ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

It is well established that negative stereotypes can undermine women's performance on mathematics tests. Despite considerable laboratory evidence for the role of “stereotype threat” in girls' and women's math test performance, the relevance of such findings for the “real world” gender test-score gap remains unclear and debates about causes focus primarily on innate sex differences in cognitive capacity. Reported here are results of a field experiment that tested the usefulness of the stereotype threat formulation for understanding women's performance in upper levels of college mathematics — men and women who are highly motivated and proficient mathematicians and who are in the pipeline to mathematics and science professions. Our primary hypothesis was confirmed. Test performance of women in a stereotype-nullifying presentation of the test in an experimental group was raised significantly to surpass that of the men in the course. In a control group, in which test-takers were given the test under normal test instructions, women and men performed equally. The pattern of results suggests that even among the most highly qualified and persistent women in college mathematics, stereotype threat suppresses test performance.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women's
achievement in high-level math courses
Catherine Good
a,
, Joshua Aronson
b,
, Jayne Ann Harder
c
a
Barnard College, Columbia University, United States
b
New York University, United States
c
The University of Texas, Austin, United States
Available online 20 November 2007
Abstract
It is well established that negative stereotypes can undermine women's performance on mathematics tests. Despite considerable
laboratory evidence for the role of stereotype threatin girls' and women's math test performance, the relevance of such findings
for the real worldgender test-score gap remains unclear and debates about causes focus primarily on innate sex differences in
cognitive capacity. Reported here are results of a field experiment that tested the usefulness of the stereotype threat formulation for
understanding women's performance in upper levels of college mathematics men and women who are highly motivated and
proficient mathematicians and who are in the pipeline to mathematics and science professions. Our primary hypothesis was
confirmed. Test performance of women in a stereotype-nullifying presentation of the test in an experimental group was raised
significantly to surpass that of the men in the course. In a control group, in which test-takers were given the test under normal test
instructions, women and men performed equally. The pattern of results suggests that even among the most highly qualified and
persistent women in college mathematics, stereotype threat suppresses test performance.
© 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Stereotype threat; Gender stereotypes; Math; Sex differences; Achievement; Adolescence
1. Introduction
How much of the gender gap in math and science achievement is attributable to nature and how much to nurture is a
source of concern, debate, and political thin ice for many who have a stake in understanding the issue of why boys and
girls appear to differ in mathematical ability. Although some studies have found evidence for biological differences
(e.g., Halpern, 1992), there remains considerable debate on the extent to which biology explains the differential
A
vailable online at www.sciencedirect.com
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 17 28
Preparation of this paper was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0217251) to Catherine Good and Carol S. Dweck,
and by a National Science Foundation CAREER award (BCS-9875957) to Joshua Aronson.
Corresponding authors. Good is to be contacted at Baruch College, CUNY, Vertical Campus Room 8-215, One Bernard Baruch Way, New York,
NY 10010. Tel.: 646 207 4413; fax: 646 312 3781. Aronson, New York University, Department of Applied Psychology, 239 Greene Street, 5th Floor,
New York, NY 10003, United States.
E-mail addresses: Catherine_Good@baruch.cuny.edu (C. Good), joshua.aronson@nyu.edu (J. Aronson).
0193-3973/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2007.10.004
outcomes in math achievement (Benbow & Stanley, 1980; Eccles, Adler, & Meece, 1984; Gottfredson, 2002; Pinker,
2005; Spelke, 2005; Stipek & Gralinski, 1991). The depth and political charge of this debate was underscored recently
when then-president of Harvard University, Lawrence Summers, suggested during a speech (Summers, 2005) that one
of the primary reasons men vastly outnumber women at the upper rungs of science and mathematics professions is
biological differences that result in different availability of aptitude at the high end.In other words, he argued that
women are not significantly hindered merely by social forces; rather their limitation is mostly attributable to innate
biological differences that make them less capable than men of doing high-level mathematics.
In this paper, we present data that cast doubt on the different availability of aptitude at the high endhypothesis.
Specifically, we report data from a field study examining the relevance of stereotype threat an environmental
impediment to women's math performance to the test-score gap between men and women in a context that is at the
epicenter of the nature-nurture debate in the advanced mathematics courses at a major university. Such courses are
important because they are the pipelinesto hard-science careers. For students to enter the fields where women are
traditionally under-represented, they must succeed in these courses. The design of the experiment comparing a group
of men and women for whom the stereotype is nullified with a control group who receives the same test under standard
testing procedures permits us to evaluate whether stereotype threat is a significant factor in the test-score gap among
these students. Our data show that there are significant (albeit not equal) numbers of talented women in the pipeline to
hard-science professions. Moreover, the data strongly suggest that not only are women able to achieve grades that are
on par with men's grades in the most difficult mathematics courses, but once disruptive social forces are minimized,
women can even surpass men on difficult math tests.
1.1. Background: females' math achievement and stereotype threat
The gap in mathematics performance that has rekindled the nature-nurture debate can be documented at practically
every level of schooling, especially when looking at the highest levels of achievers (National Science Foundation,
2006). For example, a higher percentage of males than females perform at or above proficiency in mathematics in the
fourth (35% versus 30%), eighth (30% versus 27%), and twelfth (19% versus 14%) grades. Moreover, although the gap
between males and females on the SAT has shrunk over the years, males still outperform females by 34 points on this
high-stakes test (College Board, 2005). What is more, these early differences in performance may lay the foundation for
future career aspirations. For example, although women earn nearly half of all science and engineering doctoral
degrees, they are vastly under-represented in the so-called hardsciences. Specifically, in 2003 women earned over
half of the doctoral degrees in the social and behavioral sciences, yet they earned only 29% of doctoral degrees in the
physical sciences, 24% of doctoral degrees in mathematics, and only 19% of doctoral degrees in engineering (National
Science Foundation, 2006).
What explains these sex-based performance and participation differences? Although biological factors may
represent a potent factor (see Halpern, 1992 for a review), the role of sociocultural forces, such as sex stereotypes,
has also been well established (Eccles, 1994; Eccles & Jacobs, 1992; Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990; Good, Dweck, &
Rattan, in preparation-a,b; see Aronson & Steele, 2005; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002 for reviews). For
example, one line of research suggests that parents are important socializers of sex-based achievement
differences in mathematics (Eccles, 1994; Eccles & Jacobs, 1992; Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990). Expectancies
set early in childhood by parents may lay the foundation for later underperformance, interest, and participation during
adolescence.
Another growing line of research suggests that girls and women suffer negative performance outcomes on math
tests, not necessarily because they are socialized by parents to lack ability, but because of their vulnerability to negative
stereotypes disseminated in the broader culture. The research shows that when stereotypes are not activated, or if they
are nullified by other cues in the environment, girls and women perform better. When negative stereotypes are
activated and left unchecked, they trigger a number of disruptive psychological processes that undermine test
performance (Croizet et al., 2004; Dar-Nimrod & Heine, 2006; Davies, Spencer, Quinn, & Gerhardstein, 2002; Good,
Aronson, & Inzlicht, 2003; Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Johns, Schmader, & Martens, 2005; McGlone & Aronson,
2006; McIntyre, Paulson, & Lord, 2003; Schmader, 2002; Schmader & Johns, 2003; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999).
This phenomenon is called stereotype threatand has been applied to the underperformance of African-Americans,
Latinos, and a variety of other minority groups (for reviews see Aronson & Steele, 2005; Steele, Spencer & Aronson,
2002).
18 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
This research has offered a better understanding of the situations that are most likely to lead to underperformance
(Good et al., in preparation-a,b; Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Spencer et al., 1999), the age at which females are likely to
experience stereotype threat (Good & Aronson, in preparation), and methods of overcoming stereotype threat
(Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002; Cohen, Garcia, & Master, 2006; Good et al., 2003; Johns et al., 2005). For example,
researchers have found that certain situations, such as taking a diagnostic test in a domain in which one's group faces
negative stereotypes, is sufficient to invoke underperformance on the test (Steele & Aronson, 1995). In addition,
anything that makes one's social identity salient such as indicating one's race prior to taking a diagnostic verbal test
can also lead to stereotype-based underperformance (Steele & Aronson, 1995). Even the sex composition of the testing
situation can influence vulnerability to stereotype threat: researchers have found that women's performance on a math
test went down as a function of the number of men taking the test in the same room (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000).
Studies such as these clearly implicate both the social situation and the temporary mindset of the performer in the
underperformance of stereotyped groups, such as women in math.
Yet despite considerable empirical support, stereotype threat is not widely accepted as a factor in the mathematics
gender gap. One reason for this, we believe, is that much of the research showing that stereotype threat undermines test
performance has been conducted in psychology laboratories. As such, results can easily be discounted as irrelevant to
test-score gaps existing in the real world. For example, Sackett and his colleagues (Sackett, Hardison, & Cullen, 2004)
recently argued that stereotype threat experiments conducted by Steele and Aronson (1995) which demonstrated
large effects of stereotypes on the test scores of African-Americans were interesting but irrelevant to the test-score
gap. In essence, this argument suggests that the experiments have shown that an existing gap can be made larger by
inducing stereotype threat but that the studies fail to show that reducing stereotype threat in the real world of the
classroom or testing center would narrow the actual gap between black and white students.
Scientific evidence at odds with this argument is accumulating, demonstrating specifically that reducing
stereotype threat significantly narrows the gap between stereotyped and non-stereotyped students. For example, a
large study conducted by Stricker and Ward (2004) for the Educational Testing Service (ETS) found that simply
moving the standard demographic inquiry about test-taker gender to the end of the test (and thus reducing the level of
stereotype threat during the test) resulted in significantly higher performance among women who took the AP
calculus test. The results showed that if ETS made official this simple change in procedure, 4700 additional female
students would receive Advanced Placement credit in calculus each year. Perhaps attesting to the politicized nature of
research in this area, Stricker and Ward presented their data as indicating no significant effect of asking about gender
on women's performance. Yet in fact, a reanalysis of the ETS data (Danaher & Crandall, in press)showedthese
effects to be substantial and significant. Other studies have shown the relevance of stereotype threat by showing that
interventions that reduce stereotype threat also produce significant reductions of performance gaps relative to control
groups (Brown & Day, 2006; Cohen et al., 2006; McGlone & Aronson, 2006). This strongly suggests the role of
stereotype threat in extant gaps between men and women in mathematics and minorities and whites in all areas of
cognitive ability.
1.2. Developmental trends in stereotype threat
Research investigating the contribution of stereotype threat to women's mathematics performance has focused, for
the most part, on women in college-level mathematics (see Steele et al., 2002, for a review), leaving unanswered the
question of when stereotype-based underperformance begins to emerge. The few studies that have tackled this question
suggest that stereotyped individuals may be vulnerable to stereotype threat as early as elementary school. For example,
African-American and Latino children as young as six years old performed more poorly on a cognitive task when that
task was described as a measure of ability than when the same task was described as a problem-solving task (McKown
& Weinstein, 2003). However, this difference emerged only for those children who were aware of broadly held
stereotypes about academic ability. Moreover, race/ethnicity-based stereotypes and sex-based stereotypes do not
necessarily operate in the same fashion. For example, stereotype threat has been found to undermine girls' math
performance in the sixth grade, but not in the fourth or fifth grades (Good & Aronson, in preparation). Regardless of the
specific age or grade level at which negative stereotypes were found to affect performance, these studies illustrate that
stereotype threat begins to undermine performance in children's elementary years. And by late adolescence, its
cumulative effects can undermine students' career plans (Good et al., in preparation-a), social identities (Good, Dweck,
& Aronson, 2007), and performance on high-stakes evaluations.
19C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
1.3. Methods of overcoming stereotype threat
In recent years, some researchers have turned their attention toward finding methods of alleviating stereotype threat.
One line of research has addressed the underlying message of the stereotype that stereotyped individuals are
inherently limited because of their group membership. This approach has encouraged stereotyped individuals to reject
the idea that intelligence is a fixed trait and instead to adopt the mindset that intelligence is a quality that can be
increased with hard work and effort. In one study, African-American college students who were exposed to this view
received higher grade point averages and later reported that academic achievement was more important to their senses
of selves (Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002). In a related study, college students mentored middle school students and
helped them create web-pages advocating the view that intelligence is a malleable capacity (Good, Aronson, & Inzlicht,
2003). At the end of the year, the middle school females who received the malleable message earned higher scores on
the state-wide standardized test in math than their female peers who instead received an anti-drug message as part of the
intervention. In a similarly remarkable study, Cohen, Garcia and Master (2006) reduced the blackwhite GPA gap
among low-income middle school students by affirming the students' self-concepts (and presumably inoculating them
from stereotype threat) at the beginning of the school term.
These techniques, although effective, are not likely to be implemented in standard testing situations such as the SAT,
GRE, etc. Furthermore, they are unlikely to be adopted by most mathematics curricula. Thus another aim of this
research was to test whether or not stereotype threat could be reduced in a manner that would be easily replicated and
implemented in everyday school experiences.
In sum, although stereotype threat has been found to undermine the performance of females in mathematics as
early as the sixth grade, most studies investigating the effects of stereotypes on females' math achievement have
focused on college-aged women. Moreover, many of these studies have concentrated primarily on females'
performance on general mathematics tasks. Although most studies recruit participants who are highly identified
with mathematics, or who have performed well on mathematics tasks in the past (for example, Spencer et al., 1999),
none have tested whether or not stereotype threat suppresses the scores of females who are enrolled in the most
difficult mathematics courses that lay the foundation for future careers in math and science. Consequently, we need
to obtain information about those in the pipeline who aspire to be the women who would fill the void described by
Summers. For example, Spencer and his colleagues (Spencer et al., 1999) used the psychology subject pool for
recruitment of study participants. Although these participants also were screened for high math achievement (for
example, earning a B or better in calculus), the population likely included females who had no intention of pursuing
a math or math-based major or career. Consequently, these past studies left unaddressed the issue of the role of
stereotypes and stereotype threat in math performance in the upper end of the math ability distribution. Most
importantly, however, the experiments were conducted in the laboratory and thus their applicability to real-world
test-score gaps can be questioned. In sum, the goal of the present study was to test whether the negative effects of
stereotype threat extend beyond the laboratory and beyond women's mathematics performance in general to
women's underperformance in the most difficult college mathematics courses those that produce future
mathematicians, engineers, and scientists, and those that conceivably might be avoided by women vulnerable to
stereotype threat.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and design
Participants were 174 calculus students enrolled in the final course of the most rigorous and fast-paced calculus
sequence offered by the university, a large public university in the southwest. This course satisfied degree
requirements for mathematics, engineering, and many of the natural sciences such as biology, chemistry, geology,
and physics. Because students who enrolled in the course had already successfully navigated the previous semester's
gatekeepercourse, they were well on their way to mathematics, engineering, or science degrees. Moreover, the
university offered less rigorous calculus courses, but these were generally taken by students not planning to major in
math or science.
Of the 174 students who took part in the study, 100 were male, 57 were female, and 17 did not report their sex. We
analyzed data only for those participants who indicated their sex, leaving a final sample of 157 participants. Of these,
20 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
91 self-identified as Anglo-American (36 females, 55 males), 31 self-identified as Asian-American (10 females,
21 males), 17 self-identified as Hispanic-American (6 females, 11 males), 7 self-identified as African-American
(2 females, 5 males), and 11 either did not report their ethnicity or indicated other(3 females, 8 males). In addition,
because some ethnic groups had low representation in the calculus class we did not conduct analyses based on ethnicity.
We randomly assigned participants to one of two conditions in a 2 (sex) by 2 (test description: stereotype threat, non-
threat) factorial design.
2.2. Materials and procedure
We designed a calculus test, aligned with the topics the professors were discussing in their calculus courses, to be
used as a practice test for an upcoming course examination. Test items were taken from the Graduate Record
Examination (GRE) Mathematics Subject test that represented the same content as covered in the calculus course.
These items were pilot tested with 12 advanced calculus students who were enrolled in the same level course as the
participants in this study to ensure an adequate level of difficulty for the practice test.
Toward the end of the semester, the calculus professors announced that a practice test would be administered during
the discussion sections the following week. The professors explained that the practice test would be similar in content
to an upcoming course examination and thus would provide a good indicator of the students' preparedness for the
impending examination. The professors offered extra credit on the course examination based on the students'
performance on the practice test. In reality, every student who took the practice test received the same number of extra
credit points, regardless of their performance. Extra credit was offered to increase the likelihood that students would
take the test seriously and perhaps experience it psychologically as they would a regular course examination.
Additional protocols were implemented to replicate the testing conditions of standard course examinations. For
example, the sex composition of the practice test reflected that of the course, and teaching assistants were present
(as they were during regular course examinations). With these protocols, the alignment of the content of the test
with the course topics, the diagnostic nature of the test, and the potential for performance on the practice test to
influence final course grades, the testing situation was made as similar as possible to that of the students' regular
course examinations.
On the day of the practice test, the teaching assistant introduced the researcher who then told the students the
following:
The material on this practice test covers a range of calculus topics, just like your examination next week will, so your
performance on this test will be a good indicator of your readiness for your exam. Your test will be graded by your
teaching assistant so that she/he may have a better idea of the areas where each of you may need a little extra help
before next week's exam. Your score on this test will be counted as extra credit in this course. Thank you for
participating and giving the test a genuine effort.
The researcher then presented an example problem and instructed the students to indicate how confident they were
in their answer for each question (1 = low confidence; 5 = high confidence).
Next, the researcher distributed the research packets, which included the consent form, the experimental
manipulation, the practice test, a post-measure questionnaire, demographic information questions, and a debriefing
section. The packets were randomized. Although the experimenter was knowledgeable about the hypotheses of the
study, the experimenter remained blind to condition. Sealed tabs separated each section of the packet, and the
researcher instructed the students not to proceed to succeeding sections until told to do so.
Students then read and signed the consent page and then read the manipulation page. The manipulation consisted of
a description of the nature of the test. Students in the stereotype threat condition read a statement indicating that the test
they were about to take was designed to measure their math abilities:
For the next 20 min, you will be taking a math test aimed at measuring your mathematical abilities. Why? As you
probably know, math skills are crucial to performance in many important subjects in college. Yet surprisingly little is
known about the mental processes underlying math ability. This research is aimed at better understanding what
makes some people better at math than others. After you finish the test, your teacher will score it. This will enable us
to analyze your performance and compare it with other students taking this test.
21C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
Diagnostic statements such as these have been shown to invoke stereotype threat in the past (e.g., Steele & Aronson,
1995). Students in the non-threat condition read the same diagnostic information but with the following added
paragraph:
What about gender differences? This mathematics test has not shown any gender differences in performance or
mathematics ability. The test has been piloted in many mathematics courses across the nation to determine how
reliable and valid the test is for measuring mathematics ability. Analysis of thousands of students' test results has
shown that males and females perform equally well on this test. In other words, this mathematics test shows no
gender differences.
Telling students that a test has never shown any gender differences in the past has been shown to reduce stereotype
threat for women on general mathematics tasks (Quinn & Spencer, 2001; Spencer et al., 1999).
Importantly, the non-threat condition in this study differs from past studies in that the diagnosticity of the test was
retained. In past studies, stereotype threat was reduced either by ensuring females that the test was gender-fair while at the
same time not explicitly mentioning the diagnostic nature of the test (e.g., Quinn & Spencer, 2001; Spencer et al., 1999),
or by explicitly nullifying the assumed diagnosticity of the test (Steele & Aronson, 1995). Because removing the
diagnostic nature of course examinations is an unrealistic expectation for regular course examinations or for standardized
math testing situations, we wanted to test whether or not stereotype threat could be reduced simply by addressing the
specter of gender-based performance differences within the context an explicitly diagnostic examination.
In addition, past studies have induced stereotype threat either by explicitly stating that males do better than females on
the test or by explicitly raising the possibility of sex-based performance differences (Spencer et al., study 2, 1999). This,
however, is unlikely to be the way that stereotype threat operates in real-world settings. Instead, we relied on a more
realistic and common occurrence to create the threat simply taking a diagnostic math test. Furthermore, although
other studies have induced stereotype threat by simply telling participants that they were about to take a math test, the
extent to which their performance on this test would be indicative of underlying mathematics ability was not explicitly
addressed (e.g., Spencer et al., 1999). Our study, in contrast, explicitly stated that the test was diagnostic of math ability.
We believe that these instructions not only represent a stronger and more direct manipulation of stereotype threat, but
also generalize more to real testing situations. To summarize, the threat condition explicitly presented the calculus test as
diagnostic of ability and made no mention of sex-based performance differences; the non-threat condition also explicitly
presented the test as diagnostic of ability but included the added component that the test was gender-fair.
Students were given 20 min to work on the calculus test. This time limit was set for a number of reasons. First, we
wanted to replicate the students' classroom testing situation, which uses timed tests. Second, because the test was too
long and difficult to complete in 20 min, this added time pressure could further tax working memory, which past
research has shown is hindered in stereotype-threatening situations (Schmader & Johns, 2003). After the 20 min had
passed, students were instructed to proceed to the post-test questionnaire, demographic section, and debriefing section
at their own pace. The post-test questionnaire included a manipulation check in which participants used a 15-point
Likert-type scale to respond to the following item: This test was biased(1 = not at all; 15 = extremely). The
experimenter and teaching assistant monitored the class for the duration of the experiment and collected the research
packets as the students left the classroom. At the end of the semester, we obtained students' calculus course grades from
the calculus professors.
We predicted that the diagnosticity of the test would create a stereotype-threatening situation for women, and
consequently, that they would perform worse than men on the calculus test. We also predicted that framing the
diagnostic test as sex-fair would alleviate stereotype threat; thus, we expected women's performance in the non-threat
condition to exceed that of the women in the threat condition. We predicted that the manipulations would have no
significant effect on the men's performance.
3. Results
3.1. Manipulation check
Because two participants failed to answer the question about whether or not the test was biased, these data were
analyzed for 155 participants. The two-way ANOVA performed on the participants' responses to this question revealed
22 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
only an effect of condition, F(1, 151) = 3.94, p= .05, Cohen's D=.34,r= .17. This represents a medium effect size.
Participants in the non-stereotype threat condition (M=1.51,SD = 1.38) reported a significantly lower level of test bias
than participants in the stereotype threat condition (M=2.27,SD = 2.89). Thus, the manipulation statement indicating
that the test was gender-fair reduced the amount of bias that participants in the non-stereotype threat condition perceived.
3.2. Did stereotype threat affect women's calculus performance?
A two-way ANOVA was performed on the number of items participants answered correctly. The main effect of
Condition approached significance, F(1, 153) = 3.47, p= .06, Cohen's D= .04, r= . 02, which was qualified by a
Sex × Condition interaction that also approached the level of significance, F(1, 153) = 2.86, p= .09. Planned
comparisons indicated that in the non-threat condition, women outperformed the men, t(60) = 2.30, p= .02, Cohen's
D= .58, r= .28. This represents a medium effect size (see Table 1 for means and standard deviations).
These differences might not have yielded results that attained the level of significance because sex differences in
math performance may exist primarily among Anglo-American students (American Association of University Women,
1998; Hyde, 1994; Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990). Thus, we conducted the analyses including data obtained for
only the Anglo-American participants. Despite the reduced sample size, this analysis yielded a significant main effect
of Sex, F(1, 87) = 6.21, pb.02, Cohen's D= .44, r= .21, and a significant main effect of Condition, F(1, 87) = 4.22,
pb.05, Cohen's D= .36, r= .18. These results were qualified by a significant Sex × Condition interaction, F(1, 87) =
6.75, pb.02 (see Fig. 1). Planned comparisons indicated that the Anglo-American women in the non-threat condition
Table 1
Mean (and SD) number correct on the test (maximum = 15 items) and course grades (maximum = 4.0) for male and female participants in the non-
stereotype threat and the stereotype threat conditions
Non-stereotype threat Stereotype threat
Female Male Female Male
All Participants
# Correct 3.60 (2.00) n= 25 2.60 (1.42) n= 35 3.13 (1.95) n= 32 3.08 (1.47) n=65
Course Grades 2.88 (.97) n= 25 2.55 (1.09) n= 33 2.71 (.97) n= 31 2.76 (1.01) n=59
Anglo-American Participants
# Correct 4.44 (1.90) n= 16 2.70 (1.26) n= 20 2.85 (1.79) n= 20 2.89 (1.41) n=35
Course Grades 2.81 (1.05) n= 16 2.83 (1.04) n= 18 2.65 (1.04) n= 20 2.68 (1.09) n=34
Fig. 1. Anglo-American women's and men's scores on the calculus test as a function of stereotype threat condition. Error bars represent the standard
error of the mean.
23C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
outperformed Anglo-American participants in every other condition (see Table 2 for pairwise comparisons and effect
sizes). Most strikingly, these women not only outperformed women in the stereotype threat condition, but surprisingly,
also outperformed men in both the non-threat condition and the stereotype threat condition. The manipulation did not
significantly affect men's performance.
3.3. Did women in the stereotype threat condition answer fewer problems and were they less accurate in their responses?
To address questions about items attempted and response accuracy, we first conducted a two-way ANOVA on
number attempted. This analysis yielded a significant interaction between Sex and Condition, F(1, 87) = 6.34, pb.02.
Planned comparisons indicated that males in the stereotype threat condition (M= 9.2, SD = 3.08) answered more
questions than males in the non-threat condition (M= 7.00, SD = 2.97), t(55) = 2.59, pb.02, Cohen's D= .73, r= .34.
In addition, females in the non-threat condition (M= 9.44, SD = 3.39) answered more questions than males in the non-
threat condition (M= 7.00, SD = 2.97), t(36) = 2.40, pb.02, Cohen's D= .77, r= .36. These represent large effect
sizes. These were the only comparisons to reach significance (all other psN.26).
To determine whether reducing stereotype threat enabled females to be more accurate in their responses (see Steele
& Aronson, 1995; Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000), we conducted a two-way ANOVA on accuracy (number correct divided
by number attempted). This analysis yielded a significant main effect for Condition, F(1, 87) = 4.80, pb.03, Cohen's
D= .45, r= .22. Overall, participants in the non-threat condition (M= .46, SD = .23) were more accurate than
participants in the stereotype threat condition (M= .36, SD = .21). Planned comparisons revealed that women in the
non-threat condition (M= .51, SD = .22) were significantly more accurate than women in the stereotype threat
condition (M= .36, SD = .22), t(36) = 1.99, pb.05, Cohen's D= .68, r= .32 and than men in the stereotype threat
condition (M= .36, SD = .21), t(51) = 2.27, pb.03, Cohen's D= .70, r= .33. These were the only comparisons to
reach significance. The manipulation did not significantly affect men's accuracy.
3.4. Did stereotype threat reduce women's confidence in their responses?
For each test question answered, participants indicated on a scale from one to five (1 = low confidence; 5 = high
confidence) their confidence that their response was correct. We averaged these responses to obtain a measure of each
participant's overall confidence. The ANOVA on Confidence yielded significant main effects of Sex, F(1, 87) = 6.98,
pb.01, Cohen's D= .47, r= .23, and Condition, F(1, 87) = 5.42, pb.02, Cohen's D= .50, r= .24. Males (M= 3.51,
SD = .78) were more confident than females (M= 3.14, SD = .79); and participants in the non-threat condition (M=
3.60, SD = .78) were more confident than participants in the stereotype threat condition (M= 3.21, SD = .79). These
results are consistent with past studies showing women to be less confident in mathematics than their male
counterparts, despite the lack of performance differences (American Association of University Women, 1992, 1998;
Stipek & Gralinski, 1991).
3.5. Did males and females differ in the grades they earned in the course?
Because the conditions of our study and in particular, the stereotype threat condition mimicked the conditions
under which the participants took their regular calculus examinations, we wondered whether or not males and females
differed in the grades they earned for the course. To answer this question we conducted a one-way ANOVA on Course
Grades. This analysis yielded no significant results ( pN.9), suggesting that males (M= 2.73, SD = 1.07) and females
(M= 2.72, SD = 1.03) earned similar grades in the course.
Table 2
Pairwise comparisons and effect sizes for number correct
Non-threat females Non-threat males Threat females Threat males
Non-threat females t= 3.32, p= .001 t= 3.03, p= .003 t= 3.30, p= .001
Non-threat males D= 1.08, R= .47 t= .30, p= .76 t= .42, p= .67
Threat females D= .86, R= .40 D= .10, R= .05 t= .082, p= .94
Threat males D= .93, R= .42 D= .14, R= .07 D= .02, R= .01
Note: Upper diagonal shows t-tests and significance values; Lower diagonal shows Cohen's Dand effect sizes; Anglo-American participants only.
24 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
3.6. Are course grades accurate predictors of males' and females' performance on the practice test?
Because males and females did not differ in their course grades, we wondered how well course grades predicted males'
and females' performance on the practice test in the threat and non-threat conditions. First, to determine the prediction
equation, we conducted a regression analysis which yielded the following: Predicted Number Correct = 1.84 + .47
(Course Grade). Next, we computed the Predicted Number Correct for each participant. Finally, we conducted four
paired-samples t-tests (comparing males and females in the stereotype threat and non-threat conditions) using Predicted
Number Correct and Number Correct to determine whether there were significant differences in the predicted number
correct and actual number correct on the practice test. These analyses indicated that for females in the non-threat
condition, course grades significantly underpredicted performance on the practice test by 1.28 points ( pb.02). For all
other groups, grades were non-significant overpredictors of performance on the practice test (all psN.12).
4. Discussion
This study revealed that the effects of stereotype threat are not limited to the typical woman's performance on
general mathematics tests. Rather, even women at the upper ends of the ability distribution in college who opt to enroll
in the most difficult math courses can be vulnerable to the effects of negative stereotypes. Specifically, framing a
difficult calculus test as diagnostic of ability suppressed women's calculus performance. However, ensuring women
that the same diagnostic test was free of gender-bias reduced stereotype threat and unleashed their mathematics
potential. Not only did the women in the non-threat condition outperform women in the stereotype threat condition, but
they also outperformed the men in either testing condition. In addition, women in the non-threat condition were more
accurate on the test than women and men in the stereotype threat condition. Thus, test-diagnosticity clearly suppressed
women's test performance, both in terms of actual number correct and accuracy. But when the situational threat was
removed by assuring women that the test was unbiased, women's performance increased to a level that surpassed their
male counterparts, a finding that was both surprising and encouraging.
Why would women outperform their male counterparts in this situation? It is likely that because women are more
apt to self-select out of math and science fields early in their educational careers (American Association of University
Women, 1998; Eccles, 1994), the women in advanced mathematics classes, such as those in our sample, comprise the
most motivated and prepared female mathematics students. Men, on the other hand, may be more likely to pursue math
and science careers even if they are less prepared academically to do so. Thus, the female participants in our sample
may have been a more select group of math students than the male participants. Unconstrained by stereotype threat in
our experimental group, these women performed closer to their latent ability levels, which were higher on average than
the males in the course.
Still, if the pool of women enrolled in advanced mathematics courses was more self-selected than the pool of men,
why did we not find a female advantage in course grades, as is often the case in mathematics courses (Bridgeman &
Wendler, 1991)? One possibility is that the calculus course itself was inherently prone to induce stereotype threat in
women. That is, there may have been an ethos of diagnosticity without any mention of gender-fairness or refuting of
stereotypes that led to a suppression of females' course grades. In fact, the stereotype of male superiority in calculus
was alive and well in these calculus courses. We asked 364 calculus students who they believed was better at calculus
men or women and who they thought other people believed was better at calculus. The women believed that men and
women have equal abilities, whereas the men believed that men are superior (pb.001). We also found that both men
and women believed that other people thought men were better than women (pb.001) (Good & Aronson, 1998). And
given the men's endorsement of male superiority in calculus, this was, in fact, an accurate perception of the existing
stereotype in their learning environment.
In addition, stereotype threat may have been fostered by the sex composition of the class (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev,
2000). In this course, the men outnumbered the women by a margin of two to one, a difference that is quite
representative of mathematics departments across the country (for example, see Massachusetts Institute of Technology,
1999; National Science Foundation, 2006). Thus, the course exams, on which students' grades are primarily based,
may have been confounded with stereotype threat effects, thus suppressing women's grades by suppressing their test
performances.
Indeed, the lack of sex differences in course grades mirrors the lack of sex differences in test performance in the
stereotype threat condition. In the non-threat condition, however, course grades significantly underpredicted women's
25C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
performance on the practice test. As our pattern of data suggests, if stereotype threat had been removed from the
classroom culture, these women very likely would have earned higher grades, perhaps even higher than their male
counterparts' grades. In support of this interpretation, consider our finding that grades underpredicted women's test
performance, but only in the non-threat condition. In other words, when stereotype threat is alleviated, women may be
capable of higher calculus performance than their course grades indicate.
A second possibility for why the females outperformed the males is that females simply could have prepared more
for the practice test than the males did. Indeed, research indicates that females do as much, if not more, math homework
than males (Eccles & Jacobs, 1986; Mau & Lynn, 2000). Consequently, it may be the case that the females studied more
for this practice test and thus, did better on it. This, however, is not a complete explanation for our pattern of results. If
females and males entered the class with equal ability but the females simply prepared more than the males, then why
did the females not outperform the males in both conditions? That sex differences varied by condition suggests that
females' scores were affected by a process other than preparation namely, stereotype threat.
Whether our manipulation suppressed females' scores in the stereotype threat condition or enhanced them in the
non-threat condition cannot be determined. Nevertheless, what remains clear is that when females simply took the
diagnostic test a situation that has suppressed females' scores in past studies they did worse than when they were
assured that the test was sex-fair.
Despite women's better performance in the non-threat condition compared to the stereotype threat condition, our
manipulations did nothing to affect their level of confidence. This finding, however, should not be surprising for research
has shown that women lack confidence in mathematics even when their performance is high (Linn & Hyde, 1989).
As discussed previously, this study differs from past studies in both the selectivity of the sample used, and in the
methods of inducing and reducing vulnerability to stereotype threat. First, the men and women in our sample were
enrolled in a calculus course specifically designed for mathematics, engineering, and science majors. This, together
with the fact that women earned grades in the course that were no different from the men's grades, created a highly
selective group of women who were well poised for successful careers as scientists and mathematicians. No study that
we know of has examined the role of stereotype threat with such a select group of aspirants. Although others have used
participants who scored well on standardized tests of mathematics (see Quinn & Spencer, 2001; Spencer et al., 1999),
these participants generally were recruited from psychology courses or from the psychology subject pool. Their
intended majors and career paths, therefore, likely included non-math-based fields. Furthermore, although Quinn and
Spencer (2001) recruited highly capable females, they explicitly excluded those who scored above 700 on the SAT-I. In
contrast, we drew our sample specifically from courses that were likely to produce future mathematicians and scientists
and also demanded high math ability. Thus, the results of our study expand our knowledge of populations that are
vulnerable to stereotype threat effects: even women enrolled in the most advanced math courses that prepare students
for careers in mathematics and science can experience underperformance due to stereotype threat.
Second, our procedures for inducing stereotype threat mirror the testing procedures in most classrooms, and thus are
a truer reflection of real-world stereotype threat effects. Furthermore, simply addressing the fairness of the test while
retaining its diagnostic nature presents a more practical method of alleviating stereotype threat that is easily transferable
to any testing situation. Past studies have gone to great lengths to inoculate students against the effects of negative
stereotypes. In particular, researchers have explicitly taught students about the malleability of intelligence as one
method of countering the stereotype's message of limited, fixed ability due to group membership. These labor-
intensive methods have included showing students videos that teach about the brain's ability to form new neural
connections (Aronson et al., 2002), having students write pen-pal letters espousing the malleable view of intelligence
(Aronson et al., 2002), or constructing a mentoring program in which the focus of the mentoring relationship was to
teach students about the malleable nature of intelligence (Good et al., 2003). These methods, although highly effective,
are unlikely to be implemented in regular mathematics classrooms.
Our study, in contrast, presents a method of reducing stereotype threat that is easily-implemented not only in
mathematics classrooms but also in standardized testing situations. Specifically, testing procedures could easily be
modified to include a brief statement that the test, although diagnostic of underlying mathematics ability, is sex-fair.
Moreover, this easily-implemented procedure could be used with mathematics students in any math class elementary
through graduate school or testing situation NAEP, SAT, or GRE, for example. Doing so has the potential to
change the current dynamic of gender-based performance differences in mathematics.
Moreover, it is important to identify easily replicable methods for reducing stereotype threat because doing so
creates the potential to change not only females' performance in mathematics but also their career aspirations. For
26 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
example, research has shown that women who perceived that their college calculus classes conveyed negative
stereotypes about women's math abilities reported a lower sense of belonging to the math community that is, they felt
less like accepted members of the math community whose presence was valued than those who did not perceive a
stereotypical climate (Good et al., in preparation-a). Moreover, this threat to their identity as a future mathematician (or
scientist) had real consequences for their achievement and career aspirations: when women's sense of belonging was
reduced due to their perceptions of a stereotypical environment, they earned lower grades in the course and were less
likely to intend to take any more math classes in the future.
Thus, stereotypes can cause individuals enough discomfort to lead them to drop out of the domain and redefine their
professional identities. When the domain is something as fundamental as mathematics, domain avoidance essentially
shuts the door to potentially lucrative careers in science, engineering, and technology. And as with mathematics
performance, the effects of stereotypes on professional identity have roots early in schooling, for it has been found that
stereotypes can undermine sense of belonging for girls in math as early as middle school (Good et al., in preparation-b).
This has important consequences for girls' identities as future mathematicians and scientists, because it is precisely the
middle school years when girls' confidence in and liking of mathematics begins to wane.
The results of this study also have direct implications for the discussion of social forces as a contributing factor to
women's underperformance and under-representation in science and mathematics disciplines. These results show quite
clearly the role of non-biological factors in the mediation of test performance in the upper strata of college mathematics
curriculum. Without denying the link between biology and math abilities (e.g., Halpern, 1992), it is clear that even
among a select group of females and males with equivalent grades in high-level mathematics, stereotype threat can
prevent women from performing up to their potential. Among these participants, this meant that stereotype threat
suppressed their scores to the level of their male counterparts.
Our hope is that educators will incorporate these results into any discussion about the problem of and remedies for
the under-representation of women in the upper echelons of mathematics and science professions. Clearly, social
forces, such as negative stereotypes, remain at the forefront of the factors that contribute to the lack of women who
succeed at the highest levels of mathematics. But with wise attention to situational factors such as reassuring gender-
fair testing educators can help females at any stage of their mathematics education approach their potential and
increase their numbers in mathematics and science professions.
References
American Association of University Women. (1992). Shortchanging girls, Shortchanging America: A call to action. Washington, DC: American
Association of University Women.
American Association of University Women. (1998). Gender gaps: Where schools still fail our children. Executive summary. Washington, DC:
American Association of University Women Educational Foundation
Aronson, J., Fried, C., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of
intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,38,113125.
Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Stereotypes and the fragility of human competence, motivation, and self-concept. In C. Dweck & E. Elliot (Eds.),
Handbook of competence & motivation (pp. 436456). Guilford: New York.
Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1980). Sex differences in mathematical ability: Fact or artifact. Science,210, 12621264.
Bridgeman, B., & Wendler, C. (1991). Gender differences in predictors of college mathematics performance and in college mathematics course
grades. Journal of Educational Psychology,83, 275284.
Brown, R. P., & Day, E. A. (2006). The difference isn't black and white: Stereotypethreat and the race gap on Raven's advanced progressive matrices.
Journal of Applied Psychology,91(4), 979985.
Cohen, G., Garcia, J., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A socialpsychological intervention. Science,313, 13071310.
College Board. (2005). 2005 College-bound seniors: Total group profile report Retrieved August 1, 2006 from http://www.collegeboard.com
Croizet, J. -C., Despres, G., Gauzins, M. -E., Huguet, P., Leyens, J. -P., & Meot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by
triggering a disruptive mental load. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,30, 721731.
Danaher, K. & Crandall, C.S. (in press). Stereotype threat in applied settings re-examined. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.
Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J. (2006). Exposure to scientific theories affects women's math performance. Science,314, 435.
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat
can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,28, 16151628.
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., & Meece, J. L. (1984). Sex differences in achievement: A test of alternate theories. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,46,2643.
Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women's educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement related choices.
Psychology of Women Quarterly,18, 585609.
Eccles, J. S., & Jacobs, J. E. (1986). Social forces shape math attitudes and performance. Signs,11, 367380.
27C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
Eccles, J. S., & Jacobs, J. E. (1992). The impact of mothers' gender-role stereotypic beliefs on mothers' and children's' ability perceptions. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,63, 932944.
Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects and parents' socialization of gender differences.
Journal of Social Issues,46, 183201.
Good, C., & Aronson, J. (1998). Stereotypes in the calculus classroom: Men's and women's perceptions of sex-differences. Unpublished data. The
University of Texas at Austin.
Good, C., & Aronson, J. (in preparation). The development of stereotype threat in children. Manuscript in preparation. Barnard College.
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents' standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of
stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,24, 645662.
Good, C., Dweck, C. S., & Aronson, J. (2007). Social identity, stereotype threat, and self-theories. In A. Fuligni (Ed.), Social categories, identities
and educational participation (pp. 121145). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Good, C., Dweck, C.S., & Rattan, A. (in preparation-a). The effects of perceiving fixed-ability environments and stereotyping on women's sense of
belonging to math. Manuscript in preparation. Barnard College.
Good, C., Dweck, C.S., & Rattan, A. (in preparation-b). Do I belong here? Middle school girls' sense of belonging and achievement in math.
Manuscript in preparation. Barnard College.
Gottfredson, L. S. (2002). Where and why g matters: Not a mystery. Human Performance,15,2546.
Halpern, D. F. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hyde, J. S. (1994). Can meta-analysis make feminist transformations in psychology? Psychology of Women Quarterly,18, 451462.
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,107,
139155.
Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficitsin
the presence of males. Psychological Science,11, 365371.
Johns, M., Schmader, T., & Martens, A. (2005). Knowing is half the battle: Teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women's math
performance. Psychological Science,16, 175179.
Linn, M., & Hyde, J. (1989). Gender, mathematics, and science. Educational Researcher,18,1727.
Massachusetts Institute of Technology. (1999). MIT Faculty Newsletter, vol. XI, no. 4, March, 1999. Retrieved August 1, 2006 from http://web.mit.
edu/fnl/women/women.html#numberwomen
Mau, W., & Lynn, R. (2000). Gender differences in homework and test scores in mathematics, reading and science at tenth and twelfth grade.
Psychology, Evolution & Gender,2,119125.
McGlone, M. S., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,27,
486493.
McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women's mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,39,8390.
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype-consciousness in middle childhood. Child Development,74,
498515.
National Science Foundation. (2006). Science and Engineering Indicators, 2006. Retrieved August 1, 2006 from http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/srs/seind06
Pinker, S. (2005). The science of difference: Sex ed. The New Republic. February 14.
Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women's generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies.
Journal of Social Issues,57,5571.
Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M., & Cullen, M. J. (2004). On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American White difference on
cognitive tests. American Psychologist,59,713.
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology,38, 194201.
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology,85, 440452.
Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science: A critical review. American Psychologist,60, 950958.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,35,
428.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,69, 797811.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with images of one's group: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. In
M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,34 (pp. 379440). San Diego: Academic Press.
Stipek, D., & Gralinski, H. (1991). Gender differences in children's achievement-related beliefs and emotional responses to success and failure in
mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology,83, 361371.
Stricker, L. J., & Ward, W. C. (2004). Stereotype threat, inquiring about test takers' ethnicity and gender, and standardized test performance. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology,34, 665693.
Summers, L. (2005). Remarks at NBER Conference on Diversifying the Science & Engineering Workforce. Retrieved August 1, 2006 from http://
www.president.harvard.edu/speeches/2005/nber.html
28 C. Good et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 29 (2008) 1728
... For example, Xu et al. (2020) found a gender difference in undergraduates' conceptions of computer science learning: female students depicted less positive emotions and attitudes than male students, and fewer female students expressed human-computer interaction concepts. Previous research also demonstrated that boys and girls have significantly different performances in robot programming learning (Sullivan & Bers, 2012, which might be correlated with their learning perceptions (Good et al., 2008;Sullivan & Bers, 2019). Therefore, it is a meaningful issue to reveal conceptions of robot programming learning held by early-year children of different genders. ...
Article
Full-text available
As technology-enhanced children's learning has gained wide attention, programmable robots have been gradually introduced in early childhood education. Hence, it would be valuable to understand how young children perceive robot programming learning. Draw-a-picture technique is an ideal method to elicit ideas, thoughts, and feelings for children with limited literacy, and epistemic network analysis (ENA) is a novel analytical method to analyze children's conceptions through the visualized network model. Therefore, this study employed a draw-a-picture technique and ENA to explore 189 5-6-year-old young children's conceptions of robot programming learning and probe whether their conceptions differ by gender and learning achievements. Results revealed that most children believed that with robot programming kits, they could engage in programming activities with peers in any location and held positive emotions and attitudes. In addition, young children's conceptions of robot programming learning differ notably by gender and learning achievements. Based on the current findings, several suggestions were proposed, which could set a reference for future robot programming teaching in early childhood education.
... Há muito debate sobre as diferenças de sexo nas habilidades de atenção visual, com as mulheres mostrando menos atenção visual em comparação aos homens (Merritt et al., 2007). Na pesquisa atual, as diferenças de sexo na atenção visual mostraram estar relacionadas às diferenças cognitivas de sexo entre mulheres e homens, em disciplinas como a Matemática (Good et al., 2008), mas nossos resultados não mostram uma diferença significativa relacionada à atenção visual entre meninos e meninas. ...
Article
Full-text available
Este estudo objetiva examinar com que distância específica os alunos estão visualmente mais ou menos envolvidos com o professor nas aulas de matemática. Também busca analisar em que medida, as instruções feitas pelo professor, através de gestos de apontar, afetam o envolvimento visual dos alunos nas aulas dessa disciplina. Para isso foram selecionados, aleatoriamente, 50 alunos, 25 alunos do sexo masculino e 25 do sexo feminino, os quais, usavam uma minicâmara acoplada em óculos de lente que gravou 75 horas de videoaula. Os resultados mostram que os alunos estão mais envolvidos visualmente com as instruções do professor a uma proxêmica de 1,20 a 3,70 metros. Além disso, relata-se diferenças entre meninos e meninas e como eles estão visualmente envolvidos nas aulas de matemática. Por fim, conclui-se que os gestos de apontar realizados pelos professores podem servir como uma ferramenta para recuperar a atenção visual dos alunos.
... There is some evidence that sleep deprivation and exposure to such environments can negatively affect cognitive function, productivity, and earnings (e.g., Bessone et al., 2019;Jagnani, 2018;Kristiansson et al., 2015). Stigma and stereotype threat, related to poverty or other traits and conditions, can also reduce cognitive abilities (Good et al., 2008;Hall et al., 2014;Steele & Aronson, 1995). Dean et al. (2018) provides a detailed discussion of such effects of a life in poverty. ...
Article
Full-text available
The history of development economics has been portrayed as a succession of fads and magic bullets. This study inquires whether behavioral approaches to economic development are destined to become such a fad or whether they have long‐lasting contributions to offer. I first show that behavioral development economics is not a cohesive field with a consensual definition and propose to distinguish three different views of behavioral development economics: the scarcity view, the non‐optimal behavior view, and the psycho‐social‐cultural view. I provide a systematic review of publications in the field of behavioral development economics from 2000 to 2021, distinguishing these three fields. The nonoptimal view has been, by far, the dominant view, and risk and time preferences stand out as the single‐most studied behavioral mechanisms. Finally, I argue that the sub‐field's ability to advance the understanding of behavior relevant to the persistence or alleviation of poverty depends crucially on the inclusion of the psycho‐social‐cultural view. This view has important contributions to offer in terms of (1) the understanding of seemingly nonrational behavior, (2) the identification and measurement of behavioral mechanisms, and (3) the understanding of conditions of external validity, and should be embraced more enthusiastically.
... High stigma consciousness can reinforce stereotypes and reduce women's effectiveness in leadership (Block et al., 2011). In male-dominated fields like STEM, high stigma consciousness can lower performance and cause career exits (Good et al., 2008;Shapiro & Williams, 2012). Consequently, stigma consciousness moderates the impact of women's transformational leadership on effectiveness, thus reinforcing negative biases. ...
Article
Full-text available
Stigma consciousness is crucial for influencing the effectiveness of female leaders, particularly in transformational leadership. Despite the increasing representation of women in the workplace, they still face significant challenges in reaching the highest organizational positions. This study explores how stigma consciousness moderates the impact of women’s transformational leadership on trust and leader effectiveness, while trust in leaders mediates these relationships. The research model in this study was analyzed using the Structural Equation Model (SEM) and the AMOS 28 software. This approach allows researchers to look at multiple complex relationships at the same time and combines regression and factor analysis to obtain a full picture of how variables interact with each other. We assessed the descriptive statistics and psychometric properties of the measurement scales using SPSS 28. Data were collected from 200 team members and subordinates of female leaders at various organizations in Indonesia. The results indicate that although women’s transformational leadership significantly enhances trust in leaders and leader effectiveness, this impact is moderated by stigma consciousness levels. High stigma consciousness weakens the positive effect of women’s transformational leadership on trust in leaders, thus reducing leadership effectiveness. Conversely, lower levels of stigma consciousness reinforce this positive effect. This finding highlight stigma consciousness as a barrier to women leaders’ trust and an obstacle to achieving leadership effectiveness. Reducing stigma consciousness is essential for the effective acceptance and recognition of women’s leadership. An inclusive, supportive environment free from stigma enables women to lead more effectively and gain greater team trust.
Article
Full-text available
Modelos y referentes que influyen en los estereotipos de género en STEM: un estudio de caso en España Abstract The science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) sector is an academic and professional field with high gender disparity figures despite being a field with a low unemployment rate. The "Questionnaire with University Students on STEM Studies in Higher Education" (QSTEMHE) was designed to determine the opinion of the Spanish university population on all branches of knowledge about gender stereotypes in STEM studies. This validated instrument was applied to a university sample of 2101 people from different Spanish universities. A quantitative methodology and the non-experimental ex-post-facto method were used, employing a simple random sampling technique. This study aims to analyse the relationship established between the models and references that university students have had and their manifestation of gender stereotypes on the ability to perform in STEM higher education studies. Among the main results, gender stereotypes about STEM degrees persist, considering them masculinised and male-oriented. Furthermore, the study confirms that models and references taken into account when choosing higher education studies impact the perception of men and women regarding stereotypes in STEM. Resumen El sector de la ciencia, la tecnología, la ingeniería y las matemáticas (STEM) es un ámbito académico y profesional con altas cifras de disparidad de género, a pesar de ser un campo con una baja tasa de desempleo. El cuestionario "Questionnaire with university students on STEM studies in Higher Education" (QSTEMHE) se diseñó para conocer la opinión de la población universitaria española en todas las ramas de conocimiento sobre los estereotipos de género en los estudios STEM. Este instrumento validado se aplicó a una muestra universitaria de 2101 personas de diferentes universidades españolas. Se utilizó la metodología cuantitativa y el método no experimental ex-post-facto, empleando la técnica de muestreo aleatorio simple. Este estudio pretende analizar la relación que se establece entre los modelos y referentes que ha tenido el alumnado universitario y su manifestación de estereotipos de género sobre la capacidad de desempeño en los estudios superiores STEM. Entre los principales resultados, persisten los estereotipos de género sobre las titulaciones STEM, considerándolas masculinizadas y orientadas a los hombres. Además, el estudio confirma que los modelos y referentes tenidos en cuenta a la hora de elegir los estudios superiores inciden en la percepción de hombres y mujeres sobre los estereotipos de STEM.
Article
Youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often exhibit impairments in mathematics, but long-term math development into adulthood, particularly in females, is underexplored. We characterized trajectories of math achievement in girls with ADHD and an age- and ethnicity-matched comparison sample from childhood through early adulthood across four waves and examined childhood cognitive predictors (global executive functioning, working memory, processing speed) of trajectories. The ethnically and socioeconomically diverse sample comprised 140 girls with carefully diagnosed ADHD and 88 neurotypicals, ages 6 to 12 years at baseline. Using latent growth curve models, we examined predictors of 16-year math achievement trajectories. In both the ADHD and neurotypical groups, scores declined over time; rates of change did not differ significantly. Yet in the ADHD sample, math difficulties (defined as scores at least 1 SD below the national average) increased notably over time, with many such difficulties emerging after childhood. By adulthood, nearly half of women with ADHD exhibited clear math difficulties. Worse baseline global executive functioning predicted slower math growth over time. Girls with ADHD may benefit from math supports before concerns emerge or worsen after childhood. Additional research on preventive interventions for math difficulties, including investigation of executive functioning, is warranted.
Article
Women report that they must conform to masculine behavioral norms to progress in chemistry, with the necessity of adopting such norms pushing them from the field. Advancing gender-based equity within...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to examine mathematical bullying victimization among middle school students and its relationship with socio-demographic variables using the Mathematical Bullying Victimization Scale (MBV-S). The sample consisted of 493 middle school students selected using a disproportionate stratified sampling method from five different schools in Konya, Türkiye, during the spring semester of the 2021-2022 academic year.Data were collected using the relational screening model and analyzed with nonparametric statistical methods. The results showed that female students were more frequently subjected to mathematical bullying compared to males. No significant differences were found in MBV-S scores based on grade levels. However, the analysis of school types revealed that students in girls-only religious middle schools experienced higher levels of mathematical bullying than students in other types of schools. Additionally, students with below-average mathematics achievement were more likely to be victims of mathematical bullying, whereas students with above-average achievement reported lower levels of victimization. The findings highlight the importance of developing strategies to address mathematical bullying that consider students' mathematical abilities and social dynamics. Supportive and inclusive environments should be prioritized, particularly for students with low achievement and those in specific school settings.
Article
Many researchers have debated potential differences between male and female students’ mathematical performance. One important factor that can impact performance is perception of the content. The purpose of the study is to investigate factors that impact high-ability male and female students’ perspectives of mathematics. Participants ( n = 12; n f = 5, n m = 7) attended a 10-day residential summer camp during which the researchers conducted individual, semistructured interviews with each participant. Using a thematic analysis, the researchers examined the interview transcripts for themes by gender. Six common themes emerged between male and female students: perseverance, performance, self-efficacy, enjoyment of content, social component, and life relevance. Female students reported that adult influence was also a factor. Further examination showed that female students are more likely to report enjoyment of content as a factor, whereas males are more likely to report perseverance and performance.
Article
Full-text available
This article considers 3 claims that cognitive sex differences account for the differential representation of men and women in high-level careers in mathematics and science: (a) males are more focused on objects from the beginning of life and therefore are predisposed to better learning about mechanical systems; (b) males have a profile of spatial and numerical abilities producing greater aptitude for mathematics; and (c) males are more variable in their cognitive abilities and therefore predominate at the upper reaches of mathematical talent. Research on cognitive development in human infants, preschool children, and students at all levels fails to support these claims. Instead, it provides evidence that mathematical and scientific reasoning develop from a set of biologically based cognitive capacities that males and females share. These capacities lead men and women to develop equal talent for mathematics and science.
Article
Full-text available
Women in quantitative fields risk being personally reduced to negative stereotypes that allege a sex-based math inability. This situational predicament, termed stereotype threat, can undermine women’s performance and aspirations in all quantitative domains. Gender-stereotypic television commercials were employed in three studies to elicit the female stereotype among both men and women. Study 1 revealed that only women for whom the activated stereotype was self-relevant underperformed on a subsequent math test. Exposure to the stereotypic commercials led women taking an aptitude test in Study 2 to avoid math items in favor of verbal items. In Study 3, women who viewed the stereotypic commercials indicated less interest in educational/vocational options in which they were susceptible to stereotype threat (i.e., quantitative domains) and more interest in fields in which they were immune to stereotype threat (i.e., verbal domains).
Article
g is a highly general capability for processing complex information of any type. This explains its great value in predicting job performance. Complexity is the major distinction among jobs, which explains why g is more important further up the occupational hierarchy. The predictive validities of g are moderated by the criteria and other predictors considered in selection research, but the resulting gradients of g's effects are systematic. The pattern provides personnel psychologists a road map for how to design better selection batteries. Despite much literature on the meaning and impact of g, there nonetheless remains an aura of mystery about where and why g cognitive tests might be useful in selection. The aura of mystery encourages false beliefs and false hopes about how we might reduce disparate impact in employee selection. It is also used to justify new testing techniques whose major effect, witting or not, is to reduce the validity of selection in the service of racial goals.
Article
Each of us possesses multiple social identities. For example, our sex, age, race, social class, religion, political beliefs, and professions are all potential social identities. In certain contexts in which we find ourselves, that social identity may be devalued. For example, Democrats at the Republican National Convention, gays and lesbians at a custody hearing, a lone woman at a corporate board of directors meeting, black people in an all-white, southern neighborhood, or an Arab flight attendant with an American or European airline - all are at risk of having a component of their social identities devalued in the respective contexts. In response to this devaluation, they may find that their behavior or sense of self changes. Perhaps the female corporate board member speaks less persuasively than she is capable of speaking, or perhaps the Arab flight attendant chooses a different occupation, thus changing his professional identity. One need not be in an extreme situation to feel the weight of a devalued social identity. More subtle situations may also place a burden upon individuals who are in some way stigmatized. For example, when a woman takes a math test in the presence of men, she may be reminded about the stereotype of male superiority in mathematics that is alive in our culture (Spencer, Steele, and Quinn 1999; Steele and Aronson 1995). Being a woman, and thus, having a social identity that is devalued vis-à-vis mathematics ability, she may have a sense that she could be judged or treated in terms of the stereotype or that she might inadvertently confirm the stereotype. This sense can disrupt her ability to perform up to her potential, a predicament known as "stereotype threat" (Steele and Aronson 1995). In this chapter we will review the literature on stereotype threat as it relates to social identity. Specifically, we will discuss not only how people's social identity can either protect them from or create vulnerability to stereotype threat, but also how the experience of stereotype threat can influence their social identity. Thus, we will show that social identity and stereotype threat have a reciprocal relationship. Finally, we will discuss methods of protecting both social identity and achievement from the negative effects of stereotypes1.
Article
At the highest levels of math achievement, gender differences in favor of men persist on standardized math tests. We hypothesize that stereotype threat depresses women's math performance through interfering with their ability to formulate problem-solving strategies. In Study 1, women underperformed in comparison to men on a word problem test, however, women and men performed equally when the word problems were converted into their numerical equivalents. In Study 2, men and women worked on difficult problems, either in a high- or reduced-stereo-type-threat condition. Problem-solving strategies were coded. When stereo-type threat was high, women were less able to formulate problem-solving strategies than when stereotype threat was reduced. The effect of stereotype threat on cognitive resources and the implications for gender differences in mathematical testing are discussed.
Article
African American college students tend to obtain lower grades than their White counterparts, even when they enter college with equivalent test scores. Past research suggests that negative stereotypes impugning Black students' intellectual abilities play a role in this underperformance. Awareness of these stereotypes can psychologically threaten African Americans, a phenomenon known as “stereotype threat” (Steele & Aronson, 1995), which can in turn provoke responses that impair both academic performance and psychological engagement with academics. An experiment was performed to test a method of helping students resist these responses to stereotype threat. Specifically, students in the experimental condition of the experiment were encouraged to see intelligence—the object of the stereotype—as a malleable rather than fixed capacity. This mind-set was predicted to make students' performances less vulnerable to stereotype threat and help them maintain their psychological engagement with academics, both of which could help boost their college grades. Results were consistent with predictions. The African American students (and, to some degree, the White students) encouraged to view intelligence as malleable reported greater enjoyment of the academic process, greater academic engagement, and obtained higher grade point averages than their counterparts in two control groups.
Article
Meta-analysis is a statistical method for literature reviewing. Metaanalyses of gender differences in verbal ability, spatial ability, mathematics performance, helping behavior, and sexuality illustrate the ways in which this technique can illuminate research on gender differences. Meta-analysis can make feminist transformations in psychology by: (a) challenging long-standing beliefs in gender differences, (b) demonstrating the extent to which gendered behavior is context-dependent and the product of gender roles, (c) examining the intersection between gender and race/ethnicity, and (d) providing powerful data to counter assertions of difference and female inferiority that proliferate in the popular media.