays (see work of
Wilkins, 1986; Kennedy and Murray, 1991), and two studies
have shown impairment of visual performance in tasks
involving visual search as a result of flicker from fluorescent
lamps (e.g. Jaen et al., 2005). Under double-masked
conditions the 100Hz modulation of light from fluorescent
lamps has been shown to double the average incidence of
headaches in office workers, although this effect is
attributable to a minority that is particularly affected (Wilkins
et al., 1989).
Sensitivity effects due to flicker at frequencies above
perception have also been observed in normal people with
good vision and health. A substantial decrement in sensitivity
to visible flicker at 30 Hz, used as a testing condition, occurs
in normal observers when there is a prior exposure of only 2
minutes duration with flicker frequencies about 20% above
the observers critical fusion frequency (CFF) (Shady et al.
2004).
Computer monitors and backlights
When making a rapid jerk (saccade), for example when
reading, the eyes move at a velocity of about 180 degrees per
second. As a result, any intermittently lit contour is displaced
at a succession of retinal positions during the flight of the eye
and can sometimes be seen as a set of repetitive targets. The
LED rear lamps of motor vehicles can produce such an effect.
Some displays on netbook computers have LED backlights
and exhibit significant flicker at 60Hz. Their flicker also
results in the perception of multiple images during a saccade.
It is possible that this effect is responsible for the known
disturbance of ocular motor control by high frequency flicker,
a disturbance which, in its turn, may be responsible for the
known impairments in visual performance.
Modulation depth and the Fourier fundamental.
The effects of flicker depend not only on the frequency of
the flicker but also on the modulation depth. For visible
flicker, the amplitude of the Fourier fundamental predicts
flicker fusion (de Lange Dzn, 1961). For imperceptible
flicker the effects of different waveforms have not been
studied in detail. The peak-trough modulation depth of the
100-120Hz flicker from older fluorescent lamps with
magnetic ballasts varies with the component phosphors, some
of which exhibit persistence, varying the chromaticity of the
light through its cycle (Wilkins and Clark, 1990). The peak-
trough modulation depth known to induce headaches from
fluorescent lighting at 100Hz is about 35% (Wilkins et al.,
1989). The IEEE Standards PAR1789 working group is
developing new measures/definitions of flicker that rely on
Fourier series of the flicker. The present definitions for
modulation do not distinguish the difference between low
frequency and high frequency modulation. But for
sufficiently high flicker frequencies, there are limited human
biological effects.
C. Summary of Biological Effects
The obvious biological effects occur: 1) immediately from
2) flicker that is visible. The risks include seizures, and less
specific neurological symptoms including malaise and
headache. Seizures can be triggered by flicker in individuals
with no previous history or diagnosis of epilepsy.
The less obvious biological effects occur: 1) from flicker
that is invisible 2) after exposure of several minutes. Invisible
flicker health effects have been reported to include headaches
and eye-strain.
The table in the Appendix summarizes and categorizes the
types of flicker and the biological effects they cause.
(c) Simulation of current through HB LEDs.
Luminous intensity is proportional to current,
causing lamp to flicker at twice the line frequency
(shown periodic every 1/120 sec)
I_LED
LED LED LED
R1
-+
BRIDGE
1
4
3
2
LED string
AC 50-60Hz
LED
I_LED
LED LED LED
R2
AC 50-60Hz
LED
I_LED
LED
LED
LED
LED
(a) Rectify AC and send to LED string
(b) Directly power two LED strings with opposite
Anode/Cathode connections
Or a
capacitor
Figure 3. Two methods to drive LEDs at twice line frequency: (a) Full bridge
rectification, (b) Opposite connected parallel strings, and (c) Current/Luminous Output in
D. A Few General Implications for Practice
Visual flicker is an undesirable attribute of any lighting
system. The Appendix Table summarizes research suggesting
that, for both visible and invisible flicker (in the mentioned
frequency ranges), there may be a special at-risk population
for which flicker is more than just annoying and can be a
potential health hazard. Any hazard will, however, depend
on modulation depth, ergonomics, flicker parameters and
their relation to perception and the ability to
measure/determine the influence of these parameters with
human diagnostics. These topics are beyond the scope of this
paper and will be covered in future IEEE PAR1789
documents. However, it is possible to make general
comments about the research cited in the Appendix Table:
1. Frequency. Normally functioning fluorescent lighting
controlled by magnetic ballasts has been associated with
headaches due to the flicker produced. LEDs driven so that
they flicker at a frequency twice that of the AC supply may
have a depth of modulation greater than that from most
fluorescent lamps. The effects of the flicker are therefore
likely to be more pronounced in these cases.
2. Field of view. Point sources of light are less likely to
induce seizures and headaches than a diffuse source of light
that covers most of a person’s field of vision. Flicker from
LEDs used for general lighting is therefore more likely to be
a health hazard than that from LEDs used in instrument
panels.
3. Visual task. The invisible flicker described in the
Appendix Table is more likely to cause problems when the
visual task demands precise positioning of the eyes, as when
reading.
4. Spatial distribution of point sources of light. Spatial
arrays of continuously illuminated point sources of light have
the potential to induce seizures in patients with photosensitive
epilepsy when the field of view is large and when the arrays
provide a spatial frequency close to 3 cycles/degree (e.g.
large LED public display boards viewed from close
proximity).
IV. TYPICAL LED DRIVING METHODS IN LOW FLICKER
FREQUENCY RANGE
There are several common methods that are used to drive
LEDs that can operate with frequency of modulation in the
ranges discussed in the above table (below 120Hz, including
frequencies in the vicinity of 15Hz.) For example,
commercially available LED lamps have been reported (Rand
et al., 2007; Rand, 2005) to malfunction and produce visual
flicker in the 15Hz range when connected to a conventional
residential dimmer.
Below, we present only a few driving approaches that
modulate in frequency ranges from zero to 120Hz. The list is
not exhaustive, and the discussions are only meant to
demonstrate typical driving LED currents with frequencies in
this range.
A. LED Driving Current Frequencies in Range: ~100Hz–
120Hz
(1) Full Wave Rectifier Connected to LED String
In this approach, the AC input source is sent into a full
wave rectifier, causing the (approximate) absolute value of
the input voltage to be sent to the load. In this case, the
current through the LEDs has a waveform shape similar to a
scaled absolute value of a sine wave. That is, the rectified
sine wave may be of the form, where Vp is
wave and ω is the
the amplitude of the sine
angular frequency in radians ω = 2*π*f. In this case, the LED
current is of similar shape, as Fig. 3 shows. In a first
approximation, the LED current is equal to a scaled rectified
voltage, with the additional deadtime (zero current) caused by
the LED bias voltage. Thus, when properly functioning, the
direct full wave rectifier driving approach modulates the
LEDs at twice the line frequency, which in North America
leads to 120Hz modulation and in Europe leads to 100 Hz
modulation. As Fig. 3(a) shows, often a resistor is added in
series with the LED string for current limiting protection.
(2) Directly Drive Two Parallel LED Strings with
Opposite Anode/Cathode Connections
A second LED driving method that doubles line frequency
is shown in Fig. 3(b). Two strings of LEDs are powered in
parallel, with anode of one paralleled string connected to the
cathode of the other parallel string. When the AC line voltage
is positive, energy drives one of the LED strings. When the
AC line voltage is negative, the other paralleled LED string is
driven. At most, one of the LED strings has current through
it. The net effect is that the effective LED driving current is
modulating at 120 Hz in North America or 100 Hz in Europe.
Thus, for both driving methods illustrated in Fig. 3, the
LED current modulates at twice the line frequency. Since the
intensity of the LEDs is (ideally) proportional to the current
through the LEDs, this causes the LEDs to flicker at
frequency equal to twice the AC line frequency, i.e.
100Hz~120Hz. There are many variations of the approach in
Fig. 3 that are not shown here.
(3) Simple Dimming Pulse Width Modulated (PWM)
Circuits
It is common to dim LEDs by pulsing the current through
them intentionally.The luminous intensity of the LED can be
adjusted by varying the length of time that the LED current is
High or Low,. Thus, PWM dimming circuits may be designed
to operate at any frequency, whether the input is DC or AC.
(It should be noted that it is not uncommon for LED drivers
using AC residential phase modulated dimmer circuits to
attempt to “emulate” PWM type signals with frequency
120Hz. That is, when the AC dimmer shuts off, no current is
sent to the LEDs.)
It should be mentioned that there are alternative
approaches to dimming, such as amplitude dimming, in
which the current through the LED is continuous and not
|)sin(| tVp
ω
pulsing. By reducing the value of this continuous current
(amplitude), the brightness is dimmed. This approach does
not use flicker to adjust brightness and therefore, should not
induce flicker related health risks.
(4) Power Factor Correction Circuitry
Even when sophisticated high frequency switching power
supplies with power factor correction circuits are used to
drive LEDs from AC mains, there is commonly a frequency
component in the current (and luminous intensity) of the
LEDs at twice the line frequency. Depending on the design of
the circuitry, the harmonic content of this flicker may vary
from being small and unnoticeable to being significant in
magnitude.
B. LED Driving Current Frequencies in Range: 3Hz~70Hz
(1) Failures in rectification or LED strings: 50Hz ~ 60 Hz
Modulation
In either of the two methods of Fig. 3, there is risk of
failure that can cause LED current modulation at AC line
frequency, thereby entering the range of frequencies that may
induce photosensitive epilepsy. For example, if one of the
legs of the full wave rectifier bridge fails, then it is common
that the leg becomes an open circuit. Open circuits prevent
current flow, and therefore, the LED modulation frequency
may change. This single diode failure in the rectifier will
cause the output voltage for the full wave rectifier to become
the input voltage for half the AC line cycle, and then 0 volts
for the remaining half line cycle. This means that if the AC
Mains line frequency is f and the period is T=1/f, then non-
zero voltage is applied to the LEDs for 0.5*T seconds and
then is zero for the next 0.5*T seconds, causing the LED
current to modulate at line frequency.
Similarly, when the two strings of LEDs are connected in
parallel with opposite anodes and cathodes in each string, a
failure in one string of the LEDs may cause an open circuit to
occur in that string. The net effect is the same as before: the
current is modulating at line frequency, i.e. 50Hz ~ 60Hz.
This low frequency driving current leads to brightness flicker
in the LEDs at 50Hz~60Hz, since the current in the LEDs is
proportional to their intensity. This is in a range of
frequencies that are at risk of causing photosensitive epilepsy.
(2) Residential Dimmer Switches Can Cause Low
Frequency Flicker (~3Hz – 70Hz)
Residential dimmers for incandescent bulbs primarily
utilize phase modulating dimming through triac switches to
control the power sent to the bulb. These dimmers control the
RMS voltage applied to the bulb by suppressing part of the
AC line voltage using a triac. The effect is a chopped sine
wave as shown in Fig.4. Thus, as the dimmer switch is
manually adjusted, the value of the off-time, α (often referred
to as the phase delay) changes. As α is increased in Fig. 4,
less power goes to the incandescent bulb and brightness is
reduced
Some LED lamps and their associated drivers may not
perform properly with residential phase modulated dimmers.
Often on the LED bulb application notes or on the lamp’s
manufacturer web sites there are warnings to the user that
their bulbs may not work properly when used with residential
dimmer switches. Rand’s work (Rand et al., 2007; Rand,
2005) explains the causes of these failures and shows that low
frequency flicker may occur.
Dimmer Voltage
α
Figure 4. Residential dimmer and its output voltage sent to
the driver (Rand et al., 2007).
Fig. 5 illustrates how one type of commercially available
LED lamp flickers in the noticeable visual range when
connected to a dimmer switch. The particular lamp involved
has a common LED driver configuration (further discussed
below) of a full bridge rectifier with capacitor filter within
their Edison socket, described in more detail by Rand et al.,
2007; 2005). The results presented in the figure may be
typical of similar driving configurations. The circuit will
continuously peak charge the filter capacitor to the peak
voltage of the input waveform, i.e. 169Vdc for standard
120Vac line voltage. This high level DC voltage may then be
fed into a large string of LEDs in series. For example, some
typical lamps may have parallel strings of many Red, Blue,
Green LEDs, in series attached through a current limiting
resistor to the high level DC voltage. The particular lamp
tested utilized a combination of 64 Red, Green and Blue
LEDs to produce white light.
Figure 5. Commercial LED lamp flickers at 3.15Hz when
connected to typical residential dimmer switch.
The experimental data in Fig. 5 represents the voltage of a
photo-sensor placed directly underneath the LED lamp.
Specifically, a photoresistor circuit is used to generate a
voltage proportional to the light intensity shining on it. As the
experimental voltage shows, the bulb malfunctions when
connected to (phase modulated) residential dimmer switch. It
produces a noticeable visual flicker. In particular, the flicker
varies between around 3.0Hz and 3.3Hz, with average over
many cycles of 3.153Hz. This frequency is in the range that
has been shown to be a risk for causing photosensitive
epileptic seizures.
The flicker illustrated in Fig. 5 is typical of a few LED
lamps on the market when connected to a dimmer. However,
the precise flicker frequency is hard to predict, as it may
either be higher or lower depending on various factors such
as number of lamps on the dimmer, position of the dimmer
switch (the value of desired off-time α), and/or internal
characteristics of the lamp. However, as the experimental
oscilloscope plot shows, the flicker frequency may be in the
range that induces photosensitive seizures.
The reasons that the dimmer switch may fail when
connected to LED lamp bulbs are given in (Rand et al., 2007;
2005).
(3) Uneven Brightness in Different LED Strings When
Connected as in Fig. 3(b)- With Strings Having Opposite
Anode/Cathode Connections
Consider the circuit in Fig. 3(b). Notice that each LED
must have the same dynamic characteristics (forward voltage
and dynamic resistance) in order for the current to be
perfectly balanced in each alternating illuminated string. If
for some reason this does not occur (aging, temperature
gradients, poor design), then the current through the strings
will not be identical each cycle.
Figure 6. Unbalanced LED Current in Each String of
LEDs Using Driving Method in Fig. 3(b). The unbalanced
driving will cause uneven luminous output in the lamp and
low frequency flicker.
For example, suppose over time, aging causes degradation
of one of the two strings in Fig. 3(b) such that its string
resistance increases by 50%. This could also be caused by
improper design of each string in Fig. 3(b) so that the current
in each string is not balanced. This is quite possible since
LEDs are binned by different voltages, and further, each
string may be composed of different color LEDs that have
different nominal voltage drops for the same current. Then,
the effective LED current through the bulb will look as in
Fig.6.
For example, the effective DC LED current in the
numerical simulation of Fig. 6 has average value of around
233mA. However, the Fourier component at 60 Hz (taking
FFT) is 80mA and the Fourier component at 120Hz is nearly
240mA. Thus, in this example the low frequency component
of 60Hz represents over 33% of the DC component, while the
120 Hz component represents 100% of the DC current.
Higher frequency components of the LED current in the
above figure are also present in multiples of 60Hz. However,
the typical analysis above indicates that LED lamps may
demonstrate flicker frequency at line frequency, similar to
older fluorescent lamps (previously discussed) that aged
unevenly: the flashes/brightness with one direction of line
current may not equal those that occur in the other direction.
The above example also illustrates that it is possible for
flicker in a lamp to have harmonics with multiple low
frequency components, here at both 60Hz and 120Hz.
V. CONCLUSIONS
The purpose of this paper is to make lamp and power
electronic designers aware of biological effects of flicker and
to introduce the reader to a few LED driving methods that
will have flicker. The LED driving approaches described in
this paper are not exhaustive and are only meant to introduce
the reader to a few common approaches. Other
approaches/applications of LED lighting that may also have
flicker include, but are not limited to, pulse amplitude
modulation driving, triangle wave currents through LEDs,
using LED flicker for wireless communication (see IEEE
Standard 802), beat frequencies created through the
interaction of different lamp flicker frequencies, etc.
This paper assigns no health risk to the biological effects
of flicker in the various LED lamps. The hope is that by
discussing the issue of flicker within the power electronic
community, it will be possible to decide as a community
whether or not standards or recommended practices are
necessary. We do not attempt to do so here. However, we do
offer simple suggestions as to what should be considered
when designing lamps, such as flicker frequency, angle of
viewing, task being performed, spatial distribution, AC
dimmer flicker, etc. Further, it is not difficult to create shut-
down or other safety prevention circuits that prevent
flickering in the 3Hz-70Hz range when the lamp is in failure
mode. This is the flicker range that has risk of photosensitive
epilepsy for small minority of the population.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
This paper was assembled with input from many members
of IEEE Standards PAR1789 who at the time of writing
consisted of (including observers):
Maurizio Acosta, Ian Ashdown, Rolf Bergman, Sam
Berman, Anindita Bhattacharya, Subramanyam Chamarti,
Clint Chaplin, Yvonne de Kort, Montu Doshi, Kevin
Dowling, John Halliwell, Mark Halpin, Steve Hayes, Chris
Horton, Wijnand IJsselsteijn, Michael Jennings, Jiao
Jianzhong, David Keeser, Faisal Khan, Thorbjorn Laike,
Brad Lehman, Ihor Lys, Theron Makley, Naomi Miller,
Lesley Murawski, Brandon Oakes, Yoshi Ohno, Steve
Paolini, Radu Pitigoi Aron, Michael Poplawski, Conor Quin,
Eric Richman, Anatoly Shteynberg, Michael Shur, Roger
Shuttleworth, David Sliney, Jennifer Veitch, Joachim
Walewski, Arnold Wilkins, Howard Wolfman, Wei Yan,
Regan Zane.
REFERENCES
Primary References for This Report are below. (Please refer to the IEEE
PAR1789 website for additional references
http://grouper.ieee.org/groups/1789/ that were also used. The web site will
continuously be updated.)
Berman, S.M., Greenhouse, D.S., Bailey, I.L., Clear, R.D., and Raasch,
T.W. (1991) Human electroretinogram responses to video displays,
fluorescent lighting, and other high frequency sources. Optom Vis Sci.,
68(8),645-62.
Binnie, C.D., de Korte, R.A., and Wisman, T. (1979) Fluorescent
lighting and epilepsy. Epilepsia,20,725-7.
Burns, S.A., Elsner, A.E., and Kreitz, M.R. (1992) Analysis of
nonlinearities in the flicker ERG. Optom Vis Sci.,69(2), 95-105.
de Lange Dzn, H. (1961) Eye's Response at Flicker Fusion to Square-
Wave Modulation of a Test Field Surrounded by a Large Steady Field of
Equal Mean Luminance. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 51(4),
415.
Evans J.E., Cuthill I.C., and Bennett A.T.D. (2006) Animal Behaviour,
72(2),393-400.
Eysel, U.T., Burandt, U. (1984) Fluorescent tube light evokes flicker
responses in visual neurons. Vision Res., 24(9),943-8.
Fisher, R., Harding, G.F.A., Erba, G., Barkley, G.L., and Wilkins, A,J.
(2005) Photic- and pattern-induced seizures: a review for the Epilepsy
Foundation of America Working Group. Epilepsia, 46(9),1426-41.
http://www.essex.ac.uk/psychology/overlays/2005-168.pdf
Funatsuka, M., Fujita, M., Shirakawa, S., Oguni, H., and Osawa, M.
(2003) Pediatr Neurol, 28(1),28-36.
Halpin, S.M., Bergeron, R., Blooming, T.M., Burch, R.F., Conrad, L.E.
and Key, T.S. (2003) Voltage and lamp flicerk issues: should the IEEE adopt
the IEC approach, IEEE Transactions on Power Delivery, 18(3), 1088-1097.
Harding, G.F.A. and Harding, P.F. (2008) Photosensitive epilepsy and
image safety. Appl Ergon, Oct 16.
Harding, G.F.A. and Jeavons, P. (1994) Photosensitive Epilepsy. Mac
Keith Press.
Hazell, J. and Wilkins, A.J. (1990) Psychological Medicine, 20,591-96.
Ishiguro, Y., Takada, H., Wantanabe, K., Okumura, A., Aso, K., and
Ishikawa, T. (2004) Follow-up survey on seizures induced by animated
cartoon TV program "Pocket Monster", Epilepsia, 45(4) 377-383.
Okumura, A., Aso, K., Ishikawa, T A (2004) Follow-up survey on
seizures induced by animated cartoon TV program "Pocket Monster",
Epilepsia, 45(4) 377-383.
Jaen, M., Sandoval, J., Colombo, E., and Troscianko, T. (2005) Office
workers visual performance and temporal modulation of fluorescent lighting,
Leukos, 1(4), 27-46.
Kelly, D.H. (1969) Diffusion model of linear flicker responses, Journal
of the Optical Society of America, 59(12), 1665-1670.
Kelly, D.H. (1971) Theory of flicker and transient responses, I. Uniform
Fields, Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61(4), 537-546.
Kennedy, A., Brysbaert, M., and Murray, W.S. (1998) Q J Exp Psychol
A., 51(1), 135-51.
Kennedy, A. and Murray, W.S. (1991) The effects of flicker on eye
movement control. Q J Exp Psychol A., 43(1),79-99.
Lighting Design Glossary
http://www.schorsch.com/kbase/glossary/contrast.html
Maddocks, S.A., Goldsmith, A.R., and Cuthill, I.C. (2001) General and
Comparative Endocrinology, 124(3), 315-20.
Rand, D. (2005) Off Line Dimming for High Brightness LEDs. MS
Project, Northeastern University, Boston, MA.
Rand, D., Lehman, B. , and Shteynberg, A. (2007) Issues, Models and
Solutions for Triac Modulated Phase Dimming of LED Lamps, Proc. IEEE
Power Electronics Specialists Conference.
Rea, M.s. and Ouellette, M.J. (1988) Table-tennis under high intensity
discharge (HID) lighting, Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society,
Vol. 17, No.1, 29-35.
Shady, S., MacLeod , D.I.A. and Fisher , H.S. (2004) Adaptation from
invisible flicker. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A.,
101, 5170-5173.
Veitch, J.A. and McColl, S.L. (1995) Lighting Res. Tech., 27(4),243-
256.
Wilkins, A.J. (1986) Intermittent illumination from visual display units
and fluorescent lighting affects movements of the eyes across text. Human
Factors, 28(1),75-81.
Wilkins, A.J. (1995) Visual Stress. Oxford University Press.
http://www.essex.ac.uk/psychology/overlays/book1.pdf
Wilkins AJ, Clark C. (1990) Modulation of light from fluorescent lamps.
Lighting Research and Technology, 22(2):103-109.
Wilkins, A.J., Darby, C.E. and Binnie, C.D. (1979) Neurophysiological
aspects of pattern-sensitive epilepsy. Brain 1979; 102:1-25.
http://www.essex.ac.uk/psychology/overlays/1979-15.pdf
Wilkins, A.J., Nimmo-Smith, I.M., Slater, A. and Bedocs, L. (1989)
Fluorescent lighting, headaches and eye-strain. Lighting Research and
Technology, 21(1), 11-18.
J. F. Fuller, E. F. Fuchs, and K. J. Roesler, "Influence of harmonics on
power distribution system protection," IEEE Trans. Power Delivery, vol. 3,
pp. 549-557, Apr. 1988.
arnol
d
Digitally signed by
arnold
DN: cn=arnold
Date: 2010.07.12
14:39:21 +01'00'
APPENDIX
TABLE I
FLICKER AND BIOLOGIC AL EFFECTS
Source of flicker Fre
q
uenc
y
ran
g
eBiolo
g
ical effect Evidence
Sunlight through roadside trees
or reflected from waves
Various Seizures Clinical histories (Harding and
Jeavons, 1994)
Xenon gas discharge photo-
stimulator
3-60Hz Epileptiform EEG in
patients with
photosensitive epilepsy
Many clinical EEG studies e.g
(Harding and Jeavons, 1994)
Malfunctioning
fluorescent lighting
Large 50Hz
component
Epileptiform EEG in
patients with
photosensitive epilepsy
(Binnie et al., 1979)
Television 50Hz and 60Hz
(discounting 25Hz
component)
Epileptiform EEG in
patients with
photosensitive epilepsy
Many studies eg (Harding and
Harding, 2008; Funatsuka et al.,
2003)
Flashing televised cartoo
n
~10H
z
Seizures in children
with no previous
diagnosis of epilepsy
Major incident (Okumura et al,
2004)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100Hz (small
50Hz component)
Headache and eye strain Many anecdotes.
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100Hz (small
50Hz component)
Headache and eye strain Double-masked study (Wilkins et
al 1989)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
32% modulatio
n
Reduced speed of visual
search
Two masked studies (Jaen et al.,
2005)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (60Hz
ballast)
120H
z
Reduced visual
performance
(Veitch and McColl, 1995)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100Hz (minimal
50Hz component)
Increased heart rate in
agoraphobic individuals
(Hazell and Wilkins, 1990)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100H
z
Enlarged saccades over
text
(Wilkins, 1986)
Visual display terminals 70-110Hz raste
Changes in saccade size (Kennedy et al., 1998)
Visual display terminals ~70H
z
Raster
Many anecdotal reports of
prolonged photophobia
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting
100Hz and 120Hz Phase-locked firing of
LGN neurons in cats
(Eysel and Burandt, 1984)
Various Up to 162H
z
Human
electroretinogram
signals at light
frequency
(Berman et al.,1991; Burns et al
1992)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100H
z
Inconsistent changes in
plasma corticosterone
levels in captive
starlings
(Maddocks et al., 2001)
Normally functioning
fluorescent lighting (50Hz
ballast)
100H
z
Mate choice in captive
starlings
(Evans et al., 2006)
... In addition to color perception, it is important to address that PWM may affect humans in other ways, particularly due to flicker, as discussed in a study [17]. Contrary to common belief, electroretinogram studies show that the human retina can detect modulation frequencies up to 200 Hz, even if the flicker is too rapid to be consciously perceived [18]. Non-visual effects of flickering light, such as blood pressure modulation, have also been reported [19]. ...
... However, the experimental study presented here does not account for the impact of flicker on human perception or potential health risks associated with the application of PWM. Visible flicker can pose risks, including seizures or neurological symptoms such as headaches or malaise, while invisible flicker may lead to issues like eye strain and headaches [18]. Although the current study focuses on color perception, these non-visual flicker effects should not be overlooked in future research or practical applications of PWM. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research investigates the light quality and thermal management of InGaN and InGaAlP Light Emitting Diodes (LEDs) under various light regulation methods. We compare Continuous Wave (CW) and Pulse-Width Modulation (PWM) modes, examining their impacts on luminous flux, temperature, total color shift, and angular color shift. Our findings reveal that the CW mode offers higher luminous flux and reduced temperatures, while the PWM mode ensures enhanced color stability across different currents and viewing angles, especially for white LEDs; however, this stability does not extend to other LED types. From both optical and visual performance perspectives, the study emphasizes optimizing the driving current to meet regulatory requirements and ensure consistent color perception. These insights are crucial for automotive lighting design, contributing to improved regulatory compliance and aesthetic quality.
... This suggests that light stimuli exert influence not only on the eyes but also on the brain. Flickering stimuli are associated with largeamplitude neural responses within the visual cortex (Gentile and Aguirre 2020); more precisely, the source of discomfort is transient abnormal synchronized activity of brain cells (Wilkins et al. 2010). Photosensitive epilepsy has been documented not only in humans but also in yellow baboon (Papio cynocephalus), dogs (Canis lupus familiaris), and birds, indicating a lack of species specificity (Corcoran et al. 1979;Batini et al. 2004;Da Silva and Leal 2017;Szabó and Fischer 2021). ...
... Interestingly, the same flickering frequency of 15 Hz is also known to be highly stimulating for the human brain (Takahashi et al. 1981;Parra et al. 2007). It is well-documented that red flickering lights can activate the visual cortex through the red cones, leading to transient abnormal synchronized activity in human brain cells (Tobimatsu et al. 1999;Wilkins et al. 2010). However, there are significant structural differences between the brains of mammals and birds. ...
Article
Full-text available
Human-bird conflicts are in a critical state, involving economic losses such as agricultural losses, bird strikes on aircraft and avian influenza. Traditional technologies leveraging bird vision and hearing often lose their effectiveness over time as birds become habituated to these stimuli. To address these challenges, our study introduces a novel countermeasure technology based on neurophysiology. The human brain reacts to flickering light, which can cause symptoms like headaches, nausea, and dizziness. In extremely rare cases, 15 Hz flickering red light can even lead to epilepsy. Not only humans, but chickens also suffer from 14 Hz flickering light. This led us to consider the possibility that similar flickering light stimuli could be applicable to bird management. In our experiments conducted during the day, we used long-range flashlights. White flickering light had no effect on bird escape behavior. However, when cellophane film was attached to the flashlights to restrict the wavelength, the emitted red light induced escape behavior in birds. Additionally, employing two types of flashlights to generate flickering red + blue or red + green lights elicited escape behavior. However, the blue and green combination proved to be less effective. The most intense flickering frequency for crows was 15 Hz. These results are highly similar to those found in human neurophysiology, showing that red light alone and the combination of red and blue lights have the most significant impact on the brain. By measuring the flight initiation distance (FID) of birds, we found that illuminated areas had a significantly higher FID (137 m) compared to non-illuminated areas (12 m). These findings suggest that applying principles of human physiology to wildlife management can offer new solutions for bird damage control.
... The phantom array effect can create a pattern of contours induced by saccades (a rapid eye movement) across light sources exhibiting TLM, and has a visible frequency of up to 11 kHz (Brown et al. 2020). Most notably, TLM can cause not only unwanted visual perceptions but also adverse effects on human health and wellbeing (Inger et al. 2014;Shepherd 2010;Wilkins et al. 1989Wilkins et al. , 2010. ...
... The terms "no strobe/serious strobe" and "continuous motion/discrete motion" were used to reflect stroboscopic detectability. Visual performance, headache, and mood were included as they are often reported as the effects of TLM on health problems in offices (Inger et al. 2014;Shepherd 2010;Wilkins et al. 1989Wilkins et al. , 2010. ...
... Ratings of annoyingness were lower in France, which seemed most likely to be a problem of translation. Nonetheless, ratings of annoyingness were higher overall light sources for people who were more sensitive to pattern glare, as assessed with the Wilkins and Evans Pattern Glare test (Wilkins et al., 2010). Over the whole sample (both countries and regardless of sensitivity), the light sources with SVM = 0.9 were rated as more annoying than those of SVM = 0.4. ...
Book
This report is a comprehensive review of the scientific knowledge acquired over the last ten years on the effects of solid state lighting (SSL) on human health. The report considers issues that concern both large fractions of the population as well as small sensitive groups of people. The report is primarily intended to support policymakers, offering guidance on lighting and health, as they consider product regulatory requirements, procurement specifications, programmes to raise public awareness, and other initiatives. However, the report will also be useful to the entire lighting community, including scientists engaged in fundamental and applied lighting research, illumination engineers, manufacturers, and lighting practitioners. This report is available at https://www.iea-4e.org/ssl/news/new-comprehensive-report-on-health-effects-of-solid-state-lighting/
... where A k , ϕ k are respectively the amplitude and the initial phase of the signal, f s is the fundamental signal frequency, α is a direct current component to make b k (t) always a positive number, and D k is the harmonic order for f s , which is the natural number that is sufficiently far apart to avoid frequency interference between LED light sources. Also, we set their frequencies to higher than 165 Hz which humans do not perceive flickering and lead to health issues and biologically affecting humans [36]. The value of D k corresponds to the number of stripes in the captured image and uniquely identifies the received signal from each LED. ...
Article
Full-text available
Because of the drone's penetration into our society, the demand for their indoor positioning has increased. However, its standard technology has not been established yet. This paper describes an indoor three-dimensional tracking method for drones, using the drone's built-in camera to capture light reflected from the floor. Using a captured image and video data captured during the drone's flight, the proposed method can estimate the drone's position and trajectory. A drone's built-in camera is usually unable to capture light directly from ceiling light sources because of its limited field of view and gimbal angles. To address this problem, the proposed method captures the light indirectly, as the reflections from the floor of ceiling light-emitting diodes (LEDs), in the video stream acquired by its rolling-shutter camera. The three-dimensional position is estimated by calculating the received signal strength of each individual LED for a single video frame during the flight and fitting this data to a model generated by simulation images. In an indoor environment without external lights, we captured the reflected light from floor surfaces using the drone's camera under gimbal control and analyzed the captured images offline. Experimental results gave an absolute error of 0.34 m at the 90th percentile for 3D positioning when hovering and using a single-frame image. For a linear flight path, the error was 0.31 m. The computation time for 3D position estimation was 1.12 s. We also discussed limitations related to real-time and real-world applications, together with approaches to addressing these limitations.
... The biological impact of this factor on men is a function of modulation depth, flicker frequency, brightness, and other factors. The studies [18][19][20] summarize the results of the influence of flickering on biological processes and the conditions of their occurrence. It is noted that in the frequency range of 3-70 Hz, potential risks include convulsions, especially in persons with a diagnosis of epilepsy, as well as some specific neurological symptoms, including headache. ...
... For instance, the necessity for an eavesdropper to be in the same room as the light source, due to light's inability to penetrate walls, adds a layer of physical security. However, VLC technology has its own challenges, including the need for a direct Line of Sight (LoS) between the transmitter and receiver and potential issues such as noise, flicker, or interference effects, which are areas of ongoing research and development [12], [13]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Within the ongoing developments in digitalization and transformation towards an industrial metaverse, robust, fast and trustworthy communication is indispensable. Therefore, the developments in Beyond 5G (B5G) and Sixth Generation (6G) technologies, including enabling technologies such as Artificial Intelligence (AI) methods, Terahertz Communication, and Optical Wireless Communication (OWC) are opening up additional potential and perspectives. This work focuses in particular on Visible Light Communication (VLC) as a complementary and alternative technology for applications with particular requirements for minimal electromagnetic interference and high data security. Among other aspects like latency and throughput, these are compared with established protocols such as Wireless Fidelity (WiFi), Bluetooth, Bluetooth Low Energy (BLE) and Ethernet. The analysis is conducted by examining the application of collaborative robots (Cobots), specifically illustrated through the deployment of two Dobots MG400 robotic arms. The results indicate that depending on the use case, all traditional methods exhibit strengths and weaknesses, but VLC appears to be a comparably robust, low-latency solution. Clear advantages can be seen, particularly in critical areas where traditional Radio Frequency (RF) communications may pose security risks or suffer from interference.
Article
Neurodegenerative retinal diseases such as age-related macular degeneration, glaucoma, and diabetic retinopathy remain the leading causes of low vision and blindness worldwide. Visual rehabilitation of patients who are visually impaired due to neurodegenerative diseases of the retina requires solving problems associated with a violation of the structure of neural networks and a deficiency of visual functions provided by these networks. Despite certain successes in the application of innovative methods of therapy, it is important to develop new approaches to visual rehabilitation to improve the quality of life of visually impaired patients. In visual rehabilitation, not only medical, but also various non-pharmacological therapy strategies are widely used to protect and restore the structure of the retina and its function. Among them, a separate niche is occupied by the technologies of vision stimulation therapy (phototherapy), the analysis of the main aspects of which is the task of this review. The vector of new research in the field of phototherapy is aimed at developing methods that can maximize the plasticity of the visual system to increase the effectiveness of its protection and recovery in neurodegenerative pathology. In this regard, new technologies of fractal phototherapy have great potential in visual rehabilitation.
Article
Full-text available
Alternating current can result in flickering—or pulsing—in the brightness of light emitted by luminaires. Lighting flicker typically occurs in the range of 100 to 140 cycles per second (Hz), which is too fast for visual perception by most organisms. However, evidence indicates that many organisms perceive flicker with non-visual photoreceptors present on the retinas. Exposure to flickering lights at night disrupts the circadian rhythm of organisms, leading to symptoms similar to blue light exposure at night. The traditional method for detecting flickering is with a flickermeter held near a single light. In this paper, we explore the use of high-speed camera data in the collection of temporal profiles for groups of luminaires simultaneously at distances ranging from several meters to several kilometers. Temporal profiles are extracted for individual lighting features and the full scene. The identification of luminaire types is based on their spectral signatures. With the camera data, it is possible to identify flickering and non-flickering lights, to determine the flicker frequency, to calculate percent flicker and the flicker index, and to identify groups of lights whose flickers are synchronized. Both flickering and non-flickering luminaires can be found for LED, metal halide, fluorescent, and compact fluorescent lights. To date, flickering has been detected in all of the incandescent, high-pressure sodium, and low-pressure sodium luminaires that we measured. We found that flicker synchronization is often present for lights installed within a single facility and also for strings of streetlights. We also found that flicker exposure can come from the light reflected off of the earth’s surface. Luminaires designed to illuminate large areas often saturate high-speed camera data collection. This saturation can be reduced or eliminated using neutral density filters on the camera. Published experimental data on the impacts of flicker on organisms remains sparse. Many studies have drawn inferences on the impacts of spectral and lighting brightness on organisms without controlling for flicker. Our conclusion is that lighting flicker is a type of light pollution. The use of high-speed camera data makes it easier to include flicker as a variable in studies regarding the impacts of lighting on organisms.
Article
Full-text available
It is known that temporal modulation of the source illuminating a scene can affect visual performance. Nevertheless, there is a lack of evidence showing this in a natural setting with a simple, natural task. We devised a simple test of visual function using a pencil and paper visual search technique − identifying low-contrast printed numbers on the sheet of paper. We measured the effect of the temporal modulation of the fluorescent lighting upon visual performance on this task. We constructed two office environments lit with both tubular and compact fluorescent lamps, completely identical except for the lamp ballasting. Office A equipped with electronic ballasts, producing a light emission mainly modulated at 60 kHz having a modulation percentage of 3 percent for 100 Hz component; office B equipped with magnetic ballasts, producing a temporally modulated light which fundamental component—100 Hz frequency—has a modulation percentage of 32 percent).
Book
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
The weekly incidence of headaches among office workers was compared when the offices were lit by fluorescent lighting where the fluorescent tubes were operated by (a) a conventional switch-start circuit with choke ballast providing illumination that pulsated with a modulation depth of 43-49% and a principal frequency component at 100 Hz; (b) an electronic start circuit with choke ballast giving illumination with similar characteristics; (c) an electronic ballast driving the lamps at about 32 kHz and reducing the 100 Hz modulation to less than 7%. In a double-blind cross-over design, the average incidence of headaches and eyestrain was more than halved under high-frequency lighting. The incidence was unaffected by the speed with which the tubes ignited. Headaches tended to decrease with the height of the office above the ground and thus with increasing natural light. Office occupants chose to switch on the high-frequency lighting for 30% longer on average.
Article
Full-text available
With two electrical analogs of the brightness system it is shown that at a low luminance level (<5 photons) and in the low-frequency region (<2 cps), when under the special visual conditions no attenuation in the system occurs and with a symmetrical luminance variation such as a sinusoidal modulation or a square-wave modulation with a 1:1 on-off ratio, half the crest-to-trough value of the percentage variation at flicker fusion equals the internal threshold value r0; but with an asymmetrical variation such as a square-wave modulation with a 1:3 on-off ratio, it is the crest value of the periodical percentage variation above the mean luminance level which at flicker fusion equals the internal threshold value r0. At high luminance levels an overshoot in the low-frequency region occurs with both forms of square-wave modulation in accordance with the shape of the attenuation characteristic (AC) of the system under the experimental circumstances. At the steep slope of the ACs for the whole luminance range in cone vision, half the crest-to-trough value of the Fourier fundamental percentage variation at the site of the threshold mechanism located anywhere in the system, equals the internal threshold value r0 at flicker fusion.
Article
Full-text available
Two experiments are described in which eye movements were monitored as subjects performed a simple target-spotting task under conditions of intermittent illumination produced by varying the display-screen frame rate on a computer VDU. In Experiment 1, subjects executed a saccade from a fixation point to a target which appeared randomly at a fixed eccentricity of 14 character positions to the left or right. Saccade latency did not differ reliably as a function of screen refresh rate, but average saccade extent at 70 Hz and 110 Hz was reliably shorter than at 90 Hz and 100 Hz. Experiment 2 examined the same task using a range of target eccentricities (7, 14, and 28 character positions to the left and right) and across a wider range of screen refresh rates. The results confirmed the curvilinear relationship obtained in Experiment 1, with average saccade extent reliably shorter at refresh rates of 50 Hz and 125 Hz than at 75 Hz and 100 Hz. While the effect was greater for remote targets, analyses of the proportional target error failed to show a reliable interaction between target eccentricity and display refresh rate. In contrast to Experiment 1, there was a pronounced effect of refresh rate on saccade latency (corrected for time to write the screen frame), with shorter latencies at higher refresh rates. It may be concluded that pulsation at frequencies above fusion disrupts saccade control. However, the curvilinear functional relationship between screen refresh rate and saccade extent obtained in these studies differs from previously reported effects of intermittent illumination on the average size of "entry saccades" (the first saccade to enter a given word) in a task involving word identification (Kennedy & Murray, 1993a, 1996). This conflict of data may arise in part because within-word adjustments in viewing position, which are typical of normal reading, influence measures of average saccade extent.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of fluorescent light spectral composition and flicker rate on visual performance and visual comfort were studied on 48 undergraduate students using two different rates of flicker: conventional low-frequency flicker (120 Hz) and high frequency flicker(between 20-60 kHz); and three different light sources; full-spectrum lamps, cool-white lamps, and filtered-cool-white lamps. The design was a 2×3 (Flicker rate × Light source) mixed within-between ANOVA. Visual performance and time on visual performance task were assessed using a Landolt ring task. Visual comfort was assessed by self-report after a period of reading difficult text. Visual performance scores of 18-24-year-old male and female university students were significantly higher in the high-frequency flicker condition than the low-frequency flicker condition. There were no other statistically significant effects. Health status was unrelated to visual performance. Neurophysiological explanations are discussed. The finding that an energy-efficient means of driving fluorescent lamps also can improve visual performance provides added impetus to adopt this new technology.
Article
The temporal modulation of light from halophosphate, triphosphor and multiband fluorescent lamps (controlled by a conventional choke circuit) was measured as a function of wavelength. Within each category, all lamps had similar functions for peak-peak modulation. At the short-wavelength end of the visible spectrum all lamps showed a modulation near 100%. Halophosphate and multiband lamps had a low modulation at the long-wavelength end of the spectrum and gave the lowest overall modulation. Certain deluxe lamps had a modulation greater than 80% throughout the spectrum. The modulation of photopic energy, and energy transduced by the photoreceptors was calculated. Triphosphor lamps gave greater modulation than halophosphate, the lowest modulation being from warm-white halophosphate lamps.
Article
Office Workers Visual Performance and Temporal Modulation of Fluorescent Lighting Jaen M *, Sandoval J *, Colombo E *, and Troscianko T ** Abstract—It is known that temporal modulation of the source illuminating a scene can affect visual performance. Nevertheless, there is a lack of evidence showing this, in a natural setting with a simple, natural task. We devised a simple test of visual function using a pencil and paper visual search technique – identifying low-contrast printed numbers on the sheet of paper. We measured the effect of the temporal modulation of the fluorescent lighting upon visual performance on this task. We constructed two office environments lit with both tubular and compact fluorescent lamps, completely identical except for the lamp ballasting. Office A equipped with electronic ballasts, producing a light emission mainly modulated at 60 kHz having a modulation percentage of 3 percent for 100 Hz component; office B equipped with magnetic ballasts, producing a temporally modulated light which fundamental component—100 Hz frequency—has a modulation percentage of 32 percent). Fifty students aged 20 to 22 years performed the same simple visual search tasks (errors were not allowed) under two lighting conditions. This permits evaluation of visual performance by analysis of a single dependent variable, the time taken to complete the task. An analysis of variance (considering only the complete data of 39 subjects) shows that the “modulation” factor is statistically significant (F(1,37)�10.21, p �0.00285). At the end of the session, the observers performed a subjective appraisal of the lighting, by means of a simple questionnaire for each lighting condition. A within-subject ANOVA shows that mean values for each factor in both offices are indistinguishable within a statistical significance level of 5 percent. These results confirm the hypothesis that the observers, even though they cannot visually discriminate between the two light modulation conditions, show significantly higher visual performance when performing the task in low modulation condition. Additionally it was suggested that the method would be capable of identifying the more sensitive individuals in the group. * Departamento de Luminotecnia, Luz y Vision, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Tecnologıa, Universidad Nacional de Tucuman, Argentina - mjaen@herrera.unt.edu.ar. ** Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK. LEUKOS VOL 1 NO 4 APRIL 2005 PAGES 27– 4 6 ©2005 The Illuminating Engineering Society of North America doi: 10.1582/LEUKOS.01.04.002 Keywords—temporal light modulation, high frequency, flicker sensitivity, visual performance, visual search task.
Conference Paper
This paper discusses the difficulties in dimming Edison socket LED lamps directly from residential phase modulated dimmer switches. In order to explain these difficulties PSpice models for the dimmers are proposed that necessarily include diac characteristics to improve accuracy. A method to dim the LEDs from the residential dimmers is discussed.
Article
The visual systems of birds are hypothesized to have higher temporal resolution than those of humans, suggesting that they may be able to perceive the flicker emitted from conventional low-frequency fluorescent lights (LF; 100 Hz in Europe, 120 Hz in the U.S.A.). These lights are commonly used in the housing of captive birds and this may affect both their welfare and performance in experiments. We carried out mate choice experiments on European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, under both low- and high-frequency fluorescent lights (HF; >30 kHz, at which flicker is imperceptible). Indicators of male condition and size, together with the reflectance spectra and length of the males' throat feathers, were also recorded to ascertain which variables correlated with female preference. Females ranked males consistently under HF, but not LF, lighting, and individual females chose different males under the two lighting types. Under HF lighting, females chose to spend more time with males that had longer throat feathers. The flicker rate of the light clearly affected the choices made by the females, possibly because of nonspecific stress effects or decreased discrimination ability. Our results imply that careful interpretation of mate choice experiments is needed, especially with regard to the lighting types used, to elucidate the real cause behind any variation shown.